Skip to main content
Brain logoLink to Brain
. 2020 Mar 17;143(3):1010–1026. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa033

Neuroinflammation and protein aggregation co-localize across the frontotemporal dementia spectrum

W Richard Bevan-Jones a1,#, Thomas E Cope a2,a3,✉,#, P Simon Jones a2, Sanne S Kaalund a2, Luca Passamonti a2,a4, Kieren Allinson a5, Oliver Green a4, Young T Hong a6, Tim D Fryer a6, Robert Arnold a1, Jonathan P Coles a7, Franklin I Aigbirhio a6, Andrew J Larner a8, Karalyn Patterson a2,a3, John T O’Brien a1,#, James B Rowe a2,a3,#
PMCID: PMC7089669  PMID: 32179883

Bevan-Jones, Cope et al. report that neuroinflammation co-localizes with protein aggregation in all major types of frontotemporal dementia, both in vivo with positron emission tomography, and at post mortem. In vivo neuroinflammation patterns are disease-specific and can accurately classify patients into groups.

Keywords: microglia, tau imaging, semantic dementia, primary progressive aphasia, neuropathology

Abstract

The clinical syndromes of frontotemporal dementia are clinically and neuropathologically heterogeneous, but processes such as neuroinflammation may be common across the disease spectrum. We investigated how neuroinflammation relates to the localization of tau and TDP-43 pathology, and to the heterogeneity of clinical disease. We used PET in vivo with (i) 11C-PK-11195, a marker of activated microglia and a proxy index of neuroinflammation; and (ii) 18F-AV-1451, a radioligand with increased binding to pathologically affected regions in tauopathies and TDP-43-related disease, and which is used as a surrogate marker of non-amyloid-β protein aggregation. We assessed 31 patients with frontotemporal dementia (10 with behavioural variant, 11 with the semantic variant and 10 with the non-fluent variant), 28 of whom underwent both 18F-AV-1451 and 11C-PK-11195 PET, and matched control subjects (14 for 18F-AV-1451 and 15 for 11C-PK-11195). We used a univariate region of interest analysis, a paired correlation analysis of the regional relationship between binding distributions of the two ligands, a principal component analysis of the spatial distributions of binding, and a multivariate analysis of the distribution of binding that explicitly controls for individual differences in ligand affinity for TDP-43 and different tau isoforms. We found significant group-wise differences in 11C-PK-11195 binding between each patient group and controls in frontotemporal regions, in both a regions-of-interest analysis and in the comparison of principal spatial components of binding. 18F-AV-1451 binding was increased in semantic variant primary progressive aphasia compared to controls in the temporal regions, and both semantic variant primary progressive aphasia and behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia differed from controls in the expression of principal spatial components of binding, across temporal and frontotemporal cortex, respectively. There was a strong positive correlation between 11C-PK-11195 and 18F-AV-1451 uptake in all disease groups, across widespread cortical regions. We confirmed this association with post-mortem quantification in 12 brains, demonstrating strong associations between the regional densities of microglia and neuropathology in FTLD-TDP (A), FTLD-TDP (C), and FTLD-Pick's. This was driven by amoeboid (activated) microglia, with no change in the density of ramified (sessile) microglia. The multivariate distribution of 11C-PK-11195 binding related better to clinical heterogeneity than did 18F-AV-1451: distinct spatial modes of neuroinflammation were associated with different frontotemporal dementia syndromes and supported accurate classification of participants. These in vivo findings indicate a close association between neuroinflammation and protein aggregation in frontotemporal dementia. The inflammatory component may be important in shaping the clinical and neuropathological patterns of the diverse clinical syndromes of frontotemporal dementia.

Introduction

Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) encompasses a clinically and pathologically heterogeneous group of neurodegenerative conditions, including the behavioural variant (bvFTD) (Rascovsky et al., 2011), non-fluent variant primary progressive aphasia (nfvPPA) and semantic variant primary progressive aphasia (svPPA) (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2011). In recent years, attention has focused on understanding the pathogenic role of protein misfolding and aggregation, which is a cardinal feature of the post-mortem diagnostic criteria for frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD) (Mackenzie et al., 2010). However, there are several different pathological proteins and aggregation morphologies in FTD, with generally weak correlations between clinical syndrome and the type of pathological protein (Seelaar et al., 2011) [with the exception of svPPA, which is strongly associated with TAR DNA-binding protein 43 (TDP-43) type C neuropathology] (Spinelli et al., 2017). However, other neuropathological processes may be present in common across these diverse clinical syndromes and present potential therapeutic targets. In particular, there is converging evidence for the role of neuroinflammation in neurodegenerative dementias, including FTD, from genetic associations (Guerreiro et al., 2013; Rayaprolu et al., 2013; Broce et al., 2018), CSF (Sjogren et al., 2004; Woollacott et al., 2018), epidemiology (Miller et al., 2013, 2016), post-mortem tissue (Venneti et al., 2008; Lant et al., 2014) and animal models (Yoshiyama et al., 2007; Bhaskar et al., 2010; Yin et al., 2010). Both the intensity of neuroinflammation and its distribution across the brain may be relevant determinants of the clinical syndrome. Here we aim to build on recent in vivo studies of Alzheimer’s disease, which demonstrate that neuroinflammation correlates spatially with tau aggregation (Dani et al., 2018), by assessing whether this association extends to FTD, which is associated with many different conformations of pathological tau, or other protein aggregates such as TDP-43.

PET allows the topographic quantification of specific molecules using radioligands. In this study, we measured neuroinflammation and protein aggregation in vivo in patients with bvFTD, svPPA and nfvPPA, to answer key questions regarding the relationship of these pathophysiological processes. 11C-PK-11195, which binds to the translocator protein (TSPO) that is expressed on the outer mitochondrial membrane of activated microglia, is a robust and sensitive marker of microglial activation with an established role as a proxy for neuroinflammation in neurodegenerative diseases (Stefaniak and O’Brien, 2016). 18F-AV-1451 was originally developed to bind to paired helical filament tau in Alzheimer’s disease (Zhang et al., 2012; Chien et al., 2013; Xia et al., 2013), and has been extensively used in Alzheimer’s and non-Alzheimer’s diseases. Elevated in vivo binding is seen in tauopathies characterized by straight filaments (Bevan-Jones et al., 2016; Passamonti et al., 2018; Smith et al., 2017; Jones et al., 2018), albeit with generally lower binding affinity than in Alzheimer’s disease, and also in TDP-43-related disease (Bevan-Jones et al., 2018a, 2018b; Makaretz et al., 2018). It also has low affinity for amyloid-β and α-synuclein (Xia et al., 2013). Therefore, although the molecular interpretation of increased binding is incompletely understood (Marquié et al., 2015; Sander et al., 2016), this elevated in vivo binding suggests that 18F-AV-1451 represents a proxy index of aggregated non-amyloid-β pathological proteins across the FTD spectrum.

Given the evidence for differences in affinity of 18F-AV-1451 for different tau and TDP-43 conformational targets, our analysis strategy concentrates on the relative topographical distribution of binding across regions within each individual, rather than the simple magnitude of binding. In this way, we explicitly control for difference in binding affinity between syndromes and protein strains within each syndrome.

We test the hypotheses that, in FTD, neuroinflammation and protein aggregation are both increased in frontotemporal regions compared to controls, and that neuroinflammation and protein aggregation regionally co-localize in each FTD syndrome, consistent with the syndrome-specific neuropathological distributions (e.g. co-localization of neuroinflammation and protein aggregation in the temporal pole of patients with svPPA). We use data-driven approaches to PET imaging to elucidate the spatial modes of neuroinflammation associated with FTD, and machine learning based on multi-dimensional scaling of distributional dissimilarities, to investigate whether the cortical distribution of neuroinflammation and protein aggregation can accurately discriminate diagnostic groups thereby illustrating their mechanistic importance. The association between protein aggregation (tau or TDP-43) and inflammation (microglia) in the imaging data is supported by immunohistochemistry of post-mortem tissue from patients with FTD associated with FTLD-TDP types A and C, and FTLD-Pick’s disease.

