Skip to main content
JAMA Network logoLink to JAMA Network
. 2020 Mar 23;174(7):1–11. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.0327

Associations Between Screen Use and Child Language Skills

A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis

Sheri Madigan 1,2,, Brae Anne McArthur 1,2, Ciana Anhorn 1,2, Rachel Eirich 1,2, Dimitri A Christakis 3,4
PMCID: PMC7091394  PMID: 32202633

Key Points

Question

What is the association between screen use and children’s language skills across the extant literature?

Findings

In this systematic review and meta-analysis of data from 42 studies, greater quantity of screen use (ie, hours per day/week) was negatively associated with child language, while better quality of screen use (ie, educational programs and co-viewing with caregivers) were positively associated with child language skills.

Meaning

Findings support pediatric recommendations to limit screen exposure, to provide high-quality programming, and to co-view when possible.

Abstract

Importance

There is considerable public and scientific debate as to whether screen use helps or hinders early child development, particularly the development of language skills.

Objective

To examine via meta-analyses the associations between quantity (duration of screen time and background television), quality (educational programming and co-viewing), and onset of screen use and children’s language skills.

Data Sources

Searches were conducted in MEDLINE, Embase, and PsycINFO in March 2019. The search strategy included a publication date limit from 1960 through March 2019.

Study Selection

Inclusion criteria were a measure of screen use; a measure of language skills; and statistical data that could be transformed into an effect size. Exclusion criteria were qualitative studies; child age older than 12 years; and language assessment preverbal.

Data Extraction and Synthesis

The following variables were extracted: effect size, child age and sex, screen measure type, study publication year, and study design. All studies were independently coded by 2 coders and conducted in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses guidelines.

Main Outcomes and Measures

Based on a priori study criteria, quantity of screen use included duration of screen time and background television, quality of screen use included co-viewing and exposure to educational programs, and onset of screen use was defined as the age children first began viewing screens. The child language outcome included assessments of receptive and/or expressive language.

Results

Participants totaled 18 905 from 42 studies included. Effect sizes were measured as correlations (r). Greater quantity of screen use (hours per use) was associated with lower language skills (screen time [n = 38; r = −0.14; 95% CI, −0.18 to −0.10]; background television [n = 5; r = −0.19; 95% CI, −0.33 to −0.05]), while better-quality screen use (educational programs [n = 13; r = 0.13; 95% CI, 0.02-0.24]; co-viewing [n = 12; r = 0.16; 95% CI, 0.07-.24]) were associated with stronger child language skills. Later age at screen use onset was also associated with stronger child language skills [n = 4; r = 0.17; 95% CI, 0.07-0.27].

Conclusions and Relevance

The findings of this meta-analysis support pediatric recommendations to limit children’s duration of screen exposure, to select high-quality programming, and to co-view when possible.


This meta-analysis explores the associations between quantity (screen time and background television), quality (educational programming and co-viewing), and onset of screen use and children’s language skills.

Introduction

For decades, there has been scientific debate as well as considerable public discourse as to whether screen use, defined as television or screen exposure, helps or hinders early child development. This debate has been reignited in the last decade, as children’s access to and consumption of digital media is on the rise.1,2 The debate primarily centers around quantity vs quality of screen use. In terms of quantity of screen use (ie, hours per day/week), it has been argued that screen use can be a passive or sedentary behavior that can displace critical learning opportunities for growth and development,3,4 such as language. That is, when young children are exposed to screens, they are not engaging in verbal dyadic exchanges that have been shown to promote communication and language acquisition.5,6 However, there is research to support and refute the notion that increased quantity of screen use is linked with delayed language acquisition.3,5,7,8

In terms of quality of screen use, it is purported that context (co-viewing) and content (educational programming) may offset some of the developmental risks associated with screen use.9 That is, quality of programming may serve to augment rather than inhibit child language.10 Some researchers have disputed this claim and argued that this notion is based on misleading marketing claims that certain screen-based programming will help children garner knowledge and enhance intellect.11 From an empirical perspective, there is mixed evidence as to whether screen-based programming that is deemed educational can be effective in teaching language to children12,13 and whether co-viewing facilitates child language acquisition.6,14

To adequately evaluate all sides of the debate, this meta-analysis will examine the association between screen use and children’s language skills. Specifically, 3 components of screen use will be explored: quantity of use (ie, hours of screen time and background television), quality of use (ie, educational and co-viewing), and age at screen exposure onset. We also examine potential moderators of these associations to explain heterogeneity in study findings, including child age, because it has been argued that there may be a sensitive period in which screens may exert their influence on language15; child sex, given the proclivity for boys to lag behind girls in their early language development16; and study year, given the rapid growth in the use and accessibility of digital technology.1 We target child language in particular because it is one of the most frequently studied correlates of screen time, is considered to be one of the earliest markers of developmental risk, and is a common reason for referral to pediatric specialists.17,18 Moreover, deficits and delays in language skills are in turn linked with a myriad of negative outcomes, such as psychopathology, poor school readiness, delayed academic achievement, and poor occupational functioning.19,20 These deficits and delays in language skills can set the stage for developmental disparities. Identifying modifiable environmental factors that can augment or attenuate language skills are essential, not only for optimizing child developmental trajectories but also to inform policy recommendations.

The American Academy of Pediatrics recommends no screen exposure before age 18 months, that children between ages 2 and 5 years view no more than 1 hour of high-quality programming per day and to co-view if possible, and that children older than 6 years receive consistent limits on the quantity and quality of screen use.21 While these recommendations have been adopted by many international governing bodies,22,23,24,25,26 they have also been criticized for lacking empirical support.27,28,29 Thus, this study endeavors to resolve the existing debate and provide more definitive conclusions via meta-analyses on the role of screen use for children’s language, with the goal of informing policy, research, and practice.

