Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2019 Aug 6;51(4):1142–1153. doi: 10.1002/jmri.26891

Does traditional asian vegetables (ulam) consumption correlate with brain activity using fMRI? A study among aging adults from low‐income households

Yee Xing You 1, Suzana Shahar 1,, Mazlyfarina Mohamad 2, Hanis Mastura Yahya 3, Hasnah Haron 3, Hamzaini Abdul Hamid 4
PMCID: PMC7079031  PMID: 31386268

Abstract

Background

Working memory and cognitive flexibility are supported by the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). Aging adults from low‐income households are individuals with a high risk of cognitive decline who incorporate ulam in their daily diet.

Purpose

To examine relationship between ulam consumption and the working memory and cognitive flexibility among aging adults from low‐income households who are more susceptible to cognitive decline.

Study Type

Cross‐sectional.

Population/Subjects

Thirty‐two aging adults (45–75 years old).

Field Strength/Sequence

Task‐based fMRI, 3.0T, T1‐weighted anatomical images, T2*‐weighted imaging data.

Assessment

The dietary and ulam consumption were assessed using the respective validated Dietary History and semiquantitative Food Frequency questionnaires. Working memory and cognitive flexibility were evaluated by using neuropsychological batteries (ie, mini‐mental state examination [MMSE], Digit Span, and Rey auditory verbal learning test [RAVLT]) and task‐based fMRI (N‐back and Stroop Color Word Test [SCWT]). Brodmann's areas 9 and 46 were the regions of interest (ROIs) of DLPFC activation.

Statistical Tests

Multiple linear regression used to understand the relationship between ulam consumption and the working memory and cognitive flexibility, while analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was used to compare the difference of working memory and cognitive flexibility among four percentiles of ulam consumption, after age, gender, and education years adjustments. Significance was decided by two‐sided, P < 0.0042 and P < 0.05.

Results

The multiple linear regression revealed that ulam consumption was positively associated with the Digit Span (R2 = 0.51, β = 0.702, P < 0.001), right DLPFC activation (1‐back) (R2 = 0.34, β = 0.591, P = 0.001), left DLPFC activation (SCWT‐1) (R2 = 0.33, β = 0.553, P = 0.002), and left DLPFC activation (SCWT‐2) (R2 = 0.34, β = 0.497, P = 0.004). The ulam consumption at the 75th and 100th percentile from the ANCOVA analysis had shown a better working memory and cognitive flexibility as compared with those of the 25th and 50th percentiles (P < 0.05).

Data Conclusion

This study found that high ulam consumption was related to a high intensity of brain activation in DLPFC; however, the elucidation of the neuroprotective properties of ulam have yet to be established from clinical trial studies.

Level of Evidence: 2

Technical Efficacy: Stage 4

J. Magn. Reson. Imaging 2020;51:1142–1153.

Keywords: brain activation, cognitive, DLPFC, fMRI, ulam


COGNITIVE DECLINE is an indication of mental health and can lead to irreversible dementia if left untreated.1 As a result of the aging process,2 the prevalence of people with dementia in developing countries, particularly in Asia, is estimated to increase from 60% in 2001 to 71% by 2040,3 with more prevalent poor mental health and cognition skills observed among the lower‐income aging population in Malaysia based on the National Health Mobility Survey 2015.4 Working memory and cognitive flexibility are the executive functions supported by the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) in human brain. Impaired synaptic neuroplasticity, which is caused by dorsolateral prefrontal cortex dysfunction, may lead to mild cognitive impairment.5 A recent study has successfully shown that higher vitamin B6 intake was closely related to better working memory using N‐back task functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in DLPFC among mild cognitive impairment older adults.6

Many Asian countries have conducted a series of studies on the prevention of neurodegenerative diseases through food‐based recommendation such as those of ulam, which is a type of traditional salad that is normally consumed in raw form by the Asian populations (Southeast Asia, Japan, Korea, and India) with rice or fermented sauces. 7, 8 This traditional dish was not only found to have contained a high level of antioxidants, the polyphenol in the ulam was also reported in a review as attenuating the oxidative stress that consequently prevents cognitive decline.8 Although the neuropsychological batteries (ie, Mini‐mental State Examination, Digit Span, General Health Questionnaire) had been used for measuring the respective benefits of Polygonum minus supplements9 as well as the Centella asiatica 10 and Oenanthe javanica 11 for improving the attention and memory among middle‐aged women and the cognitive impairment associated with the neurodegenerative disorders such as Alzheimer's disease and dementia; however, brain activation using fMRI is yet to be investigated.12 As such, the fMRI technique is therefore recommended to assess the working memory performance and cognitive flexibility based on brain activation that is located at the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the subjects.

Functional MRI had been used in several studies for determining the relationship between dietary nutrients or vegetables consumption and brain activation6, 13 such as the association of vegetable consumption and the brain activation that was located at the left superior frontal gyrus of 23 Korean young adults13 and the significant relationship between the consumption of vitamin B6 and brain activation of 15 community dwelling adults,6 there is still little research known on the benefits of ulam consumption among the population with a high risk of cognitive decline. For this reason, this study aimed to examine the association between ulam consumption and the working memory and cognitive flexibility of the aging adults from low‐income households through the use of fMRI.

