Skip to main content
Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine : JCSM : Official Publication of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine logoLink to Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine : JCSM : Official Publication of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine
. 2020 Jan 15;16(1):121–128. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.8136

Simple behavioral criteria for the diagnosis of disorders of arousal

Ariana Barros 1,2, Ginevra Uguccioni 1, Victoire Salkin-Goux 1, Smaranda Leu-Semenescu 1,3, Pauline Dodet 1,3, Isabelle Arnulf 1,3,
PMCID: PMC7053005  PMID: 31957639

Abstract

Study Objectives:

This case-control study aimed to identify and validate behavioral markers supporting the diagnosis of disorders of arousal (DOA) with video polysomnography.

Methods:

All behaviors associated with 1,335 episodes of N3 interruptions were compared in 52 adult patients with DOA versus 52 participants without DOA (healthy control patients and patients with insomnia, hypersomnia, or sleep apnea syndrome).

Results:

Patients with DOA had more frequent (5.1 ± 2.4 versus 3.4 ± 1.9 interruptions/N3 time) and longer (35.8 ± 33 versus 23.1 ± 21.4 sec) arousals and awakenings from N3 than control patients. In the DOA group, the onset of behaviors was more abrupt, and behaviors including eye opening (69% versus 16%), head raising (41% versus 9%), visually exploring the environment (27% versus 1%), expression of fear/surprise (21% versus zero), speaking (18% versus 0.3%), trunk raising (13% versus 0.3%), and interacting with the environment (13% versus 0.5%), were (unlike quiet, comfort behaviors) more frequent than in control patients. A cutoff of two or more N3 interruptions containing eye opening yielded a sensitivity of 94.2% and a specificity of 76.9% for a DOA diagnosis. This accuracy was confirmed in a second set of data (second night of monitoring). Behaviors including an expression of fear/surprise (67.3%), sitting (32.7%), screaming, and standing up were specific to patients with DOA.

Conclusions:

A simple, behavioral video marker of behavioral reactions during N3 interruption (ie, opening the eyes at least two times in the same night) was sensitive, specific, and reproducible for discriminating patients with DOA from sleep laboratory control patients.

Clinical Trial Registration:

This study is a surrogate study of NCT02648568 and NCT03074578 on ClinicalTrials.gov.

Citation:

Barros A, Uguccioni G, Salkin-Goux V, Leu-Semenescu S, Dodet P, Arnulf I. Simple behavioral criteria for the diagnosis of arousal disorders. J Clin Sleep Med. 2020;16(1):121–128.

Keywords: behaviors, diagnosis, disorders of arousal, eye opening, sleepwalking, sleep terrors, video-polysomnography


BRIEF SUMMARY

Current Knowledge/Study Rationale: No behavioral criterion supporting the diagnosis of sleepwalking and/or sleep terrors has been validated with video polysomnography. Consequently, we compared the behaviors upon awakening/arousing from N3 sleep in 52 adults with sleepwalking with those in 52 adults with other sleep disorders or healthy participants.

Study Impact: A cutoff of two or more interruptions of N3 sleep with eye opening accurately discriminated adults with sleepwalking from other patients and healthy participants, providing a simple and sensitive criterion to support diagnosis in clinical practice. Only sleepwalkers displayed expressions of fear/surprise, sat, screamed, and stood up during N3 interruptions, suggesting that these specific criteria could be used for forensic purposes.

INTRODUCTION

Disorders of arousal (DOA, including sleepwalking, sleep terrors and confusional arousals) are characterized by inappropriate, mostly amnestic behaviors occurring during partial arousals from N3 sleep, reflecting various degrees of motor, emotional, and autonomic activations. The disorders expose people to risks of trauma, violence, and uncontrolled behaviors that may be life threatening. In addition, patients with DOA have more difficulties initiating and maintaining sleep, a lower quality of life, higher levels of anxiety, and more daytime fatigue and sleepiness than healthy control patients.13 Disorders of arousal are common in children but may persist (and sometimes appear) in up to 4% of adults.4

The diagnosis is mostly based on interviews of the patients and their family, as indicated in the International Classification of Sleep Disorders, Third Edition.5 In contrast to many other sleep disorders (eg, sleep apnea syndrome, rapid eye movement [REM] sleep behavior disorder, periodic leg movements disorder, and central hypersomnias), in which sleep and sometimes video monitoring are mandatory to reach the diagnosis, the role of video polysomnography in the diagnosis process of DOA is debated and not required based on the current criteria.5 Ambulation and complex behaviors during N3 arousals are rare in the sleep laboratory, and some authors suggest that “normal” polysomnographies are sometimes observed in participants with typical symptoms of DOA. However, a sleep study may document simple behaviors typical of confusional arousals, rule out differential diagnoses (REM sleep behavior disorder, epilepsy, sleep-related motor disorders) and identify potential triggers of parasomniac episodes, such as noises, sleep-related breathing disorders, or periodic leg movements. In addition, there is an unmet need (for clinical and forensic purposes) to determine the sensitivity and specificity of the signs observed in a sleep study. Some electroencephalography (EEG) markers (hypersynchronous delta sleep activities and spectral analysis) lack specificity, but recently, an index of arousals plus awakenings (greater than 6.8 per N3 time) has proven sensitive (79%) and specific (82%) for distinguishing patients referred for DOA from healthy participants.6 A slow/mixed arousal index (greater than 2.5 per N3 time) was even more sensitive (94%) and totally specific (100%). However, this analysis requires highly trained sleep scorers and 8 EEG channels.

We aimed to identify easy video-based criteria to distinguish patients with DOA from healthy control patients and from patients with other sleep disorders. For this purpose, we analyzed the behaviors during N3 arousals/awakenings in patients with DOA versus those in control patients (healthy control patients and patients with insomnia, hypersomnia, and sleep apnea syndrome) to find simple, sensitive, and specific markers of DOA.

METHODS

Study design and population

This is a retrospective, case–control study. The participants were selected from the patient and control populations of the sleep disorder center at the Pitié-Salpêtrière University Hospital. The patients with DOA were recruited between January 2016 and July 2018 and the diagnosis of sleepwalking or sleep terrors was made by the sleep physicians using the international criteria.5 They met criteria for disorders of arousal, including (1) recurrent episodes of incomplete awakening from sleep, (2) inappropriate or absent responsiveness to efforts of others to intervene or redirect the person during the episode, (3) limited or no associated cognition or dream imagery, (4) partial or complete amnesia for the episode, and (5) no better explanation for the disturbance, such as another sleep disorder, a mental disorder, a medical condition, or medication or substance use. In addition to the aforementioned criteria, patients were classified as experiencing sleepwalking if there was (1) a history of ambulation during sleep or (2) the persistence of sleep or impaired judgment during ambulation. In addition to the aforementioned criteria, patients were classified as having sleep terrors if there was (1) a history of sudden episodes of terror occurring during sleep, usually initiated by a cry or loud scream with sympathetic and behavioral manifestations of intense fear, and (2) difficulty in arousing the person, mental confusion when awakened from an episode, complete or partial amnesia from the episode, or dangerous or potentially dangerous behaviors. Because the comparison of behaviors arising during a confusional arousal in N3 and during a sleep-associated (whatever the sleep stage) epileptic seizure has already been studied,7 the patients with positive clinical history of epilepsy were excluded in both groups (DOA and control).

The control group comprised healthy participants and participants with sleep disorders but no DOA. The healthy participants had been recruited by word of mouth for another trial (COREV) that included a video and sleep study and had no sleep complaints, especially current parasomnia, as checked by an interview with the physician. The control population with sleep disorders had to be representative of the source population from which the DOA cases were extracted. For this purpose, it consisted of: (1) all consecutive participants who were referred to our sleep unit from May 2018 to July 2018; (2) those who had no DOA; (3) those who had no positive clinical history of epilepsy; (4) those who did not take any psychotropic treatment for at least several months; (5) those who were matched for age and sex with the DOA patient; and (6) those who had at least one awakening episode during N3 sleep (in order to be able to observe at least one behavior associated with one N3 interruption). This control group was matched (as a group and not 1 to 1) for age and sex with the DOA group.

Ethics

The patients with DOA and the healthy control patients gave their written consent to participate in the study, which was approved by the ethics committee Ile de France-06 as part of a larger study on hypnosis in sleepwalking (NCT02648568 on ClinicalTrials.gov) and on emotions (NCT03074578 on ClinicalTrials.gov). Following the French regulation on clinical research, the other part of the group (control patients) received before, during, and after their stay the oral and written information that their deidentified measures could be later reused by investigators for research purposes (unless they refused this use) and did not oppose that. The center had received the agreement MR003 (declaration 1999732) from the National Commission for Rights and Freedom related to Informatics (CNIL) since October 2016 for storing and reusing for research purposes deidentified clinical, polysomnographic, and video/audio measures performed in clinical routine in the sleep laboratory.

Video polysomnography

All participants completed at least 1 night with video polysomnography at the sleep unit, but the 52 patients with DOA and 16 healthy control patients underwent 2 consecutive night studies. All rooms were equipped with a single infrared camera (synchronized with the audio recording and the polysomnography), placed in front of the bed. Lights were switched off by the patients by 11:00 PM at the latest and remained off until they were turned on by staff the next morning. The recordings included at least three channels for electroencephalography (EEG; Fp1/C3, C3/A2 and C3/O1; extensive 8-electroencephalophic-channel montage in patients with DOA), two channels for electrooculography (inferior left epicanthus/A2 and superior right epicanthus/A2) and three channels for surface electromyography (chin [mentalis] muscle, and right and left tibialis anterior muscles) as well as an electrocardiogram, thoracic and abdominal belts to measure the respiratory efforts via inductive plethysmography, a measure of naso-oral airflow through nasal pressure and oral thermistors, and a measure of transcutaneous oxygen saturation. The sleep stages, arousals, apnea and hypopnea events, and periodic leg movements were scored by experienced sleep neurologists according to international criteria.8

Analysis of behaviors

All interruptions of stage N3 sleep were collected. They included classic awakenings (alpha or higher EEG background rhythm lasting longer than 15 seconds), as well as arousals (lasting 3 to 15 seconds) with an acceleration of the background EEG frequency, which was mostly in the alpha rhythm but also (especially in patients with DOA) in a theta rhythm or a mixed theta-alpha rhythm. Additionally, we included the “slow-wave arousals” characterized by sudden events with persistent delta waves (0.5–3 Hz) on central derivations associated with increased muscle activity and lasting more than 3 seconds, most of which had a high amplitude (named hypersynchronous delta activity9). The behaviors concomitant with these N3 arousals/awakenings were observed on the video/audio, and the following behaviors were scored by two scorers: (1) eye opening, (2) staring or exploring the environment, (3) head raising, (4) trunk raising, (5) sitting, (6) standing up/walking, (7) speaking, (8) screaming, (9) showing any facial expression of fear or surprise, (10) interacting with the environment, and (11) making usual comfort arousal movements (ie, rubbing, yawning, repositioning oneself). The additional features collected for each episode included the triggering event (present/absent; the nature of the trigger), the onset (abrupt or slow) and duration of the episode (defined from onset to the end of any motor activity). These categories were chosen according to local experience of their high frequency in DOA and to previous studies of behaviors in patients with DOA versus epilepsy7 or control patients.10 The scorer was not blinded to the DOA/non-DOA diagnosis of each participant.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics were used to describe continuous variables, and qualitative measures were presented as frequencies and percentages. The comparisons between patients were performed using the t test (quantitative measures) and chi-square test or Fisher exact test (qualitative measures). The levels of agreement between raters were tested in 20 randomly selected, representative cases using Cohen kappa for nominal (qualitative) variables and an interrater correlation coefficient for quantitative variables. Values of Cohen kappa and interrater correlations greater than 0.8 indicate excellent agreement (almost perfect agreement). The association between quantitative measures was tested using Pearson correlation coefficient or Spearman correlation coefficient (in cases where the normality assumption was not verified). All tests were two-sided, and a value of P < .05 was considered statistically significant. Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis was applied to evaluate the best cutoff value for diagnostic performance of the criteria to provide the best pair of values for sensitivity and specificity. To compare the diagnostic accuracy of the results, the area under the curve (AUC) with a 95% confidence interval (CI) was calculated. Likelihood ratios (LRs) for positive test results and accuracy (number of correct results divided by the total number of tests) were provided as a measure of diagnostic accuracy. The best possible threshold was defined as the highest Youden index [(specificity + sensitivity) − 1]. Diagnostic criteria based on cutoffs identified by the ROC analysis were evaluated for each test individually and for each test combined with the number of N3 interruptions/awakenings (considered positive for a participant who was positive on each test). The association between test results and the presence of the disease was evaluated by the Pearson test. The Rasch model was conducted with behavioral features of N3 interruptions to examine the model fit statistics, namely, the information-weighted goodness-of-fit statistics (INFIT), and the ordering of the behavior categories. All analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics for Mac (Version 23.0. Armonk, New York, United States).

RESULTS

Clinical and sleep characteristics of the participants

Fifty-two patients with DOA and 52 control patients (23 healthy participants, 10 patients with sleep apnea syndrome [range of apnea-hypopnea index, 15.2–90.1 events/h], 11 patients with idiopathic hypersomnia and 8 patients with insomnia, as randomly determined) were enrolled in the study. As expected by matching, age and sex were not different between the DOA and control groups (Table 1). In the DOA group, most patients (n = 40, 76.9%) had combined sleepwalking and sleep terrors, 7 (13.5%) had isolated sleepwalking, and 5 (9.6%) had isolated sleep terrors. Associated confusional arousal was diagnosed in 12 of these patients (23.1%). Sleep talking was reported by 49 patients (94.2%). The age (mean ± standard deviation) at DOA onset was 10.8 ± 5.4 years, and the disease duration was 19.9 ± 10.6 years. Twenty-nine patients (55.8%) reported having had violent or dangerous sleep-related behaviors at home. A positive family history of DOA was reported in 69.2% of patients. The total score on the Paris Arousal Disorders Severity Scale11 was 16.9 ± 5.0, the behavior subscore A was 8.0 ± 3.8, the subscore B (frequency of the behaviors) was 4.2 ± 1.1, and the subscore C (effect of the DOA) was 4.7 ± 1.4. Neurologic and psychiatric comorbidities were equally rare in the DOA (zero and 1.9%, respectively) and control (1.9% and 3.8%, respectively) groups. These comorbidities included migraine, anxiety, and depression. The EEG tracings during relaxed wakefulness and sleep stages did not disclose any paroxysmal activity in patients with DOA or in control patients. Compared with control patients, patients with DOA had longer total sleep time, higher sleep efficiency, higher REM sleep percentage, and a lower apnea-hypopnea index, as well as higher stage shifts and awakening indices (Table 1). Patients with DOA had, on average, a higher number and index of N3 interruptions, awakenings, and arousals than control patients (Table 1). The between-scorer agreement for the quantification of N3 interruptions was 0.99 (95% CI = 0.966–0.995).

Table 1.

Sleep measures in patients with disorders of arousal versus control patients (healthy participants and participants with sleep disorders but no disorders of arousal).

graphic file with name jcsm.16.1.121t1.jpg

Behavioral characteristics of N3 interruptions

The interruptions from N3 sleep were analyzed in 172 night recordings and totaled 1,335 interruptions (953 in patients with DOA and 382 in control patients). The N3 interruptions lasted longer in the DOA than in the control group (35.8 ± 33 versus 23.1 ± 21.4 seconds, P < .0001). As many as 913 N3 interruptions in the DOA group (95.8%) and 365 in the control group (95.5%) were accompanied by behaviors. In 6% of the cases, the behaviors “eye opening” “exploring the environment” and “expression of fear/surprise” could not be defined from the video images, because the patients were sleeping in the prone position. An abrupt onset of motor behavior, defined as a behavior starting at the very moment of the sleep interruption, was observed in 622 of interruptions in the DOA group (65.3%) and in none of the control group (P < .0001), whereas the motor behavior started several seconds after the N3 interruption in the control group. In all groups, 170 episodes of N3 interruptions (12.7%) were triggered by a precipitating event (with a similar frequency between groups), including ambient noises (65.7%), limb movement or myoclonus (14.5%), nurse intervention (7.6%), deep inspiration/cough (4.7%), snoring (4.1%), and apnea-hypopnea/flow limitation (3.5%). Patients with DOA opened their eyes during 653 episodes of N3 interruption (68.9%), whereas control patients opened their eyes during 59 episodes (15.5%) (Figure 1). The N3 interruptions triggered by internal stimuli (apnea, hypopnea, cough, leg movement/myoclonus) led to less frequent eye openings (14/48; 29.2%) than those triggered by external stimuli (noises, contacts), which led to eye openings in 77/124 interruptions (62.1%, P < .0001). The patients with DOA reacted more to external triggers by opening the eyes (64/90, 71.1%) than did the control patients (13/34, 38.2%, P = .0008). All types of behaviors described in Figure 1, which accompanied the N3 interruptions, were more frequent in the DOA group than in the control group (P < .0001 for each difference), including in decreasing order of frequency: head raising, visually exploring the environment, speaking, trunk raising, and interacting with the environment. Some behaviors were exclusively observed in the DOA group, including facial expressions of fear or surprise (20.7%), sitting (8.6%), screaming (3.4%), and standing up (0.3%). In contrast, usual, quiet movements (repositioning, yawning, stretching, rubbing nose or some other body part) were more frequent in the control group (n = 355, 92.9% of N3 interruptions) than in the DOA group (n = 790, 81.9%, P < .0001). Notably, the rapid arousing motor pattern (eye opening and head and trunk raising) described in Figure 1, when present, preceded the quiet usual movements in the DOA group. According to the Rasch model, there was a hierarchical structure of behaviors predicting the “propensity” for DOA during the N3 interruptions: eye opening was the “easiest” criteria (lowest propensity), and standing up/walking was the “hardest” (highest propensity), in the same order as the frequency of these behaviors (Table S1 in the supplemental material). The factor analysis (INFIT values between 0.7–1.4) of the behaviors supports their unidimensionality.

Figure 1. Type and frequency of behavior occurring during N3 interruptions (n = 1,335) in patients with disorders of arousal (plain bars) and control patients (empty bars).

Figure 1

Quantification of behaviors

Although these N3 interruptions were similarly spontaneous or triggered by an event in the two groups, the frequency of the corresponding behaviors was markedly different between groups (Table 2), including a higher number and index of eye opening, visually exploring the environment, head raising, trunk raising, sitting, speaking, screaming, expressing fear/surprise, interacting with the environment, and engaging in usual arousal movements in the DOA group. The interrater agreement for identifying the episodes of eye openings was 97.7% (κ = 0.94, 95% CI = 0.84–0.96). The other κ coefficients for individual behavior classification were close to 0.9 or above for all other components. The number of eye openings positively correlated with the number of other behaviors, except for the number of screaming and sitting behaviors.

Table 2.

Behavioral characteristics during N3 interruptions (night 1) in patients with disorders of arousal versus control patients.

graphic file with name jcsm.16.1.121t2.jpg

Cutoff identification

The results of the ROC analysis for markers are shown in Table 3 and Figure 2. The highest AUC (measure of discriminative ability of the test) was obtained for the number of interruptions with eye opening (AUC = 0.93 [95% CI, 0.89–0.98]), the number of N3 awakenings (0.86 [95% CI, 0.78–0.93]), and the number of N3 interruptions (AUC = 0.77 [95% CI, 0.685–0.864]). The best pair of values for sensitivity and specificity was found at a cutoff value of six or more N3 interruptions per night (LR+ = 2.27, accuracy = 77.1%) and 6 or more N3 awakenings per night (LR+ = 3.62, accuracy = 77.9%). For the number of episodes with eye opening, the optimal cutoff point was two per night (LR+ = 4.08, accuracy = 85.6%). Of the behaviors that were 100% specific to the DOA group during the first night, 35/52 patients (67.3%) displayed a facial expression of fear/surprise at least once, 17/52 (32.7%) sat in the bed, 10/52 (19.2%) screamed, and 2/52 (3.8%) stood up and walked.

Table 3.

Optimal cutoffs of N3 interruptions for a diagnosis of disorders of arousal, evaluated with receiver operating characteristic curves.

graphic file with name jcsm.16.1.121t3.jpg

Figure 2. Receiver operating characteristic analyses for various markers during N3 interruptions.

Figure 2

Cutoff application and test combination

With the application of the identified cutoff values (Table 4), two or more episodes of N3 interruptions with eye opening yielded a sensitivity of 94.2% and specificity of 76.9%. When this number of eye openings was combined with at least six N3 interruptions (whether arousal and/or awakenings), the sensitivity was 75%, and the specificity was 88.5%. When the number of eye openings was associated with at least six N3 awakenings, the sensitivity was 67.3%, but the specificity reached 94.2%. To retest the cutoffs in a second set of data, their accuracy was tested in the second night (52 nights of patients with DOA and 16 nights of control patients). The cutoff of two N3 interruptions with eye opening yielded a sensitivity of 94.2% and a specificity of 85.7%, which was better than during the first night.

Table 4.

Diagnostic accuracy of behavioral and electroencephalography markers for disorders of arousal.

graphic file with name jcsm.16.1.121t4.jpg

DISCUSSION

In this controlled study, patients with DOA had more frequent and longer arousals and awakenings from N3 than did control patients. Notably, the behaviors observed during the 1,335 episodes of N3 interruptions differed between groups. In the DOA group, the onset of behaviors was more abrupt (starting at the exact time of N3 interruption), and eye opening, head raising, visually exploring the environment, speaking, trunk raising, and interacting with the environment (in descending order of frequency) were more frequent than in control patients, whereas the frequency of quiet behaviors usually associated with comfort did not differ between groups. Using ROC curves, a cutoff of two or more N3 interruptions containing eye opening yielded a sensitivity of 94.2% and a specificity of 76.9% for a diagnosis of DOA. A similarly excellent sensitivity (94.2%) and specificity (88.7%) was confirmed in a second set of data. Combined markers (ie, six or more N3 awakenings in the whole night, combined with two or more eye openings during these interruptions) yielded a higher specificity (94%).

Motor phenomena during sleep have been the topic of intensive studies since the introduction of video polysomnography. The pattern of behaviors associated with N3 interruptions has been routinely observed in sleep laboratories for more than 50 years, but it has been more rarely quantified and compared to that in control patients. It has been previously studied in 30 adult sleepwalkers and 10 control patients.10 The behaviors had an abrupt onset in 55% of the episodes, which is close to the 65.3% found in our series.

Eye opening is the most frequent behavior associated with N3 interruptions in patients with DOA, as it was observed in 84% of 57 episodes in an Australian series,7 in 76% of 184 episodes in an Italian series,10 and in 68.9% of 953 episodes in our series. Of interest, eye opening also occurred during N3 interruptions in control patients (15%) here and in one of the eight head flexion/extension movements in the Italian series,10 but opening the eyes more than one time per night during N3 interruption was exceptional, to the point of providing a marker with a high sensitivity and specificity for a DOA diagnosis. Additionally, this marker was reproduced with a higher specificity/sensitivity in a second set of data, was simple to recognize, and had excellent interrater agreement. Because our control group included not only healthy control patients but also patients with sleep apnea, hypersomnia, and insomnia, this marker could be a useful indicator (due to its excellent sensitivity) for the diagnosis of DOA in a sleep clinic context. However, when nocturnal behaviors were compared between patients with DOA and patients with nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy in an Australian series, episodes with eye opening did not discriminate the groups, as 97% of frontal seizures was associated with an opening of the eyes.7 Therefore, this criterion can be used for discriminating patients with DOA from usual sleep clinic patients and from healthy control patients, but not from patients with sleep-related hypermotor epilepsy.

The specificity of this “two episodes of eye openings” marker did not reach 100% in our group and therefore cannot be used for forensic purposes. Patients with DOA more frequently opened the eyes (72%) in reaction to an external stimulus (noise, contact) triggering the N3 interruption than did control patients (38%), suggesting that patients with DOA are more reactive (at least if one considers eye opening as a motor, orientating and eventually fear behavior) than are control patients. This observation of a higher reactivity to external stimuli in patients with DOA was obtained in a clinical setting. Similarly, it has been previously shown that forced arousals provoked by strong auditory stimuli trigger abnormal confusional reactions (clumsy movements, sleep talking) in patients with DOA but not in healthy control patients.12

The pattern of behaviors accompanying N3 interruption often extended beyond the simple, immediate opening of the eyes. In patients with DOA (and much more exceptionally in control patients), the behavioral pattern was more typical of a general arousal/orientation behavior, which includes the behavior of raising the head and trunk, as well as staring, looking around, speaking, and touching things around them. Because these events follow a hierarchical order (ie, the probability that the eye is open when the head or trunk is raised is high), as shown here by the Rasch model, these behaviors may result from the activation of a central pattern generator of arousal behavior. When present, this abrupt arousing motor pattern strongly contrasts with the quiet movements associated with comfort (stretching, rubbing, repositioning oneself) performed in both groups but appear after the abrupt initial arousal pattern in the DOA group.

A facial expression of fear or surprise was observed in 20% of N3 interruptions, further reinforcing the feeling of an explosive, surprised behavior, which seems specific to patients with DOA, as expressions of fear/surprise were not observed in the control group. A similar pattern of arousal behaviors has been described at the onset of nocturnal frontal lobe seizures, making paroxysmal arousals indistinguishable from confusional arousals.7 However, we show here that these arousal behaviors are markedly distinguishable from the usual, quiet behaviors associated with arousals and awakenings from N3 in healthy participants and in patients with nonparasomniac sleep disorders (without epilepsy).

For forensic purposes, highly specific rather than highly sensitive cutoffs can be sought, to be able to determine with a high level of certainty that a participant has a DOA. In this case, several behaviors were exclusively observed in patients with DOA here (100% specific) and never observed in patients with other sleep disorders. These behaviors included showing a facial expression of fear/surprise upon N3 interruption (displayed by more than two-thirds of patients with DOA), sitting (one-third of patients), and screaming (19%). As often reported, ambulation is exceptional in the sleep laboratory, as here only 2/52 patients stood up and walked during the first night of recordings.

A recent study proposed the first video polysomnography-based criteria for the diagnosis of DOA in adults, based on quantification of the N3 interruptions (N3 fragmentation index) and the postarousal EEG pattern (fast and slow/mixed arousal indices).6 In this study, the N3 fragmentation index cutoff value of 6.8 per hour reached a sensitivity of 79% and a specificity of 82%. In our study, this cutoff of 6.8 per hour was too high, as it resulted in a lower sensitivity (19.2%), whereas the “best” cutoff of 3.6 per hour had a sensitivity of 69.2% and specificity of 59.6%. Eventually, a number of six or more N3 interruptions (a number rather than an index, which is possibly easier to collect) yielded a high level of accuracy in our series. Sample fluctuations (the sample size of Lopez and colleagues was larger than ours)6 and differences in inclusion criteria (our control patients were required to have at least one N3 interruption to describe the associated behaviors, whereas this was not an inclusion criteria in the other sample) may explain these different results. Notably, the duration of N3 interruptions was longer in patients with DOA than in control patients, suggesting that the reactivity during N3 interruption is higher not only in terms of the complexity of the behavioral response in these patients but also in the time it takes to resume sleeping.

Our study presents some methodological limitations, such as a reasonable but limited number of participants in each group. However, more than 1,300 episodes of N3 interruption were carefully examined on video, which constitutes one of the largest behavioral studies in this field. The eyes are not visible when the participants sleep in the front position, which sometimes (here in 6% of N3 interruptions) limits the ability to use the two-eye-opening marker. In these instances, the surrogate markers can be the number of awakenings from N3. Patients with nocturnal epilepsy were not included in this study. They are known to open the eyes when a seizure starts at night. However, seizures mainly take place in N1 and N2 stages, and rarely in N3,9 which may help to distinguish DOA from seizures more easily than behavioral criteria. Eventually, the scorer was not blinded to the diagnosis of DOA in participants when analyzing the videoclips.

In conclusion, we found that a simple, video polysomnography-based marker of behavior reaction during N3 interruption (ie, eye opening at least two times in the same night) was sensitive, specific, and reproducible in the discrimination of patients with DOA from sleep laboratory control patients (more than half of the control group were patients with sleep disorders), but not from sleep-related hypermotor epilepsy. This cutoff value allowed the correct classification of 85.6% of patients. Other simple markers (six or more N3 awakenings or N3 interruptions per night) isolated or in combination with eye openings were also accurate. These markers could be used in clinical settings to support the DOA diagnosis. Eventually, this behavioral work could be used in the future to develop methods based on automatic video analysis (even at home) to classify patients.

DISCLOSURE STATEMENT

All authors have seen and approved the manuscript. Work for this study was performed at Pitié -Salpêtrière Hospital, Sleep Disorders Unit, Paris, France. The HYPNOSOM study was financed by a grant from the Paris Hospital (grant P130603, promotor: APHP) to IA. The authors report no conflicts of interest.

ABBREVIATIONS

AUC

area under the curve

CI

confidence interval

DOA

disorders of arousal

INFIT

information-weighted goodness-of-fit statistics

LR

likelihood ratio

REM

rapid eye movement

ROC

receiver operating characteristic.

jcsm.16.1.121.SD1.pdf (40.6KB, pdf)

REFERENCES

  • 1.Lopez R, Jaussent I, Scholz S, Bayard S, Montplaisir J, Dauvilliers Y. Functional impairment in adult sleepwalkers: a case-control study. Sleep. 2013;36(3):345–351. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Carrillo-Solano M, Leu-Semenescu S, Golmard JL, Groos E, Arnulf I. Sleepiness in sleepwalking and sleep terrors: a higher sleep pressure? Sleep Med. 2016;26:54–59. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2015.11.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Oudiette D, Leu S, Pottier M, Buzare MA, Brion A, Arnulf I. Dreamlike mentations during sleepwalking and sleep terrors in adults. Sleep. 2009;32(12):1621–1627. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.12.1621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Ohayon MM, Mahowald MW, Dauvilliers Y, Krystal AD, Leger D. Prevalence and comorbidity of nocturnal wandering in the U.S. adult general population. Neurology. 2012;78(20):1583–1589. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182563be5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. International Classification of Sleep Disorders. 3rd ed. Darien, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Lopez R, Shen Y, Chenini S, et al. Diagnostic criteria for disorders of arousal: a video-polysomnographic assessment. Ann Neurol. 2018;83(2):341–351. doi: 10.1002/ana.25153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Derry CP, Harvey AS, Walker MC, Duncan JS, Berkovic SF. NREM arousal parasomnias and their distinction from nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy: a video EEG analysis. Sleep. 2009;32(12):1637–1644. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.12.1637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Iber C, Ancoli-Israel S, Chesson AL, Jr, Quan SF. for the American Academy of Sleep Medicine. The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology and Technical Specifications. 1st ed. Westchester, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Guilleminault C. Hypersynchronous slow delta, cyclic alternating pattern and sleepwalking. Sleep. 2006;29(1):14–15. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Loddo G, Sessagesimi E, Mignani F, et al. Specific motor patterns of arousal disorders in adults: a video-polysomnographic analysis of 184 episodes. Sleep Med. 2018;41:102–109. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2017.08.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Arnulf I, Zhang B, Uguccioni G, et al. A scale for assessing the severity of arousal disorders. Sleep. 2014;37(1):127–136. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Pilon M, Montplaisir J, Zadra A. Precipitating factors of somnambulism: impact of sleep deprivation and forced arousals. Neurology. 2008;70(24):2284–2290. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000304082.49839.86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

jcsm.16.1.121.SD1.pdf (40.6KB, pdf)

Articles from Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine : JCSM : Official Publication of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine are provided here courtesy of American Academy of Sleep Medicine

RESOURCES