Methods

Definition of Constructs

Quantity of screen use included duration of screen time, defined as duration of time spent watching television, movies, or DVDs on devices (eg, tablets or televisions), as well as background television, typically reported in hours per day/week. Quality of screen use included co-viewing or how often caregivers joined their children while exposed to screens, as well as extent of exposure to educational programs (eg, Sesame Street). We included educational programs when authors deemed them as such (see study specific definitions in eTable 1 in the Supplement). Onset of screen use was defined as the age children first began viewing screens. In terms of child language, this study included assessments of receptive (eg, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test)30 or expressive (eg, MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory)31 language assessed using parent-report questionnaires or standardized assessments.

Search Strategy

Searches were conducted in MEDLINE, Embase, and PsycINFO in March 2019 by a health sciences librarian. Both database-specific subject headings and text word fields were searched for concepts of “screen time” and “language” (see full search strategy in eTable 2 in the Supplement). Synonymous terms were first combined with the Boolean “OR.” These 2 concepts were then combined with the Boolean “AND.” The concept of children (<12 years) was searched using both the “Age Limits” function in the databases as well as with a text word search and combined with the other 2 concepts. In all databases, truncation symbols were used in text word searches when appropriate. In MEDLINE and PsycINFO, the search strategy included a publication date limit from 1960 through March 2019. The Embase segment that was accessed originated in 1974. No language limits were applied. References of all included studies, as well as review articles, were also searched.

Study Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

The titles and abstracts of all studies emerging from the search strategy were reviewed by 2 independent coders to determine inclusion criteria, which were as follows: (1) mean sample age 12.0 years and younger to capture language when it is maximally unfolding; (2) a measure of screen use; (3) a measure of language skill (ie, expressive, receptive, or combined); (4) observational study; and (5) statistical data that could be transformed into an effect size. If effect sizes could not be extracted from the study (n = 3), the corresponding author was contacted (authors provided data). As detailed in Figure 1, studies were excluded if (1) they were qualitative; (2) they included children older than 12 years; (3) the language measure was preverbal (eg, babbling) or was a composite score with other nonlinguistic skills (eg, IQ); (4) the sample had children with autism spectrum disorder or intellectual disabilities; or (5) the study was experimental and without baseline measures prior to experimentation.

Figure 1. PRISMA Flow Diagram Detailing the Search Strategy.

Figure 1.

Study Quality Assessment

The quality of included studies was assessed based on a 10-point quality assessment tool adapted from the National Institutes of Health Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-Sectional Studies32 (eTable 3 in the Supplement). Extraction of methodologic quality was conducted by a primary coder, and 20% of the studies were verified by a second coder. The intercoder agreement was 0.80, and discrepancies were resolved through consensus.

Data Extraction

In addition to the calculation of effect sizes, the following moderator variables were extracted: (1) child age (in months), (2) child sex (percentage male), (3) screen use type (mobile only vs television only vs mixture of television, mobile, computers, and/or video games), (4) study publication year, (5) study design (cross-sectional vs longitudinal), and (6) study methodologic quality. All studies meeting inclusion criteria were independently coded by 2 trained coders. Reliability for continuous moderators ranged from 0.86 to 1.00 and for categorical moderators, the mean percentage agreement was 81%. Discrepancies were resolved through consensus.

Data Synthesis

If there were multiple studies based on the same data set, we selected the study with the largest sample size, readily available statistics, and psychometrically sound measurement. If adjusted and unadjusted effect sizes were provided, we selected adjusted (eTable 4 in the Supplement). If a single study assessed screen time and, for example, background television, both effect sizes were extracted and examined in separate meta-analyses. If more than 1 measure of language was provided (eg, receptive and general language), the most global assessment of child language was selected. If language was assessed at multiple points, we selected the latest point to capture the most developed language skills.33,34 When cross-sectional and longitudinal correlations were provided, we selected the most temporally distant effect size. If within a study, effect sizes from independent cohorts were provided, they were entered into the meta-analysis separately.

Data Analysis

Comprehensive Meta-Analysis Software, version 3.0 (BioStat)35 was used to estimate pooled effect sizes and conduct moderator analyses. When nonsignificant findings (n = 4) were reported without accompanying statistical information, a P value of .50 was entered, consistent with recommendations by Rosenthal.36 Pooled effect sizes are represented as correlations (r) with 95% CIs. Funder and Ozer37 suggest that correlations of 0.1, 0.2, and 0.3 are indicative of small, medium, and large effects sizes, respectively. Calculations were based on a random-effects model using the DerSimonian and Laird estimator38 to account for existing heterogeneity among studies.36 Outlier detection was examined in SPSS (IBM)39 using visual inspection of box plots. Test of heterogeneity of effect sizes were examined with and without outliers to determine whether moderator analyses should be explored. To assess for heterogeneity of effect sizes, we computed the Q and I2 statistics. Moderators should be explored when a significant Q statistic is detected and/or when an I2 is less than 50%.24 Significance of categorical and continuous moderators was determined by the Q statistic and by mixed-effects model meta-regressions (method of moments), respectively.36,38,40 Consistent with recommendations by Borenstein et al,38 categorical moderators were only analyzed when k was at least 10 and a minimum cell size of k greater than 3 was available for each categorical comparison. To detect for publication bias, we used the Egger test and an examination of funnel plots.

Results

Studies Selected

The PRISMA flow diagram (Figure 1) details the search strategy and results. The initial search identified 26 751 records. Full-text review occurred for 337 articles. A total of 42 studies (18 905 participants) met inclusion criteria and were included in the final analyses.

Sample Characteristics

Of the studies included (Table),3,5,6,7,8,10,13,14,15,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68 24 were cross-sectional and 17 were longitudinal, sample size ranged from 19 to 2335 participants, and publication year ranged from 1973 to 2019. Children were approximately aged 35.7 and 44.4 months at the screen use and language measurements, respectively, and sex was on average 50.2% male (n = 9490). Measurement included questionnaires (n = 17; 41.5%), with a smaller number of studies using screen time diary (n = 13; 31.7%), interview (n = 10; 23.4%), or observer (n = 1; 0.2%) methods. Most studies examine screen use via television exclusively (70.7%) or a composite of television and/or computers, mobile use, and video games (24.4%), and a minority examined mobile use only (4.8%). The mean (SD) quality score was 6.21 (1.69) (range, 3.00-9.00; eTable 4 in the Supplement).