Materials and Methods

Study Design and Sampling

A cross‐sectional study was conducted on 32 subjects (aged 45–75 years old) that were recruited from the low‐income residential areas of Klang Valley, Malaysia, and were screened earlier for eligibility by basing on the inclusion and exclusion criteria. In this study, the inclusion criteria consisted of adults between the age of 45–75 who are able to speak Malay or English languages, while the exclusion criteria were a history of mental health illness (ie, Alzheimer's disease, schizophrenia, history of stroke), physical disabilities, chronic kidney disease, undergoing dialysis, alcohol and drug users, being claustrophobic, and having internal metallic or electronic implants. The basic health profiles and medical history were obtained by referring to a personal medical book and self‐reported by the subjects. The sample size was calculated using the formula14:

n=+C2+3=1.96+0.8420.612+3=24

in which = confidence interval 95% = 1.96; = 80% power = 0.842; C = 0.5*ln[(1 + r)/(1‐r)] = 0.61; r = correlation coefficient13 = 0.55, and additional dropout 20%; thus, the total sample size was 32 subjects. The fMRI scan had been carried out at the selected hospital by a trained fMRI radiographer with ethical approval (NN‐2018‐097) granted from the Institutional Research and Ethics Committee.

Procedures

This study was conducted according to the guidelines as stated in the Declaration of Helsinki 1964. A detailed information sheet that consisted of the purpose of the study, procedures, instructions on completing a required task, and risks was then distributed and explained to the subjects before the attainment of an informed consent. Apart from using the surveys to gauge the sociodemographic background, the trained fieldworkers had also obtained the dietary intake information of the subjects such as the type, amount, cooking method, and the frequency of the common food consumption in the past 7 days through the use of a validated Dietary History Questionnaire (DHQ).15 The identification on the types, frequency, and quantity of ulam consumption by the subjects was validated using the vegetables and fruits Food Frequency Questionnaire (FFQ) of 58 vegetables, 24 ulam, and 26 fruits16 were then used as a checklist to complete the DHQ data, where the dietary intake would be analyzed with the Nutritionist ProTM (Axxya Systems, Stafford, TX) and compared against those of the Recommended Nutrient Intake 2017 (RNI) for Malaysians.17

This study utilized three validated neuropsychological batteries (ie, Mini‐mental State of Examination,18 Digit Span,19 and Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test20) in assessing the global cognitive function, working and episodic memory of the subjects, and the scaled Digit Span score was calculated based on the age‐specific tables of the manual in its analysis purpose.19

fMRI Protocol

N‐back Task

The two conditions of the N‐back task that were used in this study consisted of the 0‐back and 1‐back that were employed by a previous study,6 which had been created and displayed by using the Superlab 5 (Cedrus, San Pedro, CA). There are four blocks for each 0‐back and 1‐back tasks. The duration of each block is 30 seconds; there were 30 seconds rest between blocks and the total duration to complete the task was 510 seconds. Prior to conducting the fMRI scan, a trained fieldworker would demonstrate the completion of the N‐back task by first showing the diagram of the four corresponding 0‐back and 1‐back blocks of each condition (Fig. 1) to the subjects, where they are required to respond to a presented stimulus and to identify if the location of the target had been the same as the initial block (predefined stimulus) under the 0‐back condition and if the target location displayed had been the same as the one preceding it under the 1‐back condition. A trained fMRI radiographer then instigated a 3‐minute anatomical brain scan before proceeding with the 8.5‐minute duration of the N‐back tasks.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

0‐back and 1‐back paradigm.

Stroop Color Word Test (SCWT)

This task consisted of two paradigms with a respective four task blocks of 15 trials as suggested by a previous study.21 There are four blocks for each SCWT‐1 and SCWT‐2 tasks. The duration of each block is 30 seconds; there is 30 seconds rest between blocks and the total duration to complete the task is 510 seconds. In the first paradigm (SCWT‐1), the subjects were initially presented with a series of colored words (white, blue, yellow, green, red), which may be matched accordingly to the word (eg, the word "yellow" is displayed in yellow ink) or had appeared as a different color (eg, the word "red" is displayed in blue ink) and had to indicate the color in which the word is written, hence disregarding the matching of the word itself. In the second paradigm (SCWT‐2), a white colored word would be displayed under a colored word that is printed in different colors, where the meaning of the lower word would sometimes be congruent with the color that was indicated by the upper word (eg, the lower word is written as red, while the upper word "orange" is displayed in red ink) or is different from those of the upper word (eg, the lower word is written as blue, while the upper word "yellow" is displayed in green ink) (Fig. 2) and the subjects were required to indicate if the color of the upper word corresponded with the meaning of the lower word. This task was created and displayed by using Superlab 5 (Cedrus).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

SCWT paradigm.

fMRI Data Acquisition

Single‐shot spin‐echo echoplanar imaging (EPI) was acquired with the fMRI data and the fMRI images were performed on a 3.0T magnetic resonance (MR) scanner (Magnetom, Trio, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) with each of the subjects being subjected to high‐resolution T1‐weighted anatomical images (repetition time [TR] = 1900 msec, echo time [TE] = 2.35 msec, voxel dimensions 1.0 × 1.0 × 1.0 mm, 250 × 250 voxels, 176 slices, slice thickness = 1 mm), while those of the N‐back task and SCWT were conducted via the T2*‐weighted imaging data (TR = 3000 msec, TE = 30 msec, 3 mm isotropic voxels, flip angle = 90°, 27 slices, slice thickness = 4 mm).

