Abstract
Background.
Black-White differences are reported in social, psychological, behavioral, medical, and biological correlates of depression. This study was conducted to compare Black and White older adults for the association between neuroticism polygenic risk score (N-PRS) and chronicity of depressive symptoms over 20 years.
Methods.
Data came from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), 1990 – 2012, a nationally representative sample of Americans above age 50. Current analysis followed 9,249 individuals (7,924 Whites and 1,325 Blacks) for up to 22 years. Depressive symptoms were measured every two years between 1992 and 2012 using the 8-item Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale (CES-D-8). The independent variable was N-PRS. The dependent variable was average depressive symptoms between 1992 and 2012. Linear regression was used for data analysis.
Results.
In the pooled sample, higher N-PRS was associated with higher average depressive symptoms over the 20-year follow up period [b=0.01, 95%CI=0.00 to 0.04], net of all covariates. We also found an interaction between race and N-PRS [b=−0.02, 95%CI=−0.03 to 0.00], suggesting a stronger effect of N-PRS on 20-year average depressive symptoms for Whites than Blacks. Based on our race-specific linear regression models, higher N-PRS was associated with higher depressive symptoms from 1992 to 2012 for Whites [b=0.01, 95%CI=0.01 to 0.02] but not Blacks [b=0.00, 95%CI=−0.02 to 0.02].
Conclusion.
Black and White older adults may differ in the salience of the existing N-PRS for depressive symptoms, which better reflects the burden of depression for Whites than Blacks. This may be because the existing PRSs are derived from mostly or exclusively White samples, limiting their applicability in other race groups. Racial variation in psychosocial, clinical, and biological correlates of depression needs further research.
Keywords: African Americans, Whites, Ethnic groups, Neuroticism, Depression, Genetics, Polygenic Risk Score
1. Background
Neuroticism (N), a relatively stable personality trait with major public health significance (1), reflects some of the between-individual variation in the tendency to respond to threats with negative emotion. N is of high public health significance because it is a strong predictor of a wide range of undesired physical health outcomes such as heart disease, stroke, hypertension, diabetes, and obesity (2,3). N is also associated with an increased risk of premature mortality (3). N predicts quality of life, health service use, and mental disorders including depression (1–3).
N is believed to reflect vulnerability to anxiety and depression (4–8). Higher scores of N predict more frequency and intensity of negative emotional reactions in response to stress (2). Individuals with a higher N score are also more sensitive to negative emotional information (9,10). Therefore, individuals with higher N frequently experience emotional arousal which is a well-accepted risk factor for a wide range of negatively charged emotions (e.g. sadness, anger, anxiety, fear, worry, frustration, distress, loneliness, and depression) (11–13).
N, however, may not be universally harmful. The health consequences of N may depend on the context, culture, and health outcome, probably through differential behavioral and physiological consequences of N trait across sub-populations (14–17). At high-risk environments, high N may help individuals avoid exposures (18). In such contexts, N may become protective, as individuals with high N may have a higher tendency to avoid risks due to their higher sensitivity to the potential costs associated with environmental exposures (19). In this view, contextual factors such as culture and environment should be regarded as moderating factors that alter the health effects of N (18). In a study, N was a risk and protective factor for cardiovascular mortality in women with low and high socioeconomic status (SES) respectively (20). N differently moderates the effect of social support on health across cultures (19). Thus, the health effects of N may depend on sociodemographic factors.
A recent study suggested that N may better reflect the future risk of depression for Whites than Blacks (21). In the Americans’ Changing Lives (ACL) data that included 847 Whites and 372 Blacks higher N at baseline was associated with higher risk major depressive disorder (MDD) 25 years later (OR=2.23) in the pooled sample. The study showed an interaction between race and baseline N on subsequent risk of MDD (OR=0.37), suggesting a weaker effect for Blacks compared to Whites. Race-specific models showed an effect for Whites (OR=2.55) but not Blacks (OR=0.90) (21).
According to the “cultural moderation” hypothesis, correlates of N and other domains of negative affectivity depend on culture, race, and ethnicity (22). As proposed by the “differential effects hypothesis” (23), psychosocial mechanisms that shape populations’ health and illness are group-specific and not universal. As suggested by this hypothesis, such mechanisms vary across race and ethnic groups. These hypotheses conceptualize race and ethnicity as potential effect modifiers that may alter the effects of the very same risk factors on the very same health outcomes (24). Considerable empirical data support these hypothesis (25), as multiple Black-White differences have been found in correlates of psychosocial constructs such as negative affect, anger, and N (22,27).
Race, ethnicity, culture, and SES may alter the salience of N as a psychological risk factor for depression (19,20). N may not have the same salience for the majority as the minority groups (21,28). To better understand racial and ethnic differences in the association between N polygenetic risk (N-PGS) on depressive symptoms, the current study compared Blacks and Whites aged 50 or older for the association between N-PGS and average depressive symptoms over a 20 year follow up period, using a national sample of older adults in the United States.
2-. Methods
Study design
Data were from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), 1990–2012. HRS is an ongoing state of the art longitudinal cohort study with a nationally representative sample of U.S. adults over the age of 50. The HRS has collected extensive data on SES, psychological factors, health behaviors, physical health, mental health, and health care utilization, with the primary goal of understanding the healthy transition of populations into retirement.
