Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Mar 12.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Med. 2018 Jun 6;49(4):675–684. doi: 10.1017/S003329171800137X

Accelerated alcohol use across adolescence predicts early adult symptoms of alcohol use disorder via reward-related neural function

Rebecca Waller 1,2, Laura Murray 1, Daniel S Shaw 3,4, Erika E Forbes 3,4,5, Luke W Hyde 1,5,6,7,*
PMCID: PMC7066874  NIHMSID: NIHMS1560697  PMID: 29871712

Abstract

Background

Alcohol use is commonly initiated during adolescence, with earlier onset known to increase risk for alcohol use disorder (AUD). Altered function in neural reward circuitry is thought to increase risk for AUD. To test the hypothesis that adolescent alcohol misuse primes the brain for alcohol-related psychopathology in early adulthood, we examined whether adolescent alcohol consumption rates predicted reward responsivity in the ventral striatum (VS), and in turn, AUD symptoms in adulthood.

Methods

139 low income, racially diverse urban males reported on their alcohol use at ages 11, 12, 15, and 17; completed self-reports of personality, psychiatric interviews, and an fMRI scan at age 20; and completed a psychiatric interview at age 22. We measured adolescent alcohol use trajectories using latent growth curve modeling and measured neural responses to monetary reward using a VS region of interest. We tested indirect effects of adolescent alcohol use on AUD symptoms at age 22 via VS reward-related reactivity at age 20.

Results

Greater acceleration in adolescent alcohol use predicted increased VS response during reward anticipation at age 20. VS reactivity to reward anticipation at age 20 predicted AUD symptoms at age 22, over and above concurrent symptoms. Accelerated adolescent alcohol use predicted AUD symptoms in early adulthood via greater VS reactivity to reward anticipation.

Conclusions

Prospective findings support a pathway through which adolescent alcohol use increases risk for AUD in early adulthood by impacting reward-related neural functioning. These results highlight increased VS reward-related reactivity as a biomarker for AUD vulnerability.

Keywords: Adolescence, Alcohol Use Disorder, fMRI, Prospective, Reward, Trajectory

Introduction

Alcohol use disorder (AUD) is a widespread illness in the US with estimated 12-month and lifetime prevalence rates of 13.9% and 29.1% (Grant et al., 2015). AUD harms individuals by disrupting educational attainment and interpersonal functioning, and increasing risk for negative physical and psychiatric outcomes (Grant et al., 2015, Rehm, 2011). AUD is also costly to society through lower productivity, crime, and motor vehicle accidents. The prevalence of AUD increases steadily across adolescence followed by dramatic increases in early adulthood (Chassin et al., 2004, Grant et al., 2015). Alcohol use commonly begins in adolescence (Johnston et al., 2015), making this a critical period to investigate the developmental origins of AUD.

One way that alcohol use might increase risk for AUD is by impacting the developing brain, particularly subcortical and prefrontal regions, which undergo significant neurodevelopmental change during adolescence (Casey et al., 2011, Doremus-Fitzwater et al., 2010, Gogtay et al., 2004). During adolescence, there is pruning of dopamine receptors in reward-related areas of the brain, such as the ventral striatum (VS; Seeman, 1987, Teicher et al., 1995). These neurodevelopmental changes are thought to be important for understanding the neural underpinnings of reward-driven behavior and risk for alcohol misuse (Spear, 2016), especially because drugs of abuse enhance dopamine neurotransmission (Robinson and Berridge, 1993). Thus, adolescence is thought to be a time when the brain may be particularly vulnerable to the neurotoxic effects of alcohol (Casey and Jones, 2010, Wiers et al., 2007). Animal research supports this hypothesis, showing that the effects of alcohol on behavioral and cognitive functioning, as well as brain structure, are more pronounced during adolescence relative to adulthood (Spear, 2016). In humans, cross-sectional fMRI studies also document differences in the reactivity of reward-related neural systems among adolescents with AUD relative to healthy controls (Ewing et al., 2014). Thus, greater exposure to alcohol during adolescence may result in altered neurocircuitry of the reward system, including sensitization of VS dopaminergic pathways, which could increase vulnerability for developing AUDs later in life.

However, the majority of studies that have examined associations between adolescent alcohol use and reward-related neural functioning are limited by being cross-sectional in design, utilizing small sample sizes, focusing only on clinical samples with extreme use (Tapert et al., 2004, Wetherill et al., 2013, Xiao et al., 2013), and/or examining reward-related neural functioning only among children of alcoholics (Heitzeg et al., 2010, Yau et al., 2012). Longitudinal studies are needed to provide a better test of whether adolescent alcohol use impacts reward-related neural functioning. Moreover, evidence is needed from naturalistic samples beginning with alcohol use onset, rather than from youth who are already abusing alcohol. Thus, prospective studies of community youth using repeated assessments represent a powerful way to examine change in alcohol use over time (Duncan and Duncan, 1995, Duncan et al., 1994), and establish whether different (or even normative) rates of alcohol consumption across adolescence impact reward-related neural functioning and persistent alcohol misuse.

In the current study, we addressed two questions centered on alcohol use from adolescence to early adulthood via VS reward-related functioning. We focused on a low income, urban male sample because urban males are exposed to more risk factors linked to alcohol misuse (Elliott et al., 2012) and there are higher rates of AUD in males relative to females (Grant et al., 2015). First, we examined whether the trajectory of alcohol use (i.e., rate of increase in use) across adolescence predicted VS reward processing at age 20. We focused on a VS region-of-interest because of the centrality of the VS to reward processing (Haber and Knutson, 2010) and links between VS reactivity and AUD in adulthood (Heitzeg et al., 2015, Nikolova et al., 2016). We tested whether the relationship between adolescent alcohol use and VS reactivity was dependent on reward phase (i.e., reward anticipation vs. receipt). We hypothesized that because of increased incentive anticipation and “wanting” of reward linked to repeated alcohol exposure (Robinson and Berridge, 1993, Silveri and Spear, 2002), greater adolescent alcohol use would be related to increased VS reactivity during reward anticipation, but not reward receipt. Second, we examined whether trajectories of alcohol use across adolescence were related to AUD symptoms at age 22 via VS reward processing at age 20, testing longitudinal, indirect pathways. We hypothesized that adolescent alcohol use would predict VS hypersensitivity during reward anticipation at age 20, which in turn, would predict increases in AUD symptoms at age 22 (Figure 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Hypothesized associations between trajectories of adolescent alcohol use, ventral striatum reactivity at age 20, and increases in symptoms of Alcohol Use Disorder at age 22.

Note. * At age 12, clinician consensus data were not available for the KSADS, thus we did not include Conduct Disorder or ADHD diagnoses from this assessment point. ADHD=attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; MDD=major depressive disorder; GAD=generalized anxiety disorder; SPD=social phobia; PTSD=post-traumatic stress disorder; APD=antisocial personality disorder; AUD=alcohol use disorder; DUD=drug use disorder; DV=dependent variable; IV=independent variable. To address Aim 1, we tested whether individual differences in the rate of alcohol use across adolescence from ages 11, 12, 15 & 17, based on latent growth curve modeling, were related to ventral striatum reward reactivity at age 20 assessed via fMRI. To address Aim 2, we tested whether individual differences in ventral striatum reward reactivity at age 20 were related to increases in symptoms of Alcohol Use Disorder at age 22 (i.e., controlling for symptoms of Alcohol Use Disorder at age 20 and overlapping DUD and APD symptoms at age 22). The indirect (mediated) effect of adolescent alcohol use on later symptoms of Alcohol Use Disorder at age 22 via ventral striatum reactivity at age 20 was also examined. Models controlled for other salient personality, psychiatric, and demographic confounds.

Methods and Materials

Participants

139 participants were drawn from the Pitt Mother & Child Project, a longitudinal study of 310 racially diverse and low-income boys and their families (Shaw et al., 2012). The sample is at risk for externalizing outcomes based on being male, urban, and from low-income families (Shaw et al., 2012). Boys and their mothers were seen in person almost yearly from ages 1.5–22 in their home and/or the lab (assessments at 1 1/2, 2, 3 1/2, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 15, 17, 20, and 22 years old). In the current study, we focused on the latter six assessment points that covered adolescence and early adulthood: 11, 12, 15, 17, 20 and 22 years old. Assessments included questionnaires and a psychiatric interview. At age 20, the assessment also included an fMRI scan at age 20. Participants were reimbursed after assessments and procedures were approved by the University of Pittsburgh IRB.

Attrition to the age 20 and 22 visits was low for such a long-term study (252 and 256 men participated at ages 20 and 22, respectively 81% and 83% retention across more than 20 years) (Murray et al., 2017, Shaw et al., 2012). Of the 256 at age 20, 144 men had usable fMRI reward data and 139 had both fMRI and adolescent alcohol use data (Supplemental Table 1 summarizes details on attrition). The 139 males with fMRI data at age 20 did not differ significantly from the full sample retained at age 20 based on self-reported alcohol (p>.39) or marijuana (p>.76) consumption via the Alcohol and Drug Consumption Questionnaire (Cahalan et al., 1969) or self-reported antisocial behavior (p>.37) via the Self-Report of Delinquency Questionnaire (Elliott et al., 2012).

Measures

Adolescent Alcohol Use (ages 11, 12, 15, and 17)

We assessed adolescent alcohol use using items from the Self-Report Delinquency Questionnaire (Elliott et al., 2012), which assesses engagement in antisocial activities in the past year via a three-point scale (0=never, 1=once/twice, 2=more often; α=.90). Three separate items assessing consumption of beer, liquor, or wine were summed to create alcohol frequency scores at each age, which were subjected to latent growth curve modeling to derive alcohol trajectories (see Supplemental Table 2). Rates were similar to those reported via surveys of adolescents within community samples (Johnston et al., 2015, Miech et al., 2015).

Symptoms of Alcohol Use Disorder (ages 20 and 22)

To assess alcohol use disorder (AUD) at ages 20 and 22, we used interviewer assessments from the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I (SCID-I) (American Psychiatric Association, 2000, First et al., 1995) (Supplemental Methods outline the training and reliability procedures). Results are presented using an AUD symptom count combining total number of alcohol use and dependence symptoms. To confirm these results within a DSM-5 framework, which removed the abuse-dependence distinction, we compared findings for a symptom count excluding “recurrent substance-related legal problems” that does not appear in DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). We also found similar results using separate alcohol abuse versus dependence symptom counts. At age 22, approximately 21% of our sample met lifetime criteria for Alcohol Abuse or Dependence diagnoses. These estimates are consistent with epidemiological surveys of community samples (Grant et al., 2004).

Personality Confounds (age 20)

To ensure that effects were not accounted for by stable personality traits linked to hypersensitivity to reward or alcohol misuse, we controlled for impulsivity, which has been linked to VS reward-related reactivity and alcohol use (Beck et al., 2009), and extraversion, which predicts brain reward-related functioning (Cohen et al., 2005) and alcohol use (Kuntsche et al., 2006). Impulsivity was assessed at age 20 via self-report using the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-Version II, a 30 item measure tapping several aspects of impulsivity, including deficits in behavioral control (Patton and Stanford, 1995) (α=.79) Extraversion was assessed age 20 via self-report on the 12-item extraversion subscale of the NEO Personality Inventory-Revised (NEO PI-R Short Form) (α=.64) (Costa and McCrae, 1997).

Adolescent comorbid psychiatric disorders

To confirm that relationships between adolescent alcohol use and brain reward-related functioning were not due to earlier ADHD or Conduct Disorder (CD), both well-established risk factors for AUD (van Emmerik-van Oortmerssen et al., 2012), we included ADHD and CD diagnoses from the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School Age Children (Kaufman et al., 1997), a semi-structured psychiatric interview using DSM-IV criteria. At ages 11, 15, and 17 years old, examiners administered interviews to boys and their mothers and diagnoses were made through clinician consensus. Training and reliability procedures are described in Supplemental Methods. Earlier ADHD or CD were included as a present/absent dichotomous variable (i.e., present=diagnosis at any prior assessment).