Behavioral Performance Data

The percentage of accuracy and the mean response time (RT) on the N‐back and SCWT task of each subjects were then recorded in the calculation of the average data. Correct response (CR) is the percentage of correct responses from total response performed by each subject. A new index Response timeCorrect response was used to analyze the behavioral performance of data.

Preprocessing and Functional Imaging Data Analysis

A quality control test for the MRI machine was done routinely by the professional engineer to make sure that the quality of the functional images was good. The preprocessing and data analysis stage utilized the Statistical Parametric Mapping software implemented in MatLab 9.4.0 R2018a (MathWorks, Natick, MA). The first ten volumes were discarded because of equilibration effects. Functional images were first slice time‐corrected and subjected to a realignment of motion correction according to the mean image of the series. These functional images would then be coregistered to the subject's mean T1‐weighted image and estimated against a standardized Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) stereotaxic space, where the spatial normalization procedure would involve a six‐parameter affine transformation with a spatial transformation matrix. After undergoing the normalization process, all of the functional volumes were then subjected to spatial smoothing with a 6‐mm full‐width half‐maximum of isotropic Gaussian kernel as a way of increasing the signal‐to‐noise level through the removal of the high‐frequency information and the reduction of its intersubject variability.

Region of Interest (ROI) Analysis

The DLPFC that was selected had been defined by the WFU PickAtlas with Brodmann's areas 9 and 46, since prior studies had identified this ROI as being responsible for generating the working memory and cognitive flexibility of the human brain.6, 22, 23 After the individual subject analysis had been performed and corrected significantly (P < 0.05, familywise error [FWE]‐corrected), the primary outcome of the ROI brain activation would then be represented by the respective subject's percent of signal change.

Statistical Analysis

All of the statistical analyses were performed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS, Chicago, IL) v. 23.0 software with a significance level of P < 0.05. Apart from utilizing Shapiro–Wilk for testing the normality of the data (P > 0.05), the demographic data of all the respondents were also presented in the form of percentages or mean and standard deviation. Pearson correlation was used to analyze the relationship between behavioral performance index and brain activation. While the association between ulam consumption and working memory and cognitive flexibility (ie, neuropsychological test scores and DLPFC activation) had been established from the use of a simple linear regression, the adjusted age, gender, and years of formal education were then conducted via the multiple linear regression. β represents the correlation value of the analysis. To control for the inflated FWE rates that results from performing multiple tests on the same data, the significance of these multiple linear regression at a Bonferroni‐adjusted alpha level was performed. To make adjustment, the family‐wise alpha level (0.05) was divided by the total numbers of dependent variables using the formula:

Pcorrected=0.0512=0.0042

The difference in the neuropsychological test scores and DLPFC activation among the four percentiles of ulam consumption as controlled by the age, gender, and years of formal education would be further determined by a univariate analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). The magnitude of ulam was taken daily in order to have better working memory and cognitive flexibility that could be obtained from ANCOVA analysis.

Results

As shown in Table 1, the average of ulam consumption was found to be 31.7g/day (~0.5 servings/day), while the average of reaction accuracy and reaction time was 58.7% (N‐back), 70.6% (SCWT), and 2295 (N‐back) and 2495 (SCWT) msec.

Table 1.

Descriptive Analysis (Expressed in Mean and Standard Deviation)

Parameters Total subjects (n = 32) Normal rangea,b,c
Age (years)a 57.4 ± 6.7 68
Gender (men, %)a 34.4 48
Education (years)a 11.2 ± 2.2 8
Household income (USD/month)a 800.5 ± 642.1 336
Total ulam intake (g/day)b 31.7 ± 9.4 40
Total vegetables (non‐ulam) intake (g/day)b 168.1 ± 61.7 133
Total fruits intake (g/day)b 135.9 ± 58.9 179
Dietary nutrients c
Energy (kcal/day) 1847 ± 315 1550–1920
Protein (g/day) 64.4 ± 11.5 50–61
Carbohydrate (%/day) 56.4 ± 0.9 50–60
Fat (%/day) 29.7 ± 0.7 25–35
Vitamin A (μg/day) 701.3 ± 409.1 600
Vitamin C (mg/day) 87.2 ± 22.7 70
Thiamine (mg/day) 1.2 ± 0.2 1.1–1.2
Riboflavin (mg/day) 1.2 ± 0.3 1.1–1.3
Niacin (mg/day) 14.8 ± 2.7 14–16
Sodium (mg/day) 2073.5 ± 434.8 1500
Potassium (mg/day) 2463.3 ± 507.5 4700
Calcium (mg/day) 459.2 ± 188.7 1000–1200
Iron (mg/day) 19.3 ± 5.4 11–14
Phosphorus (mg/day) 832.3 ± 108.5 700
Neuropsychological batteries
MMSE 28.7 ± 2.3 23.8
Digit span (scaled score) 10.1 ± 1.7 7.7
RAVLT immediate recall 36.6 ± 9.2 37.8
RAVLT delayed recall 8.7 ± 2.6 7.7
fMRI behavioral performance
Reaction accuracy (N‐back) (%) 58.7 ± 14.9 N/A
Reaction time (N‐back) (msec) 2295 ± 429 N/A
Reaction accuracy (SCWT) (%) 70.6 ± 8.4 N/A
Reaction time (SCWT) (msec) 2492 ± 331 N/A
fMRI brain activation
0‐back total percent signal change (%) Left DLPFC 0.51 ± 0.23 N/A
0‐back total percent signal change (%) Right DLPFC 0.68 ± 0.19 N/A
1‐back total percent signal change (%) Left DLPFC 0.49 ± 0.22 N/A
1‐back total percent signal change (%) Right DLPFC 0.69 ± 0.20 N/A
SCWT‐1 total percent signal change (%) Left DLPFC 0.78 ± 0.40 N/A
SCWT‐1 total percent signal change (%) Right DLPFC 0.90 ± 0.43 N/A
SCWT‐2 total percent signal change (%) Left DLPFC 0.91 ± 0.44 N/A
SCWT‐2 total percent signal change (%) Right DLPFC 1.05 ± 0.46 N/A
a

