Skip to main content
The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism logoLink to The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism
. 2020 Jan 17;105(4):1098–1111. doi: 10.1210/clinem/dgaa023

Brain Differences in the Prefrontal Cortex, Amygdala, and Hippocampus in Youth with Congenital Adrenal Hyperplasia

Megan M Herting 1,3,4,, Anisa Azad 1, Robert Kim 1, J Michael Tyszka 2, Mitchell E Geffner 3,4,5, Mimi S Kim 3,4,5
PMCID: PMC7058446  PMID: 31950148

Abstract

Context

Classical congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH) due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency results in hormone imbalances present both prenatally and postnatally that may impact the developing brain.

Objective

To characterize gray matter morphology in the prefrontal cortex and subregion volumes of the amygdala and hippocampus in youth with CAH as compared to controls.

Design

A cross-sectional study of 27 CAH youth (16 female; 12.6 ± 3.4 years) and 35 typically developing, healthy controls (20 female; 13.0 ± 2.8 years) with 3-T magnetic resonance imaging scans. Brain volumes of interest included bilateral prefrontal cortex and 9 amygdala and 6 hippocampal subregions. Between-subject effects of group (CAH vs. control) and sex, and their interaction (group-by-sex) on brain volumes, were studied while controlling for intracranial volume (ICV) and group differences in body mass index and bone age.

Results

Congenital adrenal hyperplasia youth had smaller ICV and increased cerebrospinal fluid volume compared to controls. In fully-adjusted models, CAH youth had smaller bilateral superior and caudal middle frontal volumes, and smaller left lateral orbitofrontal volumes compared to controls. Medial temporal lobe analyses revealed that the left hippocampus was smaller in fully-adjusted models. Congenital adrenal hyperplasia youth also had significantly smaller lateral nucleus of the amygdala and hippocampal subiculum and CA1 subregions.

Conclusions

This study replicates previous findings of smaller medial temporal lobe volumes in CAH patients and suggests that the lateral nucleus of the amygdala, as well as subiculum and subfield CA1 of the hippocampus, are particularly affected within the medial temporal lobes in CAH youth.

Keywords: amygdala, hippocampus, neurodevelopment, structural MRI, congenital adrenal hyperplasia, pediatrics


Classical congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH) is most commonly due to a mutation in the CYP21A2 gene, affecting 1 in 15 000 live births, and it is the most common cause of primary adrenal insufficiency in children (1). Inadequate production of cortisol and aldosterone, and overproduction of adrenal androgens, starting in the first trimester in utero can manifest in female newborns with the severe, classical forms of CAH as masculinized external genitalia (2, 3). Given widespread expression of androgen and glucocorticoid receptors throughout the brain (4, 5), there has been interest in understanding how hormonal imbalances related to CAH may impact distinct subregions of the developing brain (6). Several studies have begun to examine brain and behavioral alterations associated with CAH, with reported differences in emotional and memory processes in CAH patients. These include moderate to large reductions in short-term and working memory, which involve the hippocampus (7, 8). Utilizing neuroimaging, smaller amygdala volumes have been reported in children with CAH (9), with affected females exhibiting greater amygdala activity during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) emotional tasks (10) compared to control females (11). Adolescents with CAH also differ in their aversive ratings of fearful faces (11), exhibit poorer memory for negatively valenced stimuli on fMRI as compared to controls (10, 12), and have impaired motivational inhibition on a reward-based antisaccade task (13). Higher rates of anxiety disorders are also reported in youth with CAH compared to nationwide rates in healthy or other chronically ill pediatric populations (14). More recently, women (18–50 years) affected by CAH due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency were also found to have reduced hippocampal volumes and impaired cognitive performance on working memory and processing speed tests (15).

Both the amygdala and hippocampus are comprised of heterogenous cell types that can be categorized into subregions with distinct cytoarchitecture, with previous nonhuman animal studies showing functional differences between the basal and lateral nuclei (which process high-level sensory input and emotional regulation) (16, 17) and the central and basolateral nuclei (which are involved in reward learning and food intake) (18). Similarly, both structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies have reported that hippocampal subfields, including the CA1, CA3, and dentate gyrus (DG), may have different involvement in learning and memory processes (19, 20). The amygdala and hippocampus have both efferent and afferent connectivity with the prefrontal cortex (PFC), ultimately allowing for successful emotional regulation and learning and memory behaviors (21, 22). However, it has yet to be studied how the distinct subregions of the amygdala and hippocampus are affected in patients with CAH.

The goal of the current study was to utilize state-of-the-art, high-resolution neuroimaging techniques to more fully characterize gray matter morphometry of the PFC, hippocampus, and amygdala in youth with classical CAH as compared to a control group. Congenital adrenal hyperplasia provides the opportunity to further understand how alterations in prenatal androgen and cortisol deficiency, as well as postnatal androgen and glucocorticoid exposure, may impact the developing brain. By combining 3D high-resolution T1- and T2-weighted structural sequences, there is substantial improvement of structural MRI to capture tissue contrast, which subsequently allows for the enhanced segmentation of the amygdala into 9 distinct subnuclei (lateral nucleus [LA], dorsal and intermediate divisions of the basolateral nucleus [BLDI], ventral division of the basolateral nucleus and paralaminar nucleus [BLVPL], basomedial nucleus [BM], central nucleus [CEN], cortical and medial nuclei [CMN], amygdala transitionareas [ATA], amygdalostriatal transition area [ASTA], and anterior amygdala area [AAA]) (24).

Participants and Methods

Study participants

The study was cross-sectional and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Southern California (USC) and Children’s Hospital Los Angeles (CHLA). Written consent was obtained from all parents or legal guardians, and/or participants, and all minors up to 14 years of age gave assent, in accordance with The Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association. Participants were recruited via flyers posted at CHLA and the Keck School of Medicine of USC, with CAH participants recruited from the CHLA CAH Comprehensive Care Center. Health-related exclusionary criteria for all participants included prenatal drug or alcohol exposure, premature birth, serious medical illness (other than CAH), eating disorders, or psychotropic medication. Participants were screened for any significant neurological conditions (eg, epilepsy and traumatic head injury) and psychiatric/developmental disorders (eg, autism, attention deficit hyperactive disorder, schizophrenia, and self-harm tendencies) which, if present, barred participation. Participants were also screened for any factors that would prevent proper and safe usage of MRI, such as irremovable ferrous materials (eg, braces), uncorrectable vision impairments (eg, blind spots and colorblindness), need for hearing aids, or claustrophobia.

We studied a total of 62 children and adolescents between the ages of 8 and 18 years old at the time of their visit (Table 1), including 27 participants with classical CAH and 35 healthy controls with no significant medical conditions. Youth with CAH had either the salt-wasting (n = 25) or simple-virilizing form (n = 2), as diagnosed by positive newborn screen (n = 12) or biochemically and/or by genotype (n = 15; age of testing 11.2 ± 27.4 months). At the time of the study visit, patients with CAH were on daily glucocorticoid dosing (16.5 ± 4.7 mg/m2/day) with glucocorticoid dose equivalencies calculated based on growth-suppressing effects of longer-acting glucocorticoids compared to hydrocortisone (prednisone dose was multiplied by 5 and dexamethasone dose was multiplied by 80) (25). Almost all patients (n = 26) were also treated with fludrocortisone (0.11 ± 0.04 mg/day).

Table 1.