Other comorbid psychiatric disorders

Finally, to ensure that effects were not accounted for by psychiatric comorbidities related to hypersensitivity to reward or engagement in alcohol misuse, we controlled symptoms of the following DSM-IV disorders at age 20 as covariates based on SCID-I (First et al., 1995) and SCID-II (First et al., 1997) interviews: antisocial personality disorder (APD; Compton et al., 2005), major depressive disorder (MDD; Hasin et al., 2005), generalized anxiety disorder (GAD; Grant et al., 2004, Kushner et al., 2000), post-traumatic stress disorder (Kushner et al., 2000), and social phobia (SP; Kushner et al., 2000). Although we included symptom counts as covariates in models, the findings were similar if we included diagnoses. Finally, in addition to including comorbid psychiatric disorders as covariates at age 20, we explored the specificity of pathways to AUD, by including Substance Use Disorder (SUD) and APD symptoms as dependent variables at age 22.

Other Covariates.

To ensure that results relating to differences in reward functioning were not due to race or socioeconomic status, we accounted for the effects of race and income (Murray et al., 2017). As alcohol use tends to co-occur with marijuana and tobacco use (Moss et al., 2014), we also accounted for rates of comorbid adolescent marijuana and tobacco use assessed via the same method as alcohol use (i.e., trajectories across adolescence; Supplemental Table 2). Finally, all models included the age at which participants first reporting drinking as a covariate. Descriptive statistics for all study variables are presented in Table 1 and bivariate correlations between study variables in Supplemental Table 3.

Table 1.

Descriptive data for other study variables for the subsample of men for whom imaging data were available (n = 139)

N Min-Max M SD
Demographic covariate (age 20)
Family monthly income ($) 139 300.00–4000.00 1010 710
Age of drinking onset 123 9.00–20.00 15.48 2.19
Psychiatric comorbidities (DMS-IV symptom counts at age 20)
Major Depressive Disorder 139 .00–6.00 .30 1.17
Generalized Anxiety 139 .00–5.00 .12 .66
Social Phobia 139 .00–5.00 .38 1.23
Antisocial Personality Disorder 139 .00–5.00 .48 1.13
Post-traumatic Stress Disorder 139 .00–13.00 .38 2.02
Personality covariates
Impulsivity (age 20) 139 9.00–60.00 29.31 9.09
Extraversion (age 20) 139 12.00–40.00 27.00 5.37
Alcohol and substance Use (DSM-IV symptoms at age 20)
Alcohol Use & Dependence (age 20) 139 .00–9.00 .56 1.39
Drug Use & Dependence (age 20) 139 .00–8.00 .92 1.78
Dependent variables in final model
Alcohol Use & Dependence (age 22) 139 .00–9.00 .65 1.61
Drug Use & Dependence (age 22) 139 .00–9.00 .71 1.52
Antisocial Personality Disorder (age 22) 139 .00–6.00 .63 1.44

Note. Findings were similar when we accounted for psychiatric comorbidities using diagnoses instead of symptom count. The number of participants meeting diagnostic criteria was as follows at age 20: Major Depressive Disorder, N=12 (9%), Generalizing Anxiety Disorder, N=1 (1%), Social Phobia, N=10 (7%), Antisocial Personality Disorder, N=11 (8%), Post-traumatic stress disorder, N=2 (1%), and Alcohol Use and Dependence, N=18 (13%). At age 22, the number of participants meeting criteria for Alcohol Use and Dependence was N=28 (20%).

Neuroimaging Procedures

After participants completed questionnaires and clinical interviews, they underwent an fMRI scan. The fMRI reward paradigm was a slow event-related card-guessing game that evaluates neural response to the anticipation and receipt of monetary reward (Nusslock et al., 2012). During trials, participants guessed via button press whether the values of visually presented cards, with possible values of 1–9, were higher or lower than 5 (4s), learned the trial type (possible-win) to anticipate feedback (6s), and receive feedback (e.g., win money/no change; 1s plus 9s inter-trial interval) (Nusslock et al., 2012). Participants were told that their performance would determine a monetary reward after the scan ($1 per win). Trials were presented in pseudorandom order with predetermined outcomes in a single, 8-minute run, with 24 trials and a balanced number of trial types. This task has previously been shown to differentiate anticipation versus receipt phases of reward processing with large task-based effect sizes in the VS (Hasler et al., 2012, Nusslock et al., 2012; see Supplemental Figure 1 for a fuller task description).

Bold fMRI acquisition parameters

As described previously (Murray et al., 2017), participants were scanned with a research-dedicated Siemens 3-T Trio scanner with an 12-channel head coil at the University of Pittsburgh. Blood oxygenation level–dependent (BOLD) functional images were acquired with a gradient-echo echoplanar imaging sequence repetition time (TR)/echo time (TE)/flip angle=2000/29 milliseconds/90°, field of view=200×200mm, matrix=64×64), that covered 34 interleaved axial slices (3mm slice thickness) aligned with the AC-PC plane and encompassing the entire cerebrum and most of the cerebellum to maximize coverage of limbic structures.

MRI data analysis

Image processing and analysis.

Analyses were completed using the general linear model of SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). Images for each participant were segmented, realigned to the first volume in the time series, unwarped to correct for head motion, co-registered to high-resolution structural scans, spatially normalized into Montreal Neurological Institute space using a 12-parameter affine model, and smoothed to minimize noise and residual difference in gyral anatomy with a 6mm FWHM Gaussian filter. Voxelwise signal intensities were ratio-normalized to the whole-brain global mean. After preprocessing, Artifact detection Tools software (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/artifact_detect/) was used by detecting global mean intensity and translation or rotational motion outliers (>4.5 SD from the mean global brain activation, >2mm movement, or 2× translation in any direction) by creating a regressor within each participant’s first-level analysis to account for confounding effects of volumes with large motion deflections or intensity spikes. Single-subject BOLD fMRI data were only included in analyses if there was a minimum of 85% VS coverage using a bilateral, anatomically-based VS ROI. Participants with less than 80% task responding were excluded from analysis (Murray et al., 2017).

BOLD fMRI data analysis.

Linear contrasts employing canonical hemodynamic response functions were used to estimate condition-specific BOLD activation for each individual. Individual contrast images were used in second-level random effects models to determine mean reward-related reactivity using one-sample t-tests for: reward anticipation>baseline and reward outcome>baseline. Baseline was defined as the last 3 seconds of the 9-second inter-trial interval as previously described (Nusslock et al., 2012). As the VS is central to reward processing (Haber and Knutson, 2010), we examined results within the VS ROI masking for main effects of the task using a Family-Wise Error (FWE) correction. A bilateral VS ROI was constructed in the WFU PickAtlas Tool v2.4 using two spheres of 10mm radius around MNI coordinates x=+/−12, y=12, z=−10.

Analytic strategy

Deriving Trajectories of Alcohol Use across Adolescence.

To measure trajectories of alcohol use across adolescence, we used latent growth curve modeling (LGCM) in Mplus version 7.2 (Muthén and Muthén, 2014) using alcohol frequency scores at ages 11, 12, 15, and 17 (Supplemental Table 2). We estimated latent factors for the intercept, slope, and quadratic slope. Comparative Fit Index (CFI: cut-off value .95), and Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA: cut-off value .06) were used to assess model fit. We compared a no-change (intercept-only) model, linear model, and quadratic model, judging the best-fitting model based on CFI and RMSEA values and whichever had the lowest Bayesian Information Criterion (Hu and Bentler, 1999).

Adolescent Alcohol Use, Brain Reward-Related Functioning, and AUD in adulthood.

First, regression analysis was performed in SPM8 to determine whether adolescent alcohol use was associated with neural response during reward anticipation or outcome. We first ran a model where we included only starting levels of alcohol, race, and income (i.e., “no covariates models”). Second, we ran a model including the following covariates: race, income, alcohol starting levels, impulsivity, extraversion, psychiatric diagnoses, and adolescent marijuana and nicotine use (Table 1 presents descriptive data).

To link differences in neural response to reward to later alcohol use, we used two approaches. First, we employed conjunction analysis in SPM8 (Nichols et al., 2005) to determine whether AUD symptoms at age 22 were related to reward responsivity in overlapping regions associated with adolescent alcohol use. Second, as conjunction analyses cannot test indirect effects, we tested a path model in Mplus to examine whether adolescent alcohol use was related to AUD symptoms at age 22 via brain reward-related functioning at age 20. To avoid double correlation issues inherent in extracting data from SPM8 based on our variables of interest (i.e., alcohol use) (Kriegeskorte et al., 2009), we extracted left and right VS peaks within an anatomical VS mask from the main effects of the reward task using the VOI tool (Supplemental Figure 2) (for examples of this approach, see Hyde et al., 2014, Waller et al., 2016). The indirect pathway from adolescent alcohol use to AUD symptoms at age 22 via VS reactivity at age 20 was estimated as the product of the coefficients (AB) (“Sobel test”). To test the specificity of effects to AUD, we included direct and indirect pathways from to SUD and APD symptoms at age 22.

Results

Main Effects of Task in the Ventral Striatum

The reward task yielded robust bilateral activity within the VS ROI with significant clusters emerging for reward anticipation>baseline (Supplemental Figure 2) and reward receipt>baseline (Supplemental Figure 3) (Murray et al., 2017).

Trajectories of Alcohol Use across Adolescence

A quadratic LGCM showed the best fit to the adolescent alcohol use data (Figure 2). The model included significant intercept and quadratic terms, as well as significant variance for the quadratic term. This model suggests that alcohol use increased at a non-linear, accelerating rate (Grimm et al., 2011) (Figure 2). We extracted intercept (i.e., to control for starting level) and quadratic factor scores to examine effects on brain reward-related functioning. Similar to the alcohol LGCM, the best fitting models for adolescent marijuana and tobacco use were quadratic (results available on request). We included the quadratic factor scores for marijuana and tobacco as covariates within models to establish unique effects of alcohol use.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Alcohol use accelerates across adolescence in a sample of low-income males

Note. We used latent growth curve modeling (LGCM) in Mplus version 7.2 and robust maximum likelihood (MLR) estimation based on alcohol frequency scores at ages 11, 12, 15, and 17 (see Supplemental Table 2). We estimated latent factors for the intercept (mean starting level), slope (linear change over time), and quadratic slope (nonlinear change over time/acceleration). A quadratic LGCM showed the best fit to the alcohol use data across adolescence: χ2=1.52, df=1, p= 22, CFI=.99, RMSEA=.04). The model included significant intercept (B=.06, SE=.02, p=.002) and quadratic (B=.04, SE=.01, p<.001) terms, including significance variance for the quadratic term (B=.004, SE=.001, p=.01). The red line represents estimated mean accelerated rate of alcohol use across time in the sample, which was estimated separately and superimposed onto individual curves to aid interpretation of findings. We examined whether latent classes existed within this overall sample trajectory, but found no evidence for distinct classes represented within either a 2- or 3- class solution.

Association between adolescent alcohol use and brain reward-related functioning

During reward anticipation, greater acceleration (i.e., quadratic factor scores) in adolescent alcohol use was associated with higher response in the left, but not right, VS at age 20 (t=4.21, k=7; x=−14, y=18, z=−12, p<.05FWE). This association emerged in a model controlling only for initial alcohol use (intercept), race, and income (Figure 3A & 3B). We also found a significant association between adolescent alcohol use and higher response in the left VS in a stringent model that accounted for adolescent marijuana and tobacco use, race, income, adolescent ADHD and CD diagnoses, extraversion and impulsivity at age 20, and MDD, PTSD, GAD, SP, and APD symptoms at age 20 (t=4.21, k=1; x=−14, y=18, z=−12, p<.05FWE). Results were specific to reward anticipation as there was no association between adolescent alcohol use and VS reactivity during reward outcome.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Greater acceleration in alcohol use across adolescence predicts increased ventral striatum reactivity during the anticipation of rewards at age 20

Note. Greater acceleration in adolescent alcohol use is related to increased left VS reactivity in the left VS ROI (centered at peak voxel: t=4.21, k=7; x=−14, y=18, z=−12, p<.05FWE), controlling for alcohol starting level, race, and income. A smaller cluster was also significant (not shown in figure) centered at peak voxel: t=3.82, k=5, x=−12, y=10, z=−12, p<.05FWE). The relationship between adolescent alcohol use and increased left VS reactivity also emerged after controlling for alcohol starting level, marijuana and tobacco use across adolescence, race, family income, earlier ADHD and CD, extraversion and impulsivity at age 20, and MDD, PTSD, GAD, SP, and APD symptoms at age 20 (t=4.21, k=1; x=−14, y=18, z=−12, p<.05FWE).

Association between brain reward-related functioning and later AUD symptoms

Conjunction analyses suggested that more symptoms of AUD at age 22 were associated with increased left VS response to reward anticipation in an overlapping region that had been associated with adolescent alcohol use based on the more stringent model and even after controlling for AUD symptoms at age 20 (t=1.80, k=1; x=−14, y=18, z=−12, p<.05FWE).

We also tested a path model in Mplus vs. 7.2 focusing on three peaks that had emerged within anatomical VS ROIs from the task main effects. There were significant direct pathways from adolescent alcohol use to increased reactivity in all three VS ROIs during reward anticipation at age 20, even controlling for their overlap (Figure 4). From a peak in the left VS ROI, there was also a pathway from higher response to reward anticipation to increases in AUD symptoms at age 22, but not SUD or APD symptoms at age 22, controlling for symptoms of all three disorders at age 20. The indirect path from adolescent alcohol use to AUD symptoms at age 22 through left VS reactivity at age 20 was significant at a trend level, confirming the results of the conjunction analysis within a more conservative model (Figure 4). Finally, because traditional significance testing using p-values represents a weaker form of inference, we computed a log-transformed Bayes factor (BF10) for the indirect pathway (Dienes and Mclatchie, 2017). The BF10 was 3.92 indicating “substantial” evidence for the alternative hypothesis (Jeffreys, 1998). That is, the data from the indirect pathway we reported are 3.92 times more likely to have occurred under the alternative hypothesis than the null hypothesis. Thus, all three approaches for testing the indirect effect converged in supporting the hypothesis that accelerated adolescent alcohol use predicted symptoms of AUD in early adulthood via reward-related VS reactivity.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Accelerated alcohol use in adolescence predicts increased VS reactivity during the anticipation of monetary reward at age 20, and in turn, left VS reactivity predicts increases in AUD symptom severity from ages 20 to 22.

Note. **p<.01, p<.05. The model shows only significant pathways but we modeled all pathways from predictors to outcomes and within-time covariance (i.e., between all measures at ages 20 and 22; see Supplemental Table 4). We tested whether accelerated adolescent alcohol use was related to AUD symptoms at age 22 via brain reward-related reactivity (Figure 1). The model included a quadratic factor of adolescent alcohol use extracted from LGCM (Figure 2) and three significant peaks that emerged within the anatomic VS ROI from the main effects of the reward task in the left and right VS (Supplemental Figure 1). The main variables of interest are shown in bold text. Covariates and correlated outcomes (i.e., to test specificity and uniqueness of effects) are shown in smaller, italicized, non-emphasized text. Direct paths were examined for statistical significance. Indirect pathways were estimated as the product of the coefficients (“Sobel test”) as an index of effect size but we also present bootstrapped CI of the effects. To examine whether VS reward-related reactivity uniquely predicted increases in AUD symptoms at age 22, we controlled for AUD symptoms at age 20. To isolate effects of adolescent alcohol use, we also accounted for symptoms of other drug use disorders and antisocial personality disorder both when the fMRI scan was completed at age 20 and concurrently to the outcome of AUD symptoms at age 22. The model accounted for the effects of race and income. Results were unchanged when we included concurrent marijuana and tobacco use across adolescence, impulsivity and extraversion at age 20, and other comorbid psychiatric disorders at age 20.

Discussion

We provide evidence that greater acceleration in alcohol use from ages 11–17 is related to increased VS reactivity during reward anticipation at age 20, which in turn predicted symptoms of AUD at age 22. We accounted for a host of covariates, suggesting that adolescent alcohol use independently contributes to reward-related neural function and is not attributable to comorbid psychopathology. Moreover, the association between alcohol use and VS reactivity in early adulthood was maintained after adjusting for adolescent marijuana and tobacco use and co-occurring drug use and APD, emphasizing specificity in the effects on AUD. These findings highlight a mechanistic neural pathway through which greater adolescent alcohol use is associated with reward responsivity at age 20, leading to risk for persistent, clinically-significant AUD symptoms at age 22, a time of heightened vulnerability for AUD development (Chassin et al., 2004, Grant et al., 2015).

Consistent with prior findings from community samples (Duncan and Duncan, 1995, Duncan et al., 1994), we found escalation in alcohol consumption across adolescence within an urban, low-income male sample, a group known to be at risk for AUD (Elliott et al., 2012, Grant et al., 2015). Greater escalation in rate of adolescent alcohol use was related to brain reward-related functioning during the period of emerging adulthood involving ongoing brain maturation (Casey and Jones, 2010, Casey et al., 2011). Results are consistent with animal findings suggesting that adolescent drinking primes the brain for alcohol-related psychopathology through increased reward sensitivity (Spear, 2016). For example, alcohol exposure during adolescence was linked to persistence of an “adolescent-like phenotype” in adult rodents characterized by greater wanting of alcohol (Spear and Swartzwelder, 2014). Several receptor systems are thought to underpin increased reward sensitivity to alcohol (Silveri and Spear, 2002), including the N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptor system, expressed in striato-cortical structures, such as the VS (Schramm et al., 2002). This system undergoes significant developmental change during adolescence, providing a molecular mechanism through which adolescents may experience increased responsiveness to the positive aspects of alcohol (Silveri and Spear, 2002, Spear and Swartzwelder, 2014). Thus, via changes to NMDA signaling, greater adolescent alcohol exposure may confer hypersensitivity of striato-cortical reward circuits, promoting adolescent-like, impulsive alcohol misuse that continues into adulthood, including AUD symptomatology (Conrod and Nikolaou, 2016).

We also found an association between higher VS reactivity to reward anticipation at age 20 and increases in AUD symptom severity from ages 20–22. Although neurobiological models of psychopathology emphasize that individual differences in brain reactivity precede changes in symptomatology, only a handful of empirical studies have established such “neuroprediction” pathways, including for depression (Mattson et al., 2016) and criminal recidivism (Aharoni et al., 2013). Our results suggest that hypersensitive reward-related neural reactivity represents a predictive biomarker of risk for AUD. At the same time, caution is warranted as our results do not preclude the possibility that pre-existing neural alterations in reward circuity predisposed individuals to adolescent alcohol misuse or AUD. This possibility is supported by studies of adolescents with a family history of alcoholism who show functional and structural differences in reward circuitry relative to healthy controls prior to alcohol use onset (Yau et al., 2012). Although a limitation of this study is that we did not measure parent history of AUD, we did control for the effects of impulsivity and extraversion, which could be proxies for stable biologically-based predispositions for adolescent alcohol use or neural hypersensitivity to reward. Future prospective studies capable of identifying individual differences in reward circuity prior to initiation of alcohol use would help to address these issues. Notably, these issues do not apply to the neuroprediction pathway we established during early adulthood, where VS hypersensitivity to reward at age 20 predicted increases in AUD symptoms at age 22.

Finally, we found a relationship between adolescent alcohol use and increased VS reactivity to reward anticipation, but not reward receipt. The anticipation phase is associated with appetitive processing, or “wanting” reward, whereas the receipt phase is linked to consumption, or “liking” a gained reward (Robinson and Berridge, 1993). Our results are consistent with a recent study that used a similar task, reporting increased VS activation during the anticipation of monetary reward among alcohol-dependent adults versus healthy controls (Becker et al., 2016). However, findings differ in direction from studies of adult alcoholics, who are typically reported to show reduced VS activation during monetary reward anticipation (Beck et al., 2009, Wrase et al., 2007). Thus, a developmental dissociation may exist, such that adolescent drinking initially primes the brain for increased response to non-substance and substance-associated reward cues. However, following chronic alcohol exposure associated with alcohol dependency, a “hijacking” of the reward system via dopaminergic modulation of signals in the VS may occur that “flips” motivational responses specifically to non-substance related rewards (Deserno et al., 2015, Forbes et al., 2014, Robinson and Berridge, 1993). Future studies employing multiple follow-up scans of participants across adolescence and adulthood are needed to test this developmental dissociation hypothesis.

The current study had a number of strengths, including a prospective, naturalistic design, well-established task for eliciting VS reward-related reactivity, and sophisticated quantitative modeling, all within a relatively large community sample. However, several limitations are worth noting. First, our sample consisted only of males raised in low-income, urban homes. Thus, the findings may not generalize to other populations, particularly women and/or those not living in low-income, urban environments. We focused on males because they are more likely to engage in risky behavior and drug use (Compton et al., 2005, Grant et al., 2015). However, there are important sex-specific volumetric developmental changes that occur in the VS across adolescence in the VS (cubic for males and linear for females) (Goddings et al., 2014). Accordingly, future studies of sex-balanced samples at risk for AUD are needed to explore potentially sex-specific neural pathways that impact VS reward-related responsivity and differentially increase risk for AUD. Second, the measure we used to assess alcohol and marijuana use across time had a limited frequency range (i.e., 0–2), which may have led to an underestimation of effect sizes. Future prospective studies are needed with measures that can more precisely assess alcohol consumption (e.g., Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test; Bush et al., 1998). Finally, although we assessed symptoms of AUD using a standardized clinical interview with extensive training and oversight of interviewers, inter-rater reliability of interviews was not assessed.

The current study is the first to use a large community sample of low-income, racially diverse males to examine how individual differences in the rate of alcohol use across adolescence impact later functioning of mesolimbic reward circuitry and risk for AUD. We found that greater acceleration in adolescent alcohol use was related to increased VS reactivity during reward anticipation. In turn, this pathway predicted increases in AUD symptoms during early adulthood. Results provide evidence to support the hypothesis that greater acceleration in adolescent alcohol consumption is related to hypersensitivity of the brain to reward anticipation and in turn, clinically-significant alcohol use, which persists into adulthood. Thus, increased VS reward-related reactivity may represent a biological mechanism to be targeted within novel strategies for more effective treatment of AUD.

Supplementary Material

Supplemental Material

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by Grant R01 MH050907 from the National Institutes of Health to Daniel S. Shaw, Grant R01 DA02622 to Daniel S. Shaw and Erika E. Forbes. Rebecca Waller was supported by a NIAAA T32 Fellowship in the Addiction Center, Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan (2T32 AA007477-24A1). The authors thank the staff and study families of the Pitt Mother and Child Project.