Nationwide aging population research in Malaysia.38

b

Vegetables and fruits intake among population in Malaysia.39

c

Recommended Nutrient Intake for Malaysian population.17

DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; RAVLT, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging.

Table 2 shows the correlation between the behavioral performance index and brain activation. Significant negative correlations were observed between the behavioral performance index and right DLPFC activation and on 0‐back (r = –0.456, P = 0.009), 1‐back (r = –0.484, P = 0.005), and left DLPFC activation while subjects performed SCWT‐1 (r = –0.353, P < 0.05) and SCWT‐2 (r = –0.356, P < 0.05).

Table 2.

Correlation Between Behavioral Performance Index and DLPFC Activation

Brain activation Behavioral performance index (RT/CR)
Correlation (r) P‐value
0‐back total percent signal change (%) Left DLPFC –0.139 0.448
0‐back total percent signal change (%) Right DLPFC –0.456* 0.009
1‐back total percent signal change (%) Left DLPFC –0.141 0.441
1‐back total percent signal change (%) Right DLPFC –0.484* 0.005
SCWT‐1 total percent signal change (%) Left DLPFC –0.353* 0.047
SCWT‐1 total percent signal change (%) Right DLPFC –0.129 0.482
SCWT‐2 total percent signal change (%) Left DLPFC –0.356* 0.046
SCWT‐2 total percent signal change (%) Right DLPFC –0.062 0.735

Significant at P < 0.05.

CR: correct response; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; RT: response time; SCWT, Stroop Color Word Test.

Apart from demonstrating a correlation between ulam consumption and the Digit Span (R2 = 0.45, β = 0.672, P < 0.001), the univariate analysis also showed that ulam consumption was correlated with the right DLPFC activation (0‐back) (R2 = 0.28, β = 0.533, P = 0.002), right DLPFC activation (1‐back) (R2 = 0.31, β = 0.556, P = 0.001), left DLPFC activation (SCWT‐1) (R2 = 0.27, β = 0.523, P = 0.002) and the left of DLPFC activation (SCWT‐2) (R2 = 0.24, β = 0.490, P = 0.004) (Table 3). The use of multiple linear regression had also shown the relationship of ulam consumption as being specifically significant with Digit Span (R2 = 0.51, β = 0.702, P < 0.001), right DLPFC activation (1‐back) (R2 = 0.34, β = 0.591, P = 0.001), left DLPFC activation (SCWT‐1) (R2 = 0.33, β = 0.553, P = 0.002), and left DLPFC activation (SCWT‐2) (R2 = 0.34, β = 0.497, P = 0.004) after the age, gender, and years of formal education adjustments (Table 3). The relationship between ulam consumption and DLPFC activation while subjects performed N‐back and SCWT is shown in Figs. 3 and 4.

Table 3.

Association Between Ulam Consumption With Cognitive Function and Brain Activation

Variables Ulam consumption
Simple linear regression Multiple linear regression
Neuropsychological batteries and brain activation R2 Adjusted odd ratio (95% CI) P‐value R2 Adjusted odd ratio (95% CI) P‐value
MMSE 0.04 0.207 (–0.019–0.068) 0.257 0.10 0.233 (–0.018–0.073) 0.225
Digit span 0.45 0.672 (0.035–0.083)* <0.001 0.51 0.702 (0.038–0.099)* <0.001
RAVLT (total immediate recall) 0.20 0.446 (0.057–0.395) 0.010 0.32 0.439 (0.054–0.391) 0.012
RAVLT (delayed recall) 0.15 0.382 (0.004–0.099) 0.034 0.37 0.422 (0.013–0.101) 0.013
0‐back right DLPFC activation 0.28 0.533 (0.002–0.008)* 0.002 0.30 0.511 (0.002–0.008) 0.005
0‐back left DLPFC activation 0.07 0.266 (–0.001–0.008) 0.142 0.10 0.233 (–0.002–0.008) 0.226
1‐back right DLPFC activation 0.31 0.556 (0.003–0.009)* 0.001 0.34 0.591 (0.003–0.010)* 0.001
1‐back left DLPFC activation 0.08 0.280 (–0.001–0.007) 0.120 0.09 0.284 (–0.001–0.008) 0.144
SCWT‐1 right DLPFC activation 0.11 0.333 (0.000–0.015) 0.062 0.16 0.348 (–0.001–0.016) 0.066
SCWT‐1 left DLPFC activation 0.27 0.523 (0.004–0.018)* 0.002 0.33 0.553 (0.005–0.019)* 0.002
SCWT‐2 right DLPFC activation 0.08 0.287 (–0.002–0.015) 0.112 0.19 0.317 (–0.001–0.016) 0.085
SCWT‐2 left DLPFC activation 0.24 0.490 (0.004–0.019)* 0.005 0.34 0.497 (0.004–0.020)* 0.004
*

Significant at P < 0.0042.

MLR model was adjusted by age, gender and years of formal education.

CI, confidence interval; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; RAVLT, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; SCWT, Stroop Color Word Test.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Association between ulam intake and brain activation. (a) Activated brain region in DLPFC when performing the 0‐back task (P < 0.05, FWE‐corrected). (b) Activated brain region in DLPFC when performing the 1‐back task (P < 0.05, FWE‐corrected). (c,d) Scatterplots of positive association between ulam intake and DLPFC activation. FWE: familywise error; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Association between ulam intake and brain activation. (a) Activated brain region in DLPFC when performing SCWT‐1 (P < 0.05, FWE‐corrected). (b) Activated brain region in DLPFC when performing SCWT‐2 (P < 0.05, FWE‐corrected). (c,d) Scatterplots of positive association between ulam intake and DLPFC activation. FWE: familywise error; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; MFG, middle frontal gyrus.

A further analysis from the univariate ANCOVA is shown in Table 4 with the age, gender, and years of formal education adjustments, which also discovered the ulam consumption at the 100th percentile as having a significantly higher Digit Span score (P < 0.001, f = 1.06) and Rey auditory verbal learning test (RAVLT) (total immediate recall) (P < 0.05, f = 0.67) than those of the 25th percentile's, while the ulam consumption at the 100th and 75th percentile demonstrated a significantly higher right DLPFC activation for the 0‐back condition (P < 0.05, f = 0.61), 1‐back condition (P < 0.01, f = 0.85), SCWT‐1 condition (P < 0.05, f = 0.90), and the SCWT‐2 condition (P < 0.05, f = 0.65) as compared with the 25th and 50th percentiles.

Table 4.

Univariate Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) Between Ulam Consumption and Cognitive Parameters

Variables Ulam consumption P‐value Effect size Cohen's f
Cognitive parameters 25th percentile (<13g) (n = 8) 50th percentile (13‐37g) (n = 8) 75th percentile (38‐46g) (n = 8) 100th percentile (≥47g) (n = 8)
MMSE 27.25 ± 3.33 29.75 ± 0.46 29.88 ± 0.35 28.13 ± 2.42 0.332 0.70
Digit span 8.88 ± 0.64a 8.87 ± 0.99b 10.63 ± 1.85 11.63 ± 1.19ab <0.001 1.06
RAVLT (total immediate recall) 27.38 ± 6.28abc 40.50 ± 8.60a 41.75 ± 7.21b 39.13 ± 10.00c 0.024 0.67
RAVLT (delayed recall) 6.88 ± 1.73 8.88 ± 1.81 10.29 ± 3.04 9.00 ± 2.98 0.127 0.52
0‐back right DLPFC activation 0.58 ± 0.18a 0.60 ± 0.18b 0.69 ± 0.16 0.82 ± 0.15ab 0.048 0.61
0‐back left DLPFC activation 0.52 ± 0.25 0.39 ± 0.19 0.45 ± 0.20 0.67 ± 0.23 0.070 0.56
1‐back right DLPFC activation 0.59 ± 0.13ac 0.24 ± 0.22bd 0.82 ± 0.13ab 0.79 ± 0.16cd 0.003 0.85
1‐back left DLPFC activation 0.48 ± 0.15 0.40 ± 0.27 0.49 ± 0.14 0.59 ± 0.29 0.446 0.33
SCWT‐1 right DLPFC activation 0.84 ± 0.21 0.85 ± 0.50 0.93 ± 0.43 1.19 ± 0.39 0.131 0.50
SCWT‐1 left DLPFC activation 0.65 ± 0.18a 0.46 ± 0.22bc 1.09 ± 0.49ab 0.95 ± 0.36c 0.002 0.90
SCWT‐2 right DLPFC activation 0.83 ± 0.46 0.72 ± 0.49 1.02 ± 0.38 1.06 ± 0.44 0.229 0.43
SCWT‐2 left DLPFC activation 0.86 ± 0.14b 0.81 ± 0.45ac 1.21 ± 0.45a 1.30 ± 0.53bc 0.030 0.65

a, b, c, dSignificant at P < 0.05 using least significance difference (LSD) post‐hoc test.

The model was adjusted by age, gender, and years of formal education.

CI, confidence interval; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; RAVLT, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; SCWT, Stroop Color Word Test.