Table. Characteristics of All Studies Included in the Meta-analysis.

Source No. Age, Mean, mo % Male SES Study design Assessment Screen Language type
Screens Language Language Screen Use variable Type
Allen et al,41 1992 53 127.0 127.0 60.0 D CS SA Dr ST TV R
Alloway et al,42 2014 30 36.0 36.0 57.0 D CS SA Q ST, ED TV R
Arraf et al,43 1990 173 45.0 45.0 50.0 M/H CS SA Dr ST, ED, CoV TV G
Barr et al,44 2010 53 15.8 49.9 46.7 M/H L SA Dr ST, ED TV R
Bittman et al,45 2011
Cohort 1 2335 30.0 54.0 50.0 D L SA Dr ST, BG, CoV TV, C R
Cohort 2 2233 84.0 102.0 50.0 D L SA Dr ST TV R
Blankson et al,15 2015 228 36.0 60.0 48.0 M/H L SA Q ST TV R
Byeon and Hong et al,46 2015 1778 27.0 27.0 51.3 D CS SA Q ST TV G
Castles et al,47 2013 1539 48.0 48.0 50.6 D CS SA Q ST TV, C G
Chonchaiya and Pruksananonda et al,6 2008 166 31.5 31.5 63.0 NS CS SA I ST, CoV, On TV G
Christakis et al,3 2009 329 18.6 22.7 51.0 D L O Dr BG TV G
Duch et al,48 2013 73 21.1 21.1 38.1 Low CS SA Dr ST TV, C, M G
Hudon et al,49 2013 84 21.3 21.3 43.5 NS CS SA Q ST, ED, BG, CoV, On TV E
Lee et al,7 2017 1870 36.0 36.0 51.1 D CS SA I ST, CoV TV G
Levin et al,50 1978 60 48.0 48.0 50.0 M/H CS SA Q ST TV R
Lin et al,51 2015 150 24.8 24.8 72.0 NS CS SA Q ST TV G
Linebarger and Walker,13 2005 51 12.0 30.0 45.1 D L SA Dr ST TV E
Linebarger et al,52 2013 121 68.4 68.4 45.0 Low CS SA Dr ST, CoV TV G
Masur et al,53 2016 25 13.0 17.0 44.0 M/H L SA Q BG TV E
McKean et al,54 2015 763 48 84.0 50.5 Low L SA Q ST TV G
Mendelsohn et al,55 2010 253 6.0 14.0 46.6 Low L SA I ST, CoV TV, VG G
Moon et al,56 2018 39 48.0 48.0 53.8 NS CS SA Q ST M G
Nelson et al,57 1973 19 12.0 24.0 38.9 NS L O Dr ST, BG TV E
Pagani et al,58 2013 1999 29.0 60.0 50.0 NS L SA I ST TV R
Patterson et al,59 2002 64 23.7 23.7 50.0 NS CS SA I ST TV E
Rice et al,10 1990
Cohort 1 115 36.0 60.0 50.0 NS L SA Dr ED, CoV TV R
Cohort 2 118 60.0 84.0 50.0 NS L SA Dr ED, CoV TV R
Richert et al,60 2010 88 72.7 72.7 54.1 NS CS SA Dr ST, On TV G
Rosenqvist et al,61 2016 381 102.0 102.0 50.0 NS CS SA Q ST TV, C G
Ruangdaraganon et al,8 2009 203 6.0 24.0 53.0 NS L SA Q ST TV G
Schmidt et al,62 2009 872 6.0 36.0 49.7 D L SA Q ST TV R
Selnow and Bettinghaus,63 1982 93 48.0 48.0 54.8 M/H CS SA Dr ST, ED TV R
Taylor et al,64 2018
Cohort 1 51 12.0 12.0 46.6 NS CS SA Q ST TV, C, M, VG G
Cohort 2 39 27.5 27.5 46.6 NS CS SA Q ST TV, C, M, VG, G
Tomopoulos et al,65 2010 259 6.0 14.0 47.1 Low L SA I ST, ED TV, VG G
van den Heuvel et al,66 2019 893 18.7 18.7 54.1 D CS SA Q ST M E
Wright et al,67 2001
Cohort 1 90 24.0 60.0 50.0 D L SA I ST, ED TV R
Cohort 2 92 48.0 84.0 50.0 D L SA I ST, ED TV R
Yang et al,14 2017 119 55.7 55.7 49.0 M/H CS SA Q ST, ED, CoV, On TV, M, C R
Zimmerman et al,5 2007
Cohort 1 345 21.0 21.0 50.0 M/H CS SA I ST, ED, CoV TV G
Cohort 2 384 12.0 12.0 50.0 M/H CS SA I ST, ED, CoV TV G
Zimmerman et al,68 2009 275 21.0 21.0 49.0 M/H CS SA O ST TV G

Abbreviations: BG, background television; C, computer; CoV, co-viewing; CS, cross-sectional; D, diverse incomes; Dr, screen time diary; E, expressive language; ED, educational programming; G, general language; I, interview; L, longitudinal; M, mobile; M/H, middle/high incomes; NS, income not specified; O, observer rating; On, age at onset of screens; Q, screen time use questionnaire; R, receptive language; SA, structured assessment; SES, socioeconomic status; ST, screen time; TV, television or DVD; VG, video games.

Meta-analytic Results for Quantity of Screen Use

Duration of Screen Use

A total of 38 studies (18 313 participants) were available for this random-effects meta-analysis, which produced a significant and negative combined effect size r = −0.14 (95% CI, −0.18 to −0.10; Figure 2). Thus, greater quantity of screen use was associated with lower child language. The funnel plot inspection revealed asymmetry (eFigure 1 in the Supplement), and the Egger test suggested that studies with smaller sample sizes had more extreme effect sizes. A sensitivity analysis was conducted to determine the presence of potential outliers, and 1 study was identified. There was evidence of significant between-study heterogeneity of effect sizes with (QB = 226.58; P < .001; I2 = 83.67) and without (QB = 169.57; P < .001; I2 = 78.77) the outlying study. All moderators are reported in eTable 5 in the Supplement; none emerged as significant.

Figure 2. Forest Plots of the Effect Sizes for Each Study Included in the Meta-analyses on Quantity of Screen Use (Screen Time and Background Television) and Child Language.

Figure 2.

Contributing studies are sorted in reverse chronological order. Square data markers represent effect size estimates (r), with size of the markers corresponding to 95% CIs and diamond data markers representing the overall effect size based on included studies.

Background Television

A total of 5 studies (2792 participants) produced a pooled effect size of r = −0.19 (95% CI, −0.33 to −0.05; Figure 2). Thus, background television was associated with decreased language skills. No outliers were detected. Asymmetry was revealed in the funnel plot (eFigure 2 in the Supplement), but the Egger test did not suggest publication bias. There was evidence of effect size heterogeneity (QB = 17.00; P < .001; I2 = 76.47), but no moderators examined explained between-study variation (eTable 6 in the Supplement).

Meta-analytic Results for Quality of Screen Use

Educational Content

A total of 13 studies (1955 participants) produced a significant and positive combined effect size r = 0.13 (95% CI, 0.02-.24; Figure 3). Thus, viewing educational content was associated with increased language skills. No publication bias was detected (eFigure 3 in the Supplement). There was evidence of significant between-study heterogeneity of effect sizes with (QB = 67.12; P < .001; I2 = 82.12), and without (QB = 32.36; P < .001; I2 = 66.01) the outlying study. Moderators were explored, but none emerged as significant (eTable 7 in the Supplement).

Figure 3. Forest Plots of the Effect Sizes for Each Study Included in the Meta-analyses on Quality of Screen Use (Educational Programming and Co-viewing) and Child Language.

Figure 3.

Contributing studies are sorted in reverse chronological order. Square data markers represent effect size estimates (r), with size of the markers corresponding to 95% CIs and diamond data markers representing the overall effect size based on included studies.

Co-Viewing

A total of 12 studies (6083 participants) produced a significant and positive combined effect size r = 0.16 (95% CI, 0.07-0.24; Figure 3). No publication bias was detected (eFigure 4 in the Supplement). Two outliers were identified; however, there was evidence of significant between-study heterogeneity with (QB = 152.91; P < .001; I2 = 92.81) and without (QB = 66.21; P < .001; I2 = 86.41) the outlying studies. Results of all moderator analyses can be found in eTable 8 in the Supplement. Child sex emerged as a significant moderator (eFigure 5 in the Supplement) for every 2.5% unit increase in the percentage of boys in a sample, the effect size increased by 0.03 (95% CI, 0.01-0.05).

Meta-analytic Results for Age at Onset

A total of 4 studies (457 participants) produced a significant pooled effect size of r = 0.17 (95% CI, 0.07-0.27; Figure 4), suggesting that as the age at onset of screen use increased, stronger language skills were observed. No publication bias (eFigure 6 in the Supplement) or outliers were detected. There was no evidence of heterogeneity of effect sizes (QB = 3.36; P = .34; I2 = 10.59); thus, moderator analyses were not explored.

Figure 4. Forest Plot of the Effect Sizes for Each Study Included in the Meta-analysis on Age at Onset of Screen Exposure and Child Language.

Figure 4.

Contributing studies are sorted in reverse chronological order. Square data markers represent effect size estimates (r), with size of the markers corresponding to 95% CIs and diamond data markers representing the overall effect size based on included studies.

Discussion

To address the ongoing debate as to the risks and benefits of screen use on child language, results from this series of meta-analyses suggest that greater quantity of screen use (screen time and background television) was associated with lower child language, while better quality of screen use (educational and co-viewing) was positively associated with child language skills. Age at onset of screen use was also positively associated with language, suggesting that language benefits of screen exposure were more likely to be later vs earlier in childhood. These results are consistent with current pediatric guidelines that suggest no screen exposure prior to age 18 months and, for those older than 18 months, to limit the duration of screen exposure. Moreover, caregivers should ensure that programing is high quality and, when possible, to co-view with the child.

An important caveat prior to a fuller discussion of the meta-analytic findings is that the magnitude of the associations observed were small to moderate.37 Moreover, given the multideterministic nature of child development, these findings should be placed in the context of other important predictors of child language. For example, a 2019 meta-analysis37 demonstrated a moderate to large effect size between sensitive parenting behavior and child language (r = 0.27; 95% CI, 0.21-0.33).69 In addition, many other environmental factors have been associated with child language, for example preterm birth,70 socioeconomic status,71 and number of words spoken by caregivers.72,73 Thus, child development is multifactorial, and screen use represents one of many predictors in the child’s developmental ecology.74 Nevertheless, small effect sizes can have large public health implications, especially when the exposure, as in this case, is ubiquitous and, in principal, easy to moderate.

Greater quantity of screen use was associated with lower language skills in children. Experimental studies have demonstrated that the ability to apply information from screens to real life may be restricted in young children, known as a transfer deficit.75 Screen time viewing can also displace a variety of missed opportunities to practice developmental milestones, such as language and motor skills.9 That is, screen time may displace time spent learning, for example, to walk, talk, and draw. Moreover, screens can limit or hinder important caregiver-child interactions that are critical for enhancing child language,55,76,77 especially in vulnerable groups.70

With regard to quality of programming, the meta-analytic results suggest that educational programs may be beneficial for child language. It is important to note that the quality of educational programs varied from study to study (eTable 1 in the Supplement) and therefore, caution should be exercised in interpreting that all educational programs are beneficial to children. Nonetheless, it is possible that educational programs geared toward younger children tend to include a coherent and integrative narrative, as well as age-appropriate language, which can assist with learning.13 Educational programs that label objects, speak directly to the child, and provide opportunities to respond verbally (eg, Sesame Street and Dora the Explorer) may be particularly beneficial.13 The auditory and visual simulation of educational programs is also often appropriately paced to the child’s developmental needs, which may enhance learning.78

Exposure to co-viewing was also found to be positively associated with child language skills. Previous reports suggest that caregivers co-view approximately 50% of the time the child is watching programming (although this number may be on the decline, with solitary tablet use on the incline).79 During this time, caregivers may use co-viewing as an opportunity for linguistic interaction, such as labeling content and asking questions.80 Exposure to caregiver linguistic input, including the quantity and quality of speech,72 can promote learning for children.72,81 Caregivers may also scaffold screen content or supplement screen viewing with live interactions, which in turn can help children effectively apply learning concepts.82 It is possible that when co-viewing screens, caregivers are more attuned to the quality of programming being viewed, and therefore, children are more likely to watch programs intended for their age group, which may in turn increase comprehension and language learning.49 A greater understanding of these factors will help appropriately target screen time interventions. Also, it should be noted that emerging evidence suggests that interactive screens may diminish rather than enhance opportunities for parent-child interactions.83

The finding that boys in particular benefit from co-viewing is somewhat surprising given the preexisting sex differences in early language acquisition, with girls slightly outperforming boys in this domain at a young age.84,85 However, given this disparity, boys may receive more benefits of linguistic engagement with a caregiver when viewing screens than girls, who may have more progression in their language skills. Indeed, studies have found that when learning language, verbal encouragement and attention from mothers is particularly important for boys’ language development.86

Although better quality of screen exposure was associated with language skills, too much screen exposure, introduced too early in development, is associated with lower language skills. Thus, consistent with the pediatric guidelines, high-quality screen programming should be used in moderation and should not replace important individual or family activities and health behaviors, such as device-free family interactions, adequate sleep, book reading, and active play. Taking into account the results of this study, as well as research suggesting that excessive screen use leads to delayed learning and achievement of developmental milestones4 and is associated with behavioral difficulties, shortened or disrupted sleep, poor school readiness, and reduced physical activity,5,48,78,87,88,89,90 it is important for clinicians to review media use in the home with children and families.21 Moreover, because meta-analytic evidence suggests that merely delivering a message to reduce screen time is ineffective,91 further steps include directing caregivers to online personalized family media planning,92 encouraging parent-child co-viewing of educational programming55 to maximize its potential benefits, and supporting caregivers to prioritize face-to-face interactions with children that include stimulating exposure to language and reinforcement of communicative engagement.70

Limitations

Several limitations of this study should be noted. First, results are correlational, not causal. Second, in some cases, pooled effect sizes are based on small sample sizes, which may limit power in detecting moderator effects. Third, in all included studies, the method for measuring screen use (ie, duration, co-viewing, and/or educational) was parent report, which introduces potential bias and underreporting.93 Fourth, although we included all studies amassed to date, most studies in this meta-analysis are prior to the mass movement toward handheld devices. Co-viewing may be less likely with handheld devices compared with television1,94 and educational apps may begin to replace educational programs on television. Thus, technology is vastly outpacing research, and a clear understanding of how handheld or mobile devices are affecting child development and health is particularly needed. Finally, approximately half of the studies in our meta-analysis did not present adjusted effect sizes. It will be important in future research to include demographic covariates as well as covariates, such as cognitive stimulation, parent sensitivity, and child care quality, when evaluating the nature of the association between screen use and child outcomes.

Conclusions

Young children are growing up with increasing exposure and access to digital media and screens.1,2 Findings from this study suggest that greater quantity of screen use (ie, duration of use and background television) is associated with lower language skills. Better quality of screen exposure in older children (ie, educational and co-viewing) appears to be beneficial for child language; however, it remains that screens should continue to be used in moderation. It will be important in future research to identify which components of screen time viewing are most beneficial vs detrimental for child language (eg, interactive apps, computer use, or video streaming)83 and to examine the potential role of co-viewing, media multitasking, and household media rules on children’s outcomes.

Supplement.

eTable 1. Educational Content Description

eTable 2. Search Strategy

eTable 3. Criteria for Assessing Study Quality for All Studies Included in the Meta-analysis

eTable 4. Study Quality Scoring for Each Study Included in the Meta-analysis

eTable 5. Results of Moderator Analysis for Duration of Screen Use and Child Language

eTable 6. Results of Moderator Analysis for Background Television and Child Language

eTable 7. Results of Moderator Analysis for Educational Screen Use and Child Language

eTable 8. Results of Moderator Analysis for Co-viewing Screens and Child Language

eFigure 1. Funnel Plot of Studies Examining Quantity of Screen Use to Child Language

eFigure 2. Funnel Plot of Studies Examining Educational Programming to Child Language

eFigure 3. Funnel Plot of Studies Examining Co-viewing to Child Language

eFigure 4. Funnel Plot of Studies Examining Background Television to Child Language

eFigure 5. Meta-Regression Scatterplot Examining Child Sex on the Association between Co-Viewing and Child Language

eFigure 6. Funnel Plot of Studies Examining Age of Onset of Screen Use to Child Language

References

  • 1.Rideout V. The Common Sense Census: Media Use by kids Zero to Eight. Common Sense Media; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Radesky JS, Christakis DA. Increased screen time: implications for early childhood development and behavior. Pediatr Clin North Am. 2016;63(5):827-839. doi: 10.1016/j.pcl.2016.06.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Christakis DA, Gilkerson J, Richards JA, et al. . Audible television and decreased adult words, infant vocalizations, and conversational turns: a population-based study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(6):554-558. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.61 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Madigan S, Browne D, Racine N, Mori C, Tough S. Association between screen time and children’s performance on a developmental screening test. JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173(3):244-250. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.5056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA, Meltzoff AN. Associations between media viewing and language development in children under age 2 years. J Pediatr. 2007;151(4):364-368. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2007.04.071 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Chonchaiya W, Pruksananonda C. Television viewing associates with delayed language development. Acta Paediatr. 2008;97(7):977-982. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2008.00831.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Lee E-Y, Spence JC, Carson V. Television viewing, reading, physical activity and brain development among young South Korean children. J Sci Med Sport. 2017;20(7):672-677. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2016.11.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Ruangdaraganon N, Chuthapisith J, Mo-suwan L, Kriweradechachai S, Udomsubpayakul U, Choprapawon C. Television viewing in Thai infants and toddlers: impacts to language development and parental perceptions. BMC Pediatr. 2009;9(1):34. doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-9-34 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Christakis DA. The effects of infant media usage: what do we know and what should we learn? Acta Paediatr. 2009;98(1):8-16. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2008.01027.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Rice ML, Huston AC, Truglio R, Wright JC. Words from “Sesame Street”: Learning Vocabulary While Viewing. Vol 26 American Psychological Association; 1990:421-428. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Linn S, Poussaint AF. The truth about Teletubbies. Zero Three. 2001;22(2):24-29. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Roseberry S, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM. Skype me! socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. Child Dev. 2014;85(3):956-970. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12166 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Linebarger DL, Walker D. Infants’ and toddlers’ television viewing and language outcomes. Am Behav Sci. 2005;48(5):624-645. doi: 10.1177/0002764204271505 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Yang X, Chen Z, Wang Z, Zhu L. The Relations between television exposure and executive function in Chinese preschoolers: the moderated role of parental mediation behaviors. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1833. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01833 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Blankson AN, O’Brien M, Leerkes EM, Calkins SD, Marcovitch S. Do hours spent viewing television at ages 3 and 4 predict vocabulary and executive functioning at age 5? Merrill-Palmer Q. 2015;61(2):264-289. doi: 10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.61.2.0264 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Leaper C, Smith TE. A meta-analytic review of gender variations in children’s language use: talkativeness, affiliative speech, and assertive speech. Dev Psychol. 2004;40(6):993-1027. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.993 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Forrest CB, Glade GB, Baker AE, Bocian AB, Kang M, Starfield B. The pediatric primary-specialty care interface: how pediatricians refer children and adolescents to specialty care. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 1999;153(7):705-714. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.153.7.705 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Madigan S, Wade M, Plamondon A, Browne D, Jenkins JM. Birth weight variability and language development: risk, resilience, and responsive parenting. J Pediatr Psychol. 2015;40(9):869-877. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsv056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Stanovich KE. Matthew effects in reading: some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. J Educ. 2009;189(1-2):23-55. doi: 10.1177/0022057409189001-204 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Beitchman JH, Wilson B, Johnson CJ, et al. . Fourteen-year follow-up of speech/language-impaired and control children: psychiatric outcome. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40(1):75-82. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200101000-00019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.American Academy of Pediatrics American Academy of Pediatrics announces new recommendations for children’s media use. Published 2016. Accessed December 5, 2019. https://www.aap.org/en-us/about-the-aap/aap-press-room/Pages/American-Academy-of-Pediatrics-Announces-New-Recommendations-for-Childrens-Media-Use.aspx
  • 22.Tremblay MS, Carson V, Chaput JP. Introduction to the Canadian 24-Hour movement guidelines for children and youth: an integration of physical activity, sedentary behaviour, and sleep. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 2016;41(6)(suppl 3):iii-iv. doi: 10.1139/apnm-2016-0203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.COUNCIL ON COMMUNICATIONS AND MEDIA Media and young minds. Pediatrics. 2016;138(5):e20162591. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-2591 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Ponti M, Bélanger S, Grimes R, et al. ; Canadian Paediatric Society, Digital Health Task Force, Ottawa, Ontario . Screen time and young children: promoting health and development in a digital world. Paediatr Child Health. 2017;22(8):461-477. doi: 10.1093/pch/pxx123 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.World Health Organization Guidelines on physical activity, sedentary behaviour and sleep for children under 5 years of age. Published 2019. Accessed July 4, 2019. https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/311664 [PubMed]
  • 26.American College of Pediatricians The Impact of Media Use and Screen Time on Children, Adolescents, and Families. Published 2016. Accessed September 4, 2018. https://www.acpeds.org/the-college-speaks/position-statements/parenting-issues/the-impact-of-media-use-and-screen-time-on-children-adolescents-and-families
  • 27.Viner R; Royal College of Paediatrics and Child Health. The health impacts of screen time: a guide for clinicians and parents. Published 2019. Accessed December 5, 2019. https://www.rcpch.ac.uk/sites/default/files/2018-12/rcpch_screen_time_guide_-_final.pdf
  • 28.Straker L, Zabatiero J, Danby S, Thorpe K, Edwards S. Conflicting guidelines on young children’s screen time and use of digital technology create policy and practice dilemmas. J Pediatr. 2018;202:300-303. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2018.07.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Ferguson CJ, Beresin E. Social science’s curious war with pop culture and how it was lost: the media violence debate and the risks it holds for social science. Prev Med. 2017;99:69-76. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2017.02.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Dunn LM, Dunn LM. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. 3rd ed American Guidance Services; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Fenson L, Dale PS, Reznick JS, Bates E, Thal DJ, Pethick SJ. Variability in early communicative development. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 1994;59(5):1-173. doi: 10.2307/1166093 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.NIH National Heart Lung and Blood Institute Quality assessment tool for observational cohort and cross-sectional studies. Accessed September 03, 2019. https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools
  • 33.Bornstein MH, Hahn CS, Putnick DL. Stability of core language skill across the first decade of life in children at biological and social risk. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2016;57(12):1434-1443. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12632 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Putnick DL, Bornstein MH, Eryigit-Madzwamuse S, Wolke D. Long-term stability of language performance in very preterm, moderate-late preterm, and term children. J Pediatr. 2017;181:74-79.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.09.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Comprehensive meta-analysis: a computer program for research synthesis. Version 3.0. Computer program. Biostat; 2005.
  • 36.Rosenthal R. Writing meta-analytic reviews. Psychol Bull. 1995;118(2):183. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.118.2.183 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Funder DC, Ozer DJ. Evaluating effect size in psychological research: sense and nonsense. Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci. 2019;2(2):156-168. doi: 10.1177/2515245919847202 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JPT, Rothstein HR. Introduction to Meta-Analysis. John Wiley & Sons; 2009. doi: 10.1002/9780470743386 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 25.0. IBM Corp; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Thompson SG, Higgins JPT. How should meta-regression analyses be undertaken and interpreted? Stat Med. 2002;21(11):1559-1573. doi: 10.1002/sim.1187 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Allen L, Cipielewski J, Stanovich KE. Multiple indicators of children’s reading habits and attitudes: Construct validity and cognitive correlates. J Educ Psychol. 1992;84(4):489-503. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.84.4.489 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Alloway TP, Williams S, Jones B, Cochrane F. Exploring the impact of television watching on vocabulary skills in toddlers. Early Child Educ J. 2014;42(5):343-349. doi: 10.1007/s10643-013-0618-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Arraf S. An analysis of the effects of television viewing patterns, IQ, SES and gender on receptive and expressive language development of preschool children. Dissertation Abstracts International. 1991;52(2-A):420. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Barr R, Lauricella A, Zack E, Calvert SL. Infant and early childhood exposure to adult-directed and child-directed television programming: relations with cognitive skills at age four. Merrill-Palmer Q. 2010;56(1):21-48. doi: 10.1353/mpq.0.0038 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Bittman M, Rutherford L, Brown J, Unsworth L. Digital natives? new and old media and children’s outcomes. Aust J Educ. 2011;55(2):161-175. doi: 10.1177/000494411105500206 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Byeon H, Hong S. Relationship between television viewing and language delay in toddlers: evidence from a Korea national cross-sectional survey. PLoS One. 2015;10(3):e0120663. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0120663 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Castles A, McLean GMT, Bavin E, et al. . Computer use and letter knowledge in pre-school children: a population-based study. J Paediatr Child Health. 2013;49(3):193-198. doi: 10.1111/jpc.12126 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Duch H, Fisher EM, Ensari I, et al. . Association of screen time use and language development in Hispanic toddlers: a cross-sectional and longitudinal study. Clin Pediatr (Phila). 2013;52(9):857-865. doi: 10.1177/0009922813492881 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Hudon TM, Fennell CT, Hoftyzer M. Quality not quantity of television viewing is associated with bilingual toddlers’ vocabulary scores. Infant Behav Dev. 2013;36(2):245-254. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.01.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Levin SR. Preschool individual differences and patterns of television viewing. Dissertation Abstracts International. 1978;39(1-B):444-445. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Lin L-Y, Cherng R-J, Chen Y-J, Chen Y-J, Yang H-M. Effects of television exposure on developmental skills among young children. Infant Behav Dev. 2015;38:20-26. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.12.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Linebarger DL, Moses A, Garrity Liebeskind K, McMenamin K. Learning vocabulary from television: does onscreen print have a role? J Educ Psychol. 2013;105(3):609-621. doi: 10.1037/a0032582 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Masur EF, Flynn V, Olson J. Infants’ background television exposure during play: negative relations to the quantity and quality of mothers’ speech and infants’ vocabulary acquisition. First Lang. 2016;36(2):109-123. doi: 10.1177/0142723716639499 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.McKean C, Mensah FK, Eadie P, et al. . Levers for language growth: characteristics and predictors of language trajectories between 4 and 7 years. PLoS One. 2015;10(8):e0134251. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134251 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Mendelsohn AL, Berkule SB, Tomopoulos S, et al. . Infant television and video exposure associated with limited parent-child verbal interactions in low socioeconomic status households. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2008;162(5):411-417. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.162.5.411 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Moon JH, Cho SY, Lim SM, et al. . Smart device usage in early childhood is differentially associated with fine motor and language development. Acta Paediatr. 2019;108(5):903-910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Nelson K. Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 1973;38(1/2):1-135. doi: 10.2307/1165788 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Pagani LS, Fitzpatrick C, Barnett TA. Early childhood television viewing and kindergarten entry readiness. Pediatr Res. 2013;74(3):350-355. doi: 10.1038/pr.2013.105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Patterson JL. Relationships of expressive vocabulary to frequency of reading and television experience among bilingual toddlers. Appl Psycholinguist. 2002;23(4):493-508. doi: 10.1017/S0142716402004010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Richert RA, Robb MB, Fender JG, Wartella E. Word learning from baby videos. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010;164(5):432-437. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.24 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Rosenqvist J, Lahti-Nuuttila P, Holdnack J, Kemp SL, Laasonen M. Relationship of TV watching, computer use, and reading to children’s neurocognitive functions. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2016;46:11-21. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2016.04.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Schmidt ME, Rich M, Rifas-Shiman SL, Oken E, Taveras EM. Television viewing in infancy and child cognition at 3 years of age in a US cohort. Pediatrics. 2009;123(3):e370-e375. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-3221 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Selnow G, Bettingaus E. Television exposure and language development. J Broadcast Electron Media. 1982;26(1):469-479. doi: 10.1080/08838158209364014 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Taylor G, Monaghan P, Westermann G. Investigating the association between children’s screen media exposure and vocabulary size in the UK. J Child Media. 2018;12(1):51-65. doi: 10.1080/17482798.2017.1365737 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Tomopoulos S, Dreyer BP, Berkule S, Fierman AH, Brockmeyer C, Mendelsohn AL. Infant media exposure and toddler development. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010;164(12):1105-1111. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.235 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.van den Heuvel M, Ma J, Borkhoff CM, et al. ; TARGet Kids! Collaboration . Mobile media device use is associated with expressive language delay in 18-month-old children. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2019;40(2):99-104. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000630 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Wright JC, Huston AC, Murphy KC, et al. . The relations of early television viewing to school readiness and vocabulary of children from low-income families: the early window project. Child Dev. 2001;72(5):1347-1366. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00352 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Zimmerman FJ, Gilkerson J, Richards JA, et al. . Teaching by listening: the importance of adult-child conversations to language development. Pediatrics. 2009;124(1):342-349. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-2267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Madigan S, Prime H, Graham SA, et al. . Parenting behavior and child language: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2019;144(4):e20183556. doi: 10.1542/peds.2018-3556 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Madigan S, Wade M, Plamondon A, Browne D, Jenkins JM. Birth weight variability and language development: risk, resilience, and responsive parenting. J Pediatr Psychol. 2015;40(9):869-877. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsv056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Hart B, Risley TR. Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. Paul H Brookes Publishing; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Hoff-Ginsberg E, Shatz M. Linguistic input and the child’s acquisition of language. Psychol Bull. 1982;92(1):3-26. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.92.1.3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Wade M, Jenkins JM, Venkadasalam VP, Binnoon-Erez N, Ganea PA. The role of maternal responsiveness and linguistic input in pre-academic skill development: A longitudinal analysis of pathways. Cogn Dev. 2018;45:125-140. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.01.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Browne D, Thompson D, Madigan S. Digital media use in children: clinical versus scientific responsibilities. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(2):111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Barr R, Hayne H. Developmental changes in imitation from television during infancy. Child Dev. 1999;70(5):1067-1081. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00079 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Radesky J, Christakis D, Hill D, et al. ; COUNCIL ON COMMUNICATIONS AND MEDIA . Media and young minds. Pediatrics. 2016;138(5):e20162591. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-2591 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Radesky JS, Schumacher J, Zuckerman B. Mobile and interactive media use by young children: the good, the bad, and the unknown. Pediatrics. 2015;135(1):1-3. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-2251 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Christakis DA, Ramirez JSB, Ferguson SM, Ravinder S, Ramirez J-M. How early media exposure may affect cognitive function: a review of results from observations in humans and experiments in mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2018;115(40):9851-9858. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1711548115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Rideout V, Hamel E. The Media Family: Electronic Media in the Lives of Infants, Toddlers, Preschoolers and Their Parents. Kaiser Family Foundation; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Barr R, Zack E, Garcia A, Muentener P. Infants’ attention and responsiveness to television increases with prior exposure and parental interaction. Infancy. 2008;13(1):30-56. doi: 10.1080/15250000701779378 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Gilkerson J, Richards JA, Warren SF, Oller DK, Russo R, Vohr B. Language experience in the second year of life and language outcomes in late childhood. Pediatrics. 2018;142(4):e20174276. doi: 10.1542/peds.2017-4276 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Roseberry S, Hirsh-Pasek K, Parish-Morris J, Golinkoff RM. Live action: can young children learn verbs from video? Child Dev. 2009;80(5):1360-1375. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01338.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Munzer TG, Miller AL, Weeks HM, Kaciroti N, Radesky J. Parent-toddler social reciprocity during reading from electronic tablets vs print books. JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173(11). doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.3480 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Bornstein MH, Hahn C-S, Haynes OM. Specific and general language performance across early childhood: Stability and gender considerations. First Lang. 2004;24(3):267-304. doi: 10.1177/0142723704045681 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Eriksson M, Marschik PB, Tulviste T, et al. . Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: evidence from 10 language communities. Br J Dev Psychol. 2012;30(pt 2):326-343. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02042.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Karrass J, Braungart-Rieker JM, Mullins J, Lefever JB. Processes in language acquisition: the roles of gender, attention, and maternal encouragement of attention over time. J Child Lang. 2002;29(3):519-543. doi: 10.1017/S0305000902005196 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Hancox RJ, Milne BJ, Poulton R. Association between child and adolescent television viewing and adult health: a longitudinal birth cohort study. Lancet. 2004;364(9430):257-262. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(04)16675-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Przybylski AK, Weinstein N. Digital screen time limits and young children’s psychological well-being: evidence from a population-based study. Child Dev. 2019;90(1):e56-e65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA. Children’s television viewing and cognitive outcomes: a longitudinal analysis of national data. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2005;159(7):619-625. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.159.7.619 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Paavonen EJ, Pennonen M, Roine M, Valkonen S, Lahikainen AR. TV exposure associated with sleep disturbances in 5- to 6-year-old children. J Sleep Res. 2006;15(2):154-161. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00525.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Wahi G, Parkin PC, Beyene J, Uleryk EM, Birken CS. Effectiveness of interventions aimed at reducing screen time in children: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2011;165(11):979-986. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.122 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.American Academy of Pediatrics Family Media Plan. Accessed July 8, 2019. https://www.healthychildren.org/English/media/Pages/default.aspx?gclid=EAIaIQobChMIoq2F-eiA3QIVUFuGCh3e0gDnEAAYBCAAEgJqNPD_BwE
  • 93.Yuan N, Weeks HM, Ball R, Newman MW, Chang Y-J, Radesky JS. How much do parents actually use their smartphones? pilot study comparing self-report to passive sensing. Pediatr Res. 2019;86(4):416-418. doi: 10.1038/s41390-019-0452-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Domoff SE, Radesky JS, Harrison K, Riley H, Lumeng JC, Miller AL. A naturalistic study of child and family screen media and mobile device use. J Child Fam Stud. 2019;28(2):401-410. doi: 10.1007/s10826-018-1275-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplement.

eTable 1. Educational Content Description

eTable 2. Search Strategy

eTable 3. Criteria for Assessing Study Quality for All Studies Included in the Meta-analysis

eTable 4. Study Quality Scoring for Each Study Included in the Meta-analysis

eTable 5. Results of Moderator Analysis for Duration of Screen Use and Child Language

eTable 6. Results of Moderator Analysis for Background Television and Child Language

eTable 7. Results of Moderator Analysis for Educational Screen Use and Child Language

eTable 8. Results of Moderator Analysis for Co-viewing Screens and Child Language

eFigure 1. Funnel Plot of Studies Examining Quantity of Screen Use to Child Language

eFigure 2. Funnel Plot of Studies Examining Educational Programming to Child Language

eFigure 3. Funnel Plot of Studies Examining Co-viewing to Child Language

eFigure 4. Funnel Plot of Studies Examining Background Television to Child Language

eFigure 5. Meta-Regression Scatterplot Examining Child Sex on the Association between Co-Viewing and Child Language

eFigure 6. Funnel Plot of Studies Examining Age of Onset of Screen Use to Child Language


Articles from JAMA Pediatrics are provided here courtesy of American Medical Association

RESOURCES