Materials and methods

As part of the NIMROD study (Bevan-Jones et al., 2017), 31 patients (10 with bvFTD, 11 with svPPA and 10 with nfvPPA) underwent PET scanning with 18F-AV1451. Twenty-eight of the 31 (nine with bvFTD, nine with svPPA and 10 with nfvPPA) also underwent a PET scan with 11C-PK-11195. The order of scans was randomized. Fourteen healthy control participants underwent 18F-AV-1451 PET and, to minimize radiation exposure in healthy individuals, a different group of 15 healthy participants underwent 11C-PK-11195 PET scanning. Genetic and amyloid status (by PET or CSF biomarkers) for patients were tested if clinically indicated.

PET with 18F-AV-1451 and 11C-PK-11195 was performed on a GE Discovery 690 PET/CT (GE Healthcare) with a low dose CT for attenuation correction or on a GE Advance PET scanner (GE Healthcare) with a 15-min 68Ge/68Ga transmission scan for attenuation correction. The PET scan itself used dynamic imaging for 90 (18F-AV-1451) and 75 (11C-PK-11195) min, respectively. All radioligands were prepared at the Wolfson Brain Imaging Centre (WBIC), University of Cambridge, with high radiochemical purity (>95%). Each subject underwent contemporaneous 3T MRI using a Siemens Magnetom Skyra, Verio or Tim Trio (www.medical.siemens.com). A high-resolution T1-weighted sequence was acquired (176 slices of 1.0 mm thickness, echo time = 2.98 ms, repetition time = 2300 ms, flip angle = 9°, acquisition matrix 256 × 240; voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm3) and used for tissue segmentation (grey and white matter along with CSF), and for non-rigid registration of standard space regions of interest. For both ligands, non-displaceable binding potential (BPND) was calculated in 83 regions of interest, defined by a Hammers atlas modified to include the midbrain and the dentate nucleus of the cerebellum, by kinetic modelling using a simplified reference tissue model, with cerebellar grey matter as reference region for 18F-AV-1451 (Passamonti et al., 2018) and supervised cluster analysis used to define the 11C-PK-11195 reference region (Yaqub et al., 2012). Prior to kinetic modelling, all region of interest data were corrected for CSF contamination of the region (i.e. partial volume corrected) through division by the mean region grey plus white matter fraction, determined using tissue probability maps smoothed to PET spatial resolution.

Four data analysis approaches were used, each designed to answer a different focused question and to explicitly control for expected between-subject and between-region differences in ligand affinity.

As a first-stage data exploration of between-group differences, a repeated-measures ANOVA was performed across the 83 regions, including age as a covariate and Greenhouse-Geisser penalization of degrees of freedom to correct for non-sphericity. Post hoc t-tests were then performed between each group, corrected for false discovery rate (FDR) over regions.

Second, to examine the relationship between neuroinflammation and protein aggregation in each disease group, a correlation between the regional BPND of each ligand was performed. PET scanning with any ligand characteristically results in a general pattern of lower BPND in brain regions such as temporal lobe and higher BPND in deep brain nuclei. We were concerned that such non-specific effects might drive apparent correlations, and weak correlations were observed between our cohorts of controls for each ligand (Supplementary Fig. 1). To control for this, we examined the between-ligand correlation within each disease group both with and without subtraction of the control mean BPND for each of the 83 regions of interest. We then assessed whether the association between the regional BPND of the two ligands was independent of atrophy. To do this, we first calculated a combined grey and white matter volume t-score for each brain region and disease group, representing the degree of atrophy in that region compared to controls (we observed that this combined grey and white matter t-score was more strongly related to ligand binding than either tissue class alone, and therefore report this as the most conservative correction and informative association). We then calculated partial correlations between the two ligands, with the effect of atrophy partialled out, as well as between each ligand and atrophy individually, with the effect of the other ligand partialled out.

Third, to elucidate the topographical patterns of inflammation and protein aggregation in FTD, we entered the BPND of each ligand in each of the 83 regions of interest into a principal component analysis. Components were retained by Cattell’s criterion (i.e. to the elbow of the scree plot) and then tested for group differences across diagnosis in a repeated measures ANOVA. Post hoc t-tests examined group differences in the expression of each topographical pattern. These first three analyses were performed in SPSS Statistics version 25 (IBM).

Finally, we undertook an analysis of the relative distribution of ligand binding potential for each ligand for every individual. This used previously published non-parametric methods (Bevan-Jones et al., 2016), that were explicitly designed to control for between-subject differences in the scaling of each ligand, such as might result from differences in the affinity of 18F-AV-1451 for different conformations of tau or TDP-43, as well as spatial dependence between adjacent regions in PET data due to signal spread. These methods can be conceptualized as analogous to multi-voxel pattern analysis techniques for functional MRI (Kriegeskorte et al., 2008), but rather than attempting to classify observed stimuli within an individual on the basis of their representational similarity, here we are attempting to classify individuals on the basis of the similarity of relative ligand BPND distributions within their brains, blinded to overall differences in binding affinity. To do this, for each ligand and every individual separately, the parcellated data were converted to 83-element linear vectors. For each ligand separately, the resultant vectors were non-parametrically correlated (Spearman’s rho) pairwise between individuals, resulting in two matrices that represented the similarity of each individual’s scan to each other individual for that ligand. The inverse of these matrices (i.e. the between-individual dissimilarities) were used to calculate a 2D scaling for each disease subgroup pair, using the squared metric stress distance criterion of the ‘mdscale’ function in MATLAB R2017b (Mathworks). The resulting locations in 2D space formed the inputs to a 10-fold cross-validated linear support vector machine (CV-SVM) for between-group classification based on each ligand separately. Statistical significance of the classification was assessed by comparison of the loss function of the CV-SVM against a null distribution of loss functions created by 1000 repetitions of the same procedure for identical data but shuffled group assignment labels. For those individuals who underwent scanning with both ligands, the CV-SVM process was repeated on multi-modal, 4D scaling. To confirm that our machine learning results were not significantly influenced by age or sex, we attempted supplementary classifications based on these factors.

Additionally, we performed quantitative immunohistochemistry on 12 post-mortem cases from the Cambridge Brain Bank to augment the imaging results. From our database of specimens, we selected three cases of FTLD-Pick's (a tauopathy), three cases of FTLD-TDP type C and three cases of FTLD-TDP type A, and compared these to three cases of Braak stage V Alzheimer’s disease. For each case, fixed brain tissue was sampled from the prefrontal cortex [Brodmann area (BA) 44], middle temporal cortex (BA22/21), parietal cortex (BA7), and occipital cortex (BA17/18). These were embedded in paraffin, and sectioned at 10 µm. For each region, two neighbouring sections were stained by immunohistochemistry using antibodies directed against CD68 (clone PG-M1, Dako), a marker of microglia and macrophages, and against the relevant pathological protein: hyperphosphorylated tau (AT8, MN1020, Thermo Scientific) or TDP43 (TIP-PTD-P02, Cosmo Bio Co Ltd.).

The number of protein aggregates, microglia, and cell nuclei were counted in series of fields of view placed uniformly randomly onto each section: a virtual grid with uniform distances between lines in the x and y direction, 1875 µm × 1875 µm, was superimposed onto each section. The position of fields of view was at the intersections of the grid lines, but only where lines crossed overlapping grey matter. The fields of view were 125 µm × 125 µm and counting was done under a 40× objective lens.

A microglial cell was counted when CD68 reactivity was visible over or around a cell nucleus. Based on the morphology, cells positive for the CD68 staining were divided into ramified microglia, amoeboid microglia and macrophages. In FTLD-Pick's, Pick bodies and glial tau inclusions were counted. In FTLD-TDP inclusions type A and C, dystrophic neurites, and lentiform intranuclear inclusions and neuronal cytoplasmic inclusions, respectively were counted. In Alzheimer’s disease, neurofibrillary tangles and neuritic plaques were counted. The densities of pathology, microglia and cell nuclei were calculated by dividing the total counts by the area in which they were counted and expressed per square millimetre. Analyses of the relationship between protein pathology and microglia were controlled for atrophy by partialling out the density of cell nuclei in correlations, and accounting for this as a co-variate of no interest in generalized linear models.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, upon reasonable request.