Meanwhile, the activated brain region in DLPFC (Brodmann's areas 9 and 46) when performing the N‐back task (P < 0.05, FWE‐corrected) as depicted in Table 5 and Figs. 3 and 4. The total voxels activated in DLPFC for N‐back and SCWT was 1081 and 773 voxels, respectively, with the highest activation observed in the middle frontal gyrus for all of the N‐back and SCWT conditions (P < 0.05, FWE‐corrected) and the other activated regions were observed in the superior frontal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, and precentral gyrus.

Table 5.

Activated Brain Regions When Performing SCWT (P < 0.05, FWE Corrected)

Anatomical region L/R Coordinates x,y,z Voxels activated Maximum T value
0‐back
Middle frontal gyrus L –40 2 32 99 8.61
Inferior frontal gyrus R 44 10 30 324 8.53
Superior frontal gyrus R 4 30 38 37 8.05
L –4 30 38 24 8.02
Middle frontal gyrus L –46 12 30 40 7.83
L –36 4 32 3 7.05
R 46 30 24 17 6.38
R 34 42 30 5 5.54
L –40 36 20 7 5.51
R 44 38 24 4 5.22
Precentral gyrus R 38 4 32 6 5.16
Middle frontal gyrus L –34 32 34 2 4.97
Inferior frontal gyrus R 54 26 14 2 4.93
1 back
Middle frontal gyrus L –40 2 32 130 9.50
R 48 20 28 258 9.07
R 46 30 22 28 6.65
R 38 4 32 6 6.54
L –36 4 32 3 6.48
R 46 32 34 30 6.12
Superior frontal gyrus R 10 26 36 20 6.07
L –4 30 36 14 6.06
Middle frontal gyrus L –42 38 24 3 5.29
L –52 22 28 11 5.22
L –44 16 28 4 5.03
L –32 42 30 1 4.81
R 34 42 30 1 4.79
R 32 36 30 1 4.73
Superior frontal gyrus R 4 34 34 1 4.64
SCWT‐1
Middle frontal gyrus R 44 10 32 148 9.73
L –40 2 32 51 9.29
L –42 32 18 45 7.82
L –36 4 32 3 7.61
L –46 10 32 24 6.59
L –46 20 26 8 5.98
R 48 40 22 1 5.88
R 38 4 32 1 5.07
R 46 32 24 1 4.89
Superior frontal gyrus R 6 34 38 4 4.88
SCWT‐2
Middle frontal gyrus L –46 4 36 164 9.61
R 50 10 36 176 9.12
L –42 3 18 68 8.37
L –36 4 32 3 8.07
R 42 34 36 18 7.11
Superior frontal gyrus R 8 28 36 36 7.10
Middle frontal gyrus R 38 4 30 6 6.91
R 44 36 20 11 6.02
R 48 32 24 2 5.21
L –44 20 34 2 5.02
L –44 32 34 1 4.90

L: left, R: right.

Discussion

Our findings demonstrated that high ulam consumption was related to greater right DLPFC activation while the subjects were performing the N‐back task. 0‐back and 1‐back tasks were designed to assess the cognitive functions like attention, working memory, and short and long‐term memory in middle‐age and older age. The N‐back task has face validity as a working memory task since it seems to require maintaining continuous updating and processing of information.24 These findings are also been supported by a previous study in which a significant improvement was reported in the executive functioning and working memory of 20 healthy Malaysian participants after 3 weeks of being supplemented with the Superulam capsules (a combination of ulam or traditional Asian vegetables extracts).25 Another systematic review on the common ulam, Centella asiatica, was also reported in Thailand, where the herb was not only discovered for improving working memory and alertness, but also in relieving anger as well.10

The high level of antioxidants and polyphenols in ulam 26 was also found to be a major contributor to the cognitive pathway, where the latter is known for attenuating apoptosis, removing reactive oxygen species, preventing oxidative stress that causes cognitive decline,8 as well as initiating the activation of sirtuins in the regulation of the human body cells that alleviates the occurrence of neurodegenerative illnesses.27 Also, the flavonoid from the plants would scavenge for free radicals and decrease the neuron apoptosis that impedes neuroinflammation.28 The association of the ulam metabolites with the improvement of working memory, however, is yet to be elucidated by future research.

This study had indicated that a high ulam consumption resulted in the better cognitive processing strategies, adaptation and flexibility,29 an activity that is closely related to DLPFC involvement.30 For this reason, although it had seemed that the flavonoids from the ulam had entered a specific brain area (ie, DLPFC) and led to the improved cognitive flexibility, further randomized control trials and pharmacological studies would still be required to validate this presumption. The effect size of the ANCOVA analysis from this study was also considerably large,31 where the subjects with higher percentiles of ulam consumption (at least ½ servings per day) had shown substantially higher Digit Span and RAVLT test scores and greater DLPFC activation than those with lower percentiles of ulam consumption.