The baseline HRS interview was conducted in 1992. Follow-up interviews are conducted every two years, using alternating face-to-face and telephone interviews. Probability sampling was used to select households in all 50 states, and primary respondents were selected from age-eligible household members. Spouses or partners of primary respondents were also enrolled in the study. The core sample in the HRS was comprised of people who were born between 1931 and 1941 and were 51–61 years of age at the time of enrollment. To maintain the sample size (due to attrition) and the representative nature of the data to U.S. adults older than 50 years, HRS has recruited additional participants over time and now includes over 37,000 respondents. More information on the HRS study design, measures, and methodology are available elsewhere (29–31).
Analytic sample
This study included only White or Black individuals from the HRS core sample with data on N-PRS and depressive symptoms. The White analytic sample consisted of respondents who self-reported being White and met criteria for genetically-determined White ancestry (see HRS QC report for additional details) (48); the same procedure was used to create the Black analytic sample. The current analysis included 9,249 individuals (7,924 Whites and 1,325 Blacks).
Ethics
The University of Michigan Institutional Review Board (IRB) approved the HRS study protocol. All participants signed written informed consent documents. All study procedures were in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (institutional and national), as determined by the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, and revised in the year 2000. Participants received financial compensation for their participating in the study.
Measures
Demographics.
All demographics were taken from the baseline interview (1992). Demographic factors included sex (1 for female, 0 for male), age (continuous measure in years), and race (a dichotomous variable, Whites=0, Blacks=1).
Socio-economic status (SES).
We took SES measures including education and income from the 1992 survey. We used continuous variables for years of education as well as household income.
Depressive symptoms.
An eight -item version of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) (33) was applied to measure frequency of depressive symptoms (32,33). This CES-D, applied at each wave, asked respondents about the extent to which they felt depressed, lonely, sad, and happy in past week. All of the item responses were dichotomous. An average score was calculated with a potential range from 0 to 8. A higher score indicated more depressive symptoms.
Chronic medical conditions (CMC).
The HRS obtained self-reported information on respondents’ CMC at all waves. Respondents were asked whether a physician has ever diagnosed them with each condition. The following seven conditions were evaluated: high blood pressure, diabetes, cancer, lung disease, heart disease, stroke, and arthritis.
Self-rated health (SRH).
HRS asked respondents’ overall health using a 5-point scale. Responses were excellent, very good, fair, good, and poor. SRH was treated as a continuous variable, with a potential range from 1 to 5, with a higher score indicating worse SRH (34,35).
Body mass index (BMI).
From 1992 to 2004, BMI was calculated based on self-reported weight and height. BMI was then directly measured starting in 2006, during face to face interviews. Weight and height were originally collected in pounds (1 pound = 0.453 kilograms), and feet (1 foot = 0.3048 meters) / inches (1 inch = 0.0254 meters). As people have a tendency to overestimate their height and underestimate their weight, BMI based on self-reported data is on average an underestimation of actual BMI, particularly for women. However, BMI based on self-reported weight and height strongly correlates with directly measured BMI (36,37).
Neuroticism PRSs (N-PRS).
Using the llumina HumanOmni2.5 BeadChips (HumanOmni2.5-4v1, HumanOmni2.5-8v1), genotype data was obtained from the HRS participants, based on about 2.4 million SNPs. Individuals were removed if they had first-degree relatives in the HRS or missing call rates more than 2%. SNPs with call rates less than 98% or Hardy-Weinberg p-value less than 0.0001 were removed. Individuals were evaluated for chromosomal anomalies, and all affected SNPs were removed from the analysis if an anomaly was detected. Genotype data was based on saliva samples (DNA) that were collected in 2006, 2008, or 2010.
The N-PRS was created using beta coefficients from a GWAS meta-analysis for neuroticism conducted on approximately 171,000 participants (38), with the HRS sample removed. The Whites’ PRS contains 1,152,920 SNPs that overlapped between the HRS genotype data and the GWAS meta-analysis; Blacks’ PRS contains 1,148,174 SNPs. The N-PRSs were standardized within racial groups (mean=0, standard deviation=1) (39,40,74).
2.7. Statistical Analysis
We used SPSS for Windows 21.0 (IBM Inc. Armonk, NY) for data analysis. Descriptive and frequency tables were used for univariate analysis. Four linear regression models were used for multivariable analysis. In all models, N-PRS was the main independent variable, average depressive symptoms over 20 years from 1992 to 2012 was the main outcome, and demographics (age and sex), SES (education and income), and health (CMC, SRH, and BMI), and baseline depressive symptoms were covariates. First, we ran two models in the pooled sample. Model 1 tested the main effects of N, race, and covariates. Model 2 also included an interaction term between race and N. Then we conducted separate race-specific models for Whites (Model 3) and Blacks (Model 4). We reported unstandardized regression coefficients (b), their 95% CI, and p values for each variable. P-values <0.05 were considered significant.
3. Results
Current analysis included 9,249 individuals who were either Whites (n=7,924, 85.7%) or Blacks (n=1,325, 14.3%). Table 1 summarizes the descriptive statistics of the pooled sample and for each racial group. Blacks had lower education and income and worse physical health status (i.e. CMC, SRH, and BMI). Blacks also had higher average depressive symptoms over time compared to Whites (Table 1).