Results

Summary demographics are outlined in Table 1, and neuropsychological test scores, motor features, genetic and CSF status for each participant are provided in Table 2. Within the bvFTD group, two patients were positive for pathogenic mutations in the microtubule associated protein tau (MAPT) and three for expansions in C9 open reading frame 72 (C9ORF72). One of the nfvPPA group had a mutation in progranulin (GRN). CSF or PET amyloid status was assessed in six participants (four with svPPA, and two with nfvPPA), all of whom were negative.

Table 1.

Summary demographics and neuropsychometry

Group n M:F * Mean age #,$ Education, years*,#,$ ACE-R /100*,#,$, MMSE/30 ,#,$ FAB/18 FTDRS_Logit FTDRS_Percent/100
nfvPPA 10 3:7 71 12 79 27 11 1.92 71.7
svPPA 11 9:2 68 14 63 25 12 0.74 52.8
bvFTD 10 5:5 60 13 57 22 8 −2.46 17.4
Tau controls 14 7:7 67 16 95 29
PK controls 15 7:8 69 14 92 29

Pairwise comparisons are by t-test for each demographic except sex comparison by χ2.

*

P < 0.05 significant pairwise comparison nfvPPA versus combined control group.

#

F-test significant P < 0.05 across all groups.

$

P < 0.05 significant pairwise comparison bvFTD versus combined control group.

P < 0.05 significant pairwise comparison svPPA versus combined control group,

F = female; FAB = Frontal Assessment Battery; FTDRS = FTD Rating Scale; M = male; MMSE = Mini-Mental Status Examination.

Table 2.

Demographics, neuropsychological testing, genetic/amyloid status and motor phenotype for each disease participant

Case Diagnosis Gene/amyloid status Sex Entry age Education, years ACE-R /100 MMSE/30 FAB/18 FTDRS Logit score Motor features
1 nfvPPA Aβ−ve (CSF) M 55 14 93 29 15 3.35
2 nfvPPA F 67 16 88 28 13 2.19
3 nfvPPA Aβ−ve (CSF) F 62 11 90 27 15 1.92 +
4 nfvPPA F 84 11 85 30 11 5.39 +
5 nfvPPA F 81 10 78 28 15 0.16
6 nfvPPA F 74 10 40 16 7 −0.8
7 nfvPPA GRN M 66 10 76 22 9 −0.2
8 nfvPPA F 77 11 86 30 13 0.34 +
9 nfvPPA M 74 11 87 30 10 1.47 +
10 nfvPPA F 70 11 71 25 6 5.39
11 svPPA M 77 16 45 22 11 −1.27
12 svPPA M 69 16 77 28 11 −1.54
13 svPPA Aβ−ve (CSF) M 61 15 79 30 16 −0.4
14 svPPA Aβ−ve (PiB) F 65 18 72 27 16
15 svPPA Aβ−ve (CSF) M 67 17 71 27 17 2.49
16 svPPA Aβ−ve (CSF) M 65 13 68 27 13 5.39
17 svPPA M 72 13 63 25 12 1.26
18 svPPA F 63 10 59 26 14 0.7
19 svPPA M 69 18 85 30 14 2.19
20 svPPA M 63 10 61 27 8 −0.8
21 svPPA M 72 9 9 3 0 −0.59
22 bvFTD F 63 12 79 29 11 −3.09
23 bvFTD M 61 11 47 15 5 −2.18
24 bvFTD MAPT F 50 16 43 21 9 −3.8
25 bvFTD M 75 16 68 21 6 −0.4 +
26 bvFTD MAPT F 70 16 38 14 7 −3.09
27 bvFTD F 67 11 71 28 8 −0.8
28 bvFTD M 51 14 81 29 11 −2.58
29 bvFTD C9orf72 M 56 10 53 25 6 −1.03 +
30 bvFTD C9orf72 F 51 10 41 16 7 −3.8
31 bvFTD C9orf72 M 58 9 46 17 5 −3.8

Aβ−ve = negative tests for amyloid-β by CSF biomarkers or PiB PET scan. ACE-R = Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination Revised; FAB = Frontal Assessment Battery; FTDRS = FTD Rating Scale; MMSE = Mini-Mental Status Examination; PiB = Pittsburgh compound B.

Group comparisons of frontotemporal dementia with controls

The repeated-measures ANOVA of regional 11C-PK-11195 binding across the FTD groups and controls, controlled for age and sex, demonstrated a significant interaction between region and diagnosis [F(37.4,423.5) = 3.54, P <0.001]. T-maps from the post hoc pairwise comparisons between the control group and each disease group are shown in Fig. 1. After correction for FDR, regions with significantly elevated binding were (i) in the bvFTD group: bilateral superior frontal gyri and putamen, right nucleus accumbens, left posterior orbital gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus and middle frontal gyrus; and (ii) in the svPPA group: left insula, middle and inferior temporal gyri, right superior parietal gyrus, middle and inferior temporal gyri, bilateral postcentral gyri, superior temporal gyrus, parahippocampal and ambient gyri, amygdala, inferior lateral anterior temporal lobe, medulla, nuclei accumbens, medial anterior temporal lobe, fusiform gyri. Left medial anterior and inferior lateral anterior temporal lobe, and superior, middle and inferior temporal gyri also survived Bonferroni correction. In the nfvPPA group no differences survived FDR correction but the peak t-score was in left inferior frontal gyrus [t(23) = 2.17, uncorrected P =0.04], which would be expected a priori to be the disease epicentre (Rogalski et al., 2011).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Regional ligand binding by group. Unthresholded regional t-scores for each disease group compared to the control group for 11C-PK-11195 BPND on the left and 18F-AV-1451 BPND on the right.

The repeated measures ANOVA of regional 18F-AV-1451 binding across the FTD groups and controls, controlled for age and sex, showed a significant interaction between region and diagnosis [F(33.6,403.6) = 3.80, P <0.001]. T-maps from the post hoc pairwise comparisons between the control group and each disease group are shown in Fig. 1. After correction for FDR, significantly elevated binding was seen in svPPA, in the following regions: left amygdala, fusiform, medial anterior temporal lobe, middle and inferior temporal gyri and superior temporal gyrus, bilateral inferolateral anterior temporal lobes. Left medial anterior temporal lobe and middle and inferior temporal gyri also survived Bonferroni correction. For bvFTD and nfvPPA, the group differences did not survive FDR correction.

There was a trend towards patients with bvFTD due to C9ORF72 having globally higher 11C-PK-11195 binding than those with no genetic diagnosis, while those with abnormalities of MAPT had intermediate binding [main effect of gene F(2,3) = 8.26, P =0.060; Tukey’s HSD P =0.049 for C9ORF72 (mean = 0.176, ±0.017) versus no genetic diagnosis (mean = 0.094, ±0.016), but non-significant for either group versus MAPT (mean = 0.123, ±0.018)]. There was no significant interaction between genetic diagnosis and region [F(5.3,7.8) = 1.47, P =0.300].

There was no effect of bvFTD genetic status on 18F-AV-1451 binding [main effect of gene F(2,4) = 0.85, P =0.492; interaction between gene and regional binding F(6.2,12.3) = 1.78, P =0.183].

For the patients with nfvPPA, the single patient with GRN mutation was not significantly different from those without a genetic diagnosis in binding of either 11C-PK-11195 [main effect of gene F(1,6) = 0.38, P =0.559; interaction between gene and regional binding F(4.8,28.7) = 1.17, P =0.349] or 18F-AV-1451 [main effect of gene F(1,6) = 0.1.66, P =0.244; interaction between gene and regional binding F(3.2,19.2) = 0.47, P =0.720].

Correlation of 11C-PK-11195 with 18F-AV-1451 in frontotemporal dementia

Regional control-adjusted group mean 11C-PK-11195 BPND was strongly correlated with regional group mean 18F-AV1451 BPND in each group both before and after the subtraction of the control group values in every region: svPPA [r(81) = 0.727, P <0.0001 before, r(81) = 0.883, P <0.0001 after], bvFTD [r(81) = 0.582, P <0.0001 before, r(81) = 0.499, P <0.0001 after], and nfvPPA [r(81) = 0.427, P <0.0001 before, r(81) = 0.589, P <0.0001 after] (Fig. 2). These correlations were only slightly affected by partialling out the effect of regional brain atrophy. For svPPA r(80) = 0.634, P <0.0001 [partial correlation between AV and atrophy r(80) = −0.398, P =0.0002; PK and atrophy r(80) = −0.330, P =0.0024], for bvFTD r(80) = 0.459, P <0.0001 [partial correlation between AV and atrophy r(80) = −0.020, P =0.8576; PK and atrophy r(80) = −0.360, P =0.0009], and for nfvPPA r(80) = 0.542, P <0.0001 [partial correlation between AV and atrophy r(80) = −0.321, P =0.0033; PK and atrophy r(80) = −0.048, P =0.6722].