Another main finding of this study was the highest DLPFC activation that was found in the middle frontal gyrus during the fMRI scan as well as the other activated regions that had appeared in the precentral, superior frontal, and inferior frontal gyrus. While the attention, working memory, and executive functioning are controlled by the prefrontal cortex that is located at the frontal part of the brain,32 the working memory, on the other hand, was reported to have been supported by the left superior frontal, right middle frontal gyrus, and the inferior frontal gyrus.33 The involvement of the middle frontal gyrus in the working memory tasks such as word‐reading and numerical operations has also been proven from previous studies.6, 33 Since our findings had shown the significance of right DLPFC activation from performing the N‐back tasks, we can therefore surmise that the subjects with higher ulam consumption as demonstrating a greater right‐hemispheric dominance during the visual‐spatial processing stage as compared with lower ulam consumption.34

As the involvement of the middle frontal gyrus while performing the SCWT had been proven from a previous study,23 our findings too had revealed that the subjects with higher ulam consumption as demonstrating better left DLPFC activation for both of the SCWT tasks, with the left hemisphere displaying more of the brain activity during the assigned word recognition task. The latest evidence therefore reported the left hemisphere of the human brain as being more affected by the interference than the right hemisphere when naming the color of a word that conflicted with the displayed word (eg, the word "yellow" printed in "orange"). Since the left DLPFC had been selectively activated during the high conflict of the SCWT trials,35 this had thus demonstrated a higher left DLPFC activation through the coordinated recruitment of brain systems and, consequently, a higher cognitive skill was observed among the subjects with higher ulam consumption.36 Although the right DLPFC activation had not been significantly associated with ulam consumption, its activation could have been due to the macro‐adjustments of the cognitive control, which minimized the subsequent attentional conflict during the frequent congruent and incongruent SCWT.23

The sample size of this study was also calculated and justified sufficiently, where the investigation on the association between ulam consumption and brain activation had been successfully conducted through the use of the fMRI; hence, corresponding to several previous studies that were conducted on the nutrients intake and brain activation with a small sample (n = 15–30).6, 13, 37 The neuroimaging approach that was complemented with the neuropsychological batteries for the evaluation of cognitive domains such as working memory and cognitive flexibility can thus be regarded as a strength of this investigation, since the variations in brain functions that arose from the initial signs of Alzheimer's disease could be examined by this sort of MRI.

The main limitation of this study was its inability for elucidating the cause–effect relationship between ulam consumption and brain activation from a cross‐sectional study design, randomized control trials through the use of fMRI, and the metabolomic approach are therefore recommended to further determine the efficacy of ulam for improving brain activation as well as to gauge a better understanding of its neuroprotective properties. In addition, we analyzed the brain activation using ROI analysis (ie, DLPFC); however, whole‐brain analysis is recommended to investigate the ulam consumption related to other activated brain regions in future studies.

In conclusion, this study found that high ulam consumption was related to a high intensity of brain activation in DLPFC using the fMRI technique. However, there is a need to adopt whole‐brain analysis and randomized controlled trials to investigate the role of ulam as a neuroprotective agent affirmatively.

Acknowledgments

We thank the subjects and local authorities for their cooperation throughout the data collection. Gratitude is expressed towards the contribution, support, and assistance of all coauthors in reviewing this article.

Contract grant sponsor: Dana Padanan Kolaborasi Contract grant number: DPK‐2017‐004; Contract grant sponsor: National University of Malaysia.

References

  • 1. Hugo J, Ganguli M. Dementia and cognitive impairment: Epidemiology, diagnosis, and treatment. Clin Geriatr Med 2014;30:421–442. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Deary IJ, Gow AJ, Penke L, et al. Age‐associated cognitive decline. Br Med Bull 2009;92:135–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Alzheimer's Disease International. World Alzheimer Report 2015: The Global Impact of Dementia. London; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Institute for Public Health (IPH). National Health and Morbidity Survey 2015. (NHMS 2015) Volume II: Non‐communicable diseases, risk factors and other health problems. Ministry of Health Malaysia; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol 2013;64:135–168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Lau H, Shahar S, Mohamad M, et al. Relationships between dietary nutrients intake and lipid levels with functional MRI dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activation. Clin Intervent Aging 2018;14:43–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Seyedreihani SF, Tan T‐C, Alkarkhi AFM, Easa AM. Total phenolic content and antioxidant activity of Ulam raja (Cosmos caudatus) and quantification of its selected marker compounds: Effect of extraction. Int J Food Propert 2017;20:260–270. [Google Scholar]
  • 8. You Y, Shahar S, Haron H, Yahya H. More ulam for your brain: A review on the potential role of ulam in protecting against cognitive decline. Sains Malaysiana 2018;47:2713–2729. [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Yahya H, Shahar S, Nur Arina Ismail S, Aziz A, Normah CD, Hakim BN. Mood, cognitive function and quality of life improvements in middle aged women following supplementation with Polygonum minus extract. Sains Malaysiana 2017;46:245–254. [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Puttarak P, Dilokthornsakul P, Saokaew S, et al. Effects of Centella asiatica (L.) Urb. on cognitive function and mood related outcomes: A systematic review and meta‐analysis. Sci Rep 2017;7:10646. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Young Won B, Young Shin K, Jee Ha H, et al. Effect of dropwort (Oenanthe javanica) extracts on memory improvement in Alzheimer's disease animal model, Tg2576 mice. Korean J Food Sci Technol 2015;47:779–784. [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Beglinger LJ, Gaydos B, Tangphao‐Daniels O, et al. Practice effects and the use of alternate forms in serial neuropsychological testing. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2005;20:517–529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. He Q, Xiao L, Xue G, et al. Altered dynamics between neural systems sub‐serving decisions for unhealthy food. Front Neurosci 2014;8:350. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Hulley SB, Cummings SR, Browner WS, Grady DG, Newman TB. Designing clinical research, 4th ed. San Francisco: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Shahar S, Earland J, Abdulrahman S. Validation of a Dietary History Questionnaire against a 7‐D weighted record for estimating nutrient intake among rural elderly Malays. Malay J Nutr 2000;6:33–44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Shahar S, Teng K‐T, Rajab F, Arshad F. Association between oxidative DNA damage, fruits and vegetables intake with breast cancer: A preliminary study in Klang valley. J Health Sci Malay 2008;6:61–77. [Google Scholar]
  • 17. National Coordinating Committee on Food and Nutrition . Malaysia Recommended Nutrient Intake. 2017; http://nutrition.moh.gov.my.
  • 18. Ibrahim NM, Shohaimi S, Chong HT, et al. Validation study of the Mini‐Mental State Examination in a Malay‐speaking elderly population in Malaysia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2009;27:247–253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Weshsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale‐III. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Jamaluddin R, Othman Z, Musa KI, Alwi MNM. Validation of the Malay Version of Auditory Verbal Learning Test (Mvavlt) among schizophrenia patients in Hospital Universiti Sains Malaysia (Husm), Malaysia. ASEAN J Psychiatry 2009;10. [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Zysset S, Müller K, Lohmann G, von Cramon DY. Color‐word matching Stroop task: Separating interference and response conflict. NeuroImage 2001;13:29–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Townsend J, Bookheimer SY, Foland‐Ross LC, Sugar CA, Altshuler LL. fMRI abnormalities in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during a working memory task in manic, euthymic and depressed bipolar subjects. Psychiatry Res 2010;182:22–29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Vanderhasselt M‐A, De Raedt R, Baeken C. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and Stroop performance: Tackling the lateralization. Psychon Bull Rev 2009;16:609–612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Gajewski PD, Hanisch E, Falkenstein M, Thönes S, Wascher E. What does the n‐back task measure as we get older? Relations between working‐memory measures and other cognitive functions across the lifespan. Front Psychol 2018;9:2208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Udani JK. Effects of superulam on supporting concentration and mood: A randomized, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled crossover study. Evid Based Complement Alternat Med 2013;2013:238454–238454. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. S Reihani SF, Easa A. Antioxidant activity and total phenolic content in aqueous extracts of selected traditional Malay salads (Ulam). Int Food Res J 2012;19:1439–1444. [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Rahnasto‐Rilla M, Tyni J, Huovinen M, et al. Natural polyphenols as sirtuin 6 modulators. Sci Rep 2018;8:4163–4163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Zhang S, Lu B, Han X, et al. Protection of the flavonoid fraction from Rosa laevigata Michx fruit against carbon tetrachloride‐induced acute liver injury in mice. Food Chem Toxicol 2013;55:60–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Li Y, Grabell AS, Wakschlag LS, Huppert TJ, Perlman SB. The neural substrates of cognitive flexibility are related to individual differences in preschool irritability: A fNIRS investigation. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2017;25:138–144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Yun J‐Y, Lee DY, Seo EH, et al. Neural correlates of Stroop performance in Alzheimer's disease: A FDG‐PET Study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord Extra 2011;1:190–201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, vol. 2 Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Lara AH, Wallis JD. The role of prefrontal cortex in working memory: A mini review. Front Syst Neurosci 2015;9:173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Koyama MS, O'Connor D, Shehzad Z, Milham MP. Differential contributions of the middle frontal gyrus functional connectivity to literacy and numeracy. Sci Rep. 2017;7:17548. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Pisella L, Alahyane N, Blangero A, Thery F, Blanc S, Pelisson D. Right‐hemispheric dominance for visual remapping in humans. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 2011;366:572–585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Gier VS, Kreiner DS, Solso RL, Cox SL. The hemispheric lateralization for processing geometric word/shape combinations: The Stroop‐shape effect. J Gen Psychol 2010;137:1–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Husain S, Kuzminski S, Shad MU, et al. Cognition and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex volume in corticosteroid‐treated patients given lamotrigine. Stress Health 2011;27:18–22. [Google Scholar]
  • 37. van der Laan LN, Charbonnier L, Viergever MA, van Meer F, Smeets PAM, Adan RAH. Developmental differences in the brain response to unhealthy food cues: An fMRI study of children and adults. Am J Clin Nutr 2016;104:1515–1522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Shahar S, Omar A, Vanoh D, et al. Approaches in methodology for population‐based longitudinal study on neuroprotective model for healthy longevity (TUA) among Malaysian older adults. Aging Clin Exp Res 2016;28:1089–1104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Nurul Izzah A, Abdullah A, Pauzi AM, Lee YH, Wan Mahiyuddin WR, Fatimah DS. Patterns of fruits and vegetable consumption among adults of different ethnics in Selangor, Malaysia. Int Food Res J 2012;19:1095–1107. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES