Abstract
Prior research has identified two resting EEG biomarkers with potential for predicting functional outcomes in depression: theta current density in frontal brain regions (especially rostral anterior cingulate cortex) and alpha power over posterior scalp regions. As little is known about the discriminant and convergent validity of these putative biomarkers, a thorough evaluation of these psychometric properties was conducted toward the goal of improving clinical utility of these markers. Resting 71-channel EEG recorded from 35 healthy adults at two sessions (one-week retest) were used to systematically compare different quantification techniques for theta and alpha sources at scalp (surface Laplacian or current source density [CSD]) and brain (distributed inverse; exact low resolution electromagnetic tomography [eLORETA]) level. Signal quality was evaluated with signal-to-noise ratio, participant-level spectra, and frequency PCA covariance decomposition. Convergent and discriminant validity were assessed within a multitrait-multimethod framework. Posterior alpha was reliably identified as two spectral components, each with unique spatial patterns and condition effects (eyes open/closed), high signal quality, and good convergent and discriminant validity. In contrast, frontal theta was characterized by one low-variance component, low signal quality, lack of a distinct spectral peak, and mixed validity. Correlations between candidate biomarkers suggest that posterior alpha components constitute reliable, convergent, and discriminant biometrics in healthy adults. Component-based identification of spectral activity (CSD/eLORETA-fPCA) was superior to fixed, a priori frequency bands. Improved quantification and conceptualization of frontal theta is necessary to determine clinical utility.
Keywords: EEG biomarkers, Theta/alpha oscillations, Validity, Frequency PCA, Current Source Density (CSD), Source Localization (LORETA)
1. Introduction
Identifying disease-specific biomarkers that elucidate pathophysiology and predict treatment outcomes is a key target for clinical neuroscience. Although there has been significant progress in this regard (Kühn et al., 2009; Light et al., 2015; Monti et al., 2010; Pizzagalli et al., 2018; Weinberg, Riesel, & Hajcak, 2012) methodological heterogeneity (i.e., identification and quantification of biomarkers) presents a significant barrier to cross-study comparison, interpretation of results, replicability, and clinical application. For major depressive disorder (MDD), several electroencephalographic (EEG) biomarkers have shown promise for predicting treatment outcomes (for reviews, see Bruder, Tenke, & Kayser, 2013; Iosifescu, 2011; Widge et al., 2018). Two candidate biomarkers that rely on resting EEG activity are currently under investigation as part of a large multisite study (Trivedi et al., 2016): rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC) theta, identified via a current density distributed inverse solution (low resolution brain electromagnetic tomography [LORETA]; e.g., Pizzagalli et al., 2001, 2018) and posterior alpha, identified via scalp current source density (CSD; surface Laplacian) and frequency principal components analysis (fPCA; e.g., Tenke et al., 2011). These two biomarkers have demonstrated moderate predictive validity (Pizzagalli et al., 2018; Tenke et al., 2011; Widge et al., 2018), but in general, published results on EEG biomarkers are biased towards small studies with large effect sizes and positive results (Widge et al., 2018). Moreover, there is a notable absence of research evaluating techniques for biomarker quantification, discriminant validity between biomarkers, and convergent validity across methodologies. This study aimed to investigate questions regarding quantification and validity of these two candidate biomarkers.
1.1. Research findings on rACC theta as a candidate biomarker
There is an ongoing effort to identify biomarkers that predict which depressed patients will improve following treatment (e.g., Pizzagalli, 2011; Waters & Mayberg, 2017). Among those, greater theta-band activity in the rACC has been repeatedly linked to favorable clinical outcome (Pizzagalli, 2011; Pizzagalli et al., 2001, 2018). Specifically, enhanced theta magnitude in the rACC, and also when measured at frontal scalp locations (i.e., midfrontal theta [MF θ]), predicted symptom remission of MDD patients who received antidepressant medications (e.g., selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors [SSRIs]; Hunter, Korb, Cook, & Leuchter, 2013; Korb, Hunter, Cook, & Leuchter, 2009; Mulert et al., 2007; Pizzagalli et al., 2001, 2018; Rentzsch, Adli, Wiethoff, Gómez-Carrillo de Castro, & Gallinat, 2014), noninvasive (i.e., transcranial magnetic stimulation [TMS]; Li et al., 2016) or invasive neuromodulation (i.e., implantation of a deep-brain stimulator [DBS]; Broadway et al., 2012), and also of those assigned to a placebo control group (Pizzagalli et al., 2018). These findings suggest that frontal/rACC theta may be a nonspecific predictor of symptom improvement in individuals with MDD. It is hypothesized that individuals with low rACC theta may have difficulty with cognitive control, and consequently these individuals are more likely to have a poor clinical prognosis (e.g., Mayberg, 1997; Pizzagalli, 2011; Waters & Mayberg, 2017). Notably, two recent multisite studies reported contradictory results. Pizzagalli et al. (2018) found a positive relationship between pretreatment rACC theta and symptom improvement in a large sample of depressed patients (N = 248), indicating that MDD patients with more pretreatment rACC theta were more likely to improve after 8 weeks regardless of treatment arm (placebo vs. SSRI; for additional independent replications, see Korb et al., 2009; Mulert et al., 2007; Pizzagalli et al., 2001; Rentzsch et al., 2014). In contrast, employing an even larger sample of MDD patients (N = 1008), Arns et al. (2015) found that less pretreatment rACC theta was associated with improvement, that is, a relationship between rACC theta and symptom change opposite the expected direction. These findings prompted us to examine whether different methods of rACC theta quantification could explain these inconsistencies.
Many reports have noted that frontal theta activity is weak and infrequent in resting EEG recordings of wakeful participants (e.g., Barry & De Blasio, 2018; Cigánek, 1961; Frauscher et al., 2018; Keitel & Gross, 2016; Schacter, 1977; Shackman, McMenamin, Maxwell, Greischar, & Davidson, 2010; Tenke & Kayser, 2005; Westmoreland & Klass, 1986), meaning there is no continuous or dominant theta rhythm except for limited bursts that can be obscured by FFT averaging (e.g., Vidaurre et al., 2018). This evidence indicates that only a fraction of resting EEG recordings contain notable oscillations below 8 Hz in wakeful resting participants (Arns, Gordon, & Boutros, 2017; Keitel & Gross, 2016; Schacter, 1977; Vidaurre et al., 2018; Westmoreland & Klass, 1986). In fact, frontal theta is inversely related to default-mode network activity (Scheeringa et al., 2008). When theta is apparent in resting EEG, it is often associated with greater drowsiness and ocular artifact (Mcmenamin et al., 2010; Schacter, 1977; Strijkstra, Beersma, Drayer, Halbesma, & Daan, 2003). Moreover, data-driven approaches (i.e., principal components analysis [PCA]) often fail to identify a spectral theta component in resting EEG recordings (Barry & De Blasio, 2018; Shackman et al., 2010; Tenke & Kayser, 2005). Variation in theta amplitude as a function of EEG hardware and personnel further underscores the subtlety of this metric (Tenke et al., 2017). Based on early factor analyses identifying functionally independent EEG frequency ranges (Kubicki et al., 1979), several studies have evaluated rACC theta as a candidate biomarker by quantifying spectral activity between 6.5 and 8.0 Hz (Arns et al., 2015; Pizzagalli et al., 2001; Mulert et al., 2007); however, this limited frequency range deviates from conventional definition of theta (Chatrian et al., 1974; Kane et al., 2017) and it may also be more prone to assessing a mixture of theta and alpha oscillations. What may ultimately be even more important is that a dominating theta rhythm, which is observed during task performance involving working memory load or cognitive control, peaks at 5 Hz (e.g., Cavanagh & Shackman, 2015). Phasic and induced theta oscillations are prominent at 5 Hz over midfrontal regions, and are consistently related to cognitive and behavioral performance (Cohen & van Gaal, 2014; Hsieh & Ranganath, 2014; Li et al., 2016; Näpflin, Wildi, & Sarnthein, 2008; Olvet & Hajcak, 2009; Schacter, 1977). To the extent that this evoked 5-Hz rhythm is directly related to resting theta, its peak frequency will be missed by a 6.5 to 8.0 Hz band quantification.
Moreover, although the rACC is the typical region-of-interest (ROI) for measuring resting-state theta, dorsal and posterior ACC (dACC and PCC) regions have demonstrated stronger resting theta. For example, Li and colleagues (2016) found that theta amplitude at rest correlated with glucose uptake in the dorsal ACC (dACC), whereas theta oscillations induced by cognitive testing correlated with glucose uptake in the rACC. Although the logic of focusing on the rACC as a region critical for depression was driven by early PET findings of Mayberg et al. (1997), later confirmed by Pizzagalli et al. (2001) using EEG source localization, more recent evidence suggests that theta generators outside the rACC may be more potent indicators of theta, and these regions are closely linked to psychological constructs relevant to treatment response (Cavanagh & Shackman, 2015). In particular, an MEG study reported higher test-retest reliability of resting theta for dorsal-posterior ACC than rACC (Martín-Buro, Garcés, & Maestú, 2016) and an intracranial EEG study revealed strong theta sources near dorsal ACC regions at rest (Frauscher et al., 2018). Notably, an exploratory whole-brain analysis by Arns and colleagues (2015) found that treatment response correlated with less theta amplitude near the dACC, rather than the rACC, suggesting that the dACC may also be relevant to treatment outcome. Therefore, refocusing analyses towards stronger theta generators (e.g., dACC) could improve measurement quality and clinical prediction.
Another important consideration is the impact of computational procedures intended to improve the signal of interest. One often-used optimization technique — spatial normalization — actually mixes theta activity from different brain regions, and could impede high-fidelity measurement and confuse spatial interpretation. Spatial normalization is calculated by dividing theta current density in the rACC by the sum of theta current density across the entire brain (e.g., Pizzagalli et al., 2003; Smith, Cavanagh, & Allen, 2018). Thus, the relationship between normalized rACC theta and MDD recovery may result from more rACC theta, less theta elsewhere in the brain (e.g., PCC), or both. Notably, studies not relying on spatial normalization found that less frontal theta (Arns et al., 2015; Knott et al., 1996; Iosifescu et al., 2009; Leuchter et al., 2017) and more PCC theta (Arns et al., 2015) predicted clinical improvement.