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Scatter plot of the regional mean BPND for 11C-PK-11195 against regional mean BPND of 18F-AV-1451 by disease group. For each disease group raw values are demonstrated on the left with values adjusted for non-specific signal strength through subtraction of the regional control mean shown on the right.

Finally, to ensure that our methods for controlling for non-specific general regional differences in PET tracer retention were adequate, we repeated our analysis in each lobe separately. Across all three patient groups combined (Supplementary Fig. 2), corrected for control binding, and accounting for group with ANCOVA, significant associations were observed between 11C-PK11195 and 18F-AV1451 BPND in temporal lobe F(1,54) = 82.11, P <0.0001 [diagnosis F(2,54) = 15.67, P <0.0001, interaction F(2,54) = 2.16, P =0.12], insula and cingulate F(1,12) = 29.18, P =0.0002 [diagnosis F(2,12) = 25.82, P <0.0001, interaction F <1], frontal lobe F(1,66) = 52.88, P <0.0001 [diagnosis F(2,66) = 32.31, P <0.0001, interaction F(2,66) = 7.41, P =0.0013], parietal lobe F(1,12) = 24.3, P =0.0003 [diagnosis F(2,12) = 10.45, P <0.0024, interaction F <1], and deep nuclei F(1,24) = 27.42, P <0.0001 [diagnosis F(2,24) = 8.07, P =0.0021, interaction F(2,24) = 3.4, P =0.05]. We did not find significant associations in occipital lobe F <1 [diagnosis F <1, interaction F(2,12) = 4.92, P =0.0275], cerebellum F(1,12) = 2.55, P =0.1366 [diagnosis F(2,12) = 1.98, P =0.1809, interaction F <1], or brainstem F <1 [diagnosis F(2,9) = 1.69, P =0.2388, interaction F <1].

Principal component analysis of 11C-PK-11195 and 18F-AV-1451

Four principal components were detected in the 11C-PK11195 BPND data before the elbow of the scree plot, which together explained 64% of the variance in the data (Fig. 3). Component 1 reflected whole brain binding. Component 2 was strongly weighted to the bilateral anterior temporal lobes. Component 3 primarily comprised frontal binding with a right-sided predominance. Component 4 was not strongly loaded onto any single region but was weighted towards motor cortex. In a repeated measures ANOVA including these four principal components, there was a main effect of diagnosis [F(3,39) = 11.07, P <0.001] and a significant interaction between principal component weighting and diagnosis [F(8.03, 104.36) = 4.16, P <0.001]. After correction for age and sex these remained significant [diagnosis F(3,34) = 8.98, P <0.001; interaction between principal component weighting and diagnosis F(8.14,92.28) = 4.11, P <0.001]; age and sex were not significant predictors [age F(1,34) = 0.40, P =0.533; sex F(1,34) = 1.45, P =0.237]. Post hoc t-tests between individual disease groups and controls showed svPPA was associated with an increase in component 2 [t(10.20) = 8.30, P <0.001], bvFTD associated with both increased component 2 [t(9.30) = 3.37, P =0.008] and component 3 [t(8.85) = 3.95, P =0.003] and nfvPPA associated with increased component 3 [t(12.4) = 2.38, P =0.034] (Fig. 3). Components 1 and 4 did not significantly differ between controls and patient groups.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

First four principal components for 11C-PK-11195 and 18F-AV-1451. 18F-AV-1451 component 5 was also retained by Cattell’s criterion but was not strongly weighted to any region and did not discriminate groups so is omitted here for parsimony. The bottom row shows, for each principal component (PC), the difference between each patient group and controls, adjusted for age and sex in the repeated measures ANOVA. Error bars span ± 1 SEM (standard error of the mean for the patient group). Significance in post hoc tests: ***P <0.001, **P <0.01, *P <0.05.

Five principal components were detected in the 18F-AV-1451 BPND data before the elbow of the scree plot, which together accounted for 76% of the variance in the data. Component 1 again reflected global binding but less marked in the temporal poles, which were loaded onto component 2 (left) and component 4 (right). Component 3 was weighted asymmetrically towards frontal lobe binding. Component 5 was not strongly loaded onto any single region but was weighted towards bilateral superior temporal poles. In a repeated measures ANOVA including these five principal components, there was a main effect of diagnosis [F(3, 41) = 5.43, P =0.003] and a significant interaction between principal component weighting and diagnosis [F(11,150.6) = 3.68, P <0.001]. After correction for age and sex these remained significant [diagnosis F(3,36) = 3.80, P =0.018; interaction between principal component weighting and diagnosis F(10.9,130.5) = 3.33, P <0.001]; age was also a significant predictor [F(1,36) = 4.47, P =0.041] but sex was not [F(1, 36) = 0.11, P =0.743]. The bvFTD group had increased weightings in component 4 [t(11.58) = 3.28, P =0.007], and svPPA had increased weightings in component 2 [t(11.88) = 6.819, P <0.0001] and component 4 [t(12.9) = 2.48, P =0.028] (Fig. 3). There were no significant post hoc differences between nfvPPA and controls. Components 1, 3 and 5 did not differ significantly between controls and patient groups.

In summary (Fig. 3), patients with bvFTD demonstrated significantly elevated PK binding in spatial modes that included frontal lobe and temporal poles, and elevated AV binding in frontal regions; patients with svPPA demonstrated significantly elevated AV and PK binding in temporal poles; patients with nfvPPA demonstrated significantly elevated PK binding in frontal lobe, but to a lesser extent than patients with bvFTD, and they showed no spatial modes with elevated AV binding.

Non-parametric analysis of 11C-PK-11195 and 18F-AV-1451 distributions

The principal component analyses suggest that a large amount of the variance between subjects relates to whole brain PET signal. While this might reflect global differences in protein aggregation and neuroinflammation, it could also be explained by variations in radioligand affinity for different protein pathologies or other non-specific influences discussed below. We therefore performed an analysis of the relative distribution of PET signal for each individual scan, blinded to differences in overall signal magnitude by non-parametric rank-order statistical methods.

Multi-dimensional scaling plots of the non-parametric similarity between ligand distributions, for each subgroup pair and for all groups combined, are shown in Fig. 4. The CV-SVM classification accuracy and permutation-based statistical significance are indicated next to each plot. Classification was significantly better than chance in all cases, except for the finding that the non-parametric distribution of 18F-AV-1451 was unable to distinguish between bvFTD and nfvPPA.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Pairwise classification accuracy for each ligand. 11C-PK-11195 (left), 18F-AV-1451 (middle), and using combined data (right). The graphs represent a 2D projection of the between-individual PET signal distribution dissimilarity calculated according to the squared metric stress criterion. A 10-fold cross-validated support vector machine was applied to each plot, and the classification accuracy compared to a null distribution of 1000 randomizations for non-parametric significant testing. For each comparison, percentage classification and P-value is stated. In simple terms, this means that the similarity of the distribution of ligand binding across the brain for each individual was assessed irrespective of the absolute magnitude of binding (and therefore not determined by differences in ligand affinity for different pathological subtypes). Note how in the top left plot (nfvPPA versus svPPA for 11C-PK-11195) two groups of patients are clearly separated. By contrast, in the second column third row (bvFTD versus nfvPPA for 18F-AV-1451) the points are intermingled, with only chance-level classification.

For those FTD participants that underwent scanning with both ligands, the classification procedure was repeated after combining the multi-dimensional scaling data such that the CV-SVM operated on four dimensions rather than two. This resulted in an improvement in the differentiation of bvFTD and svPPA compared to either ligand alone (88.9% classification accuracy, P <0.001). Multimodal nfvPPA versus svPPA classification accuracy matched the performance of 11C-PK-11195 at 100%, P <0.001, but bvFTD versus nfvPPA classification performance was intermediate compared to each ligand alone, at 73.7%, P =0.033.

This method was not able to classify participants based on age or sex (Supplementary Fig. 3).

Post-mortem immunohistochemistry

Representative post-mortem immunohistochemistry for protein aggregate inclusions and microglia are shown in Fig. 5. The quantitative relationship between protein aggregation and microglia is shown for each disease group individually in Fig. 6.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Immunohistochemistry from cases with FTLD-TDP43 types A and C, FTLD-P (Pick’s disease), and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) at Braak stage V. Areas of low (visual cortex BA17/18) and high (temporal cortex, BA21/22) disease burden are shown. Scale bars = 100 µm. Representative micrographs from the same location on adjacent sections stained for the relevant protein aggregate (phosphorylated-tau or TDP43) and CD68 (expressed by microglia), respectively.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

The relationship between protein aggregation and microglia in each post-mortem sample. Densities are quantified as the number of microglia or pathological inclusions per square millimetre. Each point represents a single brain region in a single individual. The Pearson correlation (r), and the partial correlation (ρ) after factoring out the density of cell nuclei are shown for each relationship, along with the corresponding P-value. Trend lines are emboldened when both correlation and partial correlation were significant at α < 0.05.

A generalized linear model demonstrated a significant relationship between the overall density of pathology and the density of microglia [Wald χ2(1) = 21.40, P <0.001], accounting for subject factors (including age and sex) nested within diagnostic group [Wald χ2(8) = 79.55, P <0.001], brain region [Wald χ2(3) = 18.06, P <0.001], diagnosis [Wald χ2(3) = 3.95, P =0.267], the interaction between diagnosis and brain region [Wald χ2(9) = 23.63, P =0.005], and the density of cell nuclei [Wald χ2(1) = 21.12, P <0.001].

Repeat generalized linear models with the same design demonstrated that this effect was driven by a significant relationship between the density of pathology and the density of amoeboid (rounded, phagocytic, activated) microglia [Wald χ2(1) = 30.83, P <0.001], but not the density of ramified (dendritic, resting) microglia [Wald χ2(1) = 0.02, P =0.898].

Discussion

This in vivo study provides insights into complementary pathophysiological processes of FTD. Taken as a whole, our findings support an important role for neuroinflammation across the FTD spectrum, corroborating suggestions from epidemiological (Venneti et al., 2008; Lant et al., 2014), genetic (Guerreiro et al., 2013; Rayaprolu et al., 2013; Broce et al., 2018), imaging (Cagnin et al., 2004; Miyoshi et al., 2010) and animal studies (Yoshiyama et al., 2007; Bhaskar et al., 2010; Yin et al., 2010). Using regional ANOVA and a principal components analysis, we have shown that neuroinflammation (indexed by 11C-PK-11195) and protein aggregates (tau or TDP43, as indexed by 18F-AV-1451) are elevated in FTD (Figs 1 and 3). Furthermore, neuroinflammation is regionally co-localized with protein aggregation within the individual syndromes, including most strongly in svPPA, where the predominant aggregated protein inclusions are TDP-43 rather than tau (Fig. 2). Principal component analysis revealed distinct spatial modes of neuroinflammation, with frontotemporal, temporal pole and global distributions (Fig. 3). The weighting of these regional distributions differs between groups, supporting the regional differences in the pair-wise comparisons. The spatial modes of protein aggregation (Fig. 3) similarly reflect the well characterized distributions of pathology in each patient group. However, the distribution of protein aggregation appears to be less focal than neuroinflammation in nfvPPA. To identify distinctive information from the patterns of inflammation and aggregation, and control for the marked individual differences in ligand binding affinity in different subtypes of FTD, we used non-parametric multi-dimensional scaling and cross-validated linear support vector machines to classify patients. We demonstrated that the distribution of neuroinflammation can accurately distinguish the FTD syndromes from controls and from each other (Fig. 4). Classification was often possible based on the distribution of protein aggregation, but with less accuracy. The greater discriminatory ability of neuroinflammation emphasizes its potential mechanistic relevance to the pathophysiology of FTD. Despite being strongly correlated at a regional level, the two PET tracers carry some unique information across these conditions, as illustrated by the improvement in distinguishing bvFTD from svPPA when multi-modal data were jointly available to the classifier.

The correlation between regional distributions of neuroinflammation and protein aggregation supports a close relationship between these processes in FTD, mirroring recent evidence from Alzheimer’s disease that neuroinflammation is correlated with tau aggregation (Dani et al., 2018), and extending this to TDP-43 associated diseases. Further, we confirm that this relationship is present throughout the disease course, by demonstrating it both in vivo with PET and post-mortem with immunohistochemistry. The partial correlations confirm that this association is over-and-above the effect of atrophy in FTD. While there were moderate associations between ligand binding and atrophy in some groups, in all cases these associations explained less of the variance than the association between 11C-PK-11195 and 18F-AV-1451, suggesting that neuroinflammation and protein aggregation may be more tightly associated with each other than either process is with volume loss. Similarly, in the post-mortem sample the density of cell nuclei explained some of the variance in the density of microglia, but additional variance was explained by the density of pathology. One interpretation of regionally co-localized neuroinflammation and protein aggregation is that microglial activation is an early pathophysiological process, which promotes or accelerates abnormal protein misfolding and aggregation. Microglia play a key role in orchestrating the innate immune response of the brain. They can be activated by misfolded proteins, and mediate responses through inflammatory pathways, cytotoxicity and changes in plasticity (Nakajima and Kohsaka, 2001; Nayak et al., 2014). In neurodegenerative diseases, this state of activation may become chronic, dysfunctional, and toxic, contributing to pathogenicity (Pasqualetti et al., 2015; Serrano-Pozo et al., 2016).

There is corollary evidence for inflammation in FTD (Heneka et al., 2014), from genetic (Guerreiro et al., 2013; Rayaprolu et al., 2013; Broce et al., 2018), CSF (Sjogren et al., 2004; Woollacott et al., 2018), epidemiology (Miller et al., 2013, 2016), post-mortem (Venneti et al., 2008; Lant et al., 2014) and animal studies (Yoshiyama et al., 2007; Bhaskar et al., 2010; Yin et al., 2010). It is well established that an innate immune response, characterized by activated microglia, is a feature of the neuropathology of FTD (Lant et al., 2014). Furthermore, mutations leading to haplo-insufficiency of GRN, a growth factor that has peripheral immune and central microglial regulatory functions (Petkau et al., 2010; Yin et al., 2010; Pickford et al., 2011), produce FTD syndromes characterized by TDP-43 pathology. Expansions in C9ORF72 have effects on microglial function as well as neurons (O’Rouke et al., 2016), and risk variants for FTD in TREM2 are associated with microglial activation (Giraldo et al., 2013). Neuroinflammation is an early feature of pathophysiology in mouse models of tauopathy, where inflammatory changes precede the accumulation of aggregated tau (Yoshiyama et al., 2007) and pro-inflammatory molecules increase tau hyperphosphorylation and aggregation (Bhaskar et al., 2010). In vivo PET studies in small samples have shown that neuroinflammation anticipates atrophy in clinically established FTD (Cagnin et al., 2004) and precedes both symptoms and the detectability of tau aggregation by PET in MAPT mutation carriers (Miyoshi et al., 2010; Bevan-Jones et al., 2019). Although neuroinflammation appears early in the pathogenesis of FTD and other neurodegenerative disorders, it remains unclear whether it is an independently initiating factor or whether it is induced by oligomeric proteins or pre-tangles.

Much of the evidence supporting the presence of inflammation in FTD comes from ex vivo studies, and indeed we provide further post-mortem evidence here. The need to improve our understanding of this process during life has led to the development of PET radioligands, but there is some controversy over the optimum ligand for imaging activated microglia. PET ligands, which target TSPO, have long been the mainstay of imaging microglia. However TSPO expression patterns in microglia are complex and the functional effects of different microglial phenotypes are incompletely understood (Gomez-Nicola and Perry, 2015). TSPO is also expressed by other cell types, notably astrocytes (McCarthy and Harden, 1981). However, in favour of the use of 11C-PK-11195 is its demonstrated selectivity for activated microglia over quiescent microglia and reactive astrocytes (Banati, 2002), its relative insensitivity to common polymorphisms in TSPO compared to second generation TSPO ligands (Zhang, 2015; Stefaniak and O’Brien, 2016), and the fact that it has well established methods of non-invasive kinetic analysis (Turkheimer et al., 2007; Passamonti et al., 2018). 11C-PK-11195 has been used in studies of other neurodegenerative diseases (Edison et al., 2013; Varley et al., 2015; Stefaniak and O’Brien, 2016; Passamonti et al., 2018). There remain some disadvantages, including non-specific binding and low brain penetration. Whilst its limited signal-to-noise ratio might explain previous negative studies using 11C-PK-11195, this does not undermine positive findings such as those shown here, especially within our multivariate analyses that explicitly control for differences in ligand penetration and affinity. When interpreting the meaning of increased 11C-PK-11195 binding one must consider the potential contribution from reactive astrocytes expressing upregulated TSPO, and while increased 11C-PK-11195 binding may still be interpreted as immune activation, a causal role of inflammation in human dementia is yet to be proven by interventional studies.

In contrast to 11C-PK-11195, the 18F-AV-1451 binding provides an ambiguous signal despite protein aggregation being an essential feature of FTD and many other dementias. We propose that 18F-AV-1451 binding is a proxy measure of aggregated non-amyloid-β proteins in FTD. In Alzheimer’s disease the sensitivity of in vivo imaging with 18F-AV-1451, and its affinity for tau in neurofibrillary tangles, is well established and has contributed significantly to our understanding of its pathogenesis and progression. However, the situation in FTD is more complex due in part to pathological heterogeneity and the differential affinity for tau aggregates arising from different isoforms and with different ultrastructure (Jones et al., 2018). The molecular target in FTD associated with TDP43 is also as yet undetermined, but is unlikely to be TDP43 itself (Marquié et al., 2015; Lowe et al., 2016; Sander et al., 2016). This heterogeneity is problematic for univariate regional analyses, and although six of our patients have genetic mutations, and six others were amyloid biomarker negative (Tables 1 and 2), we cannot definitively state the majority of patients’ pathological type ante-mortem.

The molecular targets for 18F-AV-1451 binding remain controversial, and several off-target binding possibilities need to be considered. Supporting our use of 18F-AV-1451 as a marker of non-amyloid-β protein aggregation, previous post-mortem work has demonstrated some binding to FTLD pathologies, albeit at a lower magnitude than that seen with Alzheimer’s pathology (Marquié et al., 2015; Lowe et al., 2016; Mcmillan et al., 2016; Sander et al., 2016). This is corroborated by in vivo studies of patients with a straight filament 4-repeat tauopathy and clinical FTD resulting from MAPT mutations, showing binding in areas typically affected in FTD and affected at post-mortem (Bevan-Jones et al., 2016; Smith et al., 2016), and by the elevated binding in the affected brain regions of patients with svPPA (Bevan-Jones et al., 2018a, Makaretz et al., 2018) and bvFTD due to C9orf72 expansions (Bevan-Jones et al., 2018b), who have TDP-43 rather than tau pathology. However, even within genetically determined FTD, binding affinity varies according to different tau isoforms and strains (Jones et al., 2018) supporting varying affinity to different morphologies of tau.

18F-AV-1451 can have other non-tau, non-TDP-43, targets such as neuromelanin (Marquié et al., 2015) and mono-amine oxidase (MAO) (Vermeiren et al., 2018), and such off-target binding is an important caveat in clinical studies using 18F-AV-1451 (Baker et al., 2019). Neuromelanin in the catecholaminergic brainstem nuclei is a particular concern in studies of brainstem tau pathology in progressive supranuclear palsy, and artefactual binding in Parkinson’s disease. However, neuromelanin is not expressed by cortex even in tauopathies (Passamonti et al., 2018) and is unlikely to account for our FTD results. MAO subtypes are expressed by both neurons (MAOA, especially in basal ganglia) and reactive astrocytes (mainly MAOB) (Fowler et al., 2005; Ben Haim et al., 2015). If 18F-AV-1451 binding were driven by ‘off target’ binding to reactive astrocytes, which are induced by activated microglia (Liddelow et al., 2017), this would provide further evidence for the importance of neuroinflammation in FTD, but it would undermine the inference we make on the relationship between protein aggregation and inflammation. Although 18F-AV-1451 has weak affinity for both types of MAO (Drake et al., 2019), selective blockade of MAOA and MAOB leads to only minor changes in the estimated cortical binding from PET in non-human primates (Drake et al., 2019), while clinical treatment with MAO inhibitors does not significantly change estimated 18F-AV-1451 uptake in humans (Hansen et al., 2018; Smith et al., 2018). Moreover, the presymptomatic dissociation of 11C-PK-11195 and 18F-AV-1451 binding (Bevan-Jones et al., 2019) argues strongly against simple cross-affinity. Similarly, in our own post-mortem cases we see regional co-localization of both FTD-tau and FTD-TDP-43 aggregates with microglial activation, to at least as great a degree as is observed in Alzheimer’s disease (Fig. 6). Together, these observations suggest that neuroinflammation and protein aggregation co-occur in symptomatic stages, but in early stage disease neuroinflammation can occur in the absence of 18F-AV-1451 binding. Neuroinflammation and protein aggregation are at least partially dissociable processes by current PET ligands.

In the face of uncertainty about molecular targets and variations in affinity, it is important to emphasize that through our classification analysis we focus on distribution rather than quantification of binding, using a non-parametric method that is insensitive to absolute binding values and instead reflects the spatial pattern of binding. This takes into account the potential differences in affinity of 18F-AV-1451 for different protein targets. Overall, whilst it is clear that 18F-AV-1451 does not bind exclusively to tau aggregates, the distribution of binding regionally co-localizes and varies with that expected of aggregated protein in these diseases, and post-mortem immunohistochemistry of tau. Indeed, 18F-AV-1451 may provide a usefully non-selective marker of non-amyloid-β aggregated protein, whether tau or TDP-43, allowing in vivo examination across the spectrum of sporadic FTD syndromes. Whilst in the complex setting of FTLD we interpret 18F-AV-1451 binding as a non-specific marker of non-amyloid-β neuropathology, the biological relevance of elevated binding in non-Alzheimer’s disease neurodegenerative disease remains incompletely understood. Further work examining 18F-AV-1451 binding across large post-mortem cohorts of FTLD pathology will be required to independently validate our hypothesis.

The main limitation of this study is group size which, although larger than most previous PET studies in FTD, is still small for each individual diagnosis. The small sample size reduces the power of the study to find parametric group differences in binding, particularly given that both ligands have a degree of insensitivity to their targets, as well as limiting the ability to detect associations with clinical features and severity. Characterization of the groups is also limited in that the genotyping and amyloid assays were based on clinical indications and consent: we did not directly examine amyloid status in all individuals and whilst there is a mix of both genetic and sporadic cases, we did not genotype every participant. The inability to perform pathological subtyping in vivo makes interpretation of results more difficult in view of the generally poor relationship between phenotype and underlying neuropathology in FTD. Consequently, we cannot use the clinical diagnostic groups alone to draw conclusions about the relationship between microglial activation and specific forms of protein aggregation. We are also limited in the inferences about the predilection for immune dysregulation in a particular neuropathological subtype, such as the relationship suggested between immune dysfunction and FTLD-TDP-43 (Miller et al., 2013, 2016), except for the cases with genetic mutations.

To conclude, we provide in vivo evidence for neuroinflammation in FTD, which has a close relationship with 18F-AV-1451 binding, taken in this study to represent a marker of either FTLD-tau or FTLD-TDP-43 neuropathology. PET measurement of inflammation provided a more accurate classification of syndromes than did protein aggregation emphasizing its potential importance in shaping the clinical and neuropathological patterns of the diverse clinical syndromes of FTD. A causal role for neuroinflammation in neurodegeneration would inform future drug targets and potential clinical trials in FTD. Our findings therefore warrant further longitudinal mechanistic investigation into the role of neuroinflammation in early-stage neurodegeneration, its relationship to specific protein aggregation and to clinical progression.

Supplementary Material

awaa033_Supplementary_Data

Acknowledgements

We thank our volunteers for participating in this study, the radiographers/technologists at the Wolfson Brain Imaging Centre and PET/CT Unit, Addenbrooke’s Hospital, for their invaluable support in data acquisition, and Poonam Singh from the Cambridge Brain bank for sectioning the neuropathological samples. We thank the NIHR Dementias and Neurodegenerative Diseases Research Network for help with subject recruitment. We also thank Drs Istvan Boros, Joong-Hyun Chun, and WBIC RPU for the manufacture of the radioligands. We thank Avid (Lilly) for supplying the precursor for the manufacturing of 18F-AV-1451 for use in this study.

Funding

The work was supported by National Institute for Health Research Cambridge Biomedical Research Centre including the Cambridge Brain Bank; the Wellcome Trust (J.B.R., 103838); the Cambridge Centre for Parkinson-plus; the Medical Research Council (MR/P01271X/1); the National Institute for Health Research, Association of British Neurologists and the Patrick Berthoud Charitable Trust (T.E.C.); and the Lundbeck Foundation (S.S.K.).

Competing interests

The AV-1451 precursor used in this study was supplied by Avid (Lilly). Prof O’Brien reports personal fees from TauRx, personal fees from Axon, personal fees from GE Healthcare, grants and personal fees from Avid/Lilly, personal fees from Eisai, grants from Alliance Medical, personal fees from Roche, grants from Merck, outside the submitted work. Prof Rowe has received research grants from Janssen, Lilly, AZ-Medimmune and honoraria/consultancies from UCB, Asceneuron, GE Healthcare, Althira, and Biogen, all unrelated to the current work. The other authors report no competing interests.

Glossary

Abbreviations

BPND =

non-displaceable binding potential;

bvFTD =

behavioural variant FTD;

FTD =

frontotemporal dementia;

nfvPPA =

non-fluent variant primary progressive aphasia;

