Abstract
Background
In developmental science, there is an extensive literature on non-food related self-regulation in childhood, where several domains relating to emotions, actions and cognitions have been identified. There is now growing attention to food related self-regulation in childhood, especially difficulties with ASR, and the consequences for weight gain and adiposity. The aim of this narrative review was to conduct a reciprocal analysis of self-regulation in the food and non-food domains in childhood (referred to as appetite self-regulation (ASR) and general self-regulation (GSR) respectively). The focus was on commonalities and differences in key concepts and underpinning processes.
Methods
Databases and major journals were searched using terms such as self-regulation, appetite self-regulation, or self-regulation of energy intake, together with associated constructs (e.g., Executive Function, Effortful Control, delay-of-gratification). This was followed by backward and forward snowballing.
Results and discussion
The scholarship on GSR in childhood has had a focus on the role of the cognitively-oriented Executive Function (EF), the temperamentally-based Effortful Control (EC) and the recursive interplay between bottom-up (reactive, emotion driven, approach or avoidance) and top-down (cognitive, conscious decision-making) processes. “Hot” and “cool/cold” EF and self-regulation situations have been distinguished. There were some parallels between GSR and ASR in these areas, but uncertainty about the contribution of EF and EC to ASR in young children. Possible differences between the contribution to ASR-related outcomes of delay-of-gratification in food and non-food tasks were apparent. Unique elements of ASR were identified; associated with psychological, biological and neurological responses to food and bottom-up processes. A diverse number of situations or elements connected to ASR exist: for example, energy balance homeostasis, caloric compensation, hunger regulation, satiation, satiety, energy density of food, eating in the absence of hunger, emotional eating, etc.
Conclusions
Self-regulation in food and non-food domains are amenable to a reciprocal analysis. We argue that self-regulation of appetite should be added as a domain under the umbrella of self-regulation in childhood along with the other non-food related domains. This could lead to a broader understanding of self-regulation in childhood, and generate novel lines of enquiry.
Keywords: Appetite regulation, Energy intake, Executive function, Homeostasis, Self-regulation, Disinhibited eating, Effortful control, Inhibitory control, Top-down, Bottom-up
Background
Self-regulation is important for children’s healthy functioning and development, as shown by its associations with a wide range of developmental outcomes, including behavioral, social, emotional and academic adjustment [1–8], school readiness [9, 10] positive health outcomes [11] and overweight/obesity [7, 12–16]. Self-regulation has been identified as a central aspect of development in the early years [4, 8, 11, 17–19].
To clarify different forms of self-regulation, Saltzman and colleagues [20] and Anderson and Keim [21] drew a separation between food and non-food related self-regulation in childhood and referred to non-food related self-regulation as “general” self-regulation. In the childhood developmental science literature, Saltzman et al. and Anderson and Keim’s “general” self-regulation is simply referred to as self-regulation. It has received extensive research and theoretical attention [4, 18, 22–25]. In this literature, self-regulation has been conceived as an umbrella term, with the development of self-regulation occurring across a number of domains or levels of functioning including physiological arousal, attentional engagement and disengagement, emotional regulation, behavioral regulation, and executive cognitive control processes [3, 5, 22, 23], but not appetite. For present purposes, we refer to the “self-regulation” from developmental science as general self-regulation (GSR).
Scholarship on food-related self-regulation in childhood has emerged rapidly in recent years. It has included attention to self-regulation of energy intake (SREI) [26–29], and more generally to appetite self-regulation (ASR) or self-regulation of eating [13, 20, 30–34] where self-regulation difficulties have been repeatedly associated with poor dietary intakes and weight status in children.
ASR is a general construct and incorporates the roles of both hunger and satiety in prompting and stopping energy intake [20]. It includes not only energy intake, but diet quality (e.g., selection of healthy or unhealthy food), and is closely linked with energy expenditure. ASR covers the positive aspects of regulation, but also the inverse in terms of the disruption of ASR, in the form of disinhibited eating and related concepts. The significance of food related self-regulation is recognized by claims [35–37] that healthy eating and food decisions require effortful and goal-directed self-regulation. At the same time, self-regulation is only one of many factors that contribute to dietary intake and the development of overweight and obesity (OW/OB) [16, 31, 38–42]. Nevertheless, difficulty with appetite self-regulation has been recognized as a possible pathway in the development of OW/OB in some children [16, 32, 43], and is often a target in preventive interventions [11, 28, 31, 44–47].
The importance of helping children develop SREI was identified two decades ago [48]. The evidence base about SREI and ASR in childhood has substantially expanded in recent years, and has begun to draw on the constructs, evidence, theories and methodologies associated with GSR, especially constructs such as the neurocognitively oriented Executive Function (EF) [20, 21, 49–57] the temperament-based Effortful Control (EC) [12, 29, 58–60], Michel’s delay-of-gratification paradigm [24, 61–63], and emotion regulation/dysregulation [14, 26, 64]. However, despite some cross-fertilization, to date, there do not appear to have been efforts to systematically compare and contrast research and theory in what remain as relatively separate bodies of scholarship in ASR and GSR.
The purpose of the present narrative review is to conduct a reciprocal analysis of ASR and GSR in childhood through an examination of (1) key concepts and processes in GSR and ASR, (2) evidence about the possibility of common processes underpinning GSR and ASR, and (3) the extent to which GSR could be implicated in ASR-related outcomes such as disinhibited eating, Body Mass Index (BMI) and obesity. A reciprocal analysis implies that there are likely to be parallels as well as contrasts between ASR and GSR, with the potential for GSR to provide insights for ASR as well as ASR to provide insights for a wider treatment of GSR. The primary focus is on childhood, meaning from infancy to about ages 6 or 7. All areas included in the review themselves represent substantial bodies of knowledge. As a consequence, at times the coverage is somewhat introductory.
The evidence suggests that there are relatively unique features of ASR when compared to GSR. As a consequence, we argue there is value in an integration of research and theory about GSR and ASR, so that the broad field of self-regulation in childhood encompasses a number of domains including those related to both non-food self-regulation (e.g., emotions, actions and cognitions) and ASR. The discussion includes implications for research and theory that arise from linking GSR and ASR under a common self-regulation umbrella.
The international literature on weight gain and obesity has recognized that the overconsumption of foods and beverages, especially palatable and energy dense food, is an intractable problem in current obesogenic environments [31, 39, 41, 65, 66]. Drivers of food consumption are complex and multifactorial and cannot easily be solved with simple interventions. There is a need to better understand how healthy food consumption can be supported. Better knowledge of processes and mechanisms underlying individual differences in the development and disruption of appetite regulation and behavior could help to explain how and why individuals respond differently to food environments and to eating interventions. Even with a widespread application of policy levers to effect change in food environments, understanding the development of healthy self-regulation in children could contribute new information on possible solutions to the problem of achieving healthy food choices and intakes in obesogenic environments.
Methods
To identify literature and relevant journals, we searched titles, abstracts and key words in databases (PubMed, PsycINFO, SCOPUS, Web of Science, Google Scholar) using the terms ‘self-regulation’, ‘appetite self-regulation’, or ‘self-regulation of energy intake’ together with associated constructs (e.g., Executive Function, Effortful Control, delay-of-gratification). We followed up with backward and forward snowballing. Given the quickly developing nature of the field, special efforts during the time of writing and up to submission were directed at “in press” articles from the key journals that were identified. Selection of articles was mainly limited to children or childhood, with a focus from infancy to age 6 or 7 years. The search also yielded literature from middle childhood, adolescence and adulthood and a selection of these publications was used to add insights about possible processes in GSR and ASR.
Results
The results are presented first for key concepts and processes in general self-regulation followed by parallel literature on appetite self-regulation. Evidence pertaining to common underpinnings of GSR and ASR is presented. Finally, theory and evidence about links between food and non-food delay-of-gratification and ASR-related outcomes is examined.
Key concepts and processes in general self-regulation
In developmental science, self-regulation is often conceived as a general goal-directed behavior, with self-control seen as an instance of successful self-regulation [3, 8, 67]. However, self-control and other concepts such as delay-of-gratification, temporal discounting, inhibitory control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control “have all been related to, and are often treated as synonymous with, self-regulation” ([68] p. 91). At the broadest level, self-regulation/self-control has been conceived as involving an ability, capacity or use of strategies to override or change one’s inner responses such as desires or impulses, as well as to interrupt undesired behavioral tendencies and refrain from acting on them [69], “the process or behavior of overcoming a temptation or prepotent response in favor of a competing goal (either concurrent or longer term)” ([67] p. 80), or as executive processes modulating prepotent responses [68].
While there seems to be broad definitional agreement, a number of reviews have summarized differences in the core constructs, measures or definitions pertaining to GSR in childhood, such as EF, EC, self-regulation, inhibitory control and delay-of-gratification [3, 24, 68, 70]. In terms of the precise definitions of self-regulation and its central constructs, there can be considerable differences as shown by Cole et al.’s [68] table of selected theories and definitions of self-regulation, a parallel table by Nigg [3] and other critical analyses (e.g., [67]). Cole et al. ([68] p. 91) noted that the US National Institutes of Health “lamented the plethora of terms, conceptualizations, and methods used to define and study self-regulation”, arguing that this hindered our understanding of the basic mechanisms associated with important health and developmental outcomes.
Despite this complexity, there is some agreement about core constructs and processes in general self-regulation. Much of this agreement is captured in the reviews by Cole et al. [68] Gagne [24], Nigg [3] and Bridgett et al. [71], from which the core features of what Nigg [3] described as a domain-general model of self-regulation can be outlined. From these reviews and other literature, there appear to be four main elements of a domain-general model of self-regulation. First, the model involves recursive top-down and bottom-up processes in the regulation of action, emotion and delay of gratification. Top-down and bottom-up processes can be differentiated conceptually, but also at the level of neurobiology [72]. Top down processes have been described as “reflecting more effortful/executive control process served by cortical structures in the anterior cingulate cortex” ([71] p. 603). Bottom-up processes have been described as “reflecting more automatic (reactive) processes served primarily by subcortical structures” ([71] p. 603) and include spontaneous, emotion-driven appetitive processes. In addition to bottom-up processes being the targets of self-regulation, they can also be the source of self-regulation [3]. For example, fear of or avoidance of weight gain could contribute to regulation when palatable food is available.
Second, in a domain-general model, self-regulation is a dynamic process involving an interplay between top-down and bottom-up processes over time [68]. Third, there are two major elements of bottom-up processes, namely bottom-up avoidance (e.g., behavioral inhibition/fear, anxiety or behavioral over-control) and bottom-up approach (e.g., impulsivity/behavioral under-control) [3, 71]. Fourth, in a domain-general model, EF and EC are centrally implicated in top-down processes [3, 71].
The impetus for self-regulatory strategies often derives from reactive, spontaneous, appetitive or avoidant bottom-up processes. The power of bottom-up processes is reflected in the choice of Cole et al. [68] to describe these as “prepotent responses”. There is a substantial body of evidence in the literature on GSR (and as we show below, an essentially parallel literature about ASR) concerning the role of both behavioral inhibition/fear and impulsivity [3, 71]. Nevertheless, it is not a one-way process with the impetus for self-regulation always to be found in bottom up processes. As Nigg ([3] p. 365) noted, “top-down systems can activate, suppress, or bias bottom-up responses”. At the same time, top-down control is effortful, so that bottom-up processes can influence the nature and extent of action control [73].
As the terms “effortful”, “executive” and “control” imply in describing top-down processes, EF and EC pertain largely to voluntary, conscious, or deliberate cognitive processes in the regulation of behaviour. EC is a temperament-based construct and “is defined as the ability to inhibit a dominant (motor, vocal, emotional or cognitive) response and to activate a subdominant response” ([74] pp. 1-2). The components of EC include inhibitory control, planned action, effortful attention, conflict resolution, error correction and abilities to delay or wait [74, 75]. Temperament is assumed to involve genetically-based inherent characteristics and it has been reasoned that EC is an important underpinning of self-regulated behavior [18], and it has been a core aspect of research on emotion regulation [23, 76–79].
EF originated from the field of clinical neuropsychology and “refers to the more deliberate or goal-directed, top-down neurocognitive processes involved in self-regulation” ([74] p. 2). It has been considered to be a significant element of self-regulation [80]. Hendry et al. ([81] p. 2) characterized EF as involving “higher-order self-regulatory processes”. The processes include inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility and working memory [24, 74, 82–84]. Blair ([70] p. 3) said of EF that it involves “the ability to hold information in working memory, to inhibit fast and unthinking responses to stimulation, and to flexibly shift the focus of one’s mental frame, is more or less the foundation for the intentional, volitional self-directed control of behavior. The cognitive skills that make up this construct help us to limit impulsive responses, to regulate emotions, and to avoid bad decisions that might bring short-term gain but longer-term problems”. Inhibitory control is a component of both EC and EF. In the case of EC, it is conceived as a dimension of temperament, while in EF it is treated as a cognitive process. Both EC and EF contain components in addition to inhibitory control (e.g., effortful attention and conflict resolution in EC, and cognitive flexibility and working memory in EF), but for present purposes the emphasis is on inhibitory control as a core ingredient of self-regulation.
The inhibitory control component of EF has usually been assessed in childhood using behavioral or performance measures involving emotionally neutral tasks that require children to inhibit a dominant response, as assumed to be required in self-regulation [24, 85, 86]. Examples are the silly sounds Stroop task, where children are asked to make the sound of a dog to a picture of a cat and the sound of a dog to a picture of a cat, or the pencil tap task, where the child taps twice if the experiment taps once and the child taps once if the experimenter taps twice.
The assessment of EC in childhood, including inhibitory control, has been based on both parent-rated temperament for toddlers through childhood [24, 87, 88] and performance-based measures, especially for preschool-aged children [87, 89]. The latter have included comparable tasks to those used to measure inhibitory control under the umbrella of EF. For example, children saying circle when shown a square and vice-versa [89]. For present purposes, the main point to highlight about these inhibitory control tasks for both EF and EC is their emphasis on inhibiting salient responses and on top-down processes. As Lin et al. [74] argued, EC and EF have their origins in different historical disciplines, but they have a number of conceptual and empirical overlaps.
Considerable attention has been directed to the question of the empirical overlaps of EF and EC. Lin et al. [74] found that measures from the two domains could load on a single “self-regulation” factor. Commonality between EF and EC has been found via a “cognitive control” factor that included temperament-rated inhibitory control and performance on EF inhibitory control tasks [80]. Nevertheless, there is also evidence that behavioral measures of EF and self-report measures of EC in adolescents could predict academic performance in complementary and independent ways [83]. At least in adolescents, Zorza et al. [83] argued that EF and EC share some elements, but do not completely overlap. In preadolescents, Tiego et al. [90] found that behavioral ratings of EF and EC measured the same self-regulation construct. However, based on assessments at age 4 years, Backer-Grondahl et al. ([8] p. 2) argued that EF and EC are “not completely synonymous”. Lengua et al. [75] suggested some separation of the executive and delay components of EC, possibly because they could stem from different brain regions, have different developmental courses, predict and relate to adjustment differently, and relate differently to cortisol. In the latter case they speculate that executive, but not delay tasks could be related to cortisol. Overall, therefore, while there seems to be considerable commonality between EF and EC, especially in the measurement and role of inhibitory control, questions about the separation of the components, both within and between EF and EC remains somewhat open.
EF has been broadened beyond abstract and decontextualized tasks through a distinction between “hot” EF and “cool/cold” EF [53, 74, 86, 91, 92], with “hot” implicated in emotionally arousing, or rewarding situations (such as tasks involving rewards of snacks, toys or gifts and measures of wait times and self-control behaviors) and “cool” in emotionally neutral situations (such as the decontextualized and cognitive tasks above, where there are no rewards or punishers). The use of hot and cool EF draws on the hot/cool delay of gratification distinction earlier highlighted by Mischel and colleagues [93, 94]. So-called hot situations imply a greater role for bottom-up processes, while cool situations are more amenable to top-down cognitive processes and decision-making. The hot/cool distinction is especially relevant for a reciprocal analysis of GSR and ASR because of its centrality in both domains of self-regulation.
In relation to possible differences between hot and cool self-regulation, Zelazo and Carlson [92] argue that the neural systems supporting EF could vary as a function of the motivational significance of the situation. They also suggested that the development of hot EF might lag the development of cool EF, and that for children younger than about 6 years, the distinction between hot and cool EF is emerging, as part of the increasing specialization of neural systems. Gagne [24] argued that hot strategies involve emotional or reactive responses, while cool strategies draw on cognitive processes, with younger children more prone to be reactive than older children.
In seminal research, Mischel and colleagues investigated aspects of will-power and self-control in preschool children using the now well-established delay-of-gratification paradigm with food rewards, in what is often referred to as the “marshmallow” task [8, 62, 93, 95]. The wait or delay aspect of delay-of-gratification is also inherent in inhibitory control as conceived in both EF and EC and these aspects are now frequently used in the measurement of inhibitory control. In delay-of-gratification tasks, both food and non-food rewards (such as an appealingly wrapped gift) have been used [14, 19, 74, 96]. Because of the use of a reward in the delay-of-gratification paradigm it has often been used as a measure of hot self-regulation [8, 93].
Overview
Central in models and analyses of GSR is the notion of the recursive interplay between top-down and bottom-up processing. In the case of bottom-up, the emphasis has been on spontaneous or reactive responses that can be either approach or avoidant. Much attention has been directed to the top-down processes via research on the function of EF and EC, especially via the inhibitory control element. The dynamic interplay or mutual influence between top-down and bottom-up processes, including the potential for bottom-up processes to interfere with top-down regulation, has also been a feature of models of GSR [3, 68]. Nigg [3] referred to the self-regulation “universe” in presenting a glossary of major terms relating to self-regulation. Here we have introduced some features of that universe to facilitate a reciprocal analysis of GSR and ASR.
Key concepts and processes in appetite self-regulation
The attention to overweight and obesity in children in recent decades has probably led to an emphasis on processes associated with ASR difficulties more than on the development of ASR in childhood. In contrast, in GSR there seems to have been a greater balance between attention to (1) the development of self-regulation and positive outcomes and (2) self-regulation difficulties or deficits. The greater positive emphasis in the GSR literature could arise from a more even focus on successful adjustment outcomes (e.g., in social, emotional, and academic domains) as well as maladjustment.
In general, the GSR description of self-regulation as pertaining to overriding inner responses, interrupting undesired tendencies and refraining from acting on them, or in terms of executive processes modulating prepotent responses, has also been generally applied to ASR (e.g., 30). In the case of ASR, the inner responses are internal signals about hunger and satiety and the prepotent responses are associated with food and eating. As already noted, research and theory about ASR implicates a wide range of influences and processes in self-regulation, many of which appear to be relatively unique to ASR.
Our survey of the literature showed that a large number of constructs has been used to characterize processes or to measure ASR in childhood, including ASR difficulties. The constructs are illustrated in Table 1 and extend across the two main sources of bottom-up processes identified in the literature on GSR, namely approach (such as food responsiveness, hedonic aspects of food, emotional eating), and avoidance (such as food avoidance, food neophobia and picky eating). The breadth and diversity of constructs that are relevant to processes and analyses of ASR in childhood is evident. The constructs in Table 1 have been grouped under the three main headings of bottom-up approach (e.g., food approach), bottom-up avoidant (e.g., food neophobia), and top-down. An additional grouping captures other constructs and those that seem to incorporate aspects of both top-down and bottom-up processes. The constructs in Table 1 cover research where ASR-related measures have been the dependent variable (such as parent influences on components of ASR, changes in ASR across development, and the heritability of ASR constructs), but also where they have been the independent variable (such as how parents respond to or perceive the components of ASR in children). The table also includes a number of reviews that discuss or examine a construct.
Table 1.
Construct | First author, publication year | Sample description/age focus for reviews | Construct Measurement | Study design | Results/selected findings |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bottom up (approach) | |||||
Food responsiveness | Carnell (2016) [97] | 4–5 year old UK children and mothers, mainly White British | CEBQ | Cross-sectional, with preloading and observed lunch intake plus parent reported eating behaviors | Higher food responsiveness was linked to greater total food intake |
Cross (2014) [98] | Low income African American and Hispanic parents and their preschool children aged 4 years | CEBQ | Cross-sectional survey of parent rated eating behaviors and parent feeding practices | Food responsiveness related to mothers’ restrictive feeding practices | |
Reward sensitivity/response to food and food cues | Adise (2019) [99] | 7–11 year old US children and families, mainly White, middle income | fMRI | Cross-sectional using neuro imaging assessment of brain responses to food and money rewards | Higher food responsiveness linked to decreased brain responses to winning food rewards. Regions associated with reward, cognitive control and emotion may play a role in the brain’s responses to food |
Yokum (2019) [100] | US adolescents 14–17 years, 77.7% European American | fMRI | Longitudinal design measuring neural activity for gained weight versus weight stable groups | Suggest that initial hyper-responsivity to palatable high-fat food tastes could be related to future weight gain | |
Shapiro (2019) [101] | From a pre-birth longitudinal cohort of US children, ethnically diverse. Tested at 4–6 years of age | fMRI | Cross-sectional with laboratory-based measurement of Eating in absence of hunger (EAH) and a brain scan | EAH was associated with activity in a major reward network, and reduced connectivity between brain regions associated with reward and those associated with response inhibition | |
Enjoyment of food | Carnell (2016) [97] | 4–5 year old UK children and mothers, mainly White British | CEBQ | Cross-sectional, with preloading and observed lunch intake plus parent reported eating behaviors | Higher enjoyment of food was linked to greater total food intake |
Cross (2014) [98] | Low income African American and Hispanic parents and their preschool children aged 4 years | CEBQ | Cross-sectional survey of parent rated eating behaviors and parent feeding practices | Higher enjoyment of food was linked to more restrictive feeding practices in African American families | |
Hedonic/reward aspects of food and hunger | Alonso-Alonso (2015) [102] | Not age-based | Review | Examined the neuroscience of food reward | Discussed homeostatic and non-homeostatic (related to the brain’s reward system) influences on the regulation of food intake |
Lowe (2007) [103] | Not age-based | Review | Examined hedonic hunger as a new eating motive | Proposed a distinction between homeostatic and hedonic eating | |
Subliminal reward signals | de Araujo (2020) [104] | Not age-based | Review | Examined human and animal research about processes associated with food reward | Proposed a two-path model of food reward that included subliminal reward signals and conscious liking |
Reward neurocircuitry | Reichelt (2015) [105] | Not age-based | Review | Examined neurocircuitry associated with the reinforcing value of foods and inhibitory control | Set out a model of food cue effects on homeostatic appetite signals and reward neurocircuitry |
Emotional eating/over-eating | Lumeng (2014) [106] | Low-income (Head Start) US children aged 3–4 years and parents | CEBQ | Cross-sectional using parent questionnaires plus child weight and cortisol measures | Family stress was linked to overweight, with this mediated by emotional eating in boys |
External eating | Jahnke (2008) [107] | German mothers of preschool children aged 3–6 years. Diverse SES | DEBQ | Cross-sectional using parent-questionnaires | Overweight children scored higher on external eating |
Consumption of problematic foods | Jahnke (2008) [107] | German mothers of preschool children aged 3–6 years, diverse SES | Parent reports of child food consumption | Cross-sectional using parent questionnaires | Parent ratings showed that children with higher weight status ate significant less problematic food |
Healthy food preferences | Anzman-Frasca (2018) [108] | Prenatal to early childhood | Review | Examined evidence about promoting healthy food preferences | Early exposure to healthy foods can support subsequent acceptance of these foods |
Russell (2016) [109] | Diverse sample of Australian preschool children aged 3–5 years and parents | Parent reports of food preferences | Cross-sectional, with measures of parent-reported child appetitive traits (CEBQ) and food preferences | Healthy food preferences were related to enjoyment of food, satiety responsiveness and fussiness | |
Eating in the absence of hunger (EAH) | Leung (2014) [58] | Low-income (Head Start) US preschool children and their caregivers, diverse in race and ethnicity | Observed EAH using the free access protocol | Cross-sectional with measures of parent-reports of temperament and obesogenic eating behaviors plus observed EAH | Higher temperamental surgency, but not effortful control, was related to more EAH |
Impulsivity | Bennett (2016) [110] | UK parents (mainly tertiary educated) and their children aged 2–4 years | Parent ratings on ECBQ, and child impulsivity, plus laboratory assessments of child impulsivity | Cross-sectional, using parent questionnaires and laboratory measures | Girls high in trait-like impulsivity and boys high in motor impulsivity could be more prone to display food approach behaviors associated with weight gain when parents monitor their intake less. |
Disinhibited eating | Shapiro (2019) [101] | From a pre-birth longitudinal cohort of US children, ethnically diverse. Tested at 4–6 years of age | Disinhibited eating measured using the EAH free access protocol | Cross-sectional with laboratory measurement of EAH and a brain scan | Provided new evidence of the neuronal correlates of disinhibited eating in young children |
Russell (2018) [29] | Childhood | Review | Narrative review of development of appetitive traits using insights from research and theory in developmental science | Outlined a biopsychosocial model of the development of appetitive traits, including disinhibited eating in childhood | |
Eating rate | Carnell (2007) [111] | UK children 4–5 years of age and parents (mainly mothers, White British and affluent) | Observed eating rate | Cross-sectional with observed eating behaviors plus parent-completed CEBQ | Faster eating was linked to higher food responsiveness and enjoyment of food. Slower eating was linked to higher satiety responsiveness |
Bottom up (avoidance) | |||||
Food neophobia/picky eating | Russell (2018) [29] | Childhood | Review | Narrative review of development of appetitive traits using insights from research and theory in developmental science | Outlined a biopsychosocial model of the development of appetitive traits, including food neophobia in childhood |
Cole (2017) [112] | Children less than 30 months of age | Review | Examined correlates of picky eating and food neophobia at different levels, for example, genetic, child, family, community | Highlighted the importance of investigating parent-child dyads and bidirectional feeding patterns | |
Russell (2008) [113] | Population-based sample of Australian children 2–5 years and parents | CFNS | Cross-sectional, with measures of parent-reported food neophobia and food preferences | Food neophobia was negatively correlated with liking for all foods in the healthy food group of Australian Healthy Eating Guide | |
Lumeng (2018) [114] | Low income US children and mothers. Entered study at 21 or 27 months of age. | CEBQ, BAMBI | Cross-lagged cohort questionnaire study at 21, 27 and 33 months of age | Concurrent association were found between picky eating and pressuring feeding, but no prospective associations | |
Food fussiness | Gregory (2010) [115] | Australian mothers of children 2–4 years mostly tertiary educated and Australian born | CEBQ | Cross-sectional using parent questionnaires about child eating behaviors, parent feeding, and concerns about child weight | Food fussiness predicted maternal pressure to eat, partially mediated by concern about child underweight |
Food avoidance | Powell (2011) [116] | UK mothers of children 3–6 years, mostly White British | CEBQ | Cross-sectional with parent reports of parent feeding behaviors and child food avoidance | Maternal feeding practices significantly predicted child food avoidance |
Emotional undereating | Bjorklund (2018) [117] | Representative community sample of Norwegian children 6–10 years and parents | CEBQ | Longitudinal with measures of child and contextual predictors of change in emotional over- and undereating | Lower family functioning at age 6 predicted emotional undereating at age 10 |
Herle (2018) [118] | Subsample from Twins Early Development study at age 4 years, mainly White British | CEBQ | Cross-sectional with measures of genetic and environmental factors contributing to emotional over-and undereating | Genetic contributions to emotional undereating were not significant. Shared environmental factors explained 77% of the variance | |
Slowness in eating | Llewellyn (2010) [98] | Population-based sample of infant twins from England and Wales | BEBQ | Cross sectional heritability analysis of scales from BEBQ | Heritability was high for slowness in eating |
Top down | |||||
Delay-of-gratification | Lelakowski (2019) [72] | Diverse US sample of mothers, children aged 24–30 months | Snack delay task | Longitudinal, with measures of child temperament, parent feeding and child BMI | Impulsivity but not inhibitory control (snack delay task) was related to BMI |
Kidd (2013) [119] | US children aged 3–5 years | Marshmallow wait task | Cross-sectional with measures of children’s wait time and beliefs about environmental reliability | Wait time reflected differences in self-control and beliefs about the stability of the world | |
Reward/delay discounting | Bennett (2019) [120] | UK children aged 7–11 years and parents, mainly White middle class | Delay discounting task as a measure of impulsivity | Cross-sectional with measures of child impulsivity, adiposity, intake during a snack, and eating behaviors | Poorer performance on delay discounting was associated with greater snack intake |
EC inhibitory control | Rollins (2014) [121] | US children aged 3–7 years and parents mainly White, middle to high income | CBQ | Short-term longitudinal with measures of restrictive feeding practices, intake of restricted food and child weight | Children with lower inhibitory control and higher approach showed greater increase in intake in association with experience of parental restriction |
Tan (2011) [122] | US parents with children 3–9 years | CBQ | Cross-sectional with measures of child self-regulation in eating, inhibitory control and parents’ feeding behavior | Self-regulation in eating was positively correlated with inhibitory control | |
EF inhibitory control | Fogel (2019) [123] | Children from an Asian cohort aged 6 years | Stop signal task as measure of inhibitory control | Cross-sectional with measures of child inhibitory control, eating behavior and adiposity | Lower inhibitory control was related to selecting larger food portion, multiple food servings and faster eating rates |
Shapiro (2019) [124] | From a pre-birth longitudinal cohort of US children, ethnically diverse. Tested at 4–6 years of age | Flanker task as measure of inhibitory control | Cross-sectional with measures of biomarkers of poor metabolic health and performance on cognitive tasks | Greater blood biomarkers of poor metabolic health were related to lower inhibitory control | |
Others/both top-down and bottom-up | |||||
Homeostatic and hedonic systems cross-talk | Higgs (2017) [125] | Not age-based | Review | Examined evidence about the integration of metabolic, reward and cognitive processes in appetite control | Favors a framework that emphasizes cross-talk between the neurochemical substrates of hedonic and homeostatic systems |
Berthoud (2017) [126] | Not age-based | Review | Examined hedonic and homeostatic controls in the regulation of body weight | Presents neural models of the interaction between homeostatic and hedonic controls | |
Interoception | Keller (2018) [127] | Children | Review | Examined the role of the brain in children’s food choice and eating behavior, including brain regions associated with interoception | Noted findings suggesting a reduced awareness of internal homeostatic cues among individuals prone to obesity |
Alliesthesia | Higgs (2017) [125] | Not age-based | Review | Examined evidence about the integration of metabolic, reward and cognitive processes in appetite control | Discussed alliesthesia: food is more liked when hungry, less so when eating when full. Noted associations with decreases in reward-related brain activations |
Berridge (2010) [128] | Not age-based | Review | Examined brain mechanisms associated with obesity or eating disorders, including alliesthesia | Suggested possible brain-based mechanisms for hunger increasing “liking” and “wanting” food | |
Caloric compensation | Carnell (2007) [111] | UK children 4–5 years of age and parents, (mainly mothers, White British and affluent) | Observed using preload protocol | Cross-sectional with measures of children’s ability to regulate intake depending on the caloric content of a preload plus parent-completed CEBQ | Higher satiety responsiveness (CEBQ) was associated with better average caloric compensation |
Compensation for energy density | Brugaileres (2019) [129] | French infants at 11 and 15 months of age and mothers | Observed using preload protocol | Short-term longitudinal with measures of changes in adjustment of intake to energy density | At both ages, infants undercompensated for the energy of the preload. Compensation ability decreased from 11 to 15 months. The greater the decrease, the higher weight status at 2 years of age |
Johnson (2000) [28] | High SES US children 4–5 years of age and parents | Preload protocol | Short-term longitudinal intervention to help children recognize cues of satiety and hunger to compensate for energy density | Large individual differences in self-regulation at baseline. The intervention improved children’s self-regulation | |
Compensation across meals and over days | Leahy (2008) [130] | US children 3–5 years of age. Parents mostly White with a university degree | Varied energy density of prepared meals | Short-term longitudinal with measures of intake in response to differences in energy density over 2 days using a cross-over design | A decrease in energy density led to a decrease in energy intake; children did not compensate in their energy intake (calories) according to the energy density of the meals |
Food choice/processed food effects | Small (2019) [131] | Not age-based | Review | Examined two systems driving food choice: metabolic signals about nutritional content, and conscious perceptions e.g., about flavor, caloric content, healthfulness | Argues there is evidence that nutritional signals about processed food are not accurately conveyed to the brain |
Food “liking” and “wanting” | Keller (2018) [127] | Children | Review | Examined the role of the brain in children’s food choice and eating behavior, including the neural drivers of food “liking” and “wanting” | Summarizes evidence about the neural drivers of affective response to food (“liking”) and the incentive salience of food (“wanting”) |
Berridge (2016) [132] | Not age-based | Review | Examined brain mechanisms associated with “wanting” a reward (including food) and “liking” the same reward | Addiction could be associated with excessive amplification of “wanting”, especially triggered by cues about anticipated rewards and pleasure. Heightened dopamine reactivity such as stress and emotions could increase “wanting” | |
Satiety responsiveness | Carnell (2016) [97] | 4–5 year old UK children and mothers, mainly White British | CEBQ | Cross-sectional, with preloading and observed lunch intake plus parent-reported eating behaviors | Higher satiety responsiveness was linked to lower total food intake |
Cross (2014) [98] | Low income African American and Hispanic parents and their preschool children | CEBQ | Cross-sectional using measures of parent-rated child eating behaviors and parent-reported feeding practices | Higher satiety responsiveness was associated with greater pressure to eat in African American families | |
Satiation and satiety | Blundell (2010) [133] | Not age-based | Review | Examined specific measures of satiation, satiety, hunger and food consumption, including “liking” and “wanting” | Sets out a model of the impact of foods on satiation and satiety. Discussed approaches to the measurement of satiation and satiety |
Bellisle (2012) [134] | Not age-based | Review | Examines the satiating power of foods with sweeteners. Included “liking” and “wanting” and their role | Highlighted methodological challenges in measuring satiation and satiety |
CEBQ Children’s Eating Behaviour Questionnaire, BEBQ Baby Eating Behaviour Questionnaire, TMCQ Temperament in Middle Childhood Questionnaire, ECBQ Early Childhood Behaviour Questionnaire, CFNS Child Food Neophobia Scale, BAMBI Brief Autism Mealtime Behaviour Inventory, CBQ Children’s Behavior Questionnaire, EF Executive Function, EC Effortful Control
The list in Table 1 reveals a wide variety of constructs that appear to be particular to ASR rather than to GSR. It seems noteworthy that homeostasis is frequently drawn on in analyses not only of appetite regulation, but also in appetite self-regulation. It is illustrated by the idea that rather than independent homeostatic and hedonic systems, there is an interaction between the neurochemical substrates of the two systems in the regulation of energy intake and weight. This has been described as involving “crosstalk” between the homeostatic and hedonic systems [125, 126, 135]. The inclusion of homeostasis in approaches to ASR seems to provide a contrast with GSR, where Nigg [3] excluded allostatic and homeostatic processes in his review of self-regulation. The constructs included in Table 1 point to the complexity and range of different components implicated in ASR.
Approaches to measurement in ASR (some referred to in Table 1) have covered three overlapping areas relating to both self-regulation and self-regulation difficulties: (1) processes, (2) individual differences and (3) regulatory skills or strategies. In the case of processes, there has been considerable use of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). For example, fMRI has been used to assess increased responsivity to palatable food receipt and cues, possible inhibitory control activation to suppress urges to consume palatable food, and reduced fat taste sensitivity that may impair satiety responses [100] as well as the origins and role of sweet taste in reward-based eating in children [136] and stress-induced cortisol effects on reward sensitivity and appetite regions of the brain [137].
In the measurement of individual differences in eating behaviors or appetitive traits in childhood, both neural and behavioural measures have been used [138]. Parent ratings of appetitive traits have been widely used. This has especially involved use of the Children’s Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (CEBQ) [139, 140] the parallel Baby Eating Behaviour Questionnaire [140, 141] and other questionnaires, such as the Child Food Neophobia Scale [142]. The CEBQ has two broad dimensions of food approach and food avoidance (thus matching the two main aspects of bottom-up processes). Individual CEBQ scales include food responsiveness, satiety responsiveness and food fussiness.
In single-session experimental and/or naturalistic contexts, the preload protocol and observations have been used to measure calorie compensation, eating in the absence of hunger, i.e., eating beyond satiation, and eating rate [28, 52, 97, 129, 143–147]. A similar strategy has been used to measure energy intake and compensation over several days [130]. Results of these measurement approaches are used either to study individual differences in ASR or as measures of self-regulatory skills (e.g., the ability to compensate for preload) or self-regulatory difficulties (e.g., eating in the absence of hunger). The widely used delay-of-gratification paradigm discussed below can also be treated as an approach to measure skills in self-regulation, or to measure individual differences. Measures of EF, EC and emotion regulation have been incorporated into research on ASR and related to child weight status [52].
Focused models and conceptions of ASR (especially as they relate to self-regulation difficulties or deficits that seem to be associated with disinhibited eating) [31–34, 101, 148–150] include at least top-down regulation, hedonic aspects of food including sweet taste preferences [151], biological processes in the control of appetite [33, 34], as well as social, physical and macro-level environments, and psychological and neural control mechanisms.
The idea of bottom-up and top-down processes and their interplay intrinsic in approaches to GSR has been part of recent conceptualizations of ASR [32, 39, 44, 61, 72, 101]. While in the case of ASR, the self-regulatory processes include top-down, cognitive and decision-making processes, regulation is also dependent on body and brain responses (such as hormonal and brain signals about satiation) to the food, processes such as alliesthesia and interactions between metabolic, reward, and cognitive processes [125]. Consciously perceptible hedonic qualities of food could play a mostly transient role in food reinforcement, with a greater contribution from subliminal gut-brain reward pathways [104]. Further, homeostatic (i.e. regulatory) and hedonic systems in relation to food have evolved to communicate and influence one another [125, 152] and there is a complex interplay of homeostatic and non-homeostatic controls [102]. Cognitive and behavioral aspects of ASR, involving social and goal-directed behaviors and decision-making involved in top-down processes [32], might interact with neural and endocrinal substrates. The interplay between bottom-up and top-down processes in ASR is reflected in the argument that appetite self-regulation occurs “at cognitive, emotional, motivational, biological, and behavioral levels” ([11], p. 71).
Cognitive processes involved in appetite regulation have been separated in terms of those operating before a meal and those during a meal [125]. In a similar way, Smethers and colleagues [153, 154] argued that there are different possible time courses in relation to ASR. One possibility is a particular eating or feeding episode which is affected by a range of factors, some of them which are unique to eating such as sensory specific satiety and food variety [155, 156]. Another relates to compensation or adjustment from a meal or snack to the next opportunity for eating, or over several days. For example, preschool children have been found not to adjust energy intake in response to energy density or portion size over a 5-day period [153, 154]. There is control of immediate gratification versus control of overall energy balance [157]. Different time courses seem to imply different bottom-up and top-down processes.
ASR also covers both the quantity and composition of dietary intake [48]. This means that ASR involves not only the regulation of energy intake, but also the choice of foods, especially with respect to “healthy” and “unhealthy” diet and food choices [34, 35, 158–162]. Finally, in the case of ASR, bottom-up processes can arise in different ways, such as from hunger (and the food could be healthy or unhealthy) versus from the attraction of palatable food, or the sweet taste [136, 151], from attraction arising from food having been restricted [163, 164], especially for children lower in inhibitory control [121], the desire to eat to regulate emotions [29, 118, 165, 166], and in response to stress [167–170]. This means that what is being responded to and what or how top-down processes might be drawn on seems variable and uncertain in the case of food and eating: is the same top-down process required in relation to self-regulating responses to foods of large portion size and foods higher in energy density, for example? And does this differ according to emotional state and hunger levels?
Evidence about common underpinnings of GSR and ASR and contributions to appetitive traits and BMI
The above comparison of GSR in developmental science and ASR highlighted possible differences between them, but also a number of parallels or overlaps. This raises the question of what the evidence indicates about whether and how GSR and ASR have common underpinnings, especially with respect to EF, EC and delay-of-gratification. The evidence reviewed above shows the importance of EF, EC and delay-of-gratification in GSR. The question is whether there is similar evidence about ASR. In addition, a linked question arises from research on delay-of-gratification. Here the question is whether and how food and non-food related delay-of-gratification is associated with children’s appetitive traits, weight and obesity. If food and non-food delay-of-gratification are associated with ASR-related outcomes such as disinhibited eating and obesity in similar ways, this could suggest a close linkage between GSR and ASR. On both questions the evidence is complex and diverse.
On the positive side, there is some evidence and claims that elements of EC contribute to ASR-related outcomes such as children’s eating behavior, weight, weight gain, or nutrition risks [21, 56, 60, 122]. This research has included preschool children [56, 60]. There is parallel evidence and claims that components of EF are also associated with ASR-related outcomes [21, 49, 50, 53, 55, 56, 171]. Much of this evidence seems to involve children of elementary school age or older [53, 55, 171–173]. There seems to be limited evidence about EF and ASR-related outcomes in younger children. Nevertheless, a recent study [49] reported that change in EF from 3 to 5 years was inversely related to change in BMI from age 2 to 5 years.
Positive results for EF and/or EC have not always been found in the literature on ASR with young children. Hughes et al. [52] used a battery of eating and non-eating self-regulation procedures and measures with a sample of Hispanic preschool children and their parents. The battery of non-eating related measures was not significantly related to child BMIz. The battery included EF tasks, EC from parent reports, delay-of-gratification with a food reward and delay-of-gratification with a non-food gift reward. In a sample of 3 to 6 year-old children, Pieper & Laugero [56] found that parent reported inhibitory control was associated with parent-reported emotional over eating, but not to observed eating in the absence of hunger. Three EF tasks (including delay-of-gratification) also were not related to observed eating behavior (but parent-reported emotional eating was related to an EF selective attention task). In a meta-analysis of research with mainly adolescents and adults, Yang et al. [57] found that broad impairments on executive function were linked to obesity but only deficits in the inhibition and working memory components of EF were found for overweight participants. Tan and Holub [122] found that parent reported inhibitory control was related to parent-reported self-regulation in eating in a sample of children aged 3 to 9 years, but not to child BMI percentile. In research with preschoolers and their caregivers, Leung et al. [58] reported no significant relationships between caregiver-reported effortful control and six measures of caregiver-reported obesogenic appetitive traits.
Evidence about contributions of food and non-food delay-of-gratification to appetitive traits and BMI
In addition, investigations of relationships between EC and EF and children’s eating behavior and weight/weight gain, there has been a body of research based on measures of delay-of-gratification. The evidence here mainly pertains to (1) whether or not BMI, obesity or weight gain is predicted by delay-of-gratification for food and non-food tasks, and (2) differences between OW/OB versus normal weight children on delay-of-gratification tasks. The findings are partly complicated by the tendency for research to claim to be investigating “self-regulation” but use food as the reward in the delay-of-gratification procedure [43], despite questions about the comparability of food and non-food rewards as the evidence below indicates.
There are findings that performance with non-food rewards in delay-of-gratification can predict ASR-related outcomes, as well as other data suggesting that only food rewards are associated with ASR-related outcomes. In the former case, Saltzman and colleagues [20] argued that general self-regulation and ASR are “strongly related”, to the extent that a pathway to ASR is via GSR. In the end, this claim relied on the results of Graziano and colleagues [95, 174]. They used delay-of-gratification for an “appealingly” gift-wrapped box at 2 years as a measure of self-regulation/inhibitory control/reward sensitivity and compared overweight/at risk versus normal weight children at age 5.5 years [96]. Overweight/at risk children were in the 85th BMIz percentile or greater. They found inhibitory control (delay-of-gratification) was significantly related to BMI at age 2. In a further follow-up at age 10 years [174] they reported that BMIz-scores and changes in BMIz-scores from 4 to 10 years were related to “self-regulation skills” at age 2. In this case “self-regulation skills” was a single factor score combining laboratory measures of sustained attention, emotion regulation and delay-of-gratification using an appealing gift-wrapped box.
In speculating about the possible mechanisms linking “self-regulation deficits” to BMI and the development of weight problems 8 years later, they suggested that it could arise from “oversensitivity to novel and pleasurable activities” (p. 941) that could be present prior to the development of obesity, but were unsure about how this could contribute to obesity. Possibilities they proposed were that it could interfere with satiety processes such as recognizing the signals of satiety and stopping eating, and could contribute to unhealthy food preferences, both of which are elements of ASR. Francis and Susman’s [157] results also suggest that performance on delay-of-gratification (they called it a “waiting game” for a toy) at 3 years was related to weight gain in children to 12 years of age. A subsequent delay-of-gratification measure using food-reward at age 5 enhanced the prediction of weight gain.
When delay-of-gratification research has directly compared food and non-food rewards, there are a number of results pointing to the independence of performance on food and non-food tasks. Miller et al. [14] measured BMI and observed toddler (mean age 33.1 months) “self-regulation” responses in food (delay of gratification for a snack) and non-food (delay of gratification for a gift) tasks as well as emotional self-regulation from negative affect in frustration eliciting tasks (one food-related- no-touch cookie tasks and one non-food related-no-touch toy task). They reported that the ability to wait in the food delay task (but not in the non-food delay task) was associated with lower concurrent child weight (BMIz) and lower odds of overweight/obese status. They suggest that food responsivity could be implicated. Better emotional self-regulation in both tasks (less prone to distress in the face of frustration) was related to lower odds of overweight/obesity. A possibility the authors raised was that parents could engage in emotional feeding as a soothing strategy for children prone to negative emotions. As noted below, emotional feeding could be a factor in the disruption of ASR in childhood.
Obese and non-obese children from early childhood and middle childhood have been found to differ on delay-of-gratification tasks where food is a reward, but not when there are non-food rewards [175, 176]. These results are consistent with the suggestion ([177] p. 411) that “children who were overweight were particularly ineffective at inhibiting their responses towards food stimuli” (p. 411). They suggested that overweight children could be especially responsive to food cues and find it difficult to inhibit responses to those cues. This possibility is consistent with a finding that performance on delay-of-gratification with a food reward at age 4 predicted BMI 30 years later [178]. These authors suggested that this result could reflect executive function abilities. It needs to be noted, however, that performance on food-reward delay-of-gratification is not always related to ASR-related outcomes. For example, in a toddler.
sample, Lelakowska et al. [72] found that performance on the snack delay task at 24 months was not related to either parent-reported emotional overeating or child BMI at 30 months.
Discussion
The present reciprocal analysis pointed to the potential for the integration of theory and evidence about GSR and ASR. Nigg [3] argued that self-regulation should be treated as domain general, and then specific terms used in relation to separate domains. For example, self-regulation of emotions, self-regulation of actions and self-regulation of cognitions. Calkins and colleagues [22, 23] have advanced similar arguments. Following this reasoning, the present analysis suggests that self-regulation of appetite should be added as a domain under the umbrella of self-regulation in childhood.
Overall, the present reciprocal analysis (an overview provided in Table 2) noted important parallels between ASR and GSR in childhood in terms of key concepts and possible underpinning processes, but, equally, ASR, like each of the other identified self-regulation domains of GSR, seems to involve a number of unique components and processes. Some of these are highlighted in Table 2. The evidence did not seem to support a conceptualization of GSR as providing a pathway to ASR. There were suggestions of common underpinnings, but the development of GSR and ASR in childhood is no doubt shaped by processes partly inherent to each domain and develop somewhat independently [52], but with increasing integration across childhood.
Table 2.
Domains | Food self-regulation (eg. ASR, SREI) | Non-food self-regulation (eg of action, emotions, cognitions) |
---|---|---|
Possible underpinning processes | EF, EC, Inhibitory Control, delay-of-gratification, recursive bottom-up and top-down processes | EF, EC, Inhibitory Control, delay-of-gratification, recursive bottom-up and top-down processes, Hot and cool/cold EF |
Possible unique components and processes | Early homeostatic regulation, satiation versus satiety, hormonal and brain responses to food, different time courses of regulation (e.g., meal versus diet), regulation of quantity versus quality of diet, different influences on bottom-up processes (such as hunger versus palatable food signals), role of disinhibited eating in the disruption of ASR, and quantity and quality of food intake effects on brain development and EF. | Unique components and processes in non-food self-regulation |
Relationships between food and non-food self-regulation | The pathway to food SR is via non-food SR (little evidence in support). | |
Food and non-food SR have common EF and EC underpinnings (limited evidence in young children, but support in middle childhood and beyond). | ||
Non-food SR contributes directly to ASR-related outcomes such as BMI and obesity (little evidence in support). | ||
Integration of aspects of non-food and food SR and the associated underpinning processes across childhood (likely and warrants further research). |
The relatively unique aspects of ASR seem to have parallels in the case of emotion regulation/dysregulation, at least with respect to neurobiological and endocrinal processes, as well as in terms of reading internal and external cues, and unique cognitive and behavioral processes [179–183]. In the case of self-regulation arising from fear/inhibition and impulsivity/disinhibition, unique features are also likely to be evident, especially in relation to demands placed on top-down processes in the regulation of cognition and action.
It is appealing to assume that comparable processes, abilities or capacities such as those inherent in EF, EC and in delay-of-gratification underpin both GSR and ASR. However, the evidence seems to be mixed for EF and EC, at least over the period from infancy to age 5 or 6 years of age. In relation to abilities associated with delay-of-gratification procedures, the evidence mainly points to differences for food and non-food related delays. In relation to ASR, there is also a question of the direction-of-effect [57, 171]. There is evidence about the impact of adiposity on the structure and function of the prefrontal cortex (which is linked to executive control processes), so that excessive consumption of appetitive calorie-dense food could contribute to impairments in executive function and then to reduced food self-regulation [150]. There is also evidence that metabolic health could impact cognitive function in preschool children [124]. Finally, both weight gain and impairments in executive function could be related to a common third factor such as stress [168, 184, 185], or genetic predispositions [171].
Collectively, processes in ASR seem to incorporate biological (e.g., bottom-up approach and avoidance), psychological (e.g., top-down inhibitory control) and social factors (e.g., external food and eating cues, and social contexts that impact both bottom-up and top-down processes). In this way, ASR appears amenable to a biopsychosocial approach in common with much developmental theory [186, 187] and in parallel with the development of appetite traits [29, 188] and overweight/obesity in childhood [16].
It is helpful to place the possibility of limited contributions of EF and EC to ASR-related outcomes in young children in a developmental context. It could be that with respect to food and eating, “children’s top-down control capacities are relatively immature compared to bottom-up processes” ([189] p. 111), where the hedonic value of food and early taste preferences contribute to the potency of bottom-up processes, and this could account for some of the results for preschool children. As we noted, however, there are consistent findings about the role of EF and EC in older children, adolescents and adults, for example, as suggested by meta-analyses [57] and literature reviews [171, 172].
Inherent in the conceptualization of self-regulation in childhood as occurring in different domains or levels, is the notion that the domains or levels build on and integrate over the course of development [3, 4, 22–24, 190]. For instance, Nigg [3] posited that “aspects of the SR universe can be organized hierarchically in relation to granularity, development, and time. Low-level components assemble into high-level components” (p. 361): “low-level operations like response inhibition and working memory support emergence of more complex operations like higher order EF” (p. 374), in a cascade type process. Nigg went on to argue that bottom-up processes emerge and mature earlier than top-down processes and that “different aspects of SR mature at different rates within bottom-up and top-down domains” (p. 375).
It seems that in developmental science research, more attention has been directed to top-down components of self-regulation than in work on ASR, as suggested at least by the body of research on the early development and precursors of EF and EC in non-food self-regulation [4, 24, 81, 89, 191, 192]. There seems to be scope for attention to similar questions about early precursors and their integration across childhood in the development of ASR. For example, whether or how early homeostatic energy regulation in infancy is linked to the emergence of different aspects of ASR in childhood. Increased research is needed on the contribution of the various dimensions of EF and EC to appetitive traits (e.g., responsiveness to food cues) across childhood. Equally, there is a need for more research on the nature and role of top-down and bottom-up processes in children’s eating and weight (e.g., 72), with attention to the elements of bottom-up processes and top-down processes, as well as how they interact, and changes in their role and interaction across childhood for different elements of ASR (such as making healthy food choices, resisting palatable foods, decisions about when to eat, decisions about stopping eating etc). Theory and evidence from GSR seem to have the potential to provide conceptual and methodological insights for approaches to ASR in these areas.
At the same time, scholarship on ASR has the potential to enrich knowledge and insights about general or non-food self-regulation in childhood. For example, by attention to possible parallels in the nature of hedonic and avoidant responses to internal or external stimuli (such as arise in relation to taste/food preferences, hunger and food), as well as the role of metabolic processes and brain structures and responses (such as arise in food-related situations). The case of ASR also suggests there is potential in adding ideas and analysis about what Cole et al. [68] referred to as Executive Processes, which they indicated involve attention, memory, reasoning and conscious decision making. For example, different executive process strategies could be implicated in responding to hunger signals versus palatable food signals and in relation to the initiation of eating versus the control of an eating episode [148] as well as to the broader control of food choice and diet. Comparable differences in executive processes might apply to non-food self-regulatory situations (such as more internally generated anger, versus external provocation, and when and how to stop an anger outburst or acting out episode).
An implication of drawing ASR under a common umbrella of self-regulation in childhood is that it generates a cross-fertilization of areas of research that have the potential, in turn, to advance knowledge about both GSR and ASR. New areas of research endeavor might include: 1) research and theory about the changing roles of top-down and bottom up processes across childhood as a function of different food and non-food regulatory situations 2) a greater integration of research and theory about the role of stress in the disruption of GSR and ASR, 3) more joint attention to situations or factors that increase (or decrease) the capacity and role of top-down and bottom-up process such as arises from visceral reactions to hyperpalatable and rewarding food cues [150], 4) expanding research on hot EF [193] to include various food-related contexts, 5) more research on when, why and how results differ for food and non-food related delay-of-gratification, 6) more attention to how the separate domains or levels of self-regulation (now including ASR) become integrated across childhood, 7) research on how homeostatic processes in energy intake and expenditure link with EF and EC, and 8) research on whether and how GSR might have parallels with food satiety and satiation.
Conclusion
The present review suggests there are some overlaps between GSR and ASR: there is commonality with respect to the overall meaning of self-regulation, in the application of constructs such as EF, EC and delay-of-gratification, and in the utilization of bottom-up and top-down processes. The overlap is shown by the relevance for both GSR and ASR of the four main elements of the domain-general model outlined in the section on key concepts and processes in general self-regulation. But, it is also evident that ASR implicates factors that seem to be unique to food-related self-regulation. Consistent with similar arguments about GSR, it is reasonable to say that there is not yet a unified definition or model of ASR in childhood. This could serve as an impediment to research and theory development about the role of ASR in OW/OB in childhood.
The recognized domains of self-regulation in childhood include, emotions, actions and cognitions. Each of these bring somewhat unique features to questions about processes in the development and functioning of self-regulation in childhood. Clearly, also with some unique features, a case can be made to include ASR as a domain under the umbrella of self-regulation in childhood. This has the potential to enrich theory and research and serve as a significant heuristic for future scholarship about self-regulation in childhood.
Acknowledgements
Not applicable.
Abbreviations
- ASR
Appetite self-regulation
- BMI
Body Mass Index
- EC
Effortful Control
- EF
Executive Function
- GSR
General self-regulation
- OW/OB
Overweight/Obese
- SREI
Self-regulation of energy intake
Authors’ contributions
CG Russell and A Russell both conceptualized and wrote the review. The author(s) read and approved the final manuscript.
Funding
None.
Availability of data and materials
Not applicable.
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Not applicable.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Catherine G. Russell, Email: Georgie.Russell@deakin.edu.au, Email: georgie.russell@deakin.edu.au
Alan Russell, Email: Alan.Russell@flinders.edu.au.
References
- 1.Denham SA, Bassett HH, Wyatt T. The socialization of emotional competence. In: Grusec JE, Hastings PD, editors. Handbook of socialization: theory and research. 2. New York: The Guilford Press; 2015. pp. 590–613. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky D, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington H, et al. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(7):2693–2698. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010076108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Nigg JT. Annual research review: on the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58(4):361–383. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Perry NB, Calkins SD, Dollar JM, Keane SP, Shanahan L. Self-regulation as a predictor of patterns of change in externalizing behaviors from infancy to adolescence. Dev Psychopathol. 2018;30(2):497–510. doi: 10.1017/S0954579417000992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Perry NB, Dollar JM, Calkins SD, Keane SP, Shanahan L. Childhood self-regulation as a mechanism through which early overcontrolling parenting is associated with adjustment in preadolescence. Dev Psychol. 2018;54(8):1542–1554. doi: 10.1037/dev0000536. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Tangney JP, Baumeister RF, Boone AL. High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J Pers. 2004;72(2):271–324. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Whitaker RC, Gooze RA. Self-regulation and obesity prevention: A valuable intersections between developmental psychology and pediatrics. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(4):386. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2008.576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Backer-Grondahl A, Naerde A, Idsoe T. Hot and cool self-regulation, academic competence, and maladjustment: mediating and differential relations. Child Dev. 2018;90:2171–2188. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Blair C, Raver CC. School readiness and self-regulation: a developmental psychobiological approach. Annu Rev Psychol. 2015;66:711–731. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Liew J. Effortful control, executive functions, and education: bringing self-regulatory and social-emotional competencies to the table. Child Dev Perspect. 2012;6(2):105–111. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00196.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Miller AL, Gearhardt AN, Fredericks EM, Katz B, Shapiro LF, Holden K, et al. Targeting self-regulation to promote health behaviors in children. Behav Res Ther. 2018;101:71–81. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2017.09.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Anzman-Frasca S, Stifter CA, Birch LL. Temperament and childhood obesity risk: a review of the literature. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2012;33(9):732–745. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31826a119f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bauer KW, Chuisano S. Intentional self-regulation of eating among children and adolescents. In: Lumeng JCF, Fisher J, editors. Pediatric food preferences and eating behaviors. London: Academic Press; 2018. pp. 255–270. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Miller AL, Rosenblum KL, Retzloff LB, Lumeng JC. Observed self-regulation is associated with weight in low-income toddlers. Appetite. 2016;105:705–712. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.07.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Moding KJ, Augustine ME, Stifter CA. Interactive effects of parenting behavior and regulatory skills in toddlerhood on child weight outcomes. Int J Obes. 2019;43(1):53–61. doi: 10.1038/s41366-018-0162-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Russell CG, Russell A. A biopsychosocial approach to processes and pathways in the development of overweight and obesity in childhood: insights from developmental theory and research. Obes Rev. 2019;20(5):725–749. doi: 10.1111/obr.12838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.De Coen V, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Verbestel V, Maes L, Vereecken C. Risk factors for childhood overweight: a 30-month longitudinal study of 3-to 6-year-old children. Public Health Nutr. 2014;17(9):1993–2000. doi: 10.1017/S1368980013002346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kochanska G, Coy KC, Murray KT. The development of self-regulation in the first four years of life. Child Dev. 2001;72(4):1091–1111. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Mulder H, van Ravenswaaij H, Verhagen J, Moerbeek M, Leseman PPM. The process of early self-control: an observational study in two- and three-year-olds. Metacogn Learn. 2019.
- 20.Saltzman JA, Fiese BH, Bost KK, McBride BA. Development of appetite self-regulation: integrating perspectives from attachment and family systems theory. Child Dev Perspect. 2018;12(1):51–57. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12254. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Anderson SE, Keim SA. Parent-child interaction, self-regulation, and obesity prevention in early childhood. Curr Obes Rep. 2016;5(2):192–200. doi: 10.1007/s13679-016-0208-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Calkins SD. The emergence of self-regulation: biological and behavioral control mechanisms supporting toddler competence. In: Brownell CA, Kopp C, editors. Socioemotional development in the toddler years: transitions and transformations. New York: Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 261–284. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Calkins SD, Fox NA. Self-regulatory processes in early personality development: a multilevel approach to the study of childhood social withdrawal and aggression. Dev Psychopathol. 2002;14(3):477–498. doi: 10.1017/S095457940200305X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Gagne JR. Self-control in childhood: a synthesis of perspectives and focus on early development. Child Dev Perspect. 2017;11(2):127–132. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12223. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kochanska G, Aksan N. Children's conscience and self-regulation. J Pers. 2006;74(6):1587–1617. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00421.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Frankel LA, Hughes SO, O'Connor TM, Power TG, Fisher JO, Hazen NL. Parental influences on Children's self-regulation of energy intake: insights from developmental literature on emotion regulation. J Obes. 2012;2012:327259. doi: 10.1155/2012/327259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hughes SO, Frankel LA, Beltran A, Hodges E, Hoerr S, Lumeng J, et al. Food parenting measurement issues: working group consensus report. Child Obes. 2013;9(Suppl(s1)):S95–102. doi: 10.1089/chi.2013.0032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Johnson SL. Improving preschoolers self-regulation of energy intake. Pediatrics. 2000;106(6):1429–1435. doi: 10.1542/peds.106.6.1429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Russell CG, Russell A. Biological and psychosocial processes in the development of Children's appetitive traits: insights from developmental theory and research. Nutrients. 2018;10(6):692. doi: 10.3390/nu10060692. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Francis LA, Riggs NR. Executive function and self-regulatory influences on Children's eating. In: Lumeng JCF, Fisher J, editors. Pediatric food preferences and eating behaviors. London: Academic Press; 2018. pp. 183–206. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Schwartz MB, Just DR, Chriqui JF, Ammerman AS. Appetite self-regulation: environmental and policy influences on eating behaviors. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2017;25(Suppl 1):S26–S38. doi: 10.1002/oby.21770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Stoeckel LE, Birch LL, Heatherton T, Mann T, Hunter C, Czajkowski S, et al. Psychological and neural contributions to appetite self-regulation. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2017;25(Suppl 1):S17–S25. doi: 10.1002/oby.21789. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Young-Hyman D. Introduction to special issue: self-regulation of appetite-it's complicated. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2017;25(Suppl 1):S5–S7. doi: 10.1002/oby.21781. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.MacLean PS, Blundell JE, Mennella JA, Batterham RL. Biological control of appetite: a daunting complexity. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2017;25(Suppl 1):S8–S16. doi: 10.1002/oby.21771. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Ha OR, Bruce AS, Pruitt SW, Cherry JB, Smith TR, Burkart D, et al. Healthy eating decisions require efficient dietary self-control in children: a mouse-tracking food decision study. Appetite. 2016;105:575–581. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.06.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Ha Oh-Ryeong, Lim Seung-Lark, Bruce Jared M., Bruce Amanda S. Unhealthy foods taste better among children with lower self-control. Appetite. 2019;139:84–89. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Lim SL, Cherry JB, Davis AM, Balakrishnan SN, Ha OR, Bruce JM, et al. The child brain computes and utilizes internalized maternal choices. Nat Commun. 2016;7:11700. doi: 10.1038/ncomms11700. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Clairman H, Dettmer E, Buchholz A, Cordeiro K, Ibrahim Q, Maximova K, et al. Pathways to eating in children and adolescents with obesity. Int J Obes. 2019;43(6):1193–1201. doi: 10.1038/s41366-018-0271-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Kremers SP, de Bruijn GJ, Visscher TL, van Mechelen W, de Vries NK, Brug J. Environmental influences on energy balance-related behaviors: a dual-process view. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2006;3(1):9. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-3-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Qasim A, Turcotte M, de Souza RJ, Samaan MC, Champredon D, Dushoff J, et al. On the origin of obesity: identifying the biological, environmental and cultural drivers of genetic risk among human populations. Obes Rev. 2018;19(2):121–149. doi: 10.1111/obr.12625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Roberto CA, Swinburn B, Hawkes C, Huang TT, Costa SA, Ashe M, et al. Patchy progress on obesity prevention: emerging examples, entrenched barriers, and new thinking. Lancet. 2015;385(9985):2400–2409. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)61744-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Smith JD, Egan KN, Montano Z, Dawson-McClure S, Jake-Schoffman DE, Larson M, et al. A developmental cascade perspective of paediatric obesity: a conceptual model and scoping review. Health Psychol Rev. 2018;12(3):271–293. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2018.1457450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Caleza C, Yanez-Vico RM, Mendoza A, Iglesias-Linares A. Childhood obesity and delayed gratification behavior: a systematic review of experimental studies. J Pediatr. 2016;169:201–7.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2015.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Hayes JF, Eichen DM, Barch DM, Wilfley DE. Executive function in childhood obesity: promising intervention strategies to optimize treatment outcomes. Appetite. 2018;124:10–23. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.05.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Hughes SO, Power TG, Beck A, Betz D, Calodich S, Goodell LS, et al. Strategies for Effective Eating Development—SEEDS: Design of an Obesity Prevention Program to Promote Healthy Food Preferences and Eating Self-Regulation in Children From Low-Income Families. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2016;48(6):405–18. e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2016.04.388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Miller AL, Horodynski MA, Herb HE, Peterson KE, Contreras D, Kaciroti N, et al. Enhancing self-regulation as a strategy for obesity prevention in head start preschoolers: the growing healthy study. BMC Public Health. 2012;12:1040. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-12-1040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.van der Veek SMC, de Graaf C, de Vries JHM, Jager G, Vereijken CMJL, Weenen H, et al. Baby’s first bites: a randomized controlled trial to assess the effects of vegetable-exposure and sensitive feeding on vegetable acceptance, eating behavior and weight gain in infants and toddlers. BMC Pediatr. 2019;19(1):266. doi: 10.1186/s12887-019-1627-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Birch LL, Fisher JA. The role of experience in the development of children's eating behavior. Why we eat what we eat: The psychology of eating. Washington, DC: US: American Psychological Association; 1996. pp. 113–141. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Blair C, Kuzawa CW, Willoughby MT. The development of executive function in early childhood is inversely related to change in body mass index: Evidence for an energetic tradeoff? Dev Sci. 2019;23:e12860. doi: 10.1111/desc.12860. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Dohle S, Diel K, Hofmann W. Executive functions and the self-regulation of eating behavior: a review. Appetite. 2018;124:4–9. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.05.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Hall PA. Executive-control processes in high-calorie food consumption. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2016;25(2):91–98. doi: 10.1177/0963721415625049. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Hughes SO, Power TG, O'Connor TM, Orlet FJ. Executive functioning, emotion regulation, eating self-regulation, and weight status in low-income preschool children: how do they relate? Appetite. 2015;89:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.01.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Lensing N, Elsner B. Cool executive functioning predicts not only mean levels but also individual 3-year growth trajectories of zBMI in elementary-school children. Int J Behav Dev. 2019;43(4):351–362. doi: 10.1177/0165025419833818. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Miller AL, Lee HJ, Lumeng JC. Obesity-associated biomarkers and executive function in children. Pediatr Res. 2015;77(1–2):143–147. doi: 10.1038/pr.2014.158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Nelson TD, James TD, Hankey M, Nelson JM, Lundahl A, Espy KA. Early executive control and risk for overweight and obesity in elementary school. Child Neuropsychol. 2017;23(8):994–1002. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2016.1183606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Pieper JR, Laugero KD. Preschool children with lower executive function may be more vulnerable to emotional-based eating in the absence of hunger. Appetite. 2013;62:103–109. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.11.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Yang Y, Shields GS, Guo C, Liu Y. Executive function performance in obesity and overweight individuals: a meta-analysis and review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;84:225–244. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Leung CY, Lumeng JC, Kaciroti NA, Chen YP, Rosenblum K, Miller AL. Surgency and negative affectivity, but not effortful control, are uniquely associated with obesogenic eating behaviors among low-income preschoolers. Appetite. 2014;78:139–146. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.03.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Stifter CA, Moding KJ. Temperament in obesity-related research: concepts, challenges, and considerations for future research. Appetite. 2019;141:104308. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.05.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.van den Heuvel M, Chen Y, Abdullah K, Maguire JL, Parkin PC, Birken CS, et al. The concurrent and longitudinal associations of temperament and nutritional risk factors in early childhood. Pediatr Obes. 2016;12:431–438. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Lundquist E, Austen M, Bermudez M, Rubin C, Bruce AS, Masterson TD, et al. Time spent looking at food during a delay of gratification task is positively associated with children's consumption at ad libitum laboratory meals. Appetite. 2019;141:104341. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.104341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Mischel W, Ebbesen EB, Zeiss AR. Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of gratification. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1972;21(2):204–218. doi: 10.1037/h0032198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Seeyave DM, Coleman S, Appugliese D, Corwyn RF, Bradley RH, Davidson NS, et al. Ability to delay gratification at age 4 years and risk of overweight at age 11 years. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(4):303–308. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Shriver Lenka, Dollar Jessica, Lawless Meg, Calkins Susan, Keane Susan, Shanahan Lilly, Wideman Laurie. Longitudinal Associations between Emotion Regulation and Adiposity in Late Adolescence: Indirect Effects through Eating Behaviors. Nutrients. 2019;11(3):517. doi: 10.3390/nu11030517. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Swinburn BA, Sacks G, Hall KD, McPherson K, Finegood DT, Moodie ML, et al. The global obesity pandemic: shaped by global drivers and local environments. Lancet. 2011;378(9793):804–814. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60813-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Swinburn B, Egger G, Raza F. Dissecting obesogenic environments: the development and application of a framework for identifying and prioritizing environmental interventions for obesity. Prev Med. 1999;29(6 Pt 1):563–570. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1999.0585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Milyavskaya M, Berkman ET, De Ridder DTD. The many faces of self-control: tacit assumptions and recommendations to deal with them. Motivation Science. 2019;5(1):79–85. doi: 10.1037/mot0000108. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Cole PM, Ram N, English MS. Toward a unifying model of self-regulation: a developmental approach. Child Dev Perspect. 2018;13(2):91–96. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Hofmann W, Meindl P, Mooijman M, Graham J. Morality and self-control: how they are intertwined and where they differ. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2018;27(4):286–291. doi: 10.1177/0963721418759317. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Blair C. Developmental science and executive function. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2016;25(1):3–7. doi: 10.1177/0963721415622634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Bridgett DJ, Burt NM, Edwards ES, Deater-Deckard K. Intergenerational transmission of self-regulation: a multidisciplinary review and integrative conceptual framework. Psychol Bull. 2015;141(3):602–654. doi: 10.1037/a0038662. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Lelakowska G, Kanya MJ, Balassone BR, Savoree SL, Boddy LE, Power TG, et al. Toddlers' impulsivity, inhibitory control, and maternal eating-related supervision in relation to toddler body mass index: direct and interactive effects. Appetite. 2019;142:104343. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.104343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Verbruggen F, McAndrew A, Weidemann G, Stevens T, McLaren IP. Limits of executive control: sequential effects in predictable environments. Psychol Sci. 2016;27(5):748–757. doi: 10.1177/0956797616631990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Lin B, Liew J, Perez M. Measurement of self-regulation in early childhood: relations between laboratory and performance-based measures of effortful control and executive functioning. Early Child Res Q. 2019;47:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Lengua LJ, Zalewski M, Fisher P, Moran L. Does HPA-Axis dysregulation account for the effects of income on effortful control and adjustment in preschool children? Infant Child Dev. 2013;22(5):439–458. doi: 10.1002/icd.1805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Dennis TA, Hong M, Solomon B. Do the associations between exuberance and emotion regulation depend on effortful control? Int J Behav Dev. 2010;34(5):462–472. doi: 10.1177/0165025409355514. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Eisenberg N, Zhou Q. Conceptions of executive function and regulation: when and to what degree do they overlap? In: Griffin JA, McCardle P, Freund LS, editors. Executive function in preschool-age children: integrating measurement, neurodevelopment, and translational research. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2016. pp. 115–136. [Google Scholar]
- 78.Eisenberg N, Spinrad TL, Eggum ND. Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children's maladjustment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2010;6:495–525. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Morales S, Perez-Edgar K, Buss K. Longitudinal relations among exuberance, externalizing behaviors, and attentional bias to reward: the mediating role of effortful control. Dev Sci. 2016;19(5):853–862. doi: 10.1111/desc.12320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Kim-Spoon J, Deater-Deckard K, Calkins SD, King-Casas B, Bell MA. Commonality between executive functioning and effortful control related to adjustment. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2019;60:47–55. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2018.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Hendry A, Jones EJH, Charman T. Executive function in the first three years of life: precursors, predictors and patterns. Dev Rev. 2016;42:1–33. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2016.06.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Matte-Gagne C, Bernier A, Sirois MS, Lalonde G, Hertz S. Attachment security and developmental patterns of growth in executive functioning during early elementary school. Child Dev. 2018;89(3):e167–ee82. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Zorza JP, Marino J, Acosta MA. Predictive influence of executive functions, effortful control, empathy, and social behavior on the academic performance in early adolescents. J Early Adolesc. 2017;39(2):253–279. doi: 10.1177/0272431617737624. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Willoughby MT, Wirth RJ, Blair CB. Family life project I. executive function in early childhood: longitudinal measurement invariance and developmental change. Psychol Assess. 2012;24(2):418–431. doi: 10.1037/a0025779. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Willoughby M, Kupersmidt J, Voegler-Lee M, Bryant D. Contributions of hot and cool self-regulation to preschool disruptive behavior and academic achievement. Dev Neuropsychol. 2011;36(2):162–180. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.549980. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Kochanska G, Murray K, Jacques TY, Koenig AL, Vandegeest KA. Inhibitory control in young children and its role in emerging internalization. Child Dev. 1996;67(2):490–507. doi: 10.2307/1131828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Hershey KL, Fisher P. Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: the Children's behavior questionnaire. Child Dev. 2001;72(5):1394–1408. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Lengua LJ, Moran L, Zalewski M, Ruberry E, Kiff C, Thompson S. Relations of growth in effortful control to family income, cumulative risk, and adjustment in preschool-age children. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2015;43(4):705–720. doi: 10.1007/s10802-014-9941-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Tiego J, Bellgrove MA, Whittle S, Pantelis C, Testa R. Common Mechanisms of Executive Attention Underlie Executive Function and Effortful Control in Children. Dev Sci. 2019:e12918. 10.1111/desc.12918. [Epub ahead of print]. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 91.Smith-Donald R, Raver CC, Hayes T, Richardson B. Preliminary construct and concurrent validity of the preschool self-regulation assessment (PSRA) for field-based research. Early Child Res Q. 2007;22(2):173–187. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2007.01.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Zelazo PD, Carlson SM. Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: development and plasticity. Child Dev Perspect. 2012;6(4):354–360. [Google Scholar]
- 93.Metcalfe J, Mischel W. A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of willpower. Psychol Rev. 1999;106(1):3–19. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Mischel W, Ayduk O. Self-regulation in a cognitive--affective personality system: attentional control in the Service of the Self. Self Identity. 2002;1(2):113–120. doi: 10.1080/152988602317319285. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Mischel W. Ebbesen, E. B. attention in delay of gratification. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1970;16(2):329–337. doi: 10.1037/h0029815. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Graziano PA, Calkins SD, Keane SP. Toddler self-regulation skills predict risk for pediatric obesity. Int J Obes. 2010;34(4):633–641. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2009.288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Carnell S, Pryor K, Mais LA, Warkentin S, Benson L, Cheng R. Lunch-time food choices in preschoolers: relationships between absolute and relative intakes of different food categories, and appetitive characteristics and weight. Physiol Behav. 2016;162:151–160. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.03.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Cross MB, Hallett AM, Ledoux TA, O'Connor DP, Hughes SO. Effects of children's self-regulation of eating on parental feeding practices and child weight. Appetite. 2014;81:76–83. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.06.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Adise S, Geier CF, Roberts NJ, White CN, Keller KL. Food or money? Children's brains respond differently to rewards regardless of weight status. Pediatr Obes. 2019;14(2):e12469. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Yokum S, Stice E. Weight gain is associated with changes in neural response to palatable food tastes varying in sugar and fat and palatable food images: a repeated-measures fMRI study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2019;110:1275. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqz204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Shapiro ALB, Johnson SL, Sutton B, Legget KT, Dabelea D, Tregellas JR. Eating in the absence of hunger in young children is related to brain reward network hyperactivity and reduced functional connectivity in executive control networks. Pediatr Obes. 2019;14(6):e12502. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12502. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Alonso-Alonso M, Woods SC, Pelchat M, Grigson PS, Stice E, Farooqi S, et al. Food reward system: current perspectives and future research needs. Nutr Rev. 2015;73(5):296–307. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nuv002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Lowe MR, Butryn ML. Hedonic hunger: a new dimension of appetite? Physiol Behav. 2007;91(4):432–439. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.de Araujo IE, Schatzker M, Small DM. Rethinking food reward. Annu Rev Psychol. 2020;71:139–164. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Reichelt AC, Westbrook RF, Morris MJ. Integration of reward signalling and appetite regulating peptide systems in the control of food-cue responses. Br J Pharmacol. 2015;172(22):5225–5238. doi: 10.1111/bph.13321. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Lumeng JC, Miller A, Peterson KE, Kaciroti N, Sturza J, Rosenblum K, et al. Diurnal cortisol pattern, eating behaviors and overweight in low-income preschool-aged children. Appetite. 2014;73:65–72. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.10.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Jahnke DL, Warschburger PA. Familial transmission of eating behaviors in preschool-aged children. Obesity. 2008;16(8):1821–1825. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Anzman-Frasca S, Ventura AK, Ehrenberg S, Myers KP. Promoting healthy food preferences from the start: a narrative review of food preference learning from the prenatal period through early childhood. Obes Rev. 2018;19(4):576–604. doi: 10.1111/obr.12658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Russell CG, Worsley T. Associations between appetitive traits and food preferences in preschool children. Food Qual Prefer. 2016;52:172–178. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.04.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Bennett C, Blissett J. Interactive effects of impulsivity and dietary restraint over snack intake in children. Appetite. 2019;146:104496. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.104496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Carnell S, Wardle J. Measuring behavioural susceptibility to obesity: validation of the child eating behaviour questionnaire. Appetite. 2007;48(1):104–113. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.07.075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Cole NC, An R, Lee SY, Donovan SM. Correlates of picky eating and food neophobia in young children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutr Rev. 2017;75(7):516–532. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nux024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Russell CG, Worsley A. A population-based study of preschoolers’ food neophobia and its associations with food preferences. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2008;40(1):11–19. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2007.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Lumeng JC, Miller AL, Appugliese D, Rosenblum K, Kaciroti N. Picky eating, pressuring feeding, and growth in toddlers. Appetite. 2018;123:299–305. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.12.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Gregory JE, Paxton SJ, Brozovic AM. Pressure to eat and restriction are associated with child eating behaviours and maternal concern about child weight, but not child body mass index, in 2-to 4-year-old children. Appetite. 2010;54(3):550–556. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.02.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Powell FC, Farrow CV, Meyer C. Food avoidance in children. The influence of maternal feeding practices and behaviours. Appetite. 2011;57(3):683–692. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Bjorklund O, Wichstrom L, Llewellyn CH, Steinsbekk S. Emotional over- and undereating in children: a longitudinal analysis of child and contextual predictors. Child Dev. 2019;90(6):e803–ee18. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Herle M, Fildes A, Llewellyn CH. Emotional eating is learned not inherited in children, regardless of obesity risk. Pediatr Obes. 2018;13(10):628–631. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12428. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Kidd C, Palmeri H, Aslin RN. Rational snacking: young children's decision-making on the marshmallow task is moderated by beliefs about environmental reliability. Cognition. 2013;126(1):109–114. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Bennett C, Blissett J. Multiple measures of impulsivity, eating behaviours and adiposity in 7-11-year-olds. Appetite. 2019;133:217–222. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Rollins BY, Loken E, Savage JS, Birch LL. Effects of restriction on children’s intake differ by child temperament, food reinforcement, and parent’s chronic use of restriction. Appetite. 2014;73:31–39. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.10.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Tan CC, Holub SC. Children's self-regulation in eating: associations with inhibitory control and parents' feeding behavior. J Pediatr Psychol. 2011;36(3):340–345. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsq089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Fogel A, McCrickerd K, Goh AT, Fries LR, Chong YS, Tan KH, et al. Associations between inhibitory control, eating behaviours and adiposity in 6-year-old children. Int J Obes. 2019;43(7):1344–1353. doi: 10.1038/s41366-019-0343-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Shapiro ALB, Wilkening G, Aalborg J, Ringham BM, Glueck DH, Tregellas JR, et al. Childhood metabolic biomarkers are associated with performance on cognitive tasks in Young children. J Pediatr. 2019;211:92–97. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2019.03.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Higgs S, Spetter MS, Thomas JM, Rotshtein P, Lee M, Hallschmid M, et al. Interactions between metabolic, reward and cognitive processes in appetite control: implications for novel weight management therapies. J Psychopharmacol. 2017;31(11):1460–1474. doi: 10.1177/0269881117736917. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Berthoud H-R, Münzberg H, Morrison CD. Blaming the brain for obesity: integration of hedonic and homeostatic mechanisms. Gastroenterology. 2017;152(7):1728–1738. doi: 10.1053/j.gastro.2016.12.050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Keller KL, Bruce AS. Neurocognitive influences on eating behavior in children. In: Lumeng JCF, Fisher JO, editors. Pediatric food preferences and eating behaviors. London: Academic Press; 2018. pp. 207–231. [Google Scholar]
- 128.Berridge KC, Ho CY, Richard JM, DiFeliceantonio AG. The tempted brain eats: pleasure and desire circuits in obesity and eating disorders. Brain Res. 2010;1350:43–64. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Brugailleres P, Issanchou S, Nicklaus S, Chabanet C, Schwartz C. Caloric compensation in infants: developmental changes around the age of 1 year and associations with anthropometric measurements up to 2 years. Am J Clin Nutr. 2019;109(5):1344–1352. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqy357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Leahy KE, Birch LL, Rolls BJ. Reducing the energy density of multiple meals decreases the energy intake of preschool-age children. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;88(6):1459–1468. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2008.26522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Small DM, DiFeliceantonio AG. Processed foods and food reward. Science. 2019;363(6425):346–347. doi: 10.1126/science.aav0556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Berridge KC, Robinson TE. Liking, wanting, and the incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Am Psychol. 2016;71(8):670–679. doi: 10.1037/amp0000059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Blundell J, de Graaf C, Hulshof T, Jebb S, Livingstone B, Lluch A, et al. Appetite control: methodological aspects of the evaluation of foods. Obes Rev. 2010;11(3):251–270. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2010.00714.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Bellisle F, Drewnowski A, Anderson GH, Westerterp-Plantenga M, Martin CK. Sweetness, satiation, and satiety. J Nutr. 2012;142(6):1149S–1154S. doi: 10.3945/jn.111.149583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Simon JJ, Skunde M, Hamze Sinno M, Brockmeyer T, Herpertz SC, Bendszus M, et al. Impaired Cross-talk between mesolimbic food reward processing and metabolic signaling predicts body mass index. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;8:359. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Mennella JA, Nolden AA, Bobowski N. Measuring sweet and bitter taste in children: individual variation due to age and taste genetics. In: Lumeng JCF, Fisher JO, editors. Pediatric food preferences and eating behaviors. London: Academic Press; 2018. pp. 1–34. [Google Scholar]
- 137.Michels N. Biological underpinnings from psychosocial stress towards appetite and obesity during youth: research implications towards metagenomics, epigenomics and metabolomics. Nutr Res Rev. 2019:1–12. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 138.Carnell S, Benson L, Pryor K, Driggin E. Appetitive traits from infancy to adolescence: using behavioral and neural measures to investigate obesity risk. Physiol Behav. 2013;121:79–88. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.02.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Wardle J, Guthrie CA, Sanderson S, Rapoport L. Development of the Children's eating behaviour questionnaire. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42(7):963–970. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Burgess B, Faith MS. Satiety responsiveness and eating rate in childhood: development, plasticity, and the family footprint. In: Lumeng JCF, Fisher JO, editors. Pediatric food preferences and eating behaviors. London: Academic Press; 2018. pp. 93–110. [Google Scholar]
- 141.Llewellyn CH, van Jaarsveld CH, Johnson L, Carnell S, Wardle J. Development and factor structure of the baby eating behaviour questionnaire in the Gemini birth cohort. Appetite. 2011;57(2):388–396. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.05.324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Faith MS, Heo M, Keller KL, Pietrobelli A. Child food neophobia is heritable, associated with less compliant eating, and moderates familial resemblance for BMI. Obesity. 2013;21(8):1650–1655. doi: 10.1002/oby.20369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Faith MS, Carnell S, Kral TV. Genetics of food intake self-regulation in childhood: literature review and research opportunities. Hum Hered. 2013;75(2–4):80–89. doi: 10.1159/000353879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Carnell S, Benson L, Gibson EL, Mais LA, Warkentin S. Caloric compensation in preschool children: relationships with body mass and differences by food category. Appetite. 2017;116:82–89. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.04.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Fox MK, Devaney B, Reidy K, Razafindrakoto C, Ziegler P. Relationship between portion size and energy intake among infants and toddlers: evidence of self-regulation. J Am Diet Assoc. 2006;106(1 Suppl 1):S77–S83. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2005.09.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Remy E, Issanchou S, Chabanet C, Boggio V, Nicklaus S. Impact of adiposity, age, sex and maternal feeding practices on eating in the absence of hunger and caloric compensation in preschool children. Int J Obes. 2015;39(6):925–930. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2015.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Tripicchio GL, Keller KL, Johnson C, Pietrobelli A, Heo M, Faith MS. Differential maternal feeding practices, eating self-regulation, and adiposity in young twins. Pediatrics. 2014;134(5):1399–1404. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-3828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Preuss H, Leister L, Pinnow M, Legenbauer T. Inhibietory control pathway to disinhibited eating: a matter of perspective? Appetite. 2019;141:104297. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.05.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Shomaker LB, Tanofsky-Kraff M, Yanovski JA. Disinhibited eating and body weight in youth. In: Preedy VR, Watson RR, Martin CR, editors. Handbook of behavior, food and nutrition. New York: Springer New York; 2011. pp. 2183–2200. [Google Scholar]
- 150.Lowe CJ, Reichelt AC, Hall PA. The prefrontal cortex and obesity: a health neuroscience perspective. Trends Cogn Sci. 2019;23(4):349–361. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Mennella JA, Bobowski NK, Reed DR. The development of sweet taste: from biology to hedonics. Rev Endocr Metab Disord. 2016;17(2):171–178. doi: 10.1007/s11154-016-9360-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Gearhardt AN. Role of reward pathways in appetitive drive and regulation. In: Lumeng JCF, Fisher JO, editors. Pediatric food preferences and eating behaviors. London: Academic Press; 2018. pp. 111–126. [Google Scholar]
- 153.Smethers AD, Roe LS, Sanchez CE, Zuraikat FM, Keller KL, Kling SMR, et al. Portion size has sustained effects over 5 days in preschool children: a randomized trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 154.Smethers AD, Roe LS, Sanchez CE, Zuraikat FM, Keller KL, Rolls BJ. Both increases and decreases in energy density lead to sustained changes in preschool children's energy intake over 5 days. Physiol Behav. 2019;204:210–218. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.02.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Hetherington MM. Sensory-specific satiety and its importance in meal termination. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1996;20(1):113–117. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(95)00048-J. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Rolls BJ. Sensory-specific satiety. Nutr Rev. 1986;44(3):93–101. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-4887.1986.tb07593.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157.Francis LA, Susman EJ. Self-regulation and rapid weight gain in children from age 3 to 12 years. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(4):297–302. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2008.579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Derks IP, Tiemeier H, Sijbrands EJ, Nicholson JM, Voortman T, Verhulst FC, et al. Testing the direction of effects between child body composition and restrictive feeding practices: results from a population-based cohort. Am J Clin Nutr. 2017;106(3):783–790. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.117.156448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Metcalfe JJ, Fiese BH, Team SKR Family food involvement is related to healthier dietary intake in preschool-aged children. Appetite. 2018;126:195–200. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.03.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160.Peeters A. Obesity and the future of food policies that promote healthy diets. Nat Rev Endocrinol. 2018;14(7):430–437. doi: 10.1038/s41574-018-0026-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161.Yee AZ, Lwin MO, Ho SS. The influence of parental practices on child promotive and preventive food consumption behaviors: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2017;14(1):47. doi: 10.1186/s12966-017-0501-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162.Russell CG, Worsley A, Campbell KJ. Strategies used by parents to influence their children's food preferences. Appetite. 2015;90:123–130. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.02.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163.Godefroy V, Champel C, Trinchera L, Rigal N. Disentangling the effects of parental food restriction on child's risk of overweight. Appetite. 2018;123:82–90. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 164.Loth KA. Associations between food restriction and pressure-to-eat parenting practices and dietary intake in children: a selective review of the recent literature. Current Nutrition Reports. 2016;5(1):61–67. doi: 10.1007/s13668-016-0154-x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 165.Steinsbekk S, Barker ED, Llewellyn C, Fildes A, Wichstrom L. Emotional feeding and emotional eating: reciprocal processes and the influence of negative affectivity. Child Dev. 2018;89(4):1234–1246. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166.Tan CC, Holub SC. The effects of happiness and sadness on Children's snack consumption. Appetite. 2018;123:169–174. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.12.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 167.Cedillo YE, Murillo AL, Fernandez JR. The association between allostatic load and anthropometric measurements among a multiethnic cohort of children. Pediatr Obes. 2019;14(6):e12501. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 168.Miller AL, Riley H, Domoff SE, Gearhardt AN, Sturza J, Kaciroti N, et al. Weight status moderates stress-eating in the absence of hunger associations in children. Appetite. 2019;136:184–192. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 169.Miller AL, Lumeng JC. Pathways of association from stress to obesity in early childhood. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2018;26(7):1117–1124. doi: 10.1002/oby.22155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 170.Jang M, Owen B, Lauver DR. Different types of parental stress and childhood obesity: a systematic review of observational studies. Obes Rev. 2019;20:1740. doi: 10.1111/obr.12930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 171.Mamrot P, Hanc T. The Association of the Executive Functions with overweight and obesity indicators in children and adolescents: a literature review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;107:59. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.08.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 172.Liang J, Matheson BE, Kaye WH, Boutelle KN. Neurocognitive correlates of obesity and obesity-related behaviors in children and adolescents. Int J Obes. 2014;38(4):494–506. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 173.Kamijo K, Khan NA, Pontifex MB, Scudder MR, Drollette ES, Raine LB, et al. The relation of adiposity to cognitive control and scholastic achievement in preadolescent children. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2012;20(12):2406–2411. doi: 10.1038/oby.2012.112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 174.Graziano PA, Kelleher R, Calkins SD, Keane SP, Brien MO. Predicting weight outcomes in preadolescence: the role of toddlers' self-regulation skills and the temperament dimension of pleasure. Int J Obes. 2013;37(7):937–942. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2012.165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 175.Johnson WG, Parry W, Drabman RS. The performance of obese and normal size children on a delay of gratification task. Addict Behav. 1978;3(3–4):205–208. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(78)90020-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 176.Sobhany MS, Rogers CS. External responsiveness to food and non-food cues among obese and non-obese children. Int J Obes. 1985;9(2):99–106. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 177.Nederkoorn C, Coelho JS, Guerrieri R, Houben K, Jansen A. Specificity of the failure to inhibit responses in overweight children. Appetite. 2012;59(2):409–413. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.05.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 178.Schlam TR, Wilson NL, Shoda Y, Mischel W, Ayduk O. Preschoolers' delay of gratification predicts their body mass 30 years later. J Pediatr. 2013;162(1):90–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2012.06.049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 179.Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of Emotion Regulation. 2. New York: Guilford Publications; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 180.Price CJ, Hooven C. Interoceptive awareness skills for emotion regulation: theory and approach of mindful awareness in body-oriented therapy (MABT) Front Psychol. 2018;9:798. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00798. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 181.Cole Pamela M., Ashana Ramsook K., Ram Nilam. Emotion dysregulation as a dynamic process. Development and Psychopathology. 2019;31(3):1191–1201. doi: 10.1017/S0954579419000695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 182.Zimmermann P, Thompson RA. New directions in developmental emotion regulation research across the life span introduction to the special section. Int J Behav Dev. 2014;38(2):139–141. doi: 10.1177/0165025413519015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 183.Gyurak A, Etkin A. A neurobiological model of implicit and explicit emotion regulation. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of Emotion Regulation. 2. New York: Guilford Press; 2014. pp. 76–90. [Google Scholar]
- 184.Duran Chelsea A.K., Cottone Elizabeth, Ruzek Erik A., Mashburn Andrew J., Grissmer David W. Family Stress Processes and Children's Self‐Regulation. Child Development. 2018;91(2):577–595. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 185.Lagasse LL, Conradt E, Karalunas SL, Dansereau LM, Butner JE, Shankaran S, et al. Transactional relations between caregiving stress, executive functioning, and problem behavior from early childhood to early adolescence. Dev Psychopathol. 2016;28(3):743–756. doi: 10.1017/S0954579416000286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 186.Dodge KA, Pettit GS. A biopsychosocial model of the development of chronic conduct problems in adolescence. Dev Psychol. 2003;39(2):349–371. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.39.2.349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 187.Perry NB, Dollar JM, Calkins SD, Bell MA. Developmental Cascade and transactional associations among biological and behavioral indicators of temperament and maternal behavior. Child Dev. 2018;89(5):1735–1751. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12842. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 188.Keller Kathleen, Kling Samantha, Fuchs Bari, Pearce Alaina, Reigh Nicole, Masterson Travis, Hickok Kara. A Biopsychosocial Model of Sex Differences in Children’s Eating Behaviors. Nutrients. 2019;11(3):682. doi: 10.3390/nu11030682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 189.Porter L, Bailey-Jones C, Priudokaite G, Allen S, Wood K, Stiles K, et al. From cookies to carrots; the effect of inhibitory control training on children's snack selections. Appetite. 2018;124:111–123. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 190.Devine RT, Ribner A, Hughes C. Measuring and predicting individual differences in executive functions at 14 months: a longitudinal study. Child Dev. 2019;90:e618. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 191.Blankenship TL, Slough MA, Calkins SD, Deater-Deckard K, Kim-Spoon J, Bell MA. Attention and executive functioning in infancy: Links to childhood executive function and reading achievement. Dev Sci. 2019;22(6):e12824. 10.1111/desc.12824. Epub 2019 Apr 2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 192.Sheese BE, Rothbart MK, Posner MI, White LK, Fraundorf SH. Executive attention and self-regulation in infancy. Infant Behav Dev. 2008;31(3):501–510. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 193.Garon N. A review of hot executive functions in preschoolers. J Self Regul Regul. 2016;2:57. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Not applicable.