Study participant characteristics for CAH and control youth

CAH (n = 27) Control (n = 35) Group Difference, p-value
Age, yr 12.63 ± 3.35 13.03 ± 2.79 CI: -2.0, 1.0; t (50.3) = -0.50; P = 0.62
Biological sex, n Male, 11; Female, 16 Male, 15; Female, 20 χ 2(1) = 0.00; P = 1
Handedness, n Right, 25; Left, 2 Right, 30; Left, 5 χ 2(1) = 1.97; P = 0.66
Hispanic/Latino, % 41 57 χ 2(2) = 2.53; P = 0.28
Race, % White, 44; Black, 7; Asian, 4; Mixed race, 0; Not reported, 30 White, 49; Black, 11; Asian, 6; Mixed race, 6; Not Reported, 20 χ 2(4) = 2.09; P = 0.72
Family income, n < $49K, 11; > $49K, 14; Not reported, 2 < $49K, 15; > $49K, 19; Not reported, 1 χ 2(1) = 0.00; P = 1
Maternal education, yr 13.85 ± 3.32, n = 26 14.85 ± 3.47, n = 34 CI: -2.8, 0.8; t (55.1) = 1.14; P = 0.26
IQ 100.22 ± 16.67 103.03 ± 15.30 CI: -11.1, 5.5; t (53.5) = 0.68; P = 0.50
BMI (kg/m2) 27.12 ± 7.28 22.52 ± 5.87 CI: 1.2, 8.0; t (49.2) = 2.67; P = 0.009
BMI z-score 1.61 ± 0.86 0.80 ± 0.95 CI: 0.3, 1.3; t (58.5) = 3.51; P = 0.0009
Tanner stage 2.81 ± 1.64 3.31 ± 1.62 CI: -1.3, 0.3; t (55.7) = 1.19; P = 0.24
Bone age, yr 13.43 ± 3.00 13.16 ± 2.77 CI: -1.2, 1.8; t (53.7) = 0.36; P = 0.72
Bone age SD 1.44 ± 3.10 0.13 ± 0.60 CI: 0.1, 2.5; t (27.5) = 2.16; P = 0.04
Brain anomaly, n Type 1 Chiari, 1; Arachnoid cyst, 2 --
CAH form, n SW, 25; SV, 2 --
Newborn screen, n Yes, 12; No, 15 --
Fludrocortisone total daily dose (mg), n = 26 0.11 ± 0.04; range: 0.05–0.2 --
Glucocorticoid total daily dose (mg/m2) 16.5 ± 4.7; range: 7.9–29.6 --
17-OHP (ng/dL) [nmol/L] 3,656 ± 4,694.8; range: 44–19,966 [110.8 ± 142.3; range: 1.3–605] --
Plasma renin activity (ng/mL/hr and μg/L/hr) 3.5 ± 2.9; range: 0.07–11.9 --
Androstenedione (ng/dL) [nmol/L] 150.5 ± 227.8; range: 10–881 [5.2 ± 8; range: 0.3–30.7] --
Testosterone (ng/dL) [nmol/L] 76.5 ± 155.3; range: 0.99–623 [2.7 ± 5.4; range: 0.03–21.6] --

Mean  ±  SD

Anthropometric measures of height (cm) and weight (kg) were obtained in all participants. Pubertal Tanner staging was assessed by a pediatric endocrinologist. Body mass index (BMI) and BMI z-score were calculated by a SAS software program based on 2000 Center for Disease Control Growth Chart data (26, 27). Bone age (BA) advancement can be a marker of prolonged and/or excess exposure to postnatal androgens, and the individual’s standard deviation (SD) for bone age (ie, BA SD) an index of BA advancement as an average (mean) for their age and sex (28). A radiograph of the left hand was used to determine BA using the Greulich–Pyle method (29), and it was read in a blinded fashion by a single pediatric endocrinologist (M.S.K.). Bone age SD was determined utilizing digital software (30). Bone age was obtained at the time of the study visit, or within several months of the visit if taken for clinical purposes. The individuals who had completed growth at the time of the study visit had their BA x-rays reviewed for earlier full maturity as adolescents.

There were no significant differences between the groups in terms of handedness, ethnicity/race composition, family income, maternal education, or IQ, as assessed by two subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WASI) IV test (31). Pubertal development was not significantly different between the 2 groups, although patients with CAH had higher BMI and BMI z-scores, as well as BA SD for their chronological age, as compared to control youth. After an overnight fast (12 hours) and prior to routine morning medications in CAH youth, all participants had their blood drawn at the CHLA Clinical Trials Unit for measurement of analytes, including 17-hydroxyprogesterone, androstenedione, plasma renin activity, and total testosterone by liquid chromatography tandem mass spectrometry (Quest Diagnostics Nichols Institute, San Juan Capistrano, California).

MRI acquisition

All images were collected on a Siemens Magnetom Prisma 3 Tesla MRI scanner using a 32-channel head coil at the University of Southern California’s Center for Image Acquisition. T1-weighted structural imaging was acquired using a sagittal whole brain MPRAGE sequence (TR = 2400 ms, TE = 2.22 ms, flip angle = 8°, BW = 220 Hz/Px, FoV = 256 mm, 208 slices, and 0.8-mm isotropic voxels, with a GRAPPA phase-encoding acceleration factor of 2). A T2-weighted variable flip angle turbo spin-echo sequence was also collected (TR = 3200 ms, TE = 563 ms, BW = 744 Hz/Px, FoV = 256 mm, 208 slices, 0.8-mm isotropic voxels, and 3.52-ms echo spacing, with a GRAPPA phase-encoding acceleration factor of 2). Anterior-posterior and posterior-anterior spin-echo field maps were also obtained (TR = 8000 ms, TE = 66.0 ms, flip angle = 90°, BW = 2290 Hz/Px, FoV = 208 mm, 72 slices, and 2.0-mm isotropic voxels, with a multiband acceleration factor of 1). A radiologist reviewed all scans for incidental findings of gross abnormalities.

MRI analysis

Whole brain segmentation

Structural image processing, including whole brain segmentation with automated labeling of different neuroanatomical structures, was performed using FreeSurfer v6.0 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu) (32, 33). Standard quality control procedures were as follows: (1) all raw images were visually inspected for motion prior to processing, and (2) postprocessed images were visually inspected by a trained operator for accuracy of segmentation for each scan per participant (34). No manual intervention (ie, subcortical editing) was performed. In addition to total gray matter, cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) and intracranial (ICV) volume were extracted as well as five a priori prefrontal regions of interest (ROI) using the Desikan-Killiany Atlas, including the superior, rostral middle, caudal middle, lateral orbitofrontal, and medial orbitofrontal regions for both the right and left hemisphere.

Amygdala and hippocampal segmentation

Details of the in vivo amygdala probabilistic atlas construction, validation, estimates of individual differences, and comparison with previous atlases have been previously published (23). Each participant’s image was registered to the CIT168 atlas using a B-spline bivariate symmetric normalization diffeomorphic registration algorithm from ANTs (35). Implementation of the inverse diffeomorphism resulted in a probabilistic segmentation of each participant’s left and right total amygdala estimates, as well as the following 9 distinct bilateral ROIs: lateral nucleus (LA), dorsal and intermediate divisions of the basolateral nucleus (BLDI), ventral division of the basolateral nucleus and paralaminar nucleus (BLVPL), basomedial nucleus (BM), central nucleus (CEN), cortical and medial nuclei (CMN), amygdala transition areas (ATA), amygdalostriatal transition area (ASTA), and anterior amygdala area (AAA). In the creation and validation of the CIT168 atlas, Tyszka and Pauli (23) established that a contrast-to-noise ratio (CNR) > 1 provides a robust estimation of the ground truth volumes. Using their established formula, the CNR for our sample was 1.21 and 1.01 for the T1- and T2-weighted scans, suggesting that we had sufficient CNR to establish amygdala boundaries using this method.

T1- and T2-weighted images for each participant were also utilized to quantify 6 hippocampal subfields, including the parasubiculum, presubiculum, subiculum, CA1, CA3, and dentate gyrus (DG) using the computational atlas (24) available in FreeSurfer v6.0 (32). This method provides hippocampal subfield volumetric measures that more closely align with histological measurements, compared to alternative automated segmentation algorithms and previous versions of the software (24).

Apportionment of subregions within the amygdala and hippocampus

The absolute volumes of amygdala subnuclei and hippocampal subfields were used to determine whether each subregion within the amygdala and hippocampus is larger or smaller in youth with CAH compared to controls. It is also important to understand what proportion of the amygdala is comprised of each of these aforementioned subregions (23), and if the proportion of each subregion differs in youth with CAH compared to controls. The relative proportion (or percentage of tissue) comprised of each subregion within the amygdala and hippocampus could further assess potential group differences in the tissue composition of the amygdala or hippocampus. Thus, we also examined if youth with CAH exhibit differences in the apportionment of subregions within the amygdala and hippocampus, compared to control youth, by calculating a relative volume fraction (RVF) for each of the 9 amygdala and 6 hippocampal subregions using the following equation: subregion volume ÷ total volume of amygdala or hippocampus (23).

Statistical analyses

All data analyses were performed in Rstudio v1.2 (Boston, Massachusetts http://www.rstudio.com/) using linear multiple regression and linear mixed models from package nlme v3.1 (https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme). Given that the BMI z-score, BA SD, and ICV were significantly different between CAH and control youth (Tables 1 and 2), these variables were included as covariates in all subsequent general linear model analyses in order to reduce the possibility of these group differences contributing to variance in brain volumes. Using ICV as a covariate in the model is a preferred method to account for differences in overall brain size when examining regional volume differences between groups (36, 37). Separate multiple regressions were first implemented to examine how group, sex, and their interaction (group-by-sex) predicted CSF and ICV, while controlling for BMI z-score and BA SD. In addition, similar models were performed for total gray matter volume, each a priori PFC regional volume, and total amygdala and hippocampus volumes for the right and left hemispheres, while including the following in the model as covariates: BMI z-score, BA SD, and ICV.

Table 2.

Brain volumes mean and SD for CAH and control youth

CAH Control
Males Females Males Females Group Difference F-statistic, P value
Total brain volumes
ICV 1559479 ± 163595.50 1438723 ± 157480.10 1647836 ± 136404.50 1515500 ± 105509.60 Group: F(1, 55) = 6.34, P = 0.01, ηp2 =0.10 Sex: F(1, 55) = 10.7, P = 0.002, ηp2 =0.17
CSF 1123.47 ± 445.98 1023.49 ± 187.06 911.70 ± 203.93 812.71 ± 145.31 Group: F(1, 56) = 6.46, P = 0.01, ηp2 =0.10 Sex: F(1, 56) = 2.1, P = 0.16, ηp2 =0.04
Total gray matter 605884.30 ± 58869.03 541305.40 ± 55596.74 645383.50 ± 39360.51 584338.20  ± 40641.82 Group: F(1, 55) = 1.48, p = 0.23, ηp2 =0.03 Sex: F(1, 55) = 10.9, P = 0.002, ηp2 =0.17
Amygdala
Right 1724.07 ± 172.76 1557.87 ± 212.42 1873.87 ± 191.82 1689.65  ± 215.5 Group: F(1, 55) = 1.10, p = 0.10, ηp2 =0.05 Sex: F(1, 55) = 1.47, p = 0.23, ηp2 =0.03
Left 1516.58 ±  166.03 1437.96 ±  216.11 1658.45 ±  176.89 1551.60 ± 176.05 Group: F(1, 55) = 1.10, p = 0.30, ηp2 =0.02 Sex: F(1, 55) = 0.0004, p = 0.98, ηp2 <0.0001
Hippocampus
Right 4069.85 ± 817.28 3843.82 ± 390.82 4288.53 ± 337.15 4136.89 ± 407.37 Group: F(1, 55) = 2.00, P = 0.16, ηp2 =0.04 Sex: F(1, 55) = 1.72, P = 0.20, ηp2 =0.03
Left 3750.32 ± 323.39 3662.90 ± 389.04 4149.08 ± 378.19 3921.53 ± 362.23 Group: F(1, 55) = 6.97, P = 0.01, ηp2 =0.11 Sex: F(1, 55) = 0.94, P = 0.34, ηp2 =0.02
Superior frontal cortex
Right 35709.82 ± 4249.97 31901.44 ± 4740.10 39502.47 ± 3196.66 35363.80 ± 3014.59 Group: F(1, 55) = 5.66, P = 0.02, ηp2 = 0.09 Sex: F(1, 55) = 4.01, P = 0.05, ηp2 = 0.07
Left 32669.64 ± 2806.22 29490.69 ± 4125.43 35863.13 ± 4071.86 32581.2 ± 2958.53 Group: F(1, 55) = 4.36, P = 0.04, ηp2 = 0.07 Sex: F(1, 55) = 3.97, P = 0.05, ηp2 = 0.07
Caudal middle frontal cortex
Right 7700.45 ± 1681.72 7104.75 ± 1613 8634.20 ± 1796.09 8832.15 ± 1426.20 Group: F(1, 55) = 8.46, P = 0.005, ηp2 =0.13 Sex: F(1, 55) = 2.92, P = 0.09, ηp2 = 0.05
Left 8221.09 ± 1427.20 7382.88 ± 1787.81 9615.33 ± 1707.55 8505.9 ± 1402.16 Group: F(1, 55) = 5.31, P = 0.02, ηp2 =0.09 Sex: F(1, 55) = 1.44, P = 0.24, ηp2 = 0.03
Rostral middle frontal cortex
Right 15967.36 ± 2311.01 13749.25 ± 1837.47 16691.80 ± 1832.05 15089.20 ± 2119.11 Group: F(1, 55) = 0.34, P = 0.56, ηp2 =0.006 Sex: F(1, 55) = 2.81, p = 0.09, ηp2 = 0.05
Left 15490.27 ± 1944.1 13004.19 ± 2068.18 16508 ± 2409.9 15561.95 ± 2010.04 Group: F(1, 55) = 7.18, P = 0.009, ηp2 =0.12 Sex: F(1, 55) = 0.32, P = 0.58, ηp2 = 0.006
Medial orbitofrontal cortex
Right 5207.55 ± 694.62 5035.12 ± 794.31 5218.93 ± 920.63 5013 ± 800.24 Group: F(1, 55) = 2.37, P = 0.13, ηp2 = 0.04 Sex: F(1, 55) = 0.91, P = 0.34, ηp2 = 0.02
Left 5194.27 ± 1266.51 5030.06 ± 563.45 5319.87 ± 661.67 4888.75 ± 641.86 Group: F(1, 55) = 1.39, P = 0.24, ηp2 = 0.02 Sex: F(1, 55) = 0.04, P = 0.84, ηp2 = 0.0007
Lateral orbitofrontal cortex
Right 11313.73 ±  1017.28 10217.62 ±  1261.01 11351 ±  1243.4 10405.1 ±  868.41 Group: F(1, 55) = 3.28, P = 0.07, ηp2 = 0.06 Sex: F(1, 55) = 2.43, P = 0.12, ηp2 = 0.04
Left 11154.36 ± 1196.43 10356.44 ± 1168.65 11593 ± 1159.35 10690.65 ± 969.28 Group: F(1, 55) = 0.07, P = 0.79, ηp2 =0.001 Sex: F(1, 55) = 0.61, P = 0.43, ηp2 = 0.01

Abbreviations: ICV, intracranial volume; CSF, cerebrospinal fluid.

Model (adjusted means for BMI z-score, bone age standard deviation, and ICV).

Model (adjusted means for BMI z-score and bone age standard deviation).

To assess absolute volumes of amygdala subnuclei and hippocampal subfields, separate multilevel models were then performed to investigate group (CAH vs control) and group-by-region effects for both the absolute volumes of the 9 amygdala subnuclei and 6 hippocampal subfields across both hemispheres (right or left as the within-subject variable), while again including the following in the model as covariates: BMI z-score, BA SD, and ICV.

To assess potential differences in the apportionment of subregions within the amygdala and hippocampus, separate multilevel models were again performed to investigate group (CAH vs. control) and group-by-region effects using the RVFs of the 9 amygdala subnuclei and 6 hippocampal subfields across both hemispheres (right or left as the within-subject variable), while including BMI z-score and BA SD as covariates. Note that ICV was not included as a covariate in the RVF models, as these values reflect the proportion of each subregion to the total amygdala or hippocampus volume of each individual participant. Post-hoc tests were then performed to probe significant interactions (group-by-region), correcting for multiple comparisons using the Holm method.

Lastly, for regions showing significant group differences in brain structure, a within-group assessment of relationships between brain structure and clinical features was performed in youth with CAH using multiple regression. These clinical features included BA SD, testosterone and androstenedione levels, and glucocorticoid daily dose (mg/m2/day).

Results

Radiological findings

During radiology review, 1 CAH patient was found to have a Type 1 Chiari malformation, and 2 CAH patients were found to have arachnoid cysts, with one cyst located near the temporal lobe and the other near the cerebellum. Because central nervous system anomalies have been previously reported in patients with CAH (15), analyses were performed including these participants; however, follow-up analyses were also examined, excluding the data from these 3 patients to ensure they were not driving group effects. Sample characteristics were similar between the groups when excluding the 3 CAH patients with brain anomalies.

Total brain volumes

A significant main effect was seen for both ICV and CSF, with youth with CAH having smaller ICV but larger CSF volumes, compared to controls (Table 2, Figs. 1A and 1B). No significant differences were seen in overall cortical gray matter (Fig. 1C). For the medial temporal lobe volumes, total amygdala and hippocampus volumes were smaller on average in CAH youth compared to controls, although in statistical testing only the total left hippocampus volume was significantly smaller in CAH (P = 0.01) (Table 2, Figs. 2A and 2B). For the PFC regions, the bilateral superior frontal, bilateral caudal middle frontal, and left rostral middle frontal regions were significantly smaller in CAH youth compared to controls (Table 2, Fig. 2C and 2D). A trend was also seen in smaller right lateral orbitofrontal volumes in CAH youth compared to controls, which did not reach statistical significance (P = 0.07). Group-by-sex effects were not significant in any of the models (data not shown).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Global brain volumes in youth with classical CAH and in healthy controls. Plots reflect means and standard error of volumes for: A) cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), B) intracranial volume (ICV), and C) cortex, gray matter (GM). A) CSF is significantly larger in CAH compared to control youth (** P ≤ 0.01). B) ICV is significantly smaller in CAH compared to control youth (** P ≤ 0.01).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Total amygdala, hippocampus, and prefrontal cortex (PFC) volumes in youth with classical CAH and controls by hemisphere. Plots reflect means and standard error of volumes for: A) amygdala, B) hippocampus, and C) prefrontal cortex (PFC) regions of interest. A) Trend level difference was seen for smaller total right amygdala volumes in CAH compared to control youth (P = 0.10). B) Left hippocampus is significantly smaller in CAH compared to control youth (** P ≤ 0.01). C) Bilateral superior frontal (SF) cortex volumes (* P ≤ 0.05) and caudal middle frontal (MF) cortex volumes (* P ≤ 0.05) were significantly smaller in CAH compared to control youth. Left rostral middle frontal volumes were significantly smaller in CAH compared to control youth (** P ≤ 0.01). Trend level difference was seen for smaller right lateral orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) volumes in CAH compared to control youth (P = 0.07). D) Significant PFC regions of interest mapped to the cortex for visualization (dark gray).

Amygdala subnuclei and hippocampal subfields

Significant group (F[1,55] = 11.70, P = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.006) and group-by-region interactions (F[8,1020] = 5.64, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.03) were seen in the absolute volumes for amygdala subnuclei (Fig. 3A). Follow-up post-hoc analyses revealed that the largest amygdala subnucleus, the LA, was significantly smaller in CAH patients as compared to controls (Table 3, Fig. 3A). No main effect of group (F[1,55] = 0.08, P = 0.77, ηp2 < 0.001) or group-by-region interaction (F[8,1020] = 1.16, p = 0.32, ηp2 = 0.008) was seen for amygdala RVF (Table 3, Fig. 3A).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Amygdala subnuclei and hippocampal subfield differences in youth with classical CAH and controls. Means and standard errors plotted for absolute (left) and relative volume fraction (RVF; right) across: A) Nine amygdala subnuclei, and B) Six hippocampal subfields. A) Absolute lateral nucleus of the amygdala (LA) volumes were significantly smaller in CAH as compared to control youth (*** P ≤ 0.001). B) Absolute subiculum and CA1 volumes were significantly smaller in CAH compared to control youth (*P ≤ 0.05 and ** P ≤ 0.01, respectively). Relative volume fractions were also significantly smaller for the presubiculum (** P ≤ 0.01), subiculum (** P ≤ 0.01), and CA1 (* P ≤ 0.05) in CAH compared to control youth. Relative volume fractions were also significantly larger for the CA3 subfield (** P ≤ 0.01). C) Three-dimensional rendering of amygdala (light gray) and hippocampal regions (dark gray) on a representative CAH participant with significantly smaller absolute volumes per region in CAH compared to control youth (black). Three-dimensional rendering was completed using QIT Software (38).

Table 3.

Amygdala subnuclei mean and SD for CAH and control youth

CAH (n = 26) Control (n = 35) Group-by-Region Post-hoc Test
Total RVF Total RVF Total†
Lateral nucleus (LA)
Right 307.22 ± 40.84 0.157 ± 0.013 331.86 ± 46.67 0.159 ± 0.012 Meansadj = -18.86, χ 2(1) = 24.88; P < 0.0001
Left 297.32 ± 42.92 0.157 ± 0.012 318.99 ± 46.3 0.158 ± 0.013
Basolateral nucleus (BLDI)
Right 192.26 ± 22.75 0.098 ± 0.004 202.12 ± 24.55 0.097 ± 0.004 Meansadj = -6.46, χ 2(1) = 2.92; P = 0.08
Left 187.24 ± 22.87 0.099 ± 0.005 198.89 ± 23.25 0.099 ± 0.004
Basomedial nucleus (BM)
Right 109.04 ± 17.47 0.056 ± 0.005 114.37 ± 15.76 0.055 ± 0.004 Meansadj = -0.79, χ 2(1) = 0.44; P = 0.83
Left 106.64 ± 14.14 0.057 ± 0.004 111.48 ± 13.88 0.056 ± 0.006
Central nucleus (CEN)
Right 46.95 ± 8.06 0.024 ± 0.003 49.04 ± 6.95 0.024 ± 0.002 Meansadj = 2.16, χ 2(1) = 0.33; P = 0.57
Left 46.28 ± 7.53 0.025 ± 0.003 48.46 ± 6.79 0.025 ± 0.003
Cortical and medial nuclei (CMN)
Right 151.04 ± 22.24 0.077 ± 0.006 156.99 ± 19.61 0.078 ± 0.007 Meansadj = -3.11, χ 2(1) = 0.67; P = 0.41
Left 147.21 ± 19.26 0.078 ± 0.006 156.07 ± 19.44 0.076 ± 0.007
Ventral division of BLDI and paralaminar nucleus (BLVPL)
Right 112.21 ± 13.2 0.058 ± 0.005 120.63 ± 16.43 0.058 ± 0.004 Meansadj = -4.39, χ 2(1) = 1.35; P = 0.25
Left 111.06 ± 14.55 0.059 ± 0.004 120 ± 15.87 0.060 ± 0.004
Amygdala transition areas (ATA)
Right 69.98 ± 10.49 0.036 ± 0.004 75.44 ± 12.05 0.037 ± 0.004 Meansadj = -0.60, χ 2(1) = 0.25; P = 0.87
Left 67.7 ± 10.61 0.036 ± 0.003 69.63 ± 9.21 0.035 ± 0.003
Amygdalostriatal transition area (ASTA)
Right 63.48 ± 8.1 0.033 ± 0.004 69.99 ± 9.4 0.034 ± 0.004 Meansadj = -2.18, χ 2(1) = 0.33; P = 0.56
Left 63.12 ± 8.6 0.034 ± 0.004 69.56 ± 10.13 0.035 ± 0.004
Anterior amygdala area (AAA)
Right 61.74 ± 7.92 0.032 ± 0.003 61.44 ± 7.49 0.030 ± 0.003 Meansadj = 3.64, χ 2(1) = 0.92; P = 0.34
Left 59.14 ± 6.84 0.032 ± 0.003 60.76 ± 7.66 0.031 ± 0.004

Total reflects larger or smaller subregion volume; RVF (relative volume fraction) reflects the fraction of the total amygdala that is included in this subregion.

Meansadj† (adjusted means for BMI z-score, bone age standard deviation, and ICV).

Significant group (F[1,56] = 8.99, p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.02] and group-by-region interactions (F[5,671] = 7.80, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.03] were also seen in the absolute volumes for the hippocampal subfields (Fig. 3B). Follow-up post-hoc analyses revealed that the subiculum and CA1 subfields were significantly smaller in CAH youth compared to controls (Fig. 3B, Table 3). For hippocampal RVF, the overall main effect of the group was not significant (F[1,57] = 1.82, P = 0.18, ηp2 = 0.002), but the group-by-region interaction was significant (F[6,671] = 5.07, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.03) (Fig. 3B, Table 4). Specifically, the RVF findings showed that the presubiculum, subiculum, and CA1 were proportionately smaller, while the CA3 region was proportionately larger within the hippocampus in patients with CAH compared to controls. Albeit significantly different, the differences in apportionment between CAH and control youth were very small in magnitude (<1% difference).

Table 4.

Hippocampal subfields mean and SD for CAH and control youth

CAH (n = 27) Control (n = 35) Group-by-Region Post-hoc Test
Total RVF Total RVF Total† RVF‡
Parasubiculum
Right 69.15 ± 16.08 0.021 ± 0.004 70.27 ± 14.83 0.020 ± 0.004 Meansadj = 7.76, χ 2(1) = 0.47; P = 0.49 Meansadj= -0.0008, χ 2(1) = 0.49; P = 0.48
Left 68.49 ± 12.81 0.022 ± 0.004 70.23 ± 12.14 0.021 ± 0.004
Presubiculum
Right 291.48 ± 67.89 0.085 ± 0.007 319.14 ± 36.62 0.089 ± 0.006 Meansadj = -20.67, χ 2(1) = 3.36; P = 0.07 Meansadj = -0.003, χ 2(1) = 7.99; P = 0.005
Left 285.38 ± 47.4 0.089 ± 0.009 317.45 ± 36.61 0.092 ± 0.005
Subiculum
Right 401.79 ± 111.56 0.117 ± 0.01 429.61 ± 55.35 0.120 ± 0.007 Meansadj = -25.76, χ 2(1) = 5.22; P = 0.02 Meansadj = -0.003, χ 2(1) = 6.29; P = 0.01
Left 381.15 ± 51.69 0.119 ± 0.007 423.23 ± 54.67 0.122 ± 0.007
CA1
Right 626.94 ± 107.6 0.184 ± 0.009 666.49 ± 87.09 0.185 ± 0.011 Meansadj = -42.40, χ 2(1) = 14.13; P < 0.0002 Meansadj= -0.0025, χ 2(1) = 4.89; P = 0.03
Left 571.1 ± 61.85 0.179 ± 0.008 634.74 ± 82.84 0.183 ± 0.009
CA3
Right 227.15 ± 39.9 0.067 ± 0.007 229.55 ± 32.18 0.064 ± 0.006 Meansadj = 4.05, χ 2(1) = 0.13; P = 0.71 Meansadj = 0.003, χ 2(1) = 6.25; P = 0.01
Left 209.96 ± 24.22 0.066 ± 0.006 217.84 ± 29.65 0.063 ± 0.006
Dentate gyrus (DG)
Right 289.33 ± 48.44 0.085 ± 0.005 304.37 ± 38.47 0.085 ± 0.005 Meansadj = -8.09, χ 2(1) = 0.51; P = 0.47 Meansadj = 0.0008, χ 2(1) = 0.46; P = 0.50
Left 272.55 ± 27.73 0.086 ± 0.004 292.08 ± 35.31 0.084 ± 0.004

Total reflects larger or smaller subregion volume; RVF (relative volume fraction) reflects the fraction of the total hippocampus that is included in this subregion.

Meansadj† (adjusted means for BMI z-score, bone age standard deviations, and ICV).

Meansadj‡ (adjusted means for BMI z-score and bone age standard deviation).

Brain volumes and clinical features in CAH youth

No associations were seen between brain volumes and CAH clinical features (data not shown), including markers of androgen excess (eg , BA SD) , total testosterone and androstenedione levels, 17-hydroxyprogesterone, or glucocorticoid daily dose (Table 1).

Finally, sensitivity analyses were performed to ensure findings were robust. Given that the BMI z-score and advancement of BA (ie, BA SD) were significantly different between CAH youth and controls, we included these variables as covariates to reduce the possibility of these group differences contributing to the variance in brain volumes between the 2 groups. However, we also performed a separate set of analyses without these covariates and found identical significance for results for all region and subregion analyses, with the only exception seen for total gray matter volume. Without adjusting for covariates, a significant group effect was seen in total gray matter volume (F[1,57] = 5.53, P = 0.022, ηp2 = 0.08), whereas the group effect for the adjusted model with covariates was not significant (F[1,55] = 1.48, P = 0.23, ηp2 = 0.02; Table 2). These findings suggest that group differences in total gray matter volume in CAH versus control youth are likely explained by their greater BMI and advancement of BA. Lastly, we also examined significant findings excluding the 3 CAH patients with brain anomalies. Results were nearly identical, although some regions, including the total left hippocampus, bilateral superior frontal cortex, and the relative volume fraction of CA1, became trend-level, and the left caudal middle frontal cortex was no longer significant (Table 5).

Table 5.

Sensitivity analyses excluding three CAH patients with brain anomalies

Brain Region Group Difference, P Value
Intracranial volume (ICV) 0.008
Cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) 0.007
Left hippocampus 0.07
Right superior frontal cortex 0.08
Left superior frontal cortex 0.09
Right caudal middle frontal cortex 0.01
Left caudal middle frontal cortex 0.11
Left rostral middle frontal cortex 0.02
Lateral nucleus (LA) <0.001
Subiculum 0.001
CA1 <0.001
Presubiculum relative volume fraction (RVF) 0.001
Subiculum relative volume fraction (RVF) 0.003
CA1 relative volume fraction (RVF) 0.07
CA3 relative volume fraction (RVF) 0.019

Discussion

Our study quantified regional differences in gray matter morphometry of the prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala in youth affected with classical CAH compared to control youth. We found smaller total volumes of the amygdala and hippocampus in CAH youth, similar to prior findings (15, 9), and expanded upon these findings to show regional volume differences in the prefrontal cortex (smaller volumes of the superior and caudal middle frontal cortex), lateral nucleus of the amygdala, and subiculum and CA1 subregions of the hippocampus in CAH youth compared to controls. We also found CAH youth to have smaller intracranial and increased cerebrospinal fluid volumes, signifying overall smaller whole brain volumes as well as regional specific differences in the prefrontal and medial temporal lobe regions during childhood and adolescent development.

Over the last 2 decades, excluding various case studies (39–41), only a small number of experimental MRI studies have examined gray matter volumes in children and adults affected with CAH. The first study included 39 patients (3 months to 26 years old), 11 of whom displayed smaller temporal lobes on T1- and T2-weighted MRI; albeit the study design did not include a true control group (42). Another study quantified total amygdala and hippocampal volumes in children with CAH as compared to controls, showing smaller amygdala volumes but no difference in hippocampal volumes, in both males and females with CAH (9). These findings are in contrast, however, to a more recent study that found only a trend-level difference in the left amygdala and a significantly smaller right hippocampus in 19 adult women with CAH compared to unaffected women (15). Of note, the latter 2 studies also reported global level differences in brain structure, including trend-level differences in total cerebral volume in female youth with CAH (9) and increased CSF in adult women with CAH (15). Our findings suggest both smaller intracranial volumes, as well as increased CSF, in both males and females with CAH, using a higher-resolution MRI scan and both T1- and T2-weighted images to improve accuracy in quantifying gray matter and white matter boundaries. Although we found that, in general, total bilateral amygdala and hippocampal volumes were smaller in CAH youth compared to controls, the differences in absolute amygdala and hippocampal volumes between CAH and control youth may be a consequence of overall smaller brain volumes given that group differences exist in ICV. After accounting for smaller ICV in our final models, only the left hippocampus was significantly smaller in both males and females with CAH compared to control youth, suggesting that differences in the amygdala may be a function of global differences in brain volumes, whereas the smaller left hippocampal volume is not a function of the overall difference in brain size.

Our interest in examining the PFC, amygdala, and hippocampus in CAH stems from the high concentrations of androgens, as well as mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptors, in these neural regions of nonhuman animals (4, 43, 44). However, nonhuman primate and human studies suggest both the amygdala and the hippocampal formation have heterogeneous subregions in terms of their cytoarchitecture and projections (45, 46). A major signaling pathway of the hippocampus, known as the perforant path, includes projections from the entorhinal cortex to both the DG and CA3, and then onto the CA1 hippocampal subfields. The DG is comprised of dense granule cells that send their projections, known as mossy fibers, to CA3, whereas the pyramidal cells in CA3 send their axons, known as the Schaffer collaterals, to CA1. The CA1 projects to the subiculum, and is the main output of the hippocampus (46). Amygdala nuclei also have distinct connectivity patterns. The basolateral portion of the amygdala, including the lateral (LA) and basal (BLDI, BM, and BLVPL regions) nuclei, receive sensory and regulatory information from the thalamus and PFC, respectively. Within the amygdala, the basolateral nuclei send projections to the central and medial nuclei, which project to the hypothalamus and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST) (45). Moreover, reciprocal connections exist between the hippocampus and amygdala, and the PFC and amygdala, with basal nuclei of the amygdala projecting to the entorhinal cortex and PFC, and the subiculum and CA1 subfields of the hippocampus projecting back to the basolateral amygdala (47). Our findings suggest that within these heterogeneous medial temporal lobe structures, the lateral nucleus of the amygdala and the subiculum and CA1 hippocampal subregions are particularly affected in youth with CAH. In addition to overall size differences, the current study also examined the relative volume fractions, or the apportionment of subregions within the amygdala and hippocampus. The subnuclei within the amygdala were found to be proportionally similar, whereas the subfields within the hippocampus were found to be significantly different in proportion, between youth with CAH and healthy controls. Specifically, in youth with CAH, the primary output of the perforant path (including CA1, presubiculum, and subiculum regions) was disproportionately smaller compared to the overall size of the hippocampus, whereas the CA3 region occupied a larger proportion of the hippocampus compared to controls. Although the group difference in relative proportions for any given hippocampal subfield was small (ie, <1% difference), these findings suggest that not only are subfield volumes affected, but that there are slight differences in the structural organization of subfields within the hippocampus, and perhaps even compensatory mechanisms, in CAH youth as compared to controls.

While structure does not necessarily imply function, additional research is needed to determine the potential real-world consequences of these structural findings in terms of emotional and reward-related behaviors. Since the lateral nuclei act as the major input of information to be processed by the amygdala, it is possible that structural differences in this region may reflect altered cognitive and sensory integration of emotional stimuli or experiences, which may help to explain previously-reported differences in subjective ratings of negative stimuli found in CAH patients (11). Given the role of the CA1 and subiculum as major outputs of the hippocampus and playing an important role in both emotional and non-emotional memory, reduced volumes in these regions may also relate to previous findings of poorer memory for negative emotional stimuli (10, 12) and the moderate to large reductions in short-term and working memory abilities seen in individuals with CAH (7, 8).

Mechanistically, it remains unclear as to what feature(s) inherent to CAH are responsible for decreases in cortical volumes in the prefrontal and medial temporal lobes. Congenital adrenal hyperplasia includes prenatal glucocorticoid deficiency, excess prenatal and postnatal androgens, and postnatal glucocorticoid treatment. Although clinical features have been found to relate to some cognitive tests or other brain biomarkers (15), we and others have not yet found markers of glucocorticoid or androgen exposure to predict amygdala or hippocampal volumes (15, 9). It has also been postulated that high levels of androgen exposure prenatally may masculinize brains of females with CAH. However, in our study, group differences in brain structure did not significantly differ between the sexes (ie, group-by-sex interaction terms were not significant in any of our models), suggesting that structural differences compared to controls are similar in both males and females with CAH. We and others have not found male-sized brain volumes in females with CAH (9), with preserved sexual dimorphism in our study in regard to females having smaller gray matter volumes compared to males. These findings suggest that the structure of the PFC and medial temporal lobe is not masculinized.

Instead of resulting from excessive androgen exposure, the smaller amygdala, hippocampus, and PFC volumes in both males and females with CAH may be a byproduct of their potentially diminished role in neural regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, as a result of the cortisol deficiency seen in CAH (48). In healthy individuals, cells within the amygdala and hippocampus project to the hypothalamus to create a vital negative feedback loop for the HPA axis in response to stress (49). In CAH, perhaps hippocampal and amygdala neurons succumb to the “use it or lose it” theory of neuroplasticity, resulting in less synaptic proliferation, or even programmed cell death, after failing to establish a typical neurofeedback circuit due to impairments in the HPA axis. Support for this hypothesis is evidenced by other pediatric clinical disorders associated with HPA dysfunction, such as hypercortisolism (Cushing’s syndrome), which is also linked to smaller total brain volumes, including the amygdala and hippocampus. In addition, suppressed cortisol awakening responses have also been associated with smaller PFC and hippocampal volumes in individuals at high risk for psychosis based on desensitized HPA responsivity (50). Further support for structural brain differences potentially being linked to HPA dysfunction in youth with CAH also stems from smaller volumes localized to the subiculum and CA1 hippocampal subfields. The CA1 responds to glucocorticoids in a dose-response fashion and the subiculum plays a key role in inhibiting the HPA axis (51, 52). Taken together, these data suggest that disruptions of HPA homeostasis may result in altered amygdala, hippocampus, and PFC volumes (53). Alternatively, a lack of association between brain volumes and exposures due to treatment or hormone concentrations may be muddled by individual differences in glucocorticoid-related genes, which may otherwise complicate hormone concentrations and clearance, as well as physiological neural responses (54). In addition, neurologic impairments, including possible brain injury, may result from adrenal crises associated with CAH, which merits further study (7). More research is needed to better understand the biological mechanisms that may contribute to smaller regional volumes of the amygdala, hippocampus, and PFC in youth with CAH.

Strengths of the current study include the high-resolution T1- and T2- weighted MRI approach, allowing for more accurate quantification of tissue as well as segmentation of the amygdala and hippocampus into distinct subregions. A few limitations should also be noted. Although equal or larger than previous studies (15, 9), our CAH sample size is still relatively small, which may have limited our ability to detect small to medium effects. In addition, the composition of CAH participants in our study limited our ability to compare salt-wasting versus simple-virilizing forms, as the majority of patients were salt-wasters. Other studies have included mostly patients with the salt-wasting form (15), which could reflect the distribution of salt-wasting to simple virilizing in clinical CAH populations. In addition, this study compared youth with CAH to youth without a chronic medical condition (although with similar age and sex characteristics). Given that youth with classical CAH are postnatally treated with glucocorticoids and must receive clinical care over their lifetime, additional controls that do not have CAH but that have been exposed to glucocorticoids from an early age, may be useful to study. Moving forward, it will be imperative for the field to conduct larger studies of patients with CAH, and multiple control groups, in order to better understand how various clinical features, including commonly seen brain anomalies, may contribute to neurological phenotypes. Previous studies have highlighted an increased prevalence of type 1 Chiari anomalies in patients with CAH (15, 42), and treatment regimens as well as adherence to the glucocorticoid and/or mineralocorticoid replacement in CAH varies greatly among patients. Pooled MRI studies across multiple clinical research centers may ultimately help to better understand both individual differences, and common patterns of altered brain structure, by clinical CAH phenotype and concurrent brain anomalies. Lastly, the current study was designed to examine structural differences in a priori gray matter in CAH versus the control youth. However, studies have also suggested impairments in white matter (15, 42), as well as brain function (7, 8, 10, 11, 13), in patients with CAH. Thus, studies are currently underway to probe additional potential differences in brain structure and function as a focus of future research.

We conclude that youth affected with classical CAH have overall smaller intracranial volumes as well as reduced regional volumes in the prefrontal cortex, amygdala, and hippocampus as compared to healthy controls, suggesting brain alterations are associated with CAH during childhood and adolescent development. Future studies to determine if volumetric differences in the PFC, lateral nucleus of the amygdala, subiculum, and CA1 hippocampal subregions map onto physiological and behavioral phenotypes are needed in patients with CAH.

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully thank all participants and their families. In addition, we would like to acknowledge Norma Martinez, Heather Ross, Christina Koppin, Kimberly Felix, Veeraya Tanawattanacharoen, and Eva Gabor for assisting with participant recruitment and data collection. Finally, we thank the CARES Foundation for their ongoing support of the CHLA CAH Comprehensive Care Center.

Financial Support: This study was supported by the National Institutes of Health K01 MH1087610 (MMH), R03HD090308 (MMH), and K23HD084735 (MSK), Conte Center Grant, Neuroimaging Core (5P50MH094258- 5388), the CARES Foundation (MEG and MSK), and the Abell Foundation (MEG).

Additional Information

Disclosure Summary: MEG receives grant support from Novo Nordisk; consultant fees from Daiichi Sankyo, Ferring, Novo Nordisk, Nutrition & Growth Solutions, Pfizer, Sandoz, and Spruce Biosciences; serves on data safety monitoring boards for Ascendis, Millendo, and Tolmar; and receives royalties from McGraw-Hill and UpToDate.

Data Availability Statement: Restrictions apply to the availability of data generated or analyzed during this study to preserve patient confidentiality or because they were used under license. The corresponding author will on request detail the restrictions and any conditions under which access to some data may be provided.

References

  • 1. Merke DP, Bornstein SR. Congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Lancet. 2005;365(9477):2125–2136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Clayton PE, Miller WL, Oberfield SE, Ritzén EM, Sippell WG, Speiser PW; ESPE/ LWPES CAH Working Group Consensus statement on 21-hydroxylase deficiency from the European Society for Paediatric Endocrinology and the Lawson Wilkins Pediatric Endocrine Society. Horm Res. 2002;58(4):188–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. New MI. Diagnosis and management of congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Annu Rev Med. 1998;49:311–328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Nuñez JL, Huppenbauer CB, McAbee MD, Juraska JM, DonCarlos LL. Androgen receptor expression in the developing male and female rat visual and prefrontal cortex. J Neurobiol. 2003;56(3):293–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Gray JD, Kogan JF, Marrocco J, McEwen BS. Genomic and epigenomic mechanisms of glucocorticoids in the brain. Nat Rev Endocrinol. 2017;13(11):661–673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Mueller SC. Magnetic resonance imaging in paediatric psychoneuroendocrinology: a new frontier for understanding the impact of hormones on emotion and cognition. J Neuroendocrinol. 2013;25(8):762–770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Collaer ML, Hindmarsh PC, Pasterski V, Fane BA, Hines M. Reduced short term memory in congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH) and its relationship to spatial and quantitative performance. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2016;64:164–173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Browne WV, Hindmarsh PC, Pasterski V, et al. . Working memory performance is reduced in children with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Horm Behav. 2015;67:83–88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Merke DP, Fields JD, Keil MF, Vaituzis AC, Chrousos GP, Giedd JN. Children with classic congenital adrenal hyperplasia have decreased amygdala volume: potential prenatal and postnatal hormonal effects. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2003;88(4):1760–1765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Mazzone L, Mueller SC, Maheu F, VanRyzin C, Merke DP, Ernst M. Emotional memory in early steroid abnormalities: an FMRI study of adolescents with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Dev Neuropsychol. 2011;36(4):473–492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Ernst M, Maheu FS, Schroth E, et al. . Amygdala function in adolescents with congenital adrenal hyperplasia: a model for the study of early steroid abnormalities. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(9):2104–2113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Maheu FS, Merke DP, Schroth EA, et al. . Steroid abnormalities and the developing brain: declarative memory for emotionally arousing and neutral material in children with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2008;33(2):238–245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Mueller SC, Daniele T, MacIntyre J, et al. . Incentive processing in Congenital Adrenal Hyperplasia (CAH): a reward-based antisaccade study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38(5):716–721. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Mueller SC, Ng P, Sinaii N, et al. . Psychiatric characterization of children with genetic causes of hyperandrogenism. Eur J Endocrinol. 2010;163(5):801–810. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Webb EA, Elliott L, Carlin D, et al. . Quantitative Brain MRI in Congenital Adrenal Hyperplasia: in Vivo Assessment of the Cognitive and Structural Impact of Steroid Hormones. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2018;103(4):1330–1341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Wan FJ, Swerdlow NR. The basolateral amygdala regulates sensorimotor gating of acoustic startle in the rat. Neurosci. 1997;76(3):715–724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Schoenbaum G, Chiba AA, Gallagher M. Neural encoding in orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala during olfactory discrimination learning. J Neurosci. 1999;19(5):1876–1884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Baxter MG, Murray EA. The amygdala and reward. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3(7):563–573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Lacy JW, Yassa MA, Stark SM, Muftuler LT, Stark CE. Distinct pattern separation related transfer functions in human CA3/dentate and CA1 revealed using high-resolution fMRI and variable mnemonic similarity. Learn Mem. 2011;18(1):15–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Eldridge LL, Engel SA, Zeineh MM, Bookheimer SY, Knowlton BJ. A dissociation of encoding and retrieval processes in the human hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2005;25(13):3280–3286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Adolphs R. Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2002;12(2):169–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Preston AR, Eichenbaum H. Interplay of hippocampus and prefrontal cortex in memory. Curr Biol. 2013;23(17):R764–R773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Tyszka JM, Pauli WM. In vivo delineation of subdivisions of the human amygdaloid complex in a high-resolution group template. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37(11):3979–3998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Iglesias JE, Augustinack JC, Nguyen K, et al. ; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative A computational atlas of the hippocampal formation using ex vivo, ultra-high resolution MRI: application to adaptive segmentation of in vivo MRI. Neuroimage. 2015;115:117–137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Finkielstain GP, Kim MS, Sinaii N, et al. . Clinical characteristics of a cohort of 244 patients with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2012;97(12):4429–4438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Guo SS, et al. . 2000 CDC growth charts for the United States: methods and development. Vital Health Stat. 2002;11(246):1–190. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. A SAS Program for the 2000 CDC Growth Charts (ages 0 to <20 years). Atlanta, GA: Division of Nutrition, Physical Activity, and Obesity, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Creo AL, Schwenk WF, 2nd. bone age: a handy tool for pediatric providers. Pediatrics. 2017;140(6). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Greulich WW, Pyle SI. Radiographic Atlas ofs Skeletal Development of the Hand and Wrist. 2 ed. California: Stanford University Press; 1959. [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Gilsanz V, Ratib O.. Hand Bone Age: A Digital Atlas of Skeletal Maturity. Germany: Springer; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. Fourth ed. San Antonio, TX: Pearson; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Reuter M, Schmansky NJ, Rosas HD, Fischl B. Within-subject template estimation for unbiased longitudinal image analysis. Neuroimage. 2012;61(4):1402–1418. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, et al. . Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. 2002;33(3):341–355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Backhausen LL, Herting MM, Buse J, Roessner V, Smolka MN, Vetter NC. Quality control of structural MRI images applied using freesurfer-a hands-on workflow to rate motion artifacts. Front Neurosci. 2016;10:558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Avants BB, Duda JT, Zhang H, Gee JC. Multivariate normalization with symmetric diffeomorphisms for multivariate studies. Med Image Comput Comput Assist Interv. 2007;10(Pt 1):359–366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. O’Brien LM, Ziegler DA, Deutsch CK, Frazier JA, Herbert MR, Locascio JJ. Statistical adjustments for brain size in volumetric neuroimaging studies: some practical implications in methods. Psychiatry Res. 2011;193(2):113–122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Sanfilipo MP, Benedict RH, Zivadinov R, Bakshi R. Correction for intracranial volume in analysis of whole brain atrophy in multiple sclerosis: the proportion vs. residual method. Neuroimage. 2004;22(4):1732–1743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Cabeen R, Laidlaw D, Toga A. Quantitative imaging toolkit: software for interactive 3D visualization, data exploration, and computational analysis of neuroimaging datasets In: Proceedings International Society for Magnetic Resonance in Medicine (ISMRM) 2018;2018:2854. [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Mnif MF, Kamoun M, Mnif F, et al. . Brain magnetic resonance imaging findings in adult patients with congenital adrenal hyperplasia: increased frequency of white matter impairment and temporal lobe structures dysgenesis. Indian J Endocrinol Metab. 2013;17(1):121–127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Sinforiani E, Livieri C, Mauri M, et al. . Cognitive and neuroradiological findings in congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1994;19(1):55–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Gaudiano C, Malandrini A, Pollazzon M, et al. . Leukoencephalopathy in 21-beta hydroxylase deficiency: report of a family. Brain Dev. 2010;32(5):421–424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Nass R, Heier L, Moshang T, et al. . Magnetic resonance imaging in the congenital adrenal hyperplasia population: increased frequency of white-matter abnormalities and temporal lobe atrophy. J Child Neurol. 1997;12(3):181–186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Lupien SJ, McEwen BS. The acute effects of corticosteroids on cognition: integration of animal and human model studies. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1997;24(1):1–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Simerly RB, Chang C, Muramatsu M, Swanson LW. Distribution of androgen and estrogen receptor mRNA-containing cells in the rat brain: an in situ hybridization study. J Comp Neurol. 1990;294(1):76–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Amaral DG, Price JL, Pikanen A, Carmichael ST. Anatomical organization of the primate amygdaloid complex. In: Aggleton JP, ed. The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction. New York: Wiley-Liss; 1992:1–66. [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Duvernoy HM, Francoise C, Risold PY.. The Human Hippocampus: Functional Anatomy, Vascularization and Serial Sections with MRI. Berlin Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Blair HT, Fanselow MS. Fear and Memory: A View of the Hippocampus Through the Lens of the Amygdala. In: Derdikman D, Knieram J eds. Space,Time and Memory in the Hippocampal Formation. Vienna: Springer; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Raff H, Sharma ST, Nieman LK. Physiological basis for the etiology, diagnosis, and treatment of adrenal disorders: cushing’s syndrome, adrenal insufficiency, and congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Compr Physiol. 2014;4(2):739–769. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Herman JP, McKlveen JM, Ghosal S, et al. . Regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical stress response. Compr Physiol. 2016;6(2):603–621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Valli I, Crossley NA, Day F, et al. . HPA-axis function and grey matter volume reductions: imaging the diathesis-stress model in individuals at ultra-high risk of psychosis. Transl Psychiatry. 2016;6(5):e797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. O’Mara S. The subiculum: what it does, what it might do, and what neuroanatomy has yet to tell us. J Anat. 2005;207(3):271–282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Tasker JG, Herman JP. Mechanisms of rapid glucocorticoid feedback inhibition of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Stress. 2011;14(4):398–406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Erickson K, Drevets W, Schulkin J. Glucocorticoid regulation of diverse cognitive functions in normal and pathological emotional states. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2003;27(3):233–246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Nebesio TD, Renbarger JL, Nabhan ZM, et al. . Differential effects of hydrocortisone, prednisone, and dexamethasone on hormonal and pharmacokinetic profiles: a pilot study in children with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Int J Pediatr Endocrinol. 2016;2016(1):17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism are provided here courtesy of The Endocrine Society

RESOURCES