Footnotes

Financial Disclosures

All authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest

References

  1. Aharoni E, Vincent GM, Harenski CL, Calhoun VD, Sinnott-Armstrong W, Gazzaniga MS & Kiehl KA (2013). Neuroprediction of future rearrest. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110, 6223–6228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Association AP (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV). American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC. [Google Scholar]
  3. Association AP (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association. [Google Scholar]
  4. Beck A, Schlagenhauf F, Wüstenberg T, Hein J, Kienast T, Kahnt T, Schmack K, Hägele C, Knutson B & Heinz A (2009). Ventral striatal activation during reward anticipation correlates with impulsivity in alcoholics. Biological psychiatry 66, 734–742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Becker A, Kirsch M, Gerchen MF, Kiefer F & Kirsch P (2016). Striatal activation and frontostriatal connectivity during non‐drug reward anticipation in alcohol dependence. Addiction Biology epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bush K, Kivlahan DR, McDonell MB, Fihn SD & Bradley KA (1998). The AUDIT alcohol consumption questions (AUDIT-C): an effective brief screening test for problem drinking. Archives of internal medicine 158, 1789–1795. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cahalan D, Cisin I & Crossley H (1969). American drinking practices Center of Alcohol Studies, Rutgers University: New Brunswick, N.J. [Google Scholar]
  8. Casey B & Jones RM (2010). Neurobiology of the adolescent brain and behavior: implications for substance use disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 49, 1189–1201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Casey BJ, Jones RM & Somerville LH (2011). Braking and accelerating of the adolescent brain. Journal of Research on Adolescence 21, 21–33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Chassin L, Flora DB & King KM (2004). Trajectories of alcohol and drug use and dependence from adolescence to adulthood: the effects of familial alcoholism and personality. Journal of abnormal psychology 113, 483–498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cohen MX, Young J, Baek J-M, Kessler C & Ranganath C (2005). Individual differences in extraversion and dopamine genetics predict neural reward responses. Cognitive Brain Research 25, 851–861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Compton WM, Conway KP, Stinson FS, Colliver JD & Grant BF (2005). Prevalence, correlates, and comorbidity of DSM-IV antisocial personality syndromes and alcohol and specific drug use disorders in the United States: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 66, 677–685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Conrod PJ & Nikolaou K (2016). Annual Research Review: On the developmental neuropsychology of substance use disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 57, 371–394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Costa PT & McCrae RR (1997). Stability and change in personality assessment: the revised NEO Personality Inventory in the year 2000. Journal of personality assessment 68, 86–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Deserno L, Beck A, Huys QJ, Lorenz RC, Buchert R, Buchholz HG, Plotkin M, Kumakara Y, Cumming P & Heinze HJ (2015). Chronic alcohol intake abolishes the relationship between dopamine synthesis capacity and learning signals in the ventral striatum. European Journal of Neuroscience 41, 477–486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dienes Z & Mclatchie N (2017). Four reasons to prefer Bayesian analyses over significance testing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Doremus-Fitzwater TL, Varlinskaya EI & Spear LP (2010). Motivational systems in adolescence: possible implications for age differences in substance abuse and other risk-taking behaviors. Brain and cognition 72, 114–123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Duncan TE & Duncan SC (1995). Modeling the processes of development via latent variable growth curve methodology. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 2, 187–213. [Google Scholar]
  19. Duncan TE, Duncan SC & Hops H (1994). The effects of family cohesiveness and peer encouragement on the development of adolescent alcohol use: a cohort-sequential approach to the analysis of longitudinal data. Journal of studies on alcohol 55, 588–599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Elliott DS, Huizinga D & Menard S (2012). Multiple problem youth: Delinquency, substance use, and mental health problems. Springer Science & Business Media. [Google Scholar]
  21. Ewing SWF, Sakhardande A & Blakemore S-J (2014). The effect of alcohol consumption on the adolescent brain: a systematic review of MRI and fMRI studies of alcohol-using youth. NeuroImage: Clinical 5, 420–437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. First MB, Gibbon M, Spitzer RL, Benjamin LS & Williams JB (1997). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis II personality disorders: SCID-II. American Psychiatric Pub. [Google Scholar]
  23. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M & Williams JB (1995). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders. New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute. [Google Scholar]
  24. Forbes EE, Rodriguez EE, Musselman S & Narendran R (2014). Prefrontal response and frontostriatal functional connectivity to monetary reward in abstinent alcohol-dependent young adults. PloS one 9, e94640. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Goddings A-L, Mills KL, Clasen LS, Giedd JN, Viner RM & Blakemore S-J (2014). The influence of puberty on subcortical brain development. Neuroimage 88, 242–251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gogtay N, Giedd JN, Lusk L, Hayashi KM, Greenstein D, Vaituzis AC, Nugent TF, Herman DH, Clasen LS & Toga AW (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National academy of Sciences of the United States of America 101, 8174–8179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Grant BF, Goldstein RB, Saha TD, Chou SP, Jung J, Zhang H, Pickering RP, Ruan WJ, Smith SM & Huang B (2015). Epidemiology of DSM-5 alcohol use disorder: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions III. JAMA psychiatry 72, 757–766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Grant BF, Stinson FS, Dawson DA, Chou SP, Dufour MC, Compton W, Pickering RP & Kaplan K (2004). Prevalence and co-occurrence of substance use disorders and independentmood and anxiety disorders: Results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and relatedconditions. Archives of general psychiatry 61, 807–816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Grimm KJ, Ram N & Hamagami F (2011). Nonlinear growth curves in developmental research. Child development 82, 1357–1371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Haber SN & Knutson B (2010). The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 4–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hasin DS, Goodwin RD, Stinson FS & Grant BF (2005). Epidemiology of major depressive disorder: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcoholism and Related Conditions. Archives of general psychiatry 62, 1097–1106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hasler BP, Dahl RE, Holm SM, Jakubcak JL, Ryan ND, Silk JS, Phillips ML & Forbes EE (2012). Weekend–weekday advances in sleep timing are associated with altered reward-related brain function in healthy adolescents. Biological psychology 91, 334–341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Heitzeg MM, Cope LM, Martz ME & Hardee JE (2015). Neuroimaging risk markers for substance abuse: recent findings on inhibitory control and reward system functioning. Current addiction reports 2, 91–103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Heitzeg MM, Nigg JT, Yau W-YW, Zucker RA & Zubieta J-K (2010). Striatal dysfunction marks preexisting risk and medial prefrontal dysfunction is related to problem drinking in children of alcoholics. Biological psychiatry 68, 287–295. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hu L. t. & Bentler PM (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural equation modeling: a multidisciplinary journal 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar]