Table 1:
All (n=9249) | Whites (n=7924) | Blacks (n=1325) | ||||
n | % | n | % | n | % | |
Sex | ||||||
Male | 3857 | 41.70 | 3374 | 42.58 | 483 | 36.45 |
Female | 5392 | 58.30 | 4550 | 57.42 | 842 | 63.55 |
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |
Age (Years) | 56.33 | 3.96 | 56.38 | 3.99 | 56.17 | 3.85 |
Education (Years)* | 12.19 | 3.24 | 12.42 | 3.12 | 11.30 | 3.19 |
Income, (Thousands of USD)* | 225.80 | 485.73 | 265.94 | 525.73 | 70.45 | 136.96 |
Chronic Medical Conditions (Baseline)* | 0.92 | 1.00 | 0.89 | 0.98 | 1.16 | 1.07 |
Self-Rated Health (Baseline)* | 2.44 | 1.13 | 2.35 | 1.11 | 2.88 | 1.12 |
Body Mass Index (Baseline)* | 27.17 | 4.90 | 26.87 | 4.64 | 28.87 | 5.86 |
N-PRS | 0.01 | 1.01 | 0.01 | 1.01 | 0.03 | 1.01 |
Depressive Symptoms (20 Year-Average)* | 0.14 | 0.25 | 0.13 | 0.24 | 0.18 | 0.28 |
Neuroticism Polygenic Risk Score (N-PRS)
p<0.05 for test of difference in means (t-test) between Whites and Blacks
Table 2 shows correlation coefficients for the study variables in the pooled sample and based on race. In the pooled sample and in Whites, N-PRS was negatively correlated with education and income and positively correlated with CMC, SRH, BMI, and average depressive symptoms over time. In Blacks, N-PRS was negatively correlated with education and income and positively correlated with CMC, SRH, and BMI, but not with average depressive symptoms over time (Table 2).
Table 2.
2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Pooled sample | |||||||||
1 Race (Black) | .068** | −0.019 | −.129** | −.137** | .099** | .168** | .147** | .005 | .074** |
2 Sex (Female) | 1 | −.132** | −.040** | −.047** | .081** | .054** | −.019 | −.001 | .137** |
3 Age (Years) | 1 | −.068** | .045** | .083** | .048** | −.023 | 0.02 | −.001 | |
4 Education (Years) | 1 | .214** | −.123** | −.352** | −.117** | −.071** | −.253** | ||
5 Income (USD) | 1 | −.096** | −.167** | −.074** | −.033* | −.112** | |||
6 Chronic Medical Conditions (Baseline) | 1 | .458** | .256** | .047** | .251** | ||||
7 Self-Rated Health (Baseline) | 1 | .218** | .058** | .337** | |||||
8 Body Mass Index (Baseline) | 1 | −.001 | .102** | ||||||
9 Depressive Symptoms (20 Year Average) | 1 | .081** | |||||||
10 N-PRS | 1 | ||||||||
Racial groups | |||||||||
1 Race (Black) | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - |
2 Sex (Female) | 1 | −.131** | −.064** | −.039** | .061** | .033* | −.078** | −0.004 | .130** |
3 Age (Years) | −.137** | 1 | −.057** | .044** | .096** | .061** | −0.012 | 0.028 | 0.006 |
4 Education (Years) | .142** | −.188** | 1 | .205** | −.111** | −.350** | −.122** | −.077** | −.251** |
5 Income (USD) | −.070* | .022 | .256** | 1 | −.087** | −.155** | −.063** | −.036* | −.109** |
6 Chronic Medical Conditions (Baseline) | .151** | .051 | −.125** | −.131** | 1 | .447** | .231** | .039* | .236** |
7 Self-Rated Health (Baseline) | .085** | −.009 | −.259** | −.111** | .474** | 1 | .201** | .058** | .325** |
8 Body Mass Index (Baseline) | .176** | −.053 | .009 | .021 | .285** | .171** | 1 | .009 | .080** |
9 Depressive Symptoms (20 Year Average) | .017 | −.02 | −.05 | .013 | .083* | .056 | −.043 | 1 | .096** |
10 N-PRS | .150** | −0.034 | −.216** | −.097** | .285** | .342** | .113** | .021 | 1 |
Neuroticism Polygenic Risk Score (N-PRS)
In the lower panel, Whites are the upper diagonal, and Blacks are the lower diagonal
p < 0.05
p < 0.01
p < 0.001
Table 3 shows two linear regression models in overall sample. These models have the N-PRS as the independent variable (predictor) and mean depressive symptoms over time as the dependent variable (outcome). The first model only included main effects. Second models also included an interaction term between race and N-PRS. According to the first model, in the pooled sample, a higher N-PRS was associated with a higher average of depressive symptoms over the follow up time [b=0.01, 95%CI=0.00 to 0.04], net of all covariates. According to the second model, we found a significant interaction between race and N-PRS on average depressive symptoms [b=−0.02, 95%CI=−0.03 to 0.00], suggesting that the association between N-PRS and average depressive symptoms is smaller for Blacks than Whites (Table 3).
Table 3:
Model 1 Main Effects |
Model 2 Model 1 + Race × N-PRS |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
b | 95% CI | b | 95% CI | |
Race (Black) | 0.02* | 0.00 to 0.04 | 0.02* | 0.00 to 0.04 |
Sex (Female) | 0.04*** | 0.03 to 0.06 | 0.04*** | 0.03 to 0.06 |
Age (Years) | 0.00 | 0.00 to 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 to 0.00 |
Education (Years) | −0.01*** | −0.01 to −0.01 | −0.01*** | −0.01 to −0.01 |
Income (USD) | 0.00 | 0.00 to 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 to 0.00 |
CMC | 0.03*** | 0.02 to 0.04 | 0.03*** | 0.02 to 0.04 |
SRH | 0.04*** | 0.04 to 0.05 | 0.04*** | 0.04 to 0.05 |
BMI | 0.00 | 0.00 to 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 to 0.00 |
N-PRS | 0.01*** | 0.01 to 0.02 | 0.01*** | 0.01 to 0.02 |
N-PRS × Race | −0.02* | −0.03 to 0.00 | ||
Intercept | 0.10# | −0.01 to 0.21 | 0.10# | −0.01 to 0.22 |
Chronic medical conditions (CMC), Body Mass Index (BMI), Self-Rated Health (SRH), Neuroticism Polygenic Risk Score (N-PGS)
p < 0.1
p < 0.05
p < 0.01
p < 0.001
Table 4 reports the results of linear regressions with N-PRS as the independent variable and average depressive symptoms over time as the dependent variable based on race. Based on these linear regression models, in Whites, higher N-PRS was associated with higher average depressive symptoms over the follow up time [b=0.01, 95%CI=0.01 to 0.02]. In Blacks, however, N-PRS was not associated with average depressive symptoms over the follow up time [b=0.00, 95%CI=−0.02 to 0.02]
Table 4:
Whites (n=7924) | Blacks (n=1325) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
b | 95% CI | b | 95% CI | |
Sex (Female) | 0.04*** | 0.03 to 0.05 | 0.06** | 0.02 to 0.10 |
Age (Years) | 0.00 | 0.00 to 0.00 | 0.00 | −0.01 to 0.00 |
Education (Years) | −0.01*** | −0.01 to −0.01 | −0.02*** | −0.02 to −0.01 |
Income (USD) | 0.00* | 0.00 to 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 to 0.00 |
CMC | 0.03*** | 0.02 to 0.04 | 0.03** | 0.01 to 0.06 |
SRH | 0.04*** | 0.03 to 0.05 | 0.06*** | 0.04 to 0.08 |
BMI | 0.00 | 0.00 to 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 to 0.00 |
N-PRS | 0.01*** | 0.01 to 0.02 | 0.00 | −0.02 to 0.02 |
Intercept | 0.09 | −0.02 to 0.21 | 0.23 | −0.15 to 0.60 |
Chronic medical conditions (CMC), Body Mass Index (BMI), Self-Rated Health (SRH), Neuroticism Polygenic Risk Score (N-PGS)
p < 0.1
p < 0.05
p < 0.01
p < 0.001
4. Discussion
We found that N-PRS is associated with average depressive symptoms for Whites but not Blacks ages 50 or older. This finding is in line with previous research that bio-psycho-social correlates of negative affect, MDD, and depressive symptoms are stronger for Whites than Blacks (36,41–47). We provide four potential explanations for the current finding. The first three explanations are not specific to N. The last explanation suggests that high N may not similarly indicate depressive symptom risk for sociodemographic groups.
Our first explanation is based on how PRSs are developed and validated. As currently available PRSs are developed and validated in exclusively or almost exclusively White samples, PRSs typically explain more outcome variability in Whites. This is partially due to the discordant patterns of linkage disequilibrium (LD) across diverse populations (48). As a result, the existing PRSs better reflect the pertaining phenotypes for Whites than other minority groups (39,40,49,50). N-PRS is no exception to this rule. Research into the development and validation PRSs in non-White populations will show whether the same percentage of the variance of phenotypes can be explained by PRSs for Whites and racial and ethnic minorities (40). Similar results are reported for other PRSs (51,52).
The second explanation is a measurement argument. Many psychosocial measures show better psychometric properties in Whites compared to Blacks as they have been developed in studies that have mostly enrolled Whites. The same is true for psychometric properties of the existing N and depression measures that better operate in Whites compared to any minority groups including Blacks. The result is systematically weaker correlates of psychosocial factors for Blacks in comparison with Whites (41–43,53,54). There is also some specific evidence showing that CES-D measure may not provide identical results for Whites and Blacks (57–60). Relevance of DSM criteria may differ for depression of Blacks and Whites (55,56), resulting in different nature of depression based on race and ethnic group. Racial and ethnic groups may also differ in what constructs that are designed to capture personality traits such as N reflect (56). This problem is not specific to N and depression, as race and ethnicity alter the meaning of almost all psychosocial measures (61,62). Race and ethnicity alter how personality traits and psychological distress covary with particular psychiatric disorders such as MDD (63). Psychiatric disorders better impact perceived mental health and perceived need to health care in Whites than Blacks (64,65). Concordance between CES-D score and clinical depression also depends on contextual factors such as race and ethnicity (66,67). Stressful life events may also better reflect risk of MDD in Whites than Blacks (45,46). The link between depressive symptoms and MDD also varies by race (90). As mentioned, the stronger associations between subjective and objective health outcomes in Whites than Blacks extends to a wide range of psychosocial and health measures (53,63,64,68).
The associations between race, SES, personality traits such as N, and health are very complex. From one side, research has shown that personality traits such as N may partially mediate the effects of SES and health. N explains some between-SES strata differences in mortality risk, as well as some individual risk heterogeneity within each SES strata. As a result, N may be shaped by individual predispositions as well as social and structural inequalities (69). At the same time, SES alters the effects of N on health. One study showed a significant interaction between sex, N, and SES on CVD mortality, so the effects of N was stronger in women with low SES, whereas the effects of N was smaller among high SES women (70). Race also changes the health effects of N (21). However, the direction of the moderation of race and low SES seems counterintuitive. One explanation is that race and SES differently engage behavioral and biological mechanisms that reflect the effects of N on physical and mental health. More research is needed in these complex patterns.
The third explanation, again not specific to N, is the unequal effects of potentials to actual outcomes due to racism in the United States. In the presence of racism, resources and assets better translate to their pertaining outcomes in Whites than Blacks. In this view, presence or absence of risk and protective factors have more consequences for the privileged than the minority group. A similar pattern is documented for several economic resources such as education, employment, neighborhood quality, and size of social network on risk of mortality (25,71–73). Similar patterns are also shown for psychological assets such as affect, anger control, self-efficacy, and perceived control over life (70,74–76).
The forth explanation is specific to N. Previous empirical research has suggested that N (19,20) and other negative affectivity measures (77–80) may have group-specific rather than universal health effects. In a recent study using American Changing Lives (ACL) data, high N at baseline predicted subsequent risk of clinical MDD 25 years later for Whites but not Blacks (21). Depressive symptoms predicted all-cause (77) and cause-specific (78) mortality for Whites but not Blacks. Hostility and anger have also predicted cardiovascular mortality for Whites but not Blacks (79). Park et al., found that N altered the link between social support and health in Japanese but not American individuals (19) and anger may even be linked to better health in some cultures (80). Park et al., showed that for White Americans, lower social standing was associated with greater expression of anger, but for Japanese individuals, high social status was associated with more anger expression. While for White Americans, anger expression was predicted by subjective social status, for Japanese, objective social status predicted anger expression (22). These racial differences are important as both current and historical racism and discrimination may have altered social and behavioral implications for N, vigilance, and sensitivity to threats for Blacks.
In line with the last explanation, Kitayama et al., have argued that N becomes a protective factor in some and a risk factor in other contexts, depending on the level of risk in the environment. As N reflects sensitivity to potential costs associated with environmental exposures, high N may be associated with avoiding exposures through vigilance (19). At least in some contexts, high N may mean less exposure, which has health implications (18,81). Blacks and Whites also differ in the effect of stress on chronic disease and depression, possibly due to their differential behavioral coping strategies (82–86). Moderating effects of context, race, and ethnicity holds for a wide range of psychosocial domains and health (26,27,77,78). This is in part because race, ethnicity and culture shape experiences and expression of emotions (87,89). More research is still needed to discern the exact mechanisms by which race, ethnicity, sex, and culture influence health outcomes such as depression.
There are limitations to this study. First, this study did not measure N. Second, we measured depressive symptoms using a self-report measure rather than risk of clinical MDD diagnosis based on structural interviews. Third, this study used an eight-item CES-D measure that may have differential validity across ethnic groups. As a result, there is a need for replication of these findings using standardized measures of depression and N. Fourth, we conceptualized N, SES, and health as fixed factors; however, all of these constructs are subject to change over time. Fifth, the sample size was not balanced between Whites and Blacks resulting in lower statistical power for Blacks. Sixth, we did not control for mental health care use, anti-depressant prescription, or access to care. Seventh, due to the higher mortality of Blacks, race is not independent of attrition in this study. This has implications for the calculation of 20-year average of depressive symptoms. Eighth, it is not clear that the results are solely due to the interaction of N-PRS with race. Future research should test the interactions between N-PRS and SES. More research is needed on validation of these findings using a wide range of data sets. Although these limitations exist, our study was one of the first attempts to explore Black-White variation in the link between N-PGS and depression.
In conclusion, Black and White older adults differ in the salience of currently accepted genetic predisposition for N on severity of depressive symptoms. The N-PRS does not operate well for Blacks, which may be because the PRS was developed and validated in Whites. This finding is in line with other previously reported Black - White differences in social, psychological, clinical, and biological correlates of depression. Other explanations may be structural racism and the cultural moderation hypothesis.
Funding:
Assari is supported by the NIH under Awards 54MD008149, R25 MD007610, 2U54MD007598, U54 TR001627, D084526–03 and CA201415–02. The National Institute on Aging (NIA) provided funding for the Health and Retirement Study (U01 AG09740).
Footnotes
Ethics: The University of Michigan IRB approved the study protocol. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants included in the study.
Conflict of Interest: Author declares no conflicts of interest.
Animal Study: None
References
- 1.Lahey BB. Public health significance of neuroticism. Am Psychol. 2009;64(4):241–56. doi: 10.1037/a0015309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Thomas SP. Neuroticism: a construct that deserves the attention of mental health researchers and clinicians. Issues Ment Health Nurs. 2009;30(12):727. doi: 10.3109/01612840903263520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Shipley BA, Weiss A, Der G, Taylor MD, Deary IJ. Neuroticism, extraversion, and mortality in the UK Health and Lifestyle Survey: a 21-year prospective cohort study. Psychosom Med. 2007;69(9):923–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Barnhofer T, Chittka T. Cognitive reactivity mediates the relationship between neuroticism and depression. Behav Res Ther. 2010;48(4):275–81. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.12.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Farmer A, Redman K, Harris T, et al. Neuroticism, extraversion, life events and depression. The Cardiff Depression Study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 2002; 181, 118 –122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Roelofs J, Huibers M, Peeters F, Arntz A, van Os J. Rumination and worrying as possible mediators in the relation between neuroticism and symptoms of depression and anxiety in clinically depressed individuals. Behav Res Ther. 2008;46(12):1283–9. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Lam D, Smith N, Checkley S, Rijsdijk F, Sham P. Effect of neuroticism, response style and information processing on depression severity in a clinically depressed sample. Psychol Med. 2003;33(3):469–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Sen S, Nesse RM, Stoltenberg SF, Li S, Gleiberman L, Chakravarti A, Weder AB, Burmeister M. A BDNF coding variant is associated with the NEO personality inventory domain neuroticism, a risk factor for depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28(2):397–401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Derryberry D, Reed MA. Temperament and attention: Orienting toward and away from positive and negative signals. J Personality and Social Psychology 1994; 66(6):1128–1139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wilson EJ, MacLeod C, Mathews A, Rutherford EM. The causal role of interpretive bias in anxiety reactivity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 2006; 115(1):103–111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Costa PT, McCrae RR. The revised NEO personality inventory (NEO-PI-R) eds Boyles G, Matthews G, Saklofske D (The SAGE handbook of personality theory and assessment: Volume 2 — Personality measurement and testing, London: ), pp 179–199. 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Costa PT, McCrae RR. Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being: Happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1980; 38(4):668–678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Rusting CL, Larsen RJ. Extraversion, neuroticism, and susceptibility to positive and negative affect: A test of two theoretical models. Personality and Individual Differences 1997; 22(5):607–612. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Nesse RM, Ellsworth PC. Evolution, emotions, and emotional disorders. American Psychologist 2009; 64(2):129–139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Marks IM, Nesse RM. Fear and fitness: An evolutionary analysis of anxiety disorders. 1994; 15:247–261. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Friedman HS. Long-Term Relations of Personality and Health: Dynamisms, Mechanisms, Tropisms. Journal of Personality 2000; 68(6):1089–1107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Turiano NA, Mroczek DK, Moynihan J, Chapman BP. Big 5 personality traits and interleukin-6: Evidence for “healthy Neuroticism” in a US population sample. Brain Behavior and Immunity 2013; 28:83–89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Karney B, Bradbury T. The longitudinal course of marital quality and stability: A review of theory, method, and research. Psychological Bulletin 1995; 118: 3–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Park J, Kitayama S, Karasawa M, Curhan K, Markus HR, Kawakami N, Miyamoto Y, Love GD, Coe CL, Ryff CD. Clarifying the links between social support and health: culture, stress, and neuroticism matter. J Health Psychol. 2013;18(2):226–35. doi: 10.1177/1359105312439731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Hagger-Johnson G, Roberts B, Boniface D, Sabia S, Batty GD, Elbaz A, Singh-Manoux A, Deary IJ. Neuroticism and cardiovascular disease mortality: socioeconomic status modifies the risk in women (UK Health and Lifestyle Survey). Psychosom Med. 2012;74(6):596–603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Assari S Neuroticism Predicts Subsequent Risk of Major Depression for Whites but Not Blacks. Behav Sci (Basel). 2017;7(4). pii: E64. doi: 10.3390/bs7040064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Park J, Kitayama S, Markus HR, Coe CL, Miyamoto Y, Karasawa M, Curhan KB, Love GD, Kawakami N, Boylan JM, Ryff CD. Social status and anger expression: the cultural moderation hypothesis. Emotion. 2013; 13(6):1122–31. doi: 10.1037/a0034273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Race Assari S. and ethnic differences in additive and multiplicative effects of depression and anxiety on cardiovascular risk. Int J Prev Med 2016; 7(1):22. doi: 10.4103/2008-7802.173931 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Assari S, Lankarani MM. Education and Alcohol Consumption among Older Americans; Black-White Differences. Front Public Health. 2016;4:67. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2016.00067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Assari S Unequal Gain of Equal Resources across Racial Groups. Int J Health Policy Manag. 2017. doi: 10.15171/ijhpm.2017.90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Assari S, Moghani Lankarani M. Depressive Symptoms and Self-Esteem in White and Black Older Adults in the United States. Brain Sci. 2018. June 11;8(6). pii:E105. doi: 10.3390/brainsci8060105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Assari S, Lankarani MM. Race and Urbanity Alter the Protective Effect of Education but not Income on Mortality. Front Public Health. 2016;4:100. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2016.00100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Gale CR, Booth T, Mõttus R, Kuh D, Deary IJ. Neuroticism and Extraversion in Youth Predict Mental Wellbeing and Life Satisfaction 40 Years Later. J Res Pers. 2013;47(6):687–697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hauser RM, Willis RJ. Survey design and methodology in the health and retirement study and the Wisconsin longitudinal study In: Waite LJ, editor. Aging, Health, and Public Policy: Demographic and Economic Perspectives. New York: The Population Council, Inc; 2005. pp. 209–35. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Heeringa SG, Connor JH. Technical description of the health and retirement survey sample design. 1995. Available from: http://www.hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/sitedocs/userg/HRSSAMP.pdf [Google Scholar]
- 31.Sonnega A, Faul JD, Ofstedal MB, Langa KM, Phillips JW, Weir DR. Cohort Profile: the Health and Retirement Study (HRS). Int J Epidemiol. 2014. ;43(2):576–85. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyu067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Steffick DE . Documentation of Affective Functioning Measures in the Health and Retirement Study HRS AHEAD Documentation Report. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Assari S, Lankarani MM, Burgard S. Black-white difference in long-term predictive power of self-rated health on all-cause mortality in United States. Ann Epidemiol. 2016;26(2):106–114. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2015.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Assari S Gender differences in the predictive role of self-rated health on short-term risk of mortality among older adults. SAGE Open Med. 2016;4:2050312116666975. doi: 10.1177/2050312116666975. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Taylor AW, Dal Grande E, Gill TK, Chittleborough CR, Wilson DH, Adams RJ, et al. How valid are self-reported height and weight? A comparison between CATI self-report and clinic measurements using a large cohort study. Aust N Z J Public Health. 2006;30(3):238–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Simon GE, Von Korff M, Saunders K, Miglioretti DL, Crane PK, van Belle G, et al. Association between obesity and psychiatric disorders in the US adult population. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(7):824–30. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.7.824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Okbay A, Baselmans BM, De Neve JE, Turley P, Nivard MG, Fontana MA, Gratten J Genetic variants associated with subjective well-being, depressive symptoms, and neuroticism identified through genome-wide analyses. Nature Genetics, 2016: 48(6), 624–633. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Ware EB, Schmitz LL, Faul JD, Gard AM, Smith JA, Mitchell CM, Weir DR, Kardia SLR. (2017). Method of Construction Affects Polygenic Score Prediction of Common Human Traits. BiorXiv. 2017 doi: 10.1101/106062 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Ware E, Schmitz L, Faul J, HRS Polygenic Scores 2006–2010 Genetic Data Survey Research Center. University of Michigan; Ann Arbor, Michigan: June 2017 [Google Scholar]
- 41.2: Assari S, Moghani Lankarani M. Secular and Religious Social Support Better Protect Blacks than Whites against Depressive Symptoms. Behav Sci (Basel). 2018. May 4;8(5). pii: E46. doi: 10.3390/bs8050046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Assari S, Sonnega A, Leggett A, Pepin RL: Residual Effects of Restless Sleep over Depressive Symptoms on Chronic Medical Conditions: Race by Gender Differences. Journal of Racial and Ethnic Health Disparities 2016, doi: 10.1007/s40615-015-0202-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Watkins DC, Assari S, Johnson-Lawrence V: Race and ethnic group differences in comorbid major depressive disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, and chronic medical conditions. Journal of Racial and Ethnic Health Disparities 2015, 2(3): 385–394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Assari S, Caldwell CH: Gender and ethnic differences in the association between obesity and depression among black adolescents. Journal of Racial and Ethnic Health Disparities 2015, 2: 481–493. Doi: 10.1007/s40615-015-0096-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Assari S, Lankarani MM. Stressful Life Events and Risk of Depression 25 Years Later: Race and Gender Differences. Front Public Health. 2016;4:49. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2016.00049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Assari S, Lankarani MM. Association Between Stressful Life Events and Depression; Intersection of Race and Gender. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities. 2016;3(2):349–56. doi: 10.1007/s40615-015-0160-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Assari S Social Determinants of Depression: The Intersections of Race, Gender, and Socioeconomic Status. Brain Sci. 2017, 7, 156. doi: 10.3390/brainsci7120156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Health and Retirement Study. Quality Control Report for Genotypic Data. 2013. http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/sitedocs/genetics/HRS2_qc_report_SEPT2013.pdf
- 49.Martin AR, Gignoux CR, Walters RK, Wojcik GL, Neale BM, Gravel S, Kenny EE Human demographic history impacts genetic risk prediction across diverse populations. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 2017; 100(4), 635–649. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Assari S Polygenic Risk Score Not Predictive of Body Mass Index for Black Men. Frontiers in Genetics. 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Assari S Racial variation in the effects of depression PGRS on Depression. Frontiers in Genetics. 2017. Under review [Google Scholar]
- 52.Assari S Racial variation in the effects of depression PGRS on body mass index. Frontiers in Genetics. 2017. Under review [Google Scholar]
- 53.Jang Y, Park NS, Kang S-Y, Chiriboga DA: Racial/ethnic differences in the association between symptoms of depression and self-rated mental health among older adults. Community mental health journal 2014, 50(3):325–330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Canady RB, Stommel M, Holzman C: Measurement properties of the Centers for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) in a sample of African-American and non-Hispanic White pregnant women. Journal of nursing measurement 2009, 17(2):91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Green JG, Gruber MJ, Kessler RC, Lin JY, McLaughlin KA, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Alegria M. Diagnostic validity across racial and ethnic groups in the assessment of adolescent DSM-IV disorders. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2012;21(4):311–20. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Scollon CN, Diener E. Love, work, and changes in extraversion and neuroticism over time. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2006;91(6):1152–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Assari S High sense of mastery reduces psychological distress for African American women but not African American men. Arch Gen Intern Med. 2019;3(1):5–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Assari S, Moazen-Zadeh E. Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the 12-Item Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale among Blacks and Whites. Front Psychiatry. 2016;7:178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Kim G, Chiriboga DA, Jang Y. Cultural equivalence in depressive symptoms in older white, black, and Mexican-American adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2009;57(5):790–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Boutin-Foster C An item-level analysis of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) by race and ethnicity in patients with coronary artery disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;23(10):1034–9. doi: 10.1002/gps.2029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Jang Y, Kwag KH, Chiriboga DA. Not saying I am happy does not mean I am not: cultural influences on responses to positive affect items in the CES-D. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2010;65(6):684–90. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbq052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Assari S Ethnic Groups Differ in How Poor Self-Rated Mental Health Reflects Psychiatric Disorders. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities. 2017. doi: 10.1007/s40615-017-0417-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Assari S, Dejman M, Neighbors HW: Ethnic Differences in Separate and Additive Effects of Anxiety and Depression on Self-rated Mental Health Among Blacks. J Racial Ethnic Health Disparities 2015:1–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Kim G, DeCoster J, Chiriboga DA, Jang Y, Allen RS, Parmelee P: Associations between self-rated mental health and psychiatric disorders among older adults: do racial/ethnic differences exist? The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 2011; 19(5):416–422. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Breslau J, Cefalu M, Wong EC, Burnam MA, Hunter GP, Florez KR, Collins RL. Racial/ethnic differences in perception of need for mental health treatment in a US national sample. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2017;52(8):929–937. doi: 10.1007/s00127-017-1400-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Moazen-Zadeh E, Assari S. Depressive Symptoms Predict Major Depressive Disorder after 15 Years among Whites but Not Blacks. Front Public Health. 2016;4:13. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2016.00013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Arango-Lasprilla JC, Kreutzer JS: Racial and ethnic disparities in functional, psychosocial, and neurobehavioral outcomes after brain injury. The Journal of head trauma rehabilitation 2010, 25(2):128–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Kim G, Bryant A, Huang C, Chiriboga D, Ma GX: Mental health among Asian American adults: association with psychiatric. Asian American Journal of Psychology 2012, 3(1):44–52. [Google Scholar]
- 69.Chapman BP, Fiscella K, Kawachi I, Duberstein PR. Personality, socioeconomic status, and all-cause mortality in the United States. Am J Epidemiol. 2010;171(1):83–92. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwp323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Assari S General Self-Efficacy and Mortality in the USA; Racial Differences. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities. 2017;4(4):746–757. doi: 10.1007/s40615-016-0278-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Assari S Health Disparities Due to Diminished Return among Black Americans: Public Policy Solutions. Social Issues and Policy Review. 2018: DOI: 10.1111/sipr.12042 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Assari S Whites but Not Blacks Gain Life Expectancy from Social Contacts. Behav Sci. 2017;7(4). pii: E68. doi: 10.3390/bs7040068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Assari S Life Expectancy Gain Due to Employment Status Depends on Race, Gender, Education, and Their Intersections. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities. 2017. doi: 10.1007/s40615-017-0381-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Ware EB, Schmitz LL, Faul JD, Gard A, Mitchell C, Smith JA, Zhao W, Weir D, Kardia SLR, Heterogeneity in polygenic scores for common human traits. https://www.biorxiv.org/content/early/2017/02/05/106062 [Google Scholar]
- 75.Assari S, Lankarani MM. Reciprocal Associations between Depressive Symptoms and Mastery among Older Adults; Black-White Differences. Front Aging Neurosci. 2017;8:279. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Race Assari S., sense of control over life, and short-term risk of mortality among older adults in the United States. Arch Med Sci. 2017;13(5):1233–1240. doi: 10.5114/aoms.2016.59740. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Assari S, Moazen-Zadeh E, Lankarani MM, Micol-Foster V. Race, Depressive Symptoms, and All-Cause Mortality in the United States. Front Public Health. 2016;4:40. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2016.00040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Assari S, Burgard S. Black-White differences in the effect of baseline depressive symptoms on deaths due to renal diseases: 25 year follow up of a nationally representative community sample. J Renal Inj Prev. 2015; 4(4):127–34. doi: 10.12861/jrip.2015.27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Hostility Assari S., Anger, and Cardiovascular Mortality Among Blacks and Whites. Res Cardiovasc Med. 2016. doi: 10.5812/cardiovascmed.34029. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Kitayama S, Park J, Boylan JM, Miyamoto Y, Levine CS, Markus HR, Karasawa M, Coe CL, Kawakami N, Love GD, Ryff CD. Expression of anger and ill health in two cultures: an examination of inflammation and cardiovascular risk. Psychol Sci. 2015;26(2):211–20. doi: 10.1177/0956797614561268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Kitayama S, et al. Neuroticism Predicts Reduced Biological Health Risk among Japanese: A Mediating Role of Behavioral Adjustment. 2015 [Google Scholar]
- 82.Hicken MT, Lee H, Mezuk B, Kershaw KN, Rafferty J, Jackson JS. Racial and ethnic differences in the association between obesity and depression in women. J Womens Health (Larchmt). 2013;22(5):445–52. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2012.4111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Jackson JS, Knight KM, Rafferty JA. Race and unhealthy behaviors: chronic stress, the HPA axis, and physical and mental health disparities over the life course. Am J Public Health. 2010;100:933–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Jackson JS, Knight KM. Race and self-regulatory health behaviors: the role of the stress response and the HPA axis in physical and mental health disparities In: Schaie KW, Carstensen LL, eds. Social structures, aging, and self-regulation in the elderly. New York: Springer; 2006. pp. 189–207. [Google Scholar]
- 85.Mezuk B, Abdou CM, Hudson D, Kershaw KN, Rafferty JA, Lee H, Jackson JS. “White Box” Epidemiology and the Social Neuroscience of Health Behaviors: The Environmental Affordances Model. Soc Ment Health. 2013; 3(2). doi: 10.1177/2156869313480892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Mezuk B, Rafferty JA, Kershaw KN, Hudson D, Abdou CM, Lee H, Eaton WW, Jackson JS. Reconsidering the role of social disadvantage in physical and mental health: stressful life events, health behaviors, race, and depression. Am J Epidemiol. 2010;172(11):1238–49. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwq283. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Mesquita B Emotions in collectivist and individualist contexts. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;80(1):68–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Imada T, Ellsworth PC. Proud Americans and lucky Japanese: cultural differences in appraisal and corresponding emotion. Emotion. 2011;11(2):329–45. doi: 10.1037/a0022855. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Mesquita B, Frijda NH. Cultural variations in emotions: a review. Psychol Bull. 1992;112(2):179–204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Assari S, Moazen-Zadeh E. Ethnic Variation in the Cross-sectional Association between Domains of Depressive Symptoms and Clinical Depression. Front Psychiatry. 2016;7:53. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]