1.2. Research findings on posterior alpha-band activity as a candidate biomarker
In comparison to resting theta oscillations, the alpha rhythm dominates the resting EEG, with most individuals showing a distinct alpha peak at about 10 Hz having a robust posterior topography (Aurlien et al., 2004; Chiang, Rennie, Robinson, van Albada, & Kerr, 2011). Moreover, the alpha rhythm is prominent (e.g., visible in raw EEG traces) and reliably quantified by different research groups using different methodologies (e.g., Barry & De Blasio, 2018; Labounek et al., 2018; Schmidt et al., 2017; Shackman et al., 2010; Sockeel, Schwartz, Pélégrini-issac, & Benali, 2016; Tenke et al., 2017). Importantly, greater posterior alpha oscillations at rest predicted a favorable clinical outcome for individuals diagnosed with MDD (Baskaran et al., 2017; Bruder et al., 2008; Jaworska, de la Salle, Ibrahim, Blier, & Knott, 2019; Kandilarova et al., 2017; Knott, Telner, Lapierre, Browne, & Horn, 1996; Tenke et al., 2011; Ulrich, Renfordt, Zeller, & Frick, 1984; Ulrich, Renfordt, & Frick, 1986; although see Arns et al., 2016, and Knott, Mahoney, Kennedy, & Evans, 2000, for unsuccessful attempts to replicate these findings). To some degree, these reports differed in methodology, including EEG montage (density, locations) and reference, preprocessing steps, and a priori selection of frequency bins. Notably, there are significant benefits from using EEG signal separation as a generic initial preprocessing step, especially with regard to improved accuracy of measuring brain activity from specific neural sources, and empirical (i.e., data-driven) determination of optimal frequency bands and recording locations (Cohen, 2017a; Delorme & Makeig, 2004; Tenke & Kayser, 2005). For example, whereas spectral amplitude within an approximate range of 8 to 13 Hz is often averaged for a pooled analysis of the (classical) “alpha” band, narrower bands in this range have distinct functional significance (Buzsáki, 2006; Klimesch, 1999; Sadaghiani & Kleinschmidt, 2016) and spatial distribution (Barzegaran et al., 2017; Tenke & Kayser, 2005). Specifically, Tenke and colleagues demonstrated the presence of two distinct spectral alpha components1, peaking at around 9 and 11 Hz, with 9 Hz component loadings crossing over to the classical theta band (i.e., well below 8 Hz), suggesting that low alpha and theta activity share variance (e.g., Tenke et al., 2011, termed this spectral component “Low Alpha/Theta”). Indeed, fPCA applied to resting-state EEG has consistently revealed two distinct alpha components: one low alpha component with a ~9 Hz peak frequency and a lateral occipitoparietal topography, and another high alpha component peaking at ~11 Hz with an occipitoparietal midline maximum (Barry & De Blasio, 2018; Barzegaran et al., 2017; Chiang et al., 2011; Shackman et al., 2010; Tenke & Kayser, 2005). As ‘true’ rACC theta variance may be weak at rest, it stands to reason that 6.5 to 8 Hz spatially-normalized rACC activity, which is influenced by posterior activity, may partly be measuring a posterior alpha covariance entity. Moreover, spectral windows within fixed frequency limits do not actually separate genuine EEG rhythms (i.e., theta and alpha) because spectral leakage conflates neighboring bands. Taken together, various suboptimal methodological choices may conflate theta and alpha metrics by mixing variance sources. Hence, the different specific techniques used to calculate these biomarkers should be directly compared.
1.3. Present report
This report aimed to systematically compare the effects of processing choices for the quantification and validity of resting EEG biomarkers. An evaluation of discriminant and convergent validity can reveal the extent to which biomarkers are robust to specific methodologies, and to what extent biomarkers are distinct from one another (Campbell & Fiske, 1959). Although decisions regarding EEG methodology are often arbitrary or based on a priori hypotheses (e.g., a priori frequency bands, EEG reference, ROIs, spatial normalization), we argue that in many cases data-driven approaches will outperform investigator-guided processing choices. Data-driven approaches will reduce unsystematic nuisance variance in EEG data (e.g., Barry & De Blasio, 2018; Delorme, Palmer, Onton, Oostenveld, & Makeig, 2012; Kayser, Tenke, & Debener, 2000; Tenke & Kayser, 2005). Hence, frequency analyses in this report were guided by an unbiased extraction of spectral components (i.e., multivariate data decomposition via fPCA; Kayser, Tenke, & Debener, 2000; Tenke & Kayser, 2005; Tenke et al., 2011). fPCA identifies spectral components from their covariance structure across conditions, electrodes, and/or participants. Most importantly, fPCA has proven to be a useful tool for the quantification of the latent structure of EEG spectra (e.g., Tenke & Kayser, 2005; Shackman et al., 2010; Barry & De Blasio, 2018).
Furthermore, ambiguity of reference choice is resolved, and spatial signal smearing due to volume conduction is mitigated, by transformation of scalp potentials into a reference-free representation of radial current flow (surface Laplacian or scalp CSD; Kayser & Tenke, 2015b; Nunez & Srinivasan, 2006; Tenke & Kayser, 2012). Source modeling of resting EEG spectra also represents a reference-free approach, although this requires – in contrast to the CSD transform – additional biophysiological assumptions (e.g., Michel et al., 2004; Nunez, Nunez, & Srinivasan, 2019; Pascual-Marqui, 2007). CSD and inverse models have the additional advantage of estimating possible locations of neural generator sources.
To this end, we analyzed current source estimates of EEG resting state recordings at sensor-level (radial current flow via scalp CSD; Tenke & Kayser, 2012, 2015b) and voxel-level (distributed inverses via exact low resolution electromagnetic tomography [eLORETA]; Pascual-Marqui, 2007) for theta and alpha oscillations in a sample of healthy adults (Tenke et al., 2017). We sought to identify cortical regions, particularly within cingulate cortex, where theta was maximal by taking signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) into account (Cohen, 2014; Cohen & Gulbinaite, 2017; Smith et al., 2017). Given previous work (Frauscher et al., 2018; Li et al., 2016; Martín-Buro et al., 2016), it was expected that dorsal and posterior cingulate regions would demonstrate greater SNR for the theta band compared to rACC and subgenual ACC (sgACC) regions. We also explored fPCA results for a frontal theta component, to facilitate identification of an optimal intracranial target to measure theta, as well as optimal frequency band limits. A multitrait-multimethod framework (MTMM; Campbell & Fiske, 1959) was used to assess discriminant and convergent validity for frontal theta and posterior alpha (see Bruder et al., 2013, and Pizzagalli, 2011, for reviews). For MTMM, patterns of correlations between methods and traits are interpreted qualitatively with respect to one another, with different patterns having implications for convergent and discriminant validity. MTMM correlations are descriptive measures of standardized covariance amongst indices, and are not interpreted as inferential statistics. An MTMM analysis can elucidate construct validity, and has the advantage of being technically parsimonious. We also applied fPCA to eLORETA distributed inverses (Barzegaran et al., 2017; Pascual-Marqui, 2007), as well as a combined data set using both CSD and eLORETA estimates.
Given prior research reviewed above, we anticipated poor discriminant validity between a posterior low-frequency (peaking around 9 Hz) alpha component and spatially-normalized rACC theta (using the typical 6.5 to 8 Hz band limits). In contrast, we anticipated good convergent validity between CSD and eLORETA spectral principal components, good discriminant validity between theta and alpha spectral components, and good test-retest reliability for spectral components. Overall, we anticipated that clarifying relationships between rACC theta and posterior alpha measures, which has not yet been systematically addressed in the literature, could aid the interpretation of each metric, help explain inconsistent findings across reports, and lead to improvements in EEG biomarker quantification and validity.
2. Method
2.1. Participants
Details regarding participant recruitment and selection are presented in Tenke et al. (2017). Briefly, a total of 35 healthy, English-fluent adults aged 18–65 years were tested as part of the Establishing Moderators and Biosignatures of Antidepressant Response in Clinical Care (EMBARC) project (Trivedi et al., 2016). Participants were locally recruited and tested at four sites: Columbia University Medical Center in New York, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center in Dallas, Massachusetts General Hospital in Boston, and University of Michigan in Ann Arbor. As part of the larger randomized clinical trial EMBARC, these participants represented the subsample of healthy controls (HC) who were free of lifetime psychiatric disorder as assessed by an interviewer trained in the administration of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders, Nonpatient Edition (First, Gibbon, Spitzer, & Williams, 1996). Each participant had two 8-min resting EEG sessions separated by about one week. Each EEG recording included four 2-min blocks with eyes closed (C) or open (O) in a fixed order (OCCO). Participants also completed a battery of biometric assays and self-report questionnaires that are not pertinent to this report. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, was approved by the institutional review board at each testing site, and all participants provided informed consent.
2.2. EEG processing
All EEG pre and postprocessing steps, including unification of EEG montage and acquisition parameters, have been detailed in Tenke et al. (2017). Briefly, data from the different research testing sites were converted to a common 72-channel EEG montage and visually inspected for recording artifacts. Missing, bad or bridged channels (Alschuler et al., 2014) were replaced by spherical spline interpolation (Perrin, Pernier, Bertrand, & Echallier, 1989, 1990). The continuous EEG data were then blink-corrected via spatial singular value decomposition and segmented into 2-s epochs with 75% overlap. Epochs were band-passed at 1–60 Hz (24 dB/octave). A semiautomated reference-free approach identified isolated EEG channels containing amplifier drift, residual eye activity, muscle or movement-related artifacts on a trial-by-trial basis (Kayser & Tenke, 2006c). Channels containing artifact were replaced by spline interpolation if less than 25% of all channels were flagged; otherwise, the epoch was rejected. Finally, an automatic threshold (±100 μV) applied to all EEG and uncorrected EOG channels removed any residual artifacts epoch-wise. The average total number of useable epochs for EO and EC were 331.4 (range 191 – 467) and 377.7 (74 – 477) in Session 1, and 329.7 (90 – 480) and 373.6 (44 – 476) in Session 2.
To be consistent with the LORETA analysis previously employed using these data (Pizzagalli et al., 2018; Tenke et al., 2017), the Nose site was excluded from these data for all analyses, including those at sensor level, rendering a 71-channel montage. Average-referenced2 and epoched data were transformed to CSD (surface Laplacian; Nunez et al., 2019; Nunez & Srinivasan, 2006; Tenke & Kayser, 2012) and eLORETA representations (Pascual-Marqui, 2007) using previously recommended parameters for CSD (spline flexibility m = 4, regularization constant λ = 10−5; e.g., Tenke & Kayser, 2005; Kayser & Tenke, 2006; Kayser, 2009) or default parameters as implemented in the eLORETA software package (Pascual-Marqui, 2007). As the capacity of CSD estimates to represent different spatial frequencies is affected by choosing more (e.g., m = 3) or less (e.g., m = 5) flexible splines (Kayser & Tenke, 2015b), their spectral quantification using fPCA (Tenke & Kayser, 2005) may be affected as well. CSD spline flexibility affects the sensitivity of CSD measures to differentially represent broader versus more focal dipole layers (i.e., the spatial scale of the underlying neuronal generator sources; see Fig. 15 in Kayser & Tenke, 2015b). We evaluated the effect of six different spline flexibilities (m = 2 … 7) on fPCA results. These analyses revealed that fPCA produced highly consistent spectral components regardless of spline flexibility (Supplementary Figs. S1 and S2 for alpha and theta components, respectively). Accordingly, only CSD findings for a medium (default, m = 4) spline flexibility are detailed here. The Fast Fourier Transform (FFT) was used for calculation of spectral amplitude from 2-s epochs (i.e., Tenke et al., 2017). CSD-based midfrontal theta (MF θ; Table 1) was operationalized as the 6.5 to 8 Hz signal amplitude from the mean of sensors FCz and Cz. Spatially-normalized MF θ was calculated as the 6.5 to 8 Hz amplitude at a sensor divided by the sum of 6.5 to 8 Hz theta amplitudes across all scalp sensors.
Table 1.
CSD | eLORETA | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Candidate Biomarkers |
MF θ | MF θ normal |
5 Hz Comp. |
9 Hz Comp. |
10.5 Hz Comp. |
rACC θ | rACC θ normal |
6 Hz Comp. |
9 Hz Comp. |
10.5 Hz Comp. |
||
CSD | MF θ | 0.76 | ||||||||||
MF θ w/ spatial normalization | 0.22 | 0.42 | ||||||||||
5 Hz Comp. | 0.67 | 0.43 | 0.91 | |||||||||
9 Hz Comp. | 0.37 | −0.52 | −0.06 | 0.91 | ||||||||
10.5 Hz Comp. | 0.41 | 0.21 | 0.28 | −0.14 | 0.91 | |||||||
eLORETA | rACC θ | 0.45 | −0.33 | 0.20 | 0.48 | 0.01 | 0.87 | |||||
rACC θ w/ spatial normalization | −0.43 | 0.25 | −0.15 | −0.17 | −0.40 | −0.26 | 0.89 | |||||
6 Hz Comp. | 0.58 | 0.41 | 0.82 | 0.04 | 0.15 | 0.11 | −0.15 | 0.89 | ||||
9 Hz Comp. | 0.09 | −0.54 | −0.26 | 0.89 | −0.44 | 0.40 | 0.03 | −0.10 | 0.82 | |||
10.5 Hz Comp. | 0.48 | −0.08 | 0.22 | 0.24 | 0.69 | −0.07 | −0.48 | 0.16 | −0.10 | 0.90 |
Note: A multitrait-multimethod matrix (MTMM) displaying correlations between candidate biomarkers calculated using different methodologies. In particular, averaging of FFT-based amplitude spectra with a priori band limits (i.e., 6.5 to 8.0 Hz; MF θ and rACC θ) versus spectral components identified by frequency PCA (fPCA). Spectral principal components in Table 1 are from separate fPCAs for CSD and eLORETA data so that the effect of EEG transformation method can be directly assessed. Blue cells indicate one-week test-retest reliability (i.e., coefficients on main diagonal); all other cells refer to the first test session only. Green cells indicate validity coefficients: coefficients on the validity diagonal that are larger coefficients than those in red and yellow cells indicate good discriminant and convergent validity. Yellow cells include monomethod-heterotrait coefficients. Red cells include heteromethod-heterotrait coefficients. Alpha and theta principal components demonstrated good convergent validity, good discriminant validity, and good test-retest reliability. Theta metrics calculated using conventional FFT averages of fixed spectral bands generally revealed good test-retest reliability, but had poor convergent and discriminant validity. MTMM correlations are descriptive measures of standardized covariance amongst indices, are not interpreted as inferential statistics, and thus statistical significance for correlations is not reported.
2.3. Signal-to-noise ratio and internal consistency
Given that calculation of signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) is arbitrary with regard to what is considered signal and noise, we considered two different SNR calculations. In both cases, SNR was calculated directly from amplitude spectra rather than spectral components. One technique (SNRavg) defined SNR as the ratio of spectral amplitude at a single frequency bin to the average amplitude of the surrounding ±5 Hz interval, excluding the ±1 Hz around each bin (i.e., SNRavg[f] = famp / mean[f−3..f−1amp + f+1.. f+3amp]). This approach characterizes subject-specific peaks in averaged amplitude spectra as having large SNR, and noise is assumed to be broadband or 1/f amplitude. A second technique (SNRsgl) calculated signal as the average cross-trial spectral amplitude, and noise as the single-trial standard deviation of an amplitude spectrum. In this case, noise is assumed to be the variance across a frequency spectrum, and then standard deviations for each trial were averaged together to create an estimate of noise across the recording. For single-trial analyses, 148 random trials (i.e., all participants had at minimum 148 artifact-free trials) were selected from the first EEG recording session (74 trials each from eyes closed [EC] and eyes open [EO] blocks) for calculation of SNRsgl. Random selection of an equal number of epochs was used so that all participants benefitted similarly from the number of trials used during averaging. Although neither SNR calculation is believed to represent ‘ground truth’, both measure the presence of a periodic signal relative to the background EEG (noise).
To obtain estimates of internal consistency, FFT spectra from the first EEG session were averaged for theta and alpha from a random split-half of 148 epochs (i.e., 74 each, irrespective of condition).
2.4. Rostral-ACC (rACC) 6.5 to 8 Hz current density
Based on reports of improvement in localization accuracy with newer iterations of the LORETA source estimation package (Pascual-Marqui, 2007; Pascual-Marqui et al., 2018), we opted to analyze rACC theta using the more recent implementation. The rACC region-of-interest (ROI) was based on a rACC ROI used by Pizzagalli and others (2001, 2018), but varied slightly with the newer eLORETA brain. This rACC ROI included 39 voxels nearest (within 10 mm) a centroid at [±5 40 −5] in MNI space (Supplementary Fig. S3). Importantly, LORETA, sLORETA, and eLORETA versions all produced very similar estimates of rACC theta (Pearson’s correlations for all pairwise comparisons between LORETA versions: for 4.5 to 7 Hz, 0.82 ≤ r ≤ 0.99; for 6.5 to 8 Hz, 0.80 ≤ r ≤ 0.99). Thus, eLORETA estimates were considered to be equivalent to LORETA or sLORETA estimates.
Although 4 to 8 Hz are typically employed as theta band limits (Chatrian et al., 1974; Kane et al., 2017), the 6.5 to 8 Hz band was used here for consistency with previous reports examining biomarker capability for rACC theta (e.g., Arns et al., 2015; Hunter et al., 2013; Korb, Hunter, Cook, & Leuchter, 2009; Pizzagalli et al., 2001). Current density was normalized (theta amplitude at each voxel / summed theta amplitude across all 6239 voxels) and averaged across the rACC ROI. For comparison, raw theta (i.e., not normalized) was calculated as the average across rACC voxels. Rather than solely examining rACC, the current analysis also examined three additional cingulate ROIs to identify and determine the strength of other possible theta generators: dorsal ACC (dACC), subgenual ACC (sgACC; Brodmann area 25), and posterior cingulate / precuneus (PCC).
2.5. CSD-fPCA, eLORETA-fPCA, and combined CSD/eLORETA-fPCA
Sensor-level (i.e., CSD) amplitude spectra consisting of 157 frequencies between 1 and 40 Hz (.25 Hz frequency resolution) for each participant (N = 35), test session (2), EO/EC condition (2), and sensor (71), resulting in a 9940-by-157 cases-by-variables data matrix, were submitted to unrestricted fPCA followed by Varimax rotation of covariance loadings (Kayser & Tenke, 2003; Tenke & Kayser, 2005).
The procedure used for the sensor-level CSD data was repeated for voxel-level spectral amplitude (i.e., square-root of power; Tenke & Kayser, 2005). First, the 6239 voxels of the eLORETA brain were parcelled into 84 Brodmann areas (BAs; 42 for each hemisphere as created by the eLORETA software). This resulted in a 11760-by-157 data matrix (i.e., 35 participants × 2 conditions × 2 sessions × 84 Brodmann areas and 157 frequency bins), which was submitted to unrestricted Varimax-PCA.
We also conducted a combined CSD/eLORETA-fPCA that encompassed 21700 cases (35 participants × 2 conditions × 2 sessions × [71 scalp sites + 84 Brodmann areas]). As the separate CSD- and eLORETA-fPCAs relied on unstandardized covariance as the association matrix for component extraction (Kayser & Tenke, 2003), and because in all likelihood CSD and eLORETA data have dissimilar covariances, each data set was scaled by their respective total covariance (i.e., the sum of the diagonal elements of the covariance matrix) so that the total covariance for each metric was equal to 1 (see Kayser & Tenke, 2015a, for a related example of scaling covariances for this purpose). A combined CSD/eLORETA-fPCA solution was obtained because of the merits afforded by a unitary model for oscillatory phenomena often presented separately at sensor/scalp (CSD) or voxel/brain (eLORETA) levels.
Frequency PCA followed by unrestricted Varimax rotation (Kayser & Tenke, 2003) yields as many factors as there are linearly-independent variables. In most instances, this will result in an fPCA solution comprising as many factors as there are variables (i.e., the total number of spectral frequencies), although the vast majority will explain little variance (Kayser & Tenke, 2003). For brevity and simplicity, we focused our analysis on the factors accounting for the top 99% of spectral variance, in line with previous work (Tenke & Kayser, 2005).
All frequency PCAs were based on the average spectrum from all available trials from both recording sessions of each participant (i.e., following the procedures described in Tenke & Kayser, 2005).
2.6. Statistics
Statistical analyses were conducted using native functions from the statistics toolbox of Matlab (2018a v 9.4, The Mathworks Inc.), and custom Matlab code. Wilcoxon sign-rank tests were used for examining paired-samples comparisons (i.e., condition difference between EO and EC epochs). A correction for multiple comparisons across brain regions (i.e., BAs) based on a surrogate null distribution of 1000 random shuffles was used for evaluation of statistical significance (i.e., the so-called “T-max” test; Holmes, Blair, Watson, & Ford, 1996). A corrected p-value < .05 was considered significant. Corrected p-values are reported unless otherwise indicated.
3. Results
3.1. Signal quality for resting theta and alpha oscillations
Internal consistency was excellent for both theta and alpha FFT amplitudes (rs > .99). Individual spectra from eLORETA source models, as well as SNR, are displayed in Figure 1. Individual spectral amplitudes indicated that only a few individuals were characterized by a noticeable theta peak (Figure 1B). Figure 1B also shows that theta amplitude was similar for EC (top panel) and EO (bottom panel). This observation was bolstered by a robust correlation between 74 random EC and 74 random EO epochs (r = .951). For SNRavg, pairwise follow-up comparisons for a significant main effect of region (F(3, 102) = 48.927, p < .001) did not reveal significant differences between the rACC, sgACC, and dACC ROIs (all ps > .57; Figure 1C). The PCC region demonstrated significantly less theta SNRavg than the rACC (t(34) = 8.121, p < .001). For SNRsgl, repeated-measures ANOVA also confirmed that SNRsgl varied across ROIs, (F(3, 102) = 4.700, p = .004). Pairwise comparisons revealed that theta was weaker at the rACC than at any other ROI (all ps < .007). In contrast, alpha was greatest at the PCC for both SNRavg and SNRsgl (all ps < .001).
3.2. CSD/eLORETA-fPCA
Factor loadings for alpha components were highly similar for CSD, eLORETA, and combined CSD/eLORETA solutions (Tucker congruence coefficient ϕ: .78 ≥ ϕs ≥ .98; ϕ ≥ .85 indicates “fair similarity” and ϕ ≥ .95 indicates that factors are considered “equal”; Lorenzo-Seva & ten Berge, 2006). The first 11 components of the combined CSD/eLORETA fPCA accounted for 99% spectral variance. Two high-variance components had peak frequencies at 8.5 (4.3%) and 10.5 Hz (30.8%), and posterior topographies consistent with previous results (e.g., Tenke & Kayser, 2005; Tenke et al., 2011; Supplementary Fig. S4, bottom panel). Reconstructed spectra using only these two alpha factors were then submitted to a second fPCA which excluded frequencies above 20 Hz (following the procedure of Tenke et al., 2011), yielding the 9 Hz and 10.5 Hz factors used for all subsequent analyses (Figure 2). Alpha component topographies and tomographies are displayed in Figure 2 separately for each resting condition (EO, EC), along with their respective difference (EC – EO, or net alpha; cf. Tenke et al., 2015). Alpha components demonstrated the strongest alpha amplitude in parietal-occipital BAs, and weak alpha amplitude in frontal regions. The EC – EO difference was maximal in parietal-occipital BAs, indicating that EC-cued enhancement of alpha oscillations in parietal-occipital brain regions, however the strength and spatial distribution of this condition effect varied with alpha component.
The 9 Hz component demonstrated a significant condition difference (EC – EO; Figure 3) in the right retrosplenial cortex and PCC (BA 30, Z = 4.59, p < .001), and inferior-lateral occipital and temporal gyri (BAs 18, 29, 41; all Zs > 4.50, all ps < .001).
In contrast, there were no significant condition differences for the 10.5 Hz component after correcting for multiple comparisons. However, when applying a more liberal statistical threshold that was used in previous work (uncorrected p < .01; Pizzagalli et al., 2003), several trend-level condition differences emerged for the 10.5 Hz component, as shown in Figure 3. These condition differences were maximal near the superior parietal lobe, right postcentral gyrus, and right fusiform gyrus (BAs 5, 3, and 37; Zs > 2.50, ps < .01).
While the CSD-fPCA solution revealed a distinct theta component (peak frequency 6 Hz, 1% explained variance; Supplementary Figure S4) in the first 99% explained variance, this was not the case for the eLORETA- and combined CSD/eLORETA-fPCA. Therefore, the search for a theta component was widened to 99.9% total variance, which revealed one low-variance (.05%) factor with a peak frequency at 5 Hz and a midfrontal maximum (Figure 4). Factor scores for this 5 Hz principal component showed several local maxima at medial anterior regions, including premotor areas (e.g., BAs 6, 24) and the dACC. There were no significant condition differences (EC vs. EO, all ps > .3).
3.3. Multitrait-multimethod matrix for rACC theta and posterior alpha
Interpretation of correlations in Table 1 is based on a multitrait-multimethod matrix (MTMM) framework that is helpful for evaluating convergent and discriminant validity of novel psychometrics (Campbell & Fiske, 1959). Significance tests are not a focus of the MTMM correlations – correlations are interpreted as descriptive, not inferential -- and are not reported. Table 1 displays MTMM correlations between theta and alpha metrics calculated from FFT averages using typical frequency bands and sensors, or principal component scores from separate CSD or eLORETA-fPCAs. The combined CSD/eLORETA-fPCA results were not examined in the MTMM, because we wanted to evaluate validity across different techniques, in this case CSD versus eLORETA.
Within the MTMM framework, theta and alpha measures are presumed to index different neurophysiological traits (i.e., constructs) pertinent to recovery from a depressive episode. For instance, rACC theta has been linked with internetwork coordination and cognitive control (Pizzagalli, 2011), whereas posterior alpha is more often linked with arousal and vigilance (Olbrich et al., 2009; Tenke, Kayser, Abraham, Alvarenga, & Bruder, 2015; Ulke et al., 2018). In contrast, CSD and eLORETA are different methods in the MTMM. The main diagonal of the MTMM (Table 1) reflects the one-week test-retest reliability, with coefficients displayed with a blue background (test-retest reliability is also reported in Tenke et al., 2017 for some of these metrics). Coefficients with a green background make up the validity diagonal. Validity coefficients that are larger than coefficients with a red or yellow background suggest good convergent and discriminant validity. On one hand, the absolute magnitude of the validity coefficient speaks to convergent validity (i.e., a large correlation between traits assessed with a different measurement type). On the other hand, the relative magnitude of the validity coefficient compared to other correlations in the matrix speaks to discriminant validity. Correlations between different methods (CSD vs. eLORETA) and different traits (theta vs. alpha) are called heteromethod-heterotrait correlations, which are displayed with a red background. High correlations in the heteromethod-heterotrait triangles speak to misspecification of intertrait relationships (e.g., traits are overlapping or nonspecific), or more generally, these correlations indicate covariance between traits and measures that would not be hypothesized to covary. Finally, the monomethod-heterotrait triangles are displayed with a yellow background. Large monomethod-heterotrait correlations indicate that a particular method contributes to significant covariance between traits. Of course, assumptions regarding what constitutes traits and methods are arguable, and MTMM interpretation is contextualized by tenability of these assumptions, as well as by extant literature and theory. Nonetheless, examination of covariance patterns between candidate biomarkers can reveal important information about test validity beyond that inferred from bivariate correlations between predictors and outcomes (i.e., predictive validity; Campbell & Fiske, 1959).
Theta metrics at scalp demonstrated good test-retest reliability, with the exception of spatially normalized MF θ (Table 1, blue). By comparison, intracranial theta metrics were reliable over time regardless of quantification approach. Theta component scores (i.e., via fPCA) demonstrated better convergent validity (r = .82; Table 1, green) than other theta metrics. Theta components also demonstrated high convergent and discriminant validity (i.e., larger than coefficients with yellow and red background). In contrast, whereas nonnormalized theta measures demonstrated modest convergent validity across CSD and eLORETA methods (r = .45), spatially-normalized theta measures demonstrated poor convergence across transformations (r = .25). Normalized and nonnormalized theta metrics also demonstrated high heterotrait correlations, indicating poor discriminant validity with posterior alpha components. Moreover, spatially-normalized theta demonstrated small correlations with fPCA theta components.
Posterior alpha components demonstrated good convergent validity across CSD and eLORETA fPCA decompositions (Table 1, green), good discriminant validity with theta metrics, and good test-retest reliability. For example, coefficients in validity diagonals for posterior alpha components (green background in Table 1) were larger than heteromethod-heterotrait (red background) and monomethod-heterotrait (yellow background) coefficients.
Although our focus was on the qualitative comparison of MTMM coefficients, validity coefficients for frequency PCA components (theta and alpha) were significantly greater than the validity coefficients for normalized theta metrics (all ps < .02), supporting the notion that frequency PCA improves convergent validity across CSD and eLORETA transformations compared to spatial normalization.
4. Discussion
This study examined the comparative validity of two resting EEG candidate biomarkers of MDD symptom improvement in a sample of healthy adults. The present findings are in agreement with prior work noting lack of prominent theta activity during wakeful rest (Barry & De Blasio, 2018; Cigánek, 1961; Keitel & Gross, 2016; Frauscher et al., 2018; Schacter, 1977; Shackman, et al., 2010; Tenke & Kayser, 2005; Westmoreland & Klass, 1986). A combination of spatial filtering, fPCA, and conventional frequency analyses indicated that theta-band activity was weak in this resting-state EEG dataset from healthy adults. By comparison, posterior alpha was prominent, reliably quantified, and persistent across data transformation (CSD vs. eLORETA). Patterns of correlations between alpha and theta metrics indicated that spectral component (fPCA) measures evidenced good convergent and discriminant validity. Theta metrics computed at brain (rACC) and sensor (MF θ) level using spatial normalization and spectral averaging of a 6.5 to 8 Hz band evidenced poor validity.
4.1. Findings and Context: Theta
A low-variance, 5 Hz theta component stemming from a combined CSD/eLORETA-fPCA solution of resting EEG was highly similar to midfrontal theta as typically described in the literature (Cavanagh & Shackman, 2015; Schacter, 1977). This 5 Hz component was most prominent over frontocentral sensors, and near the dACC using the eLORETA source model. Notably, this theta component accounted for a very small amount of spectral variance across participants, conditions, and scalp/brain regions. Indeed, only a few participants showed a noticeable theta peak in their mean amplitude spectra. These findings are consistent with the notion that theta activity is weak and infrequent during wakeful resting states (Keitel & Gross, 2016; Vidaurre et al., 2018; Westmoreland & Klass, 1986). In contrast, event-related and induced theta oscillations are prominent (Cavanagh & Shackman, 2015), especially during cognitive load (Cavanagh & Frank, 2014; Cohen, 2014a; Hsieh & Ranganath, 2014; Li et al., 2016; Olvet & Hajcak, 2009; Sauseng, Griesmayr, Freunberger, & Klimesch, 2010). When theta oscillations are observed in resting EEG, oscillations tend to be brief in duration (<1 s; Vidaurre et al., 2018). This is consistent with the idea that rapid and incidental activation of theta-band processes during resting-state may be masked by cross-trial averages dominated by default-mode activity, which leans towards alpha oscillations (Knyazev, Slobodskoj-Plusnin, Bocharov, & Pylkova, 2011; Schmidt et al., 2017; Tenke & Kayser, 2005). For example, Li and colleagues (2016) reported that frontal theta following a cognitive task predicted clinical response to antidepressant treatment, whereas baseline differences in resting frontal theta did not. It could therefore be argued that the cognitive demands studied by Li et al. (2016) likely cued theta oscillations critical for cognitive processing, which presented the researchers with measurable and pertinent variance by which to predict treatment outcome. Indeed, most work examining theta has focused on phasic theta oscillations, rather than on a stable and continuous theta rhythm (Cavanagh & Shackman, 2015; Cohen, 2014a).
Theta generators have been localized to several different regions of the brain, including the rostral ACC (Li et al., 2016; Pizzagalli et al., 2003; Scheeringa et al., 2006), dorsal ACC (Cohen, 2011; Debener, Ullsperger, Siegel, & Engel, 2006; Frauscher et al., 2018; Li et al., 2016; Onton, Delorme, & Makeig, 2005), and PCC (Martin-Buro et al., 2016). The results here indicated that although theta peaks were apparent in the sgACC and rACC for some participant-level spectra, dACC had larger SNR and factor scores, suggesting that dACC may be a preferable ROI for measuring theta at rest (Arns, et al., 2015; Li et al., 2016). Theta amplitude near the rACC is also prone to contamination by residual nonneurogenic artifact (Mcmenamin et al., 2010); moreover, EEG demonstrates generally poor SNR near the rACC (Goldenholz et al., 2009). These difficulties with robust measurement of a resting-state theta biomarker may also contribute to the apparent variability in theta amplitude across EMBARC research sites (Tenke et al., 2017), underscoring the difficulty of enhancing (“scaling-up”) theta towards reliable clinical use. Spectral component factor loadings (Figure 4), as well as visual inspection of participant spectra (Figure 1B), suggest that a 5–6 Hz peak (Cavanagh & Shackman, 2015; Chatrian et al., 1974) may be more representative of theta than the often-used 6.5 to 8.0 Hz band. In fact, recent work examining associations between theta oscillations and MDD outcomes has adopted the more conventional 4 to 8 Hz theta band (Pizzagalli et al., 2018; Whitton et al., 2018). Of course, if the effectiveness of high theta (6.5 to 8.0 Hz) to predict clinical outcomes is directly related to posterior alpha components, then the alpha components are likely superior resting EEG biomarkers. Altogether, the literature suggests that assessment of theta oscillations and their underlying neural generators may be improved upon by modifying assays to include perturbation of theta circuitry (e.g., cognitive load, event-related designs; Cohen, 2014a) and with an increased focus on empirical identification and quantification of theta.
4.2. Findings and Context: Alpha
Posterior alpha components from an eLORETA fPCA and a combined CSD/eLORETA fPCA were highly comparable to alpha components found in previous reports examining only CSD data, both in terms of peak frequencies (i.e., about 9 and 11 Hz) and band-specific topographies (lateral vs. midline occipitoparietal maxima). Source modeling yielded additional information about the differential tomographic distribution underlying these low and high alpha rhythms. Low alpha (9 Hz) was strongest in primary visual cortex and inferior-temporal gyri, but high alpha was strongest in anterior-superior-lateral parietal regions, especially on the right (also see Barzegaran, Vildavski, & Knyazeva, 2017, for a similar example). These eLORETA distributions are fully consistent with their CSD counterparts, but also extend previous CSD findings. For example, a deep medial source for the 9.0 Hz component and inferotemporal sources for both the 9.0 and 10.5 Hz components are only evident with the eLORETA source model. Whereas the low alpha component (peak at 9.0 Hz) demonstrated a robust eyes open/closed “Berger effect”, the EC - EO difference for the high alpha component (10.5 Hz) was more modest. The strongest condition differences for low alpha were near primary visual cortex, which is consistent with a role of low alpha to inhibit ongoing activity in areas important for vision while the eyes are closed (Klimesch, 1999; Sadaghiani & Kleinschmidt, 2016; Tenke et al., 2015). By comparison, high alpha was less affected by closing of the eyes, with trend-level condition differences apparent in parietal lobes; interestingly, these parietal high alpha sources overlap with brain regions important for cross-modal attention and working memory performance (Fig. 3; Haegens, Cousijn, Wallis, Harrison, & Nobre, 2014; Klimesch, 1999; Knyazev, Slobodskoj-Plusnin, Bocharov, & Pylkova, 2011; Sadaghiani & Kleinschmidt, 2016; Tenke et al., 2015). Moreover, it has been noted that high alpha is more likely to be affected by attention/cognition manipulations than low alpha (see Klimesch, 1999; Sadaghiani & Kleinschmidt, 2016, for reviews). Overall, these results align with a body of work demonstrating that narrow bands of the alpha frequency are spatially and functionally dissociable (Barzegaran et al., 2017; Buzsáki, 2006; Frauscher et al., 2018; Klimesch, 1999, 2018; Sadaghiani & Kleinschmidt, 2016).
The spectral component structure of alpha was relatively invariant to different spatial filters (CSD versus eLORETA), including spherical splines of varying flexibility that are differentially sensitive to different generator configurations (i.e., shallow vs. deep, narrow vs. broad; Kayser & Tenke, 2015b; Tenke & Kayser, 2015), suggesting that the latent structure of resting alpha activity is robust to differences in identification and quantification methodology. Indeed, the latent structure of spectral data is remarkably homogeneous across studies, given that researchers have identified spectral components with 8.6 and 10.9 Hz peaks (Tenke & Kayser, 2005), 9.0 and 10.5 Hz peaks (Tenke & Kayser, 2011), 8.0 and 10.0 Hz peaks (Shackman et al., 2010), 8.5 and 10.0 Hz peaks (Barry & de Blasio, 2018) and 9.0 and 10.0 Hz peaks (Barzegaran et al., 2017). Altogether, findings for resting EEG have been highly consistent at identifying two distinct alpha bands (8–9 Hz and 10–11 Hz) using data-driven approaches like fPCA, indicating that resting-state alpha is easily quantified at rest as well as across a wide range of conditions (Tenke et al., 2015; see also Tenke et al., 2017).
4.3. Relationships between Theta and Alpha
Interrelationships between biomarkers indicated good psychometric properties for alpha and theta spectral components (i.e. fPCA-based), whereas psychometrics for typical frontal/rACC theta measures were poor. For example, associations between spectral measures varied sizably as a function of spatial normalization (Table 1). The correlation between raw rACC and a 9.0 Hz component approached the magnitude of validity coefficients, whereas normalized rACC theta was uncorrelated with the low alpha component. The opposite relationship was observed for the 10.5 Hz alpha component. Similar results were found for normalized MF θ at the scalp. Collectively, results suggest that theta metrics are influenced by different variance sources depending on whether normalization is used.
In contrast, component-based metrics demonstrated good convergent and discriminant validity. Coefficients for alpha metrics on the validity diagonal were large, indicative of convergence across measures, and distinct from the rACC theta biomarker. A similar pattern was evidenced for theta components: theta components correlated highly with one another across methods, moderately with conventional theta metrics, and demonstrated small correlations with alpha components.
As a caveat, it should be noted that power-power coupling has been observed between theta and alpha, so relationships between these measures could reflect a combination of spurious and true neural interactions (Cohen & Van Gaal, 2013; Klimesch, 2018; Popov, Popova, Harkotte, Awiszus, & Miller, 2018). However, spectral overlap and/or leakage seems to be a more parsimonious explanation for theta-alpha correlation during the resting state. Insofar as alpha amplitudes contribute to normalized theta amplitudes, the mere variation of using spatial normalization versus raw theta may account for discrepancies between reports (compare Arns et al., 2015 and Pizzagalli et al., 2018). In fact, some researchers have used a combination of theta and alpha activity to successfully predict MDD prognosis, for example, by averaging across a 3 to 12 Hz frequency band (Hunter et al., 2011; Leuchter, Cook, Gilmer, et al., 2009; Leuchter, Cook, Marangell, et al., 2009). Altogether, the current findings strongly indicate that posterior alpha components and a 6.5 to 8.0 Hz rACC theta metric covary, and it seems likely that suboptimal measurement of rACC theta contributes to an undesirable conflation of frontal theta and posterior alpha biomarkers.
4.4. Limitations
While this study employed both design and analysis pipelines of previous biomarker research to improve comparability (Tenke et al., 2017), conventional cross-trial averaging of the resting-state is a poor representation of true dynamic internetwork communication (Allen & Cohen, 2010; Buzsáki, 2006; Vidaurre et al., 2018). Accordingly, the study is limited by a lack of parallel event-related data for examining theta and theta-alpha interactions, analyses amenable to aperiodic signals, and a direct comparison of EEG oscillations during task performance versus resting. Recordings of event-related EEG may have also facilitated interpretation of functional differences in posterior alpha components, for instance, by linking different alpha components to visual processing and/or working memory capacity (e.g., Haegens et al., 2014; Tenke et al., 2015).
Although fPCA has the advantage of providing a simple and straightforward linear representation of the latent variance structure within a given dataset, it has the disadvantage that it may not carve nature at its joints. Specifically, Varimax-fPCA, as used here, is a linear decomposition that reveals orthogonal spectral components (e.g., Tenke & Kayser, 2005), although it is probable that neural activity is neither linear nor orthogonal (e.g., Delorme, Palmer, Onton, Oostenveld, & Makeig, 2012). Whereas an orthogonal rotation provides the advantage of maintaining components that do not share variance and are in this sense parsimonious (Kayser & Tenke, 2005, 2006a), oblique rotation methods may provide superior estimates under certain conditions (Barry & De Blasio, 2018; Dien, Beal, & Berg, 2005; Scharf & Nestler, 2018). Recent work with relaxed orthogonality constraints has demonstrated a similar component structure to the present findings, albeit with an overall improved component loadings’ morphology (e.g., nonnegative component loadings; Barry & De Blasio, 2018). These methodological issues will require further study in the context of spectral decomposition. Nonetheless, the simplified representation afforded by the present approach is unique, replicable, and consistent with prior work (Tenke & Kayser, 2005; Tenke et al., 2011) and therefore adequate to address the questions posed in this report.
Notwithstanding that the present findings firmly convey the notion that a combination of weak rACC theta, spectral leakage, and spatial normalization are a parsimonious explanation for variable findings across studies, it is important to note that these studies also differed in several other critical aspects. First, the present data were from healthy adults only and did not include resting EEG from MDD patients, which have been characterized by larger theta amplitudes at rest (Arns et al., 2015). Arguably, a more valid and robust measure of theta should be a better predictor of clinical outcomes; however, this was not directly tested in this report and should therefore be a focus in future work examining comparative predictive validity for treatment outcome in MDD patients (Trivedi et al., 2016). Second, previous treatment failure with antidepressant medication (Hunter et al., 2013), differences in MDD severity (compare Arns et al., 2015, to Pizzagalli et al., 2018), use of relative frequency indices (e.g., Leuchter et al., 2017), and variation in time between EEG recording and treatment initiation (Hunter et al., 2013) constitute additional moderators (see Pizzagalli et al., 2018, for examples) of the relationship between theta and symptom improvement in MDD. Future work will need to examine how these putative moderators affect prediction of clinical outcomes in individuals with MDD.
4.5. Towards Improving Theta Quantification
The present findings argue for several methodological improvements to theta quantification. First, researchers should consider adopting reference-free (Kayser & Tenke, 2010, 2015b, 2015c; Tenke & Kayser, 2012; Nunez et al., 2019) and multivariate data-decomposition techniques (Delorme & Makeig, 2004; Tenke & Kayser, 2005; Bridwell et al., 2018) as a general strategy for analyzing EEG data. The present findings showing enhanced validity for CSD/eLORETA-fPCA spectral components attests to the importance and benefits of this strategy. Second, conventional spectral averaging of rACC theta should pivot towards event-related and induced designs rather than relying on a resting state paradigm. Third, examining resting state theta may significantly benefit from approaches that oversample theta bursts, or are otherwise sensitive to aperiodic theta oscillations. We describe these possible improvements and their rationale in more detail below.
The quantification of observations (neural, self-report, or otherwise) into latent variables that reflect unobservable constructs of interest is of considerable interest and importance to the field (e.g., Meehl, 1995; Patrick & Hajcak, 2016). EEG source separation techniques facilitate interpretation, identification, and quantification of EEG signals by reducing observed EEG variance into latent variables (e.g., components) that index some unobserved neural generators (Barry & De Blasio, 2018; Cohen, 2017a, 2017b; Delorme et al., 2012; Kayser & Tenke, 2010; Tenke & Kayser, 2005). This is important because signal mixing in the raw EEG dilutes measurement of variance pertinent to psychological constructs of interest. Spatial normalization should be avoided in this regard because it enhances signal mixing. At the very least, results from spatially normalized and raw data should be reported in parallel. Source separation techniques also facilitate localization of neural activity to specific brain regions, and produce results that are invariant to EEG reference choice (see Cohen, 2017a; Delorme & Makeig, 2004; Kayser & Tenke, 2010, 2015b; Pascual-Marqui, Michel, & Lehmann, 1994, for examples and detailed discussions of this persistent pitfall). This is important for clinical utility, as specific brain regions likely have differential relatedness to psychiatric outcomes, and EEG reference choices will vary from clinic to clinic. There are myriad techniques and tools available to accomplish these aims, yet it remains to be seen which specific techniques are best for clinical prediction (Bridwell et al., 2018; Delorme et al., 2012; Jaworska et al., 2019). Nonetheless, pivoting towards analyses focused on latent variables should substantially advance biomarker development and utility.
Task-based theta is likely to be a more robust and valid measure of cognitive control than resting theta (e.g., Li et al., 2016; Shacter, 1977), especially when using multisecond FFT averages. Resting theta is confounded by sleepiness and ocular artifact (Schacter, 1977; Strijkstra et al., 2003), resting theta oscillations are more likely to appear as transient bursts than a continuous rhythm (e.g., Keitel & Gross, 2016; Vidaurre et al., 2018), and resting theta is inversely correlated with default mode activity (Scheeringa et al., 2008). In this way, conventional FFT-based analyses computed over multiple seconds of resting EEG are poorly suited for assessing random and aperiodic theta bursts. By comparison, when theta bursts are presumed to be more frequent and sustained, FFT may be suitable as is the case when theta activity is induced by sustained cognitive load (e.g., during a memory encoding period or during mental computation; Schacter, 1977; Li et al., 2016). Similarly, event-related designs benefit from time-locked theta bursts that consistently appear over trials, whereas the influence of background EEG is attenuated by averaging. Thus, clinical neuroscience could refocus its efforts towards induced and event-related theta when applying conventional FFT analyses for quantification of frontal theta.
Alternatively, FFT analyses may be amenable to assessing resting theta when theta bursts are oversampled prior to averaging. For example, researchers can comb through the resting state for specific neural events (e.g., oscillatory bursts), and then oversample these events for averaging. Specifically, Allen and Cohen (2011) extracted the top 1% of alpha bursts from the resting EEG as their indicator of alpha activity and found that the magnitude of the top 1% of alpha bursts accounted for a remarkable 42% of the variance in prediction of depression status (history or current depression). A similar approach could also be adopted with rACC theta: researchers can apply a narrow-band temporal filter to continuous EEG for the theta band (e.g., 4 to 8 Hz), and identify time-points from the continuous EEG corresponding to large theta bursts (at FCz, for example), and then only analyze epochs with large theta bursts using a typical FFT to calculate spectral amplitude. Another approach by Cohen (2017b) created epochs time-locked to oscillatory bursts and then submitted these epochs to an EEG source separation technique (leaving out a substantial amount of the remaining background EEG). This approach guides source separation based upon experimenter theory and/or interest (i.e., the specific neural events that were oversampled); moreover, Cohen (2017b) demonstrated convincingly that this technique improves SNR, mitigates multiple comparisons, and simplifies EEG analyses (also see Parra, Spence, Gerson, & Sajda, 2005; Parra & Sajda, 2003). These results underscore the importance of transient neural events buried in the continuous EEG that are typically weakened by a conventional FFT approach that relies on averaging across all resting epochs.
Notwithstanding that this report did not directly investigate all of these considerations, known limitations of FFT spectral averaging, along with findings from previous studies, strongly suggests that implementing techniques that circumvent these pitfalls of conventional spectral analyses should improve the clinical utility of resting-state EEG dynamics.
4.6. Conclusion
In close agreement with prior research, two distinct posterior alpha components dominated the resting EEG spectrum. Most importantly, both posterior alpha components revealed psychometric qualities supportive of their continued development as candidate EEG biomarkers: good reliability across sessions, convergent validity between methods (CSD and eLORETA), and discriminant validity with rACC theta. In contrast, rACC theta showed relatively low convergent validity across methods and poor discriminant validity with posterior alpha components. Nonetheless, a low-variance theta component demonstrated substantial advantages over rACC theta as is typically calculated, especially with regard to convergent and discriminant validity. Overall, these findings argue against the continued use of the rACC theta metric as originally proposed (i.e., recorded at rest, spatial normalization, fixed band limits). Instead, task-based assays, empirical identification of meaningful theta sources, and analyses that are amenable to aperiodic theta dynamics may be a more promising avenue for identifying theta biomarkers with high clinical utility (Iosifescu, 2011; Stewart, Coan, Towers, & Allen, 2011; Tenke et al., 2011; Wade & Iosifescu, 2016; Widge et al., 2018).
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
This research was funded by National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) award MH115299 (JK). EEG data were obtained as part of the EMBARC study under NIMH awards MH092221 (MHT) and MH092250 (McGrath, P. J.; Parsey, R. V.; MMW). We greatly appreciate helpful comments from Tarik Bel-Bahar on an earlier version of this manuscript.
In the last three years, the authors report the following financial disclosures, for activities unrelated to the current research: JK: funding from NIMH and the Templeton Foundation; DAP: funding from NIMH, Brain and Behavior Research Foundation, and the Dana Foundation; consulting fees from Akili Interactive Labs, BlackThorn Therapeutics, Boehreinger Ingelheim, CompassPathway, Posit Science, and Takeda Pharmaceuticals USA and an honorarium from Alkermes for activities unrelated to the current study; MHT (lifetime disclosures): research support from the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, Cyberonics Inc., National Alliance for Research in Schizophrenia and Depression, National Institute of Mental Health, National Institute on Drug Abuse, National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases, Johnson & Johnson, and consulting and speaker fees from Abbott Laboratories Inc., Akzo (Organon Pharmaceuticals Inc.), Allergan Sales LLC, Alkermes, AstraZeneca, Axon Advisors, Brintellix, Bristol-Myers Squibb Company, Cephalon Inc., Cerecor, Eli Lilly & Company, Evotec, Fabre Kramer Pharmaceuticals Inc., Forest Pharmaceuticals, GlaxoSmithKline, Health Research Associates, Johnson & Johnson, Lundbeck, MedAvante Medscape, Medtronic, Merck, Mitsubishi Tanabe Pharma Development America Inc., MSI Methylation Sciences Inc., Nestle Health Science-PamLab Inc., Naurex, Neuronetics, One Carbon Therapeutics Ltd., Otsuka Pharmaceuticals, Pamlab, Parke-Davis Pharmaceuticals Inc., Pfizer Inc., PgxHealth, Phoenix Marketing Solutions, Rexahn Pharmaceuticals, Ridge Diagnostics, Roche Products Ltd., Sepracor, SHIRE Development, Sierra, SK Life and Science, Sunovion, Takeda, Tal Medical/Puretech Venture, Targacept, Transcept, VantagePoint, Vivus, and Wyeth-Ayerst Laboratories; MMW: funding from NIMH, the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression (NARSAD), the Sackler Foundation, and the Templeton Foundation; royalties from the Oxford University Press, Perseus Press, the American Psychiatric Association Press, and MultiHealth Systems. All other authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
Footnotes
Throughout the manuscript, “component” refers to a spectral component identified by fPCA.
As only CSD and eLORETA transformed data were analyzed (i.e., no reference-dependent surface potentials), the actual reference choice is inconsequential. CSD and eLORETA are reference-free representations of EEG data and will produce the same unique result regardless of reference scheme
References
- Allen JJB, & Cohen MX (2010). Deconstructing the “resting” state: exploring the temporal dynamics of frontal alpha asymmetry as an endophenotype for depression. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4(December), 232 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00232 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alschuler DM, Tenke CE, Bruder GE, Kayser J, 2014. Identifying electrode bridging from electrical distance distributions: a survey of publicly-available EEG data using a new method. Clinical Neurophysiology, 125, 484–490. 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.08.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arns M, Bruder G, Hegerl U, Spooner C, Palmer DM, Etkin A, … Gordon E (2016). EEG alpha asymmetry as a gender-specific predictor of outcome to acute treatment with different antidepressant medications in the randomized iSPOT-D study. Clinical Neurophysiology, 127(1), 509–519. 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.05.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arns M, Etkin A, Hegerl U, Williams LM, DeBattista C, Palmer DM, … Gordon E (2015). Frontal and rostral anterior cingulate (rACC) theta EEG in depression: Implications for treatment outcome? European Neuropsychopharmacology, 25(8), 1190–1200. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.03.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arns M, Gordon E, & Boutros NN (2017). EEG Abnormalities Are Associated With Poorer Depressive Symptom Outcomes With Escitalopram and Venlafaxine-XR, but Not Sertraline: Results From the Multicenter Randomized iSPOT-D Study. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 48(1), 33–40. 10.1177/1550059415621435 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aurlien H, Gjerde IO, Aarseth JH, Eldøen G, Karlsen B, Skeidsvoll H, & Gilhus NE (2004). EEG background activity described by a large computerized database. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115(3), 665–673. 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.10.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barry RJ, & De Blasio FM (2018). EEG frequency PCA in EEG-ERP dynamics. Psychophysiology, 55(5). 10.1111/psyp.13042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barzegaran E, Vildavski VY, & Knyazeva MG (2017). Fine Structure of Posterior Alpha Rhythm in Human EEG: Frequency Components, Their Cortical Sources, and Temporal Behavior. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1–12. 10.1038/s41598-017-08421-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baskaran A, Farzan F, Milev R, Brenner CA, Alturi S, Pat McAndrews M, … Kennedy SH (2017). The comparative effectiveness of electroencephalographic indices in predicting response to escitalopram therapy in depression: A pilot study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 227(September 2017), 542–549. 10.1016/j.jad.2017.10.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bridwell DA, Cavanagh JF, Collins AGE, Nunez MD, Srinivasan R, Stober S, & Calhoun VD (2018). Moving Beyond ERP Components: A Selective Review of Approaches to Integrate EEG and Behavior. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12(March), 1–17. 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broadway JM, Holtzheimer PE, Hilimire MR, Parks NA, Devylder JE, Mayberg HS, & Corballis PM (2012). Frontal Theta Cordance Predicts 6-Month Antidepressant Response to Subcallosal Cingulate Deep Brain Stimulation for Treatment-Resistant Depression : A Pilot Study. Neuropsychopharmacology, 37(7), 1764–1772. 10.1038/npp.2012.23 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruder GE, Sedoruk JP, Stewart JW, McGrath PJ, Quitkin FM, & Tenke CE (2008). Electroencephalographic Alpha Measures Predict Therapeutic Response to a Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitor Antidepressant: Pre- and Post-Treatment Findings. Biological Psychiatry, 63(12), 1171–1177. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.10.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruder GE, Tenke CE, & Kayser J (2013). Electrophysiological predictors of clinical response to antidepressants In Mann JJ, McGrath PJ, And, & Roose SP (Eds.), The Clinical Handbook for the Management of Mood Disorders (pp. 380–393). 10.1017/cbo9781139175869 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsáki G (2006). Rhythms of the Brain. In Rhythms of the Brain (Vol. 1). 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195301069.001.0001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell DT, & Fiske DW (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56(2), 88–105. 10.1037/h0046016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavanagh JF, & Frank MJ (2014). Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(8), 414–421. 10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavanagh JF, & Shackman AJ (2015). Frontal Midline Theta Reflects Anxiety and Cognitive Control: Meta-Analytic Evidence. Journal of Physiology: Paris, 109(0), 3–15. 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.04.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chatrian GE, Bergamini L, Dondey M, Klass DW, Lennox-Buchthal M, & Petersen I (1974). A glossary of terms most commonly used by clinical electroencephalographers. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 37(5), 538–548. 10.1016/0013-4694(74)90099-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiang AKI, Rennie CJ, Robinson PA, van Albada SJ, & Kerr CC (2011). Age trends and sex differences of alpha rhythms including split alpha peaks. Clinical Neurophysiology, 122(8), 1505–1517. 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.01.040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cigánek L (1961). Theta-discharges in the middle-line — EEG symptom of temporal lobe epilepsy. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 13(5), 669–673. 10.1016/0013-4694(61)90099-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen MX (2011). Error-related medial frontal theta activity predicts cingulate-related structural connectivity. NeuroImage, 55(3), 1373–1383. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.072 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen MX (2014a). A neural microcircuit for cognitive conflict detection and signaling. Trends in Neurosciences, 37(9), 480–490. 10.1016/j.tins.2014.06.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen MX (2014b). Analyzing neural time series data: theory and practice. MIT press. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen MX (2017a). Comparison of linear spatial filters for identifying oscillatory activity in multichannel data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 278, 1–12. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2016.12.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen MX (2017b). Multivariate cross-frequency coupling via generalized eigendecomposition. ELife, 6, 1–26. 10.7554/elife.21792 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen MX, & Gulbinaite R (2017). Rhythmic entrainment source separation: Optimizing analyses of neural responses to rhythmic sensory stimulation. NeuroImage, 147, 43–56. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen MX, & van Gaal S (2014). Subthreshold muscle twitches dissociate oscillatory neural signatures of conflicts from errors. NeuroImage, 86, 503–513. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen MX, & Van Gaal S (2013). Dynamic interactions between large-scale brain networks predict behavioral adaptation after perceptual errors. Cerebral Cortex, 23(5), 1061–1072. 10.1093/cercor/bhs069 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Debener S, Ullsperger M, Siegel M, & Engel AK (2006). Single-trial EEG – fMRI reveals the dynamics of cognitive function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(12), 558–563. 10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delorme A, & Makeig S (2004). EEGLAB : an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134, 9–21. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delorme A, Palmer J, Onton J, Oostenveld R, & Makeig S (2012). Independent EEG sources are dipolar. PLoS ONE, 7(2). 10.1371/journal.pone.0030135 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dien J, Beal DJ, & Berg P (2005). Optimizing principal components analysis of event-related potentials : Matrix type, factor loading weighting, extraction, and rotations. Clinical Neurophysiology, 116, 1808–1825. 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.11.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- First MB, Gibbon M, Spitzer RL, & Williams JB (1996). User’s guide for the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I Disorders—Research version. New York: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute. [Google Scholar]
- Frauscher B, Von Ellenrieder N, Zelmann R, Doležalová I, Minotti L, Olivier A, … Gotman J (2018). Atlas of the normal intracranial electroencephalogram: Neurophysiological awake activity in different cortical areas. Brain, 141(4), 1130–1144. 10.1093/brain/awy035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldenholz DM, Ahlfors SP, Ha MS, Sharon D, Ishitobi M, Vaina LM, & Stufflebeam SM (2009). Mapping the Signal-To-Noise-Ratios of Cortical Sources in Magnetoencephalography and Electroencephalography. Human Brain Mapping, 1086(4), 1077–1086. 10.1002/hbm.20571 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haegens S, Cousijn H, Wallis G, Harrison PJ, & Nobre AC (2014). Inter- and intra-individual variability in alpha peak frequency. NeuroImage, 92, 46–55. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.049 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holmes AP, Blair RC, Watson HJDG, & Ford I (1996). Nonparametric Analysis of Statistic Images from Functional Mapping Experiments. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, 16(1), 7–22. 10.1097/00004647-199601000-00002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hsieh L, & Ranganath C (2014). Frontal midline theta oscillations during working memory maintenance and episodic encoding and retrieval. NeuroImage, 85, 721–729. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunter AM, Cook IA, Greenwald S, Tran ML, Miyamoto KN, Leuchter AF, … Mcmahon LL (2011). The Antidepressant Treatment Response (ATR) Index and Treatment Outcomes in a Placebo-controlled Trial of Fluoxetine. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 28(5), 478–482. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.06.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunter AM, Korb AS, Cook IA, & Leuchter AF (2013). Rostral Anterior Cingulate Activity in Major Depressive Disorder: State or Trait Marker of Responsiveness to Medication? The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 25(2), 126–133. 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.11110330 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iosifescu DV (2011). Electroencephalography-derived biomarkers of antidepressant response. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 19(3), 144–154. 10.3109/10673229.2011.586549 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iosifescu DV, Greenwald S, Devlin P, Mischoulon D, Denninger JW, Alpert JE, & Fava M (2009). Frontal EEG predictors of treatment outcome in major depressive disorder. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 19(11), 772–777. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaworska N, de la Salle S, Ibrahim M-H, Blier P, & Knott V (2019). Leveraging Machine Learning Approaches for Predicting Antidepressant Treatment Response Using Electroencephalography (EEG) and Clinical Data. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9(January), 1–16. 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00768 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kandilarova SS, Terziyski KV, Draganova AI, Stoyanov DS, Akabaliev VH, & Kostianev SS (2017). Response to Pharmacological Treatment in Major Depression Predicted by Electroencephalographic Alpha Power – a Pilot Naturalistic Study. Folia Medica, 59(3), 318–325. 10.1515/folmed-2017-0040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kane N, Acharya J, Benickzy S, Caboclo L, Finnigan S, Kaplan PW, … Putten M. J. A. M. Van. (2017). A revised glossary of terms most commonly used by clinical electroencephalographers and updated proposal for the report format of the EEG findings . Revision 2017 Clinical Neurophysiology Practice, 2, 170–185. 10.1016/j.cnp.2017.07.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kayser J (2009). Current source density (CSD) interpolation using spherical splines - CSD Toolbox (Version 1.1). Retrieved from http://psychophysiology.cpmc.columbia.edu/Software/CSDtoolbox
- Kayser J, & Tenke CE (2003). Optimizing PCA methodology for ERP component identification and measurement: theoretical rationale and empirical evaluation. Clinical Neurophysiology, 114(12), 2307–2325. 10.1016/s1388-2457(03)00241-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kayser J, & Tenke CE (2005). Trusting in or breaking with convention : Towards a renaissance of principal components analysis in electrophysiology. Clinical Neurophysiology, 116(8), 1747–1753. 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.03.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kayser J, & Tenke CE (2006a). Consensus on PCA for ERP data , and sensibility of unrestricted solutions. Clinical Neurophysiology, 117(3), 703–707. 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.11.015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kayser J, & Tenke CE (2006b). Principal components analysis of Laplacian waveforms as a generic method for identifying ERP generator patterns : I . Evaluation with auditory oddball tasks. Clinical Neurophysiology, 117(2), 348–368. 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.08.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kayser J, & Tenke CE (2006c). Electrical distance as a reference-free measure for identifying artifacts in multichannel electroencephalogram (EEG) recordings. Psychophysiology, 43, S51 Retrieved from http://psychophysiology.cpmc.columbia.edu/mmedia/SPR2006/ElecDistArti.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Kayser J, & Tenke CE (2010). In search of the Rosetta Stone for scalp EEG: Converging on reference-free techniques. Clinical Neurophysiology, 121(12), 1973–1975. 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.04.030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kayser J, & Tenke CE (2015a). Hemifield-dependent N1 and event-related theta / delta oscillations : An unbiased comparison of surface Laplacian and common EEG reference choices. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 97(3), 258–270. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.12.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kayser J, & Tenke CE (2015b). Issues and considerations for using the scalp surface Laplacian in EEG/ERP research: A tutorial review. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 97(3), 189–209. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.04.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kayser J, & Tenke CE (2015c). On the benefits of using surface Laplacian (current source density) methodology in electrophysiology. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 97(3), 171–173. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.06.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kayser J, Tenke CE, & Debener S (2000). Principal components analysis (PCA) as a tool for identifying EEG frequency bands: I. Methodological considerations and preliminary findings. Psychophysiology, 37, S54 Retrieved from http://psychophysiology.cpmc.columbia.edu/mmedia/SPR2000/fPCA1.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Keitel A, & Gross J (2016). Individual Human Brain Areas Can Be Identified from Their Characteristic Spectral Activation Fingerprints. PLoS Biology, 14(6), 1–22. 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002498 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch W (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance : a review and analysis. Brain Research Reviews, 29(2–3), 169–195. 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00056-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch W (2018). The Frequency Architecture of Brain and Brain-Body Oscillations: An Analysis. European Journal of Neuroscience, 0–2. 10.1111/ejn.14192 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knott VJ, Telner JI, Lapierre YD, Browne M, & Horn ER (1996). Quantitative EEG in the prediction of antidepressant response to imipramine. Journal of Affective Disorders, 39(3), 175–184. 10.1016/0165-0327(96)00003-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knott V, Mahoney C, Kennedy S, & Evans K (2000). Pre-treatment EEG and it’s relationship to depression severity and paroxetine treatment outcome. Pharmacopsychiatry, 33(6), 201–205. 10.1055/s-2000-8356 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knyazev GG, Slobodskoj-Plusnin JY, Bocharov AV, & Pylkova LV (2011). The default mode network and EEG alpha oscillations : An independent component analysis. Brain Research, 1402, 67–79. 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.05.052 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Korb AS, Hunter AM, Cook IA, & Leuchter AF (2009). Rostral anterior cingulate cortex theta current density and response to antidepressants and placebo in major depression. Clinical Neurophysiology, 120(7), 1313–1319. 10.1016/j.jeconom.2010.01.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kubicki S, Herrmann WM, Fichte K, & Freund G (1979). Reflections on the topics: EEG frequency bands and regulation of vigilance. Pharmacopsychiatry, 12(2), 237–245. 10.1055/s-0028-1094615 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kühn AA, Tsui A, Aziz T, Ray N, Brücke C, Kupsch A, … Brown P (2009). Pathological synchronisation in the subthalamic nucleus of patients with Parkinson’s disease relates to both bradykinesia and rigidity. Experimental Neurology, 215(2), 380–387. 10.1016/j.expneurol.2008.11.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Labounek R, Bridwell DA, Mareček R, Lamoš M, Mikl M, Slavíček T, … Baštinec J (2018). Stable Scalp EEG Spatiospectral Patterns Across Paradigms Estimated by Group ICA. Brain Topography, 31(1), 76–89. 10.1007/s10548-017-0585-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leuchter AF, Cook IA, Gilmer WS, Marangell LB, Burgoyne KS, Howland RH, … Greenwald S (2009). Effectiveness of a quantitative electroencephalographic biomarker for predicting differential response or remission with escitalopram and bupropion in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 169(2), 132–138. 10.1016/j.psychres.2009.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leuchter AF, Cook IA, Marangell LB, Gilmer WS, Burgoyne KS, Howland RH, … Greenwald S (2009). Comparative effectiveness of biomarkers and clinical indicators for predicting outcomes of SSRI treatment in Major Depressive Disorder: Results of the BRITE-MD study. Psychiatry Research, 169(2), 124–131. 10.1016/j.psychres.2009.06.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leuchter AF, Hunter AM, Jain FA, Tartter M, Crump C, & Cook IA (2017). Escitalopram but not placebo modulates brain rhythmic oscillatory activity in the first week of treatment of Major Depressive Disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 84, 174–183. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.10.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li C-TT, Hsieh JC, Huang HH, Chen MH, Juan CH, Tu PC, … Su TP (2016). Cognition-Modulated Frontal Activity in Prediction and Augmentation of Antidepressant Efficacy: A Randomized Controlled Pilot Study. Cerebral Cortex, 26(1), 202–210. 10.1093/cercor/bhu191 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Light GA, Swerdlow NR, Thomas ML, Calkins ME, Green MF, Greenwood TA, … Turetsky BI (2015). Validation of mismatch negativity and P3a for use in multi-site studies of schizophrenia: Characterization of demographic, clinical, cognitive, and functional correlates in COGS-2. Schizophrenia Research, 163(1–3), 63–72. 10.1016/j.schres.2014.09.042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lorenzo-Seva U, & Berge JMF (2006). Tucker ’ s Congruence Coefficient as a Meaningful Index of Factor Similarity. Methodology, 2(2), 57–64. 10.1027/1614-2241.2.2.57 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Martín-Buro MC, Garcés P, & Maestú F (2016). Test-retest reliability of resting-state magnetoencephalography power in sensor and source space. Human Brain Mapping, 37(1), 179–190. 10.1002/hbm.23027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayberg HS (1997). Limbic-cortical dysregulation: a proposed model of depression. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 9(3), 471–481. 10.1176/jnp.9.3.471 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mcmenamin BW, Shackman AJ, Maxwell JS, Bachhuber DRW, Koppenhaver AM, Greischar LL, & Davidson RJ (2010). Validation of ICA-based myogenic artifact correction for scalp and source-localized EEG. NeuroImage, 49(3), 2416–2432. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meehl PE (1995). Bootstraps taxometrics: Solving the classification problem in psychopathology. American Psychologist, Vol. 50, pp. 266–275. 10.1037/0003-066X.50.4.266 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michel CM, Murray MM, Lantz G, Gonzalez S, Spinelli L, & Grave De Peralta R (2004). EEG source imaging. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115(10), 2195–2222. 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.06.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monti MM, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Coleman MR, Boly M, Pickard JD, Tshibanda L, … Laureys S (2010). Willful Modulation of Brain Activity in Disorders of Consciousness. New England Journal of Medicine, 362(7), 579–589. 10.1056/NEJMoa0905370 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mulert C, Juckel G, Brunnmeier M, Karch S, Leicht G, Mergl R, … Pogarell O (2007). Rostral Anterior Cingulate Cortex Activity in the Theta Band Predicts Response to Antidepressive Medication. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 38(2), 78–81. 10.1177/155005940703800209 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Näpflin M, Wildi M, & Sarnthein J (2008). Test-retest reliability of EEG spectra during a working memory task. NeuroImage, 43(4), 687–693. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nunez PL, Nunez MD, & Srinivasan R (2019). Multi-Scale Neural Sources of EEG: Genuine, Equivalent, and Representative. A Tutorial Review. Brain Topography, 0(0), 391318 10.1101/391318 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nunez PL, & Srinivasan R (2006). Electric fields of the brain: the neurophysics of EEG. USA: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Olbrich S, Mulert C, Karch S, Trenner M, Leicht G, Pogarell O, & Hegerl U (2009). EEG-vigilance and BOLD effect during simultaneous EEG / fMRI measurement. NeuroImage, 45(2), 319–332. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.11.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olvet DM, & Hajcak G (2009). The stability of error-related brain activity with increasing trials. Psychophysiology, 46, 957–961. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00848.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Onton J, Delorme A, & Makeig S (2005). Frontal midline EEG dynamics during working memory. Neuroimage, 27, 341–356. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parra LC, Spence CD, Gerson AD, & Sajda P (2005). Recipes for the linear analysis of EEG. NeuroImage, 28(2), 326–341. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parra L, & Sajda P (2003). Blind source separation via generalized eigenvalue decomposition. The Journal of Machine Learning Research, 4, 1261–1269. Retrieved from http://dl.acm.org/citation.cfm?id=945365.964305 [Google Scholar]
- Pascual-Marqui RD (2007). Discrete, 3D distributed, linear imaging methods of electric neuronal activity. Part 1: exact, zero error localization. ArXiv, 1–16. Retrieved from http://arxiv.org/abs/0710.3341 [Google Scholar]
- Pascual-Marqui RD, Faber PL, Kinoshita T, Kochi K, Milz P, Nishida K, & Yoshimura M (2018). Comparing EEG/MEG neuroimaging methods based on localization error, false positive activity, and false positive connectivity. bioRxiv, 269753. [Google Scholar]
- Pascual-Marqui RD, Michel CM, & Lehmann D (1994). Low resolution electromagnetic tomography: a new method for localizing electrical activity in the brain. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 18(1), 49–65. 10.1016/0167-8760(84)90014-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patrick CJ, & Hajcak G (2016). RDoC: Translating promise into progress. Psychophysiology, 53(3), 415–424. 10.1111/psyp.12612 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perrin F, Pernier J, Bertrand O, & Echallier JF (1989). Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 72(2), 184–187. 10.1016/0013-4694(89)90180-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perrin F, Pernier J, Bertrand O, & Echallier JF (1990). Corrigenda. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, (76), 565–566. 10.1016/0013-4694(90)90009-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pizzagalli DA (2011). Frontocingulate dysfunction in depression: Toward biomarkers of treatment response. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(1), 183–206. 10.1038/npp.2010.166 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pizzagalli DA, Oakes TR, & Davidson RJ (2003). Coupling of theta activity and glucose metabolism in the human rostral anterior cingulate cortex: an EEG/PET study of normal and depressed subjects. Psychophysiology, 40(6), 939–949. Retrieved from 10.1111/1469-8986.00112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pizzagalli DA, Pascual-Marqui RD, Nitschke JB, Oakes TR, Larson CL, Abercrombie HC, … Davidson RJ (2001). Anterior cingulate activity as a predictor of degree of treatment response in major depression: evidence from brain electrical tomography analysis. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(3), 405–415. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11229981 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pizzagalli DA, Webb CA, Dillon DG, Tenke CE, Kayser J, Goer F, … Trivedi MH (2018). Pretreatment rostral anterior cingulate cortex theta activity in relation to symptom improvement in depression: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry, 75(6), 547–554. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.0252 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Popov T, Popova P, Harkotte M, Awiszus B, & Miller GA (2018). Cross-frequency interactions between frontal theta and posterior alpha control mechanisms foster working memory. NeuroImage, 181, 728–733. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.07.067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rentzsch J, Adli M, Wiethoff K, Gómez-Carrillo de Castro A, & Gallinat J (2014). Pretreatment anterior cingulate activity predicts antidepressant treatment response in major depressive episodes. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 264(3), 213–223. 10.1007/s00406-013-0424-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sadaghiani S, & Kleinschmidt A (2016). Brain Networks and α-Oscillations: Structural and Functional Foundations of Cognitive Control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(11), 805–817. 10.1016/j.tics.2016.09.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sauseng P, Griesmayr B, Freunberger R, & Klimesch W (2010). Control mechanisms in working memory : A possible function of EEG theta oscillations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(7), 1015–1022. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schacter D (1977). EEG Theta waves and physiological phenomena : a review and analysis. Biological Psychology, 5(1), 47–82. 10.1016/0301-0511(77)90028-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scharf F, & Nestler S (2018). Principles behind variance misallocation in temporal exploratory factor analysis for ERP data: Insights from an inter-factor covariance decomposition. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 128, 119–136. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.03.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scheeringa R, Bastiaansen MCM, Petersson KM, Oostenveld R, Norris DG, & Hagoort P (2008). Frontal theta EEG activity correlates negatively with the default mode network in resting state. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 67(3), 242–251. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.05.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt FM, Sander C, Dietz M, Nowak C, Schröder T, Mergl R, … Hegerl U (2017). Brain arousal regulation as response predictor for antidepressant therapy in major depression. Scientific Reports, 7, 1–10. 10.1038/srep45187 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shackman A, McMenamin B, Maxwell J, Greischar L, & Davidson RJ (2010). Identifying Robust and Sensitive Frequency Bands for Interrogating Neural Oscillations. NeuroImage, 51(4), 1319–1333. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith EE, Cavanagh JF, & Allen JJBB (2018). Intracranial source activity (eLORETA) related to scalp-level asymmetry scores and depression status. Psychophysiology, 55(1), 1–16. 10.1111/psyp.13019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith EE, Reznik SJ, Stewart JL, & Allen JJB (2017). Assessing and conceptualizing frontal EEG asymmetry: An updated primer on recording, processing, analyzing, and interpreting frontal alpha asymmetry. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 111, 98–114. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.11.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sockeel S, Schwartz D, Pélégrini-issac M, & Benali H (2016). Large-Scale Functional Networks Identified from Resting-State EEG Using Spatial ICA. PLoS ONE, 1–18. 10.5061/dryad.v9f16 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stewart JL, Coan J. a, Towers DN, & Allen JJB (2011). Frontal EEG asymmetry during emotional challenge differentiates individuals with and without lifetime major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 129(1–3), 167–174. 10.1016/j.jad.2010.08.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strijkstra AM, Beersma DGM, Drayer B, Halbesma N, & Daan S (2003). Subjective sleepiness correlates negatively with global alpha (8–12 Hz) and positively with central frontal theta (4–8 Hz) frequencies in the human resting awake electroencephalogram. Neuroscience Letters, 340(1), 17–20. 10.1016/S0304-3940(03)00033-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tenke CE, & Kayser J (2005). Reference-free quantification of EEG spectra: Combining current source density (CSD) and frequency principal components analysis (fPCA). Clinical Neurophysiology, 116(12), 2826–2846. 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.08.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tenke CE, & Kayser J (2012). Generator localization by current source density (CSD): Implications of volume conduction and field closure at intracranial and scalp resolutions. Clinical Neurophysiology, 123(12), 2328–2345. 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.06.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tenke CE, & Kayser J (2015). Surface Laplacians (SL) and phase properties of EEG rhythms: Simulated generators in a volumeconduction model. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 97(3), 285–298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tenke CE, Kayser J, Abraham K, Alvarenga JE, & Bruder GE (2015). Posterior EEG alpha at rest and during task performance: Comparison of current source density and field potential measures. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 97(3), 299–309. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.05.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tenke CE, Kayser J, Manna CG, Fekri S, Kroppmann CJ, Schaller JD, … Bruder GE (2011). Current source density measures of electroencephalographic alpha predict antidepressant treatment response. Biological Psychiatry, 70(4), 388–394. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tenke CE, Kayser J, Pechtel P, Webb CA, Dillon DG, Goer F, … Trivedi M (2017). Demonstrating Test-Retest Reliability of Electrophysiological Measures for Healthy Adults in a Multisite Study of Biomarkers of Antidepressant Treatment Response. Psychophysiology, 54(1), 34–50. 10.1111/psyp.12758 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trivedi MH, Mcgrath PJ, Fava M, Parsey RV, Benji T, Phillips ML, … Weissman MM (2016). ESTABLISHING MODERATORS and BIOSIGNATURES OF ANTIDEPRESSANT RESPONSE IN CLINICAL CARE (EMBARC): Rationale and Design. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 78, 11–23. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.03.001.ESTABLISHING [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ulke C, Tenke CE, Kayser J, Sander C, Böttger D, Wong LYX, … Bruder GE (2018). Resting EEG Measures of Brain Arousal in a Multisite Study of Major Depression. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 50(1), 3–12. 155005941879557 10.1177/1550059418795578 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ulrich G, Renfordt E, Zeller G, & Frick K (1984). Interrelation between changes in the EEG and psychopathology under pharmacotherapy for endogenous depression. Pharmacopsychiatry, 17(06), 178–183. 10.1055/s-2007-1017433 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ulrich G, Renfordt E, & Frick K (1986). The Topographical Distribution of Alpha-activity in the Resting EEG of Endogenous-depressive In-patients with and without Clinical Response to Pharmacotherapy. Pharmacopsychiatry, 19(4), 272–273. 10.1055/s-2007-1017230 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vidaurre D, Hunt LT, Quinn AJ, Hunt BAEE, Brookes MJ, Nobre AC, … Quinn AJ (2018). Spontaneous cortical activity transiently organises into frequency specific phase-coupling networks. Nature Communications, 9(1). 10.1038/s41467-018-05316-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wade EC, & Iosifescu DV (2016). Using Electroencephalography for Treatment Guidance in Major Depressive Disorder. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 1(5), 411–422. 10.1016/j.bpsc.2016.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waters AC, & Mayberg HS (2017). Brain-Based Biomarkers for the Treatment of Depression: Evolution of an Idea. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 23(9–10), 870–880. 10.1017/s1355617717000881 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinberg A, Riesel A, & Hajcak G (2012). Integrating multiple perspectives on error-related brain activity: The ERN as a neural indicator of trait defensive reactivity. Motivation and Emotion, 36(1), 84–100. 10.1007/s11031-011-9269-y [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Westmoreland BF, & Klass DW (1986). Midline Theta Rhythm. Archives of Neurology, 43(2), 139–141. 10.1001/archneur.1986.00520020033013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitton AE, Webb CA, Dillon DG, Kayser J, Rutherford A, Goer F, … Pizzagalli DA (2018). Pretreatment Rostral Anterior Cingulate Cortex Connectivity With Salience Network Predicts Depression Recovery: Findings From the EMBARC Randomized Clinical Trial. Biological Psychiatry, 85(10), 872–880. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.12.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Widge AS, Deckersbach T, Nemeroff CB, Montana R, Bilge MT, Rodriguez CI, … Chang W (2018). Electroencephalographic Biomarkers for Treatment Response Prediction in Major Depressive Illness: A Meta-Analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 176, 44–56. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17121358 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.