  36. Hyde LW, Byrd AL, Votruba-Drzal E, Hariri AR & Manuck SB (2014). Amygdala reactivity and negative emotionality: Divergent correlates of antisocial personality and psychopathy traits in a community sample. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 123, 214–224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Jeffreys H (1998). The theory of probability. OUP Oxford. [Google Scholar]
  38. Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE, Patrick ME & Miech RA (2015). National Survey Results Monitoring The Future. In http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/citations?doi=10.1.1.719.6081.
  39. Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, Williamson D & Ryan N (1997). Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 36, 980–988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kriegeskorte N, Simmons WK, Bellgowan PS & Baker CI (2009). Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping. Nature neuroscience 12, 535–540. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kuntsche E, Knibbe R, Gmel G & Engels R (2006). Who drinks and why? A review of socio-demographic, personality, and contextual issues behind the drinking motives in young people. Addictive behaviors 31, 1844–1857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kushner MG, Abrams K & Borchardt C (2000). The relationship between anxiety disorders and alcohol use disorders: a review of major perspectives and findings. Clinical psychology review 20, 149–171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Mattson WI, Hyde LW, Shaw DS, Forbes EE & Monk CS (2016). Clinical neuroprediction: Amygdala reactivity predicts depressive symptoms 2 years later. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience 11, 892–898. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Miech R, Johnston L, O’Malley P, Bachman J & Schulenberg J (2015). Monitoring The Future National Survey Results On Drug Use, 1975–2014: Volume I, Secondary School Students, Institute for Social Research. Ann Arbor http://monitoringthefutureorg/pubshtml-monographs. [Google Scholar]
  45. Moss HB, Chen CM & Yi H. y. (2014). Early adolescent patterns of alcohol, cigarettes, and marijuana polysubstance use and young adult substance use outcomes in a nationally representative sample. Drug and alcohol dependence 136, 51–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Murray L, Shaw DS, Forbes EE & Hyde LW (2017). Reward-related neural correlates of antisocial behavior and callous-unemotional traits in young men. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging. In Press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Muthén LK & Muthén BO (2014). Mplus User’s Guide: Seventh Edition Muthén & Muthén: Los Angeles, CA. [Google Scholar]
  48. Nichols T, Brett M, Andersson J, Wager T & Poline J-B (2005). Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic. Neuroimage 25, 653–660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Nikolova YS, Knodt AR, Radtke SR & Hariri AR (2016). Divergent responses of the amygdala and ventral striatum predict stress-related problem drinking in young adults: possible differential markers of affective and impulsive pathways of risk for alcohol use disorder. Molecular psychiatry 21, 348–356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Nusslock R, Almeida JR, Forbes EE, Versace A, Frank E, Labarbara EJ, Klein CR & Phillips ML (2012). Waiting to win: elevated striatal and orbitofrontal cortical activity during reward anticipation in euthymic bipolar disorder adults. Bipolar Disord 14, 249–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Patton JH & Stanford MS (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of clinical psychology 51, 768–774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Rehm J (2011). The risks associated with alcohol use and alcoholism. Alcohol research & health: the journal of the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism 34, 135. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Robinson TE & Berridge KC (1993). The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain research reviews 18, 247–291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Schramm NL, Egli RE & Winder DG (2002). LTP in the mouse nucleus accumbens is developmentally regulated. Synapse 45, 213–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Seeman P (1987). Dopamine receptors and the dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia. Synapse 1, 133–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Shaw DS, Hyde LW & Brennan LM (2012). Early predictors of boys’ antisocial trajectories. Dev Psychopathol 24, 871–88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Silveri M & Spear L (2002). The effects of NMDA and GABAA pharmacological manipulations on ethanol sensitivity in immature and mature animals. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 26, 449–456. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Spear LP (2016). Alcohol Consumption in Adolescence: a Translational Perspective. Current Addiction Reports 3, 50–61. [Google Scholar]
  59. Spear LP & Swartzwelder HS (2014). Adolescent alcohol exposure and persistence of adolescent-typical phenotypes into adulthood: a mini-review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 45, 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Tapert SF, Schweinsburg AD, Barlett VC, Brown SA, Frank LR, Brown GG & Meloy MJ (2004). Blood oxygen level dependent response and spatial working memory in adolescents with alcohol use disorders. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 28, 1577–1586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Teicher MH, Andersen SL & Hostetter JC (1995). Evidence for dopamine receptor pruning between adolescence and adulthood in striatum but not nucleus accumbens. Developmental Brain Research 89, 167–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. van Emmerik-van Oortmerssen K, van de Glind G, van den Brink W, Smit F, Crunelle CL, Swets M & Schoevers RA (2012). Prevalence of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in substance use disorder patients: a meta-analysis and meta-regression analysis. Drug and alcohol dependence 122, 11–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Waller R, Corral-Frias N, Vannucci B, Bogdan R, Knodt AR, Hariri AR & Hyde LW (2016). An oxytocin receptor polymorphism predicts amygdala reactivity and antisocial behavior in men. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 11, 1218–1226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Wetherill RR, Squeglia LM, Yang TT & Tapert SF (2013). A longitudinal examination of adolescent response inhibition: neural differences before and after the initiation of heavy drinking. Psychopharmacology 230, 663–671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Wiers RW, Bartholow BD, van den Wildenberg E, Thush C, Engels RC, Sher KJ, Grenard J, Ames SL & Stacy AW (2007). Automatic and controlled processes and the development of addictive behaviors in adolescents: a review and a model. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 86, 263–283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Wrase J, Schlagenhauf F, Kienast T, Wüstenberg T, Bermpohl F, Kahnt T, Beck A, Ströhle A, Juckel G & Knutson B (2007). Dysfunction of reward processing correlates with alcohol craving in detoxified alcoholics. Neuroimage 35, 787–794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Xiao L, Bechara A, Gong Q, Huang X, Li X, Xue G, Wong S, Lu Z-L, Palmer P & Wei Y (2013). Abnormal affective decision making revealed in adolescent binge drinkers using a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 27, 443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Yau W-YW, Zubieta J-K, Weiland BJ, Samudra PG, Zucker RA & Heitzeg MM (2012). Nucleus accumbens response to incentive stimuli anticipation in children of alcoholics: relationships with precursive behavioral risk and lifetime alcohol use. Journal of Neuroscience 32, 2544–2551. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplemental Material

RESOURCES