svPPA =

semantic variant primary progressive aphasia

References

  1. Baker SL, Harrison TM, Maaß A, La Joie R, Jagust W.. Effect of off-target binding on 18F-Flortaucipir variability in healthy controls across the lifespan. J Nucl Med 2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Banati RB. Visualising microglial activation in vivo. Glia 2002; 206–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Ben Haim Carrillo-de Sauvage BL, Ceyzériat M-A, Escartin K.. C. Elusive roles for reactive astrocytes in neurodegenerative diseases. Front Cell Neurosci 2015; 9: 1–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bevan-Jones RW, Cope TE, Jones SP, Passamonti L, Hong YT, Fryer T, et al. [18F]AV-1451 binding is increased in frontotemporal dementia due to C9orf72 expansion. Ann Clin Transl Neurol 2018b; 5: 1292–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bevan-Jones WR, Cope TE, Jones PS, Passamonti L, Hong YT, Fryer T, et al. In vivo evidence for pre-symptomatic neuroinflammation in a MAPT mutation carrier. Ann Clin Transl Neurol 2019; 6: 373–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bevan-Jones WR, Cope TE, Jones PS, Passamonti L, Hong YT, Fryer TD, et al. [18F] AV-1451 binding in vivo mirrors the expected distribution of TDP-43 pathology in the semantic variant of primary progressive aphasia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2018a; 89: 1032–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bevan-Jones WR, Cope TE, Passamonti L, Fryer TD, Hong YT, Aigbirhio F, et al. [18F]AV-1451 PET in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia due to MAPT mutation. Ann Clin Transl Neurol 2016; 3: 940–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bevan-Jones WR, Surendranathan A, Passamonti L, Rodríguez PV, Arnold R, Mak E, et al. Neuroimaging of Inflammation in Memory and Related Other Disorders (NIMROD) study protocol: a deep phenotyping cohort study of the role of brain inflammation in dementia, depression and other neurological illnesses. BMJ Open 2017; 7: 1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bhaskar K, Konerth M, Kokiko-Cochran ON, Cardona A, Ransohoff RM, Lamb BT.. Regulation of tau pathology by the microglial fractalkine receptor. Neuron 2010; 68: 19–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Broce I, Karch CM, Wen N, Fan CC, Wang Y, Hong C, et al. Immune-related genetic enrichment in frontotemporal dementia: an analysis of genome-wide association studies. PLoS Med 2018; 15: e1002487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cagnin A, Rossor M, Sampson EL, MacKinnon T, Banati RB.. In vivo detection of microglial activation in frontotemporal dementia. Ann Neurol 2004; 56: 894–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chien DT, Bahri S, Szardenings AK, Walsh JC, Mu F.. Early clinical PET imaging results with the novel PHF-tau radioligand [F-18]-T807. J Alzheimers Dis 2013; 34: 457–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dani M, Wood M, Mizoguchi R, Fan Z, Walker Z, Morgan R, et al. Microglial activation correlates in vivo with both tau and amyloid in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 2018; 141: 2740–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Drake LR, Pham JM, Desmond TJ, Mossine AV, Lee SJ, Kilbourn MR, et al. Identification of AV-1451 as a weak, nonselective inhibitor of monoamine oxidase. ACS Chem Neurosci 2019; 10: 3839–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Edison P, Ahmed I, Fan Z, Hinz R, Gelosa G, Ray Chaudhuri K, et al. Microglia, amyloid, and glucose metabolism in Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia. Neuropsychopharmacol 2013; 38: 938–49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fowler JS, Logan J, Volkow ND, Wang G-J.. Translational neuroimaging: positron emission tomography studies of monoamine oxidase. Mol Imaging Biol 2005; 7: 377–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Giraldo M, Lopera F, Siniard AL, Corneveaux JJ, Schrauwen I, Carvajal J, et al. Variants in triggering receptor expressed on myeloid cells 2 are associated with both behavioral variant frontotemporal lobar degeneration and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 2013; 34: 2077.e11–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gomez-Nicola D, Perry VH.. Microglial dynamics and role in the healthy and diseased brain: a paradigm of functional plasticity. Neuroscientist 2015; 21: 169–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gorno-Tempini ML, Hillis AE, Weintraub S, Kertesz A, Mendez M, Cappa SF, et al. Classification of primary progressive aphasia and its variants. Neurology 2011; 76: 1006–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Guerreiro RJ, Lohmann E, Bras JM, Gibbs JR, Rohrer JD, Gurunlian N, et al. Using exome sequencing to reveal mutations in TREM2 presenting as a frontotemporal dementia-like syndrome without bone involvement. JAMA Neurol 2013; 70: 78–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hansen AK, Brooks DJ, Borghammer P.. MAO-B inhibitors do not block in vivo flortaucipir ([18 F]-AV-1451) binding. Mol Imaging Biol 2018; 20: 356–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Heneka MT, Kummer MP, Latz E.. Innate immune activation in neurodegenerative disease. Nat Rev Immunol 2014; 14: 463–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Jones DT, Knopman DS, Graff-Radford J, Syrjanen JA, Senjem ML, Schwarz CG, et al. In vivo F-AV-1451 tau-PET signal in MAPT mutation carriers varies by expected tau isoforms. Neurology 2018; 90: e947–e954. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kriegeskorte N, Mur M, Bandettini P.. Representational similarity analysis–connecting the branches of systems neuroscience. Front Syst Neurosci 2008; 2: 1–28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Lant SB, Robinson AC, Thompson JC, Rollinson S, Pickering-Brown S, Snowden JS, et al. Patterns of microglial cell activation in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol 2014; 40: 686–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Liddelow SA, Guttenplan KA, Clarke LE, Bennett FC, Bohlen CJ, Schirmer L, et al. Neurotoxic reactive astrocytes are induced by activated microglia. Nature 2017; 541: 481–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Lowe VJ, Curran G, Fang P, Liesinger AM, Josephs KA, Parisi JE, et al. An autoradiographic evaluation of AV-1451 Tau PET in dementia. Acta Neuropathol Commun 2016; 4: 58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Mackenzie IR, Neumann M, Bigio EH, Cairns NJ, Alafuzoff I, Kril J, et al. Nomenclature and nosology for neuropathologic subtypes of frontotemporal lobar degeneration: an update. Acta Neuropathol 2010; 119: 1–4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Makaretz SJ, Quimby M, Collins J, Makris N, Mcginnis S, Schultz A, et al. Flortaucipir tau PET imaging in semantic variant primary progressive aphasia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2018; 89: 1024–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Marquié M, Normandin MD, Vanderburg CR, Costantino IM, Bien EA, Rycyna LG, et al. Validating novel tau positron emission tomography tracer [F-18]-AV-1451 (T807) on postmortem brain tissue. Ann Neurol 2015; 78: 787–800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. McCarthy KD, Harden TK.. Identification of two benzodiazepine binding sites on cells cultured from rat cerebral cortex. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1981; 216: 183–91. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Mcmillan CT, Irwin DJ, Nasrallah I, Phillips JS, Spindler M, Rascovsky K, et al. Multimodal evaluation demonstrates in vivo 18F-AV-1451 uptake in autopsyã confirmed corticobasal degeneration. Acta Neuropathol 2016; 132: 935–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Miller ZA, Rankin KP, Graff-Radford NR, Takada LT, Sturm VE, Cleveland CM, et al. TDP-43 frontotemporal lobar degeneration and autoimmune disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2013; 84: 956–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Miller ZA, Sturm VE, Camsari GB, Karydas A, Yokoyama JS, Grinberg LT, et al. Increased prevalence of autoimmune disease within C9 and FTD/MND cohorts completing the picture. Neurol Neuroimmunol NeuroInflamm 2016; 3: 1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Miyoshi M, Shinotoh H, Wszolek ZK, Strongosky AJ, Shimada H, Arakawa R, et al. In vivo detection of neuropathologic changes in presymptomatic MAPT mutation carriers: a PET and MRI study. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2010; 16: 404–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Nakajima K, Kohsaka S.. Microglia: activation and their significance in the central nervous system. J Biochem 2001; 130: 169–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Nayak D, Roth TL, McGavern DB.. Microglia development and function. Annu Rev Immunol 2014; 32: 367–402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. O’Rouke JG, Bogdanik L, Yanez A, Lall D, Wolf AJ, Muhammad A, et al. C9orf72 is required for proper macrophage and microglial function in mice. Science 2016; 351: 1324–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Pasqualetti G, Brooks DJ, Edison P.. The role of neuroinflammation in dementias. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 2015; 15: 17. doi: 10.1007/s11910-015-0531-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Passamonti L, Vazquez Rodriguez P, Hong YT, Allinson K, Bevan-Jones WR, Williamson D, et al. [11C] PK11195 binding in Alzheimer’s disease and progressive supranuclear palsy. Neurology 2018; 90: e1989–e1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Petkau TL, Neal SJ, Orban PC, Macdonald JL, Hill AM, Lu G, et al. Progranulin expression in the developing and adult murine brain. J Comp Neurol 2010; 518: 3931–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Pickford F, Marcus J, Camargo LM, Xiao Q, Graham D, Mo J-R, et al. Progranulin is a chemoattractant for microglia and stimulates their endocytic activity. Am J Pathol 2011; 178: 284–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Rascovsky K, Hodges JR, Knopman D, Mendez MF, Kramer JH, Neuhaus J, et al. Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain 2011; 134: 2456–77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Rayaprolu S, Mullen B, Baker M, Lynch T, Finger E, Seeley WW, et al. TREM2 in neurodegeneration: evidence for association of the p. R47H variant with frontotemporal dementia and Parkinson’s disease. Mol Neurodegener 2013; 8: 1–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Rogalski E, Cobia D, Harrison TM, Wieneke C, Thompson CK, Weintraub S, et al. Anatomy of language impairments in primary progressive aphasia. J Neurosci 2011; 31: 3344–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Sander K, Lashley T, Gami P, Gendron T, Lythgoe MF, Rohrer JD, et al. Characterization of tau positron emission tomography tracer [(18)F]AV-1451 binding to postmortem tissue in Alzheimer’s disease, primary tauopathies, and other dementias. Alzheimers Dement 2016; 12: 1116–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Seelaar H, Rohrer JD, Pijnenburg YA, Fox NC, Van Swieten JC.. Clinical, genetic and pathological heterogeneity of frontotemporal dementia: a review. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2011; 82: 476–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Serrano-Pozo A, Betensky RA, Frosch MP, Hyman BT.. Plaque-associated local toxicity increases over the clinical course of Alzheimer disease. Am J Pathol 2016; 186: 375–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Sjogren M, Folkesson S, Blennow K, Tarkowski E.. Increased intrathecal inflammatory activity in frontotemporal dementia: pathophysiological implications. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2004; 75: 1107–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Smith R, Puschmann A, Scholl M, Ohlsson T, Van Swieten J, Honer M, et al. 18F-AV-1451 tau PET imaging correlates strongly with tau neuropathology in MAPT mutation carriers. Brain 2016; 139: 2372–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Smith R, Schöll M, Londos E, Ohlsson T, Hansson O.. 18 F-AV-1451 in Parkinson’s Disease with and without dementia and in. Sci Rep 2018; 8: 4717. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Smith R, Scholl M, Widner H, van Westen D, Svenningsson P, Hägerström D, et al. In vivo retention of 18 F-AV-1451 in corticobasal syndrome. Neurology 2017; 89: 845–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Spinelli EG, Mandelli ML, Miller ZA, Santos‐Santos MA, Wilson SM, Agosta F, et al. Typical and atypical pathology in primary progressive aphasia variants. Ann Neurol 2017; 81: 430–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Stefaniak J, O’Brien J.. Imaging of neuroinflammation in dementia: a review. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2016; 87: 21–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Turkheimer FE, Edison P, Pavese N, Roncaroli F, Anderson AN, Hammers A, et al. Reference and target region modeling of [11C]-(R)-PK11195 brain studies. J Nucl Med 2007; 48: 158–67. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Varley J, Brooks DJ, Edison P.. Imaging neuroinflammation in Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias: recent advances and future directions. Alzheimer’s Dement 2015; 11: 1110–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Venneti S, Wang G, Nguyen J, Wiley CA.. The positron emission tomography ligand DAA1106 binds with high affinity to activated microglia in human neurological disorders. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 2008; 67: 1001–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Vermeiren C, Motte P, Viot D, Mairet-Coello G, Courade J-P, Citron M, et al. The tau positron-emission tomography tracer AV-1451 binds with similar affinities to tau fibrils and monoamine oxidases. Mov Disord 2018; 33: 273–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Woollacott IOC, Nicholas JM, Heslegrave A, Heller C, Foiani MS, Dick KM, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid soluble TREM2 levels in frontotemporal dementia differ by genetic and pathological subgroup. Alzheimer’s Res Ther 2018; 10: 1–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Xia C-F, Arteaga J, Chen G, Gangadharmath U, Gomez LF, Kasi D, Lam C, et al. [ 18 F] T807, a novel tau positron emission tomography imaging agent for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Dement 2013; 9: 666–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Yaqub M, van Berckel BNM, Schuitemaker A, Hinz R, Turkheimer FE, Tomasi G, et al. Optimization of supervised cluster analysis for extracting reference tissue input curves in (R)-[(11)C]PK11195 brain PET studies. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 2012; 32: 1600–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Yin F, Banerjee R, Thomas B, Zhou P, Qian L, Jia T, et al. Exaggerated inflammation, impaired host defense, and neuropathology in progranulin-deficient mice. J Exp Med 2010; 207: 117–28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Yoshiyama Y, Higuchi M, Zhang B, Huang S-M, Iwata N, Saido TC, et al. Synapse loss and microglial activation precede tangles in a P301S tauopathy mouse model. Neuron 2007; 53: 337–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Zhang J. Mapping neuroinflammation in frontotemporal dementia with molecular PET imaging. J Neuroinflamm 2015; 12: 108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Zhang W, Arteaga J, Cashion DK, Chen G, Gangadharmath U, Gomez LF, et al. A highly selective and specific PET tracer for imaging of tau pathologies. J Alzheimers Dis 2012; 31: 601–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

awaa033_Supplementary_Data

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, upon reasonable request.


Articles from Brain are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES