Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Health Psychol. 2017 Jun 1;24(11):1536–1547. doi: 10.1177/1359105317710815

Body image in adult women: Associations with health behaviors, quality of life, and functional impairment

Carolyn B Becker 1, Christina L Verzijl 1, Lisa S Kilpela 2, Salome A Wilfred 1, Tiffany Stewart 3
PMCID: PMC7091492  NIHMSID: NIHMS1573777  PMID: 28810463

Abstract

This study examined body image in adult women along with probable correlates including health behaviors, negative affect, quality of life, and functional impairment. Adult women (N = 738, age = 25–86 years) completed an online survey assessing these domains. Women across all ages reported similar body image concerns. Negative body image significantly correlated with poorer wellness behaviors, negative affect, quality of life, and functional impairment. The inverse held for positive body image. The majority of correlations remained even when controlling for negative affect. Further research is warranted to investigate nature of the correlational relationships identified in this study.

Keywords: age, body image, health behavior, quality of life, women’s health


Substantial research indicates that negative body image (BI) is associated with numerous undesirable outcomes in adolescent girls and young adult women, including eating disorders, depression, unhealthy weight control behaviors, smoking, reduced physical activity, and reduced health-related quality of life (QOL) (e.g. Barker and Galambos, 2007; Clark et al., 2005; Neumark-Sztainer et al., 2006; Stice et al., 2000; Wilson et al., 2013). Research also indicates that BI concerns extend beyond the early stages of life for girls/women (e.g. Slevec and Tiggemann, 2011; Tiggemann, 2004; Webster and Tiggemann, 2003). For instance, in a sample of over 1800 women aged 50+ years, Gagne et al. (2012) found that 71 percent of women reported dissatisfaction with weight.

With regard to BI correlates, the literature on adult woman lags behind that for younger girls/women, despite indications that BI may be more complex later in life secondary to age-related body changes (e.g. menopause and weight gain), increased rates of disease (e.g. cancer), and pregnancy (Kilpela et al., 2015). Some research areas, however, are more underrepresented than others. For instance, whereas there is a moderately substantial and growing literature for adult women on the association between BI and aging anxiety, self-objectification, and eating disorders (Kilpela et al., 2015; Slevec and Tiggemann, 2011), relatively few studies have investigated the association between BI and health/wellness behaviors (Kilpela et al., 2015).

More specifically, four studies have investigated BI and smoking behavior in adult women, each of which largely supported a relationship (Ben-Tovim and Walker, 1991; King et al., 2000, 2005; Perkins et al., 2001). Three studies also support an association between exercise motivation and BI (Liechty et al., 2006; Prichard and Tiggemann, 2008; Segar et al., 2006). With regard to functional impairment, Muennig et al. (2008) found that ideal versus actual weight discrepancy better predicted unhealthy days over the past month as compared to body mass index (BMI) in women aged 18–65+years.

For QOL, Mond et al. (2013) examined the association of adult women’s BI and QOL in a very large sample (N = 5255). Unfortunately, Mond et al. assessed BI using only two questions, which is less than optimal assessment, and the age range only extended to 42 years. Results indicated that mental health QOL, psychosocial functioning, and some aspects of physical health functioning decreased as negative BI increased. More recently, Van Zutven et al. (2015) found that weight/shape concerns correlated with general psychological distress and worsened life satisfaction in women aged 20–44 years. This study assessed BI using a more comprehensive measure of negative BI.

Importantly, none of these studies examined the association of positive BI with QOL and functional impairment. This is a limitation. Although positive BI inversely correlates with negative BI, positive BI is not a mirror image of negative BI, but a separate construct that includes body appreciation, body acceptance, and adaptive appearance investment (Tylka and Wood-Barcalow, 2015). As such, it is important to examine whether positive BI has different relationships with health-related QOL and functional impairment compared to negative BI.

The primary aim of this study was to examine the association of BI in adult women (age=25–86 years) along with potential correlates drawn from two interrelated areas: health behaviors and QOL/functional impairment. For health behaviors, based on Kilpela et al. (2015), this study assessed self-reported consumption of nutrient-dense food, sleep, and exercise. We also investigated whether negative affect accounted for any identified relationships between BI and health behaviors. With regard to QOL, this study investigated four domains of health-related QOL, as well as self-reported functional impairment directly associated with BI concerns. We hypothesized that negative BI would be associated with worse outcomes on all health/wellness outcomes and worse health-related QOL and functional impairment. We hypothesized the inverse for positive BI.

In a recent review regarding prevalence of negative BI in the US adults, Fiske et al. (2014) identified several shortcomings. These limitations included the following: (1) reports of means and standard deviations (SDs) without sample prevalence (i.e. frequencies); (2) frequent use of single-item measures, which lack psychometric rigor, and/or use of only one BI measure even though BI is multi-dimensional; and (3) lack of sample prevalence across diverse subgroups, such as age and ethnicity. Thus, a secondary aim was to begin to address these limitations by including frequencies, using multiple BI measures, and reporting sample prevalence across different age groups. Frequencies, which are easier to understand than means, are important for (1) documenting the degree to which a problem is normative enough to be considered a potential public health problem and (2) communicating with community stakeholders, including policy makers (Becker, 2017; Fiske et al., 2014). Thus, it is important to begin to document frequencies of participants scoring at different levels of different BI measures.

A final aim was to respond to increasing recognition (Becker, 2017; Brownell and Roberto, 2015) that standard academic approaches to asking questions and presenting results (e.g., means, correlations) appear insufficient for engaging clinicians and stakeholders in translating research to meaningful day-to-day clinical impact. Becker called for increased reporting of statistics that can be used to convey scientific findings in easy-to-understand or “digestible” formats that facilitate better communication with stakeholders about both the nature of the problems clinical researchers study and interventions effects. Thus, we sought to provide digestible information about the self-reported functional impact of negative BI that may help non-researchers understand and communicate regarding the suffering that negative BI can cause.

Methods

Participants

Participants included 738 women recruited via snowball sampling with a mean BMI of 25.43 (SD = 6.31). Participants ages ranged from 25 to 86 (M = 41.17, SD = 12.58) years. Regarding race/ethnicity, 89.3 percent endorsed Caucasian, 8.8 percent Hispanic, 2.4 percent Asian, 2.3 percent Black, 2.0 percent American Indian/Alaska native, and 3.4 percent endorsed multiple races. The sample was highly educated with 84.6 percent reporting at least a bachelor’s degree.

Procedure

The study received Institutional Review Board’s approval. The study team recruited women of 25 years or older with recruitment scripts distributed via personal and professional networks through email, social networking sites (e.g. Facebook), and word of mouth. We also recruited via social media pages dedicated to women’s fitness, BI, and wellness (e.g. Black Fitness Women). All communications requested that women forward the invitation to their own networks.

The study was described as exploring adult women’s attitudes and feelings about their bodies in relation to health and wellness. After providing informed consent, 774 participants completed self-report questionnaires online. Upon completion, participants could provide email addresses to enter a lottery for a US$200 Amazon gift card. We removed 36 participants from analyses due to (1) identified age below 25 years (n = 33) or (2) missing age data (n = 3).

Measures

Demographic information.

Participants reported age, height, weight, race/ethnicity, and highest level of education. Although gathering participants’ height and weight data via self-report is not optimal, due to the survey nature of the study, we were unable to collect these objectively.

BI.

Because BI is a multi-dimensional construct, we operationalized BI using three measures. The Body Shape Questionnaire (BSQ; Cooper et al., 1987) assessed shape/weight concerns. We used an 8-item version (BSQ-8D; Evans and Dolan, 1993) to reduce participant burden. The BSQ uses a 6-point Likert scale (1 = Never, 6 = Always). Scores are summed; higher scores indicate greater concerns. A sample question is “have you been so worried about your shape that you have been feeling you ought to diet?” Research supports the BSQ’s reliability and validity (Rosen et al., 1996) and indicates that shortened versions have similar convergent and discriminant validity (Evans and Dolan, 1993). We replaced two questions from BSQ-8D (e.g. have you imagined cutting off fleshy areas of your body) with items from the BSQ-8B (e.g. have you avoided wearing clothes which make you particularly aware of the shape of your body?) to target less pathological BI concerns. Internal consistency for the modified BSQ-8D sample was excellent (Cronbach’s α = .92).

We also administered the Body Parts Satisfaction Scale–Revised (BPSS-R; Petrie et al., 2002). BPSS-R uses a 7-point Likert scale (1 = extremely dissatisfied, 7 = extremely satisfied) and asks participants to rate 14 body parts (e.g. weight, shoulders, and stomach) plus general muscle tone and overall body size/shape. Items are averaged; higher scores indicate greater satisfaction. The BPSS-R differs from other BI measures by focusing on (dissatisfaction with specific body parts versus attitudes/behaviors. Research supports construct and concurrent validity (Petrie et al., 2002); internal consistency with this sample was high (α = .92).

The 13-item Body Appreciation Scale (BAS; Avalos et al., 2005) assessed four aspects of positive BI: favorable opinions of one’s body, body acceptance despite imperfections, respect for the body by attending to its needs, and protection of the body by rejecting media ideals. Items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = never, 5 = always). Sample items include “my feelings towards my body are positive for the most part.” Responses are averaged; higher scores indicate greater body appreciation. Research supports BAS psychometric properties (Avalos et al., 2005), which evidenced excellent internal consistency in this sample (α = .95).

Wellness behaviors.

The 19-item Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI; Buysse et al., 1989) assesses past-month self-reported sleep. The PSQI includes seven component sleep scores: quality, latency, duration, efficiency, disturbances, use of sleep medication, and daytime dysfunction. A global score is computed by summing the seven scores; lower scores indicate better sleep. The PSQI has demonstrated internal consistency and reliability (α = .83; Buysse et al., 1989). We found good internal consistency (α = .83).

The International Physical Activity Questionnaire–Short Form (IPAQ-SF; Patterson, 2002) assesses physical activity over the past 7 days. The IPAQ-SF measures days and duration of walking, moderate physical activity, vigorous physical activity, and sitting. Continuous MET (METs are multiples of the resting metabolic rate) scores are calculated for the three activity subscales by multiplying the appropriate MET number by duration per day and days per week. A total score is computed by summing MET walking, moderate, and vigorous. IPAQ-SF reliability is acceptable (Craig et al., 2003).

The 8-item Physical Activity Enjoyment Scale (PACES; Mullen et al., 2011) assessed positive affect associated with physical activity (e.g. “when I am physically active I find it pleasurable”). Items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = Disagree a lot, 5 = Agree a lot). Scores are summed; higher scores indicate greater physical activity enjoyment. Internal consistency in this sample was high (α = .96).

Two items measured self-reported consumption of fruits, vegetables, and nutrient-dense foods. Item-1 asked how often participants consciously tried to increase nutrient density in meals over the past week. Item-2 asked how often participants ate fresh fruits and vegetables over the past week. Items were rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = consume at every meal, 5 = never). We summed individual items; higher scores indicate less attempts to eat nutritionally dense foods.

Negative affect.

We used 17 items comprising the fear, guilt, and sadness subscales of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule Expanded (PANAS; Watson and Clark, 1999; Watson et al., 1988) to assess negative affect. Items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = very slightly or not at all, 5 = extremely) to measure negative affect over the past 3 weeks. Higher total score indicates greater experience of negative emotions. Research supports construct validity (Watson and Clark, 1999), with high internal consistency with the current sample (α = .97).

QOL and engagement in key life activities.

The 26-item short version of the World Health Organization Quality of Life Scale (WHOQOL-BREF; Skevington et al., 2004) measures overall QOL within four domains over the last 4 weeks: physical health (e.g. activities of living), psychological health (e.g. self-esteem), social relationships (e.g. social support), and environment (e.g. financial resources) using a 5-point Likert scale. Subscales are summed for a raw score, which is transformed by subtracting the lowest possible raw score from the actual raw score and dividing by possible raw score range. This score is then multiplied by 100 to obtain a final score; higher scores indicate better QOL. Research supports the internal consistency of the subscales (Skevington et al., 2004). Internal consistency in this sample was good (α: physical health = .81, psychological=.89, environment = .82, social relationships = .76).

The 16-item Clinical Impairment Assessment Questionnaire (CIAQ; Bohn et al., 2008) assesses impairment in four domains of life (mood/self-perception, cognitive functioning, interpersonal functioning, and work performance) secondary to eating, exercise, and weight and shape concerns over the past 28 days (e.g. “over the past 28 days, to what extent have your eating habits, exercising, or feelings about your eating, shape or weight stopped you from going out with others?”). Items are rated on a 4-point Likert scale (1 = not at all, 4 = a lot) and are summed; higher scores indicate greater impairment. We excluded eating and exercise from the question stem to focus on shape/weight concerns (i.e. “… to what extent have your feelings about your shape and weight). Research supports CIAQ internal consistency and construct validity (Bohn et al., 2008). The modified version demonstrated high internal consistency (α = .96).

Results

Because our sample consisted of a wide age range, we divided participants into four groups, consistent with past research (Becker et al., 2013). We created groups with the following considerations: (1) ages that clustered in terms of life stages and (2) a manageable number of groups with reasonably large sample sizes. We consulted the lifespan literature (Levinson, 1985) and a life-span developmental psychologist in creating the groups. Groups were as follows: 25–30 years; 31–45 years; 46–60 years; and 61 years and older. Analyses of variance (ANOVAs) indicated that BMI differed across groups (F(3, 665) = 6.49, p = .0001, ηp2=.028). Post hoc analyses indicated that the youngest group reported a lower mean BMI (M = 23.83) than other groups (M range = 25.56–26.83), which did not differ from one another. Thus, we co-varied for BMI in subsequent age group analyses.

BI across age groups

Four univariate analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) for BMI indicated that age groups significantly differed on mean BAS (F(3, 664)=8.45, p=.0001, ηp2=.037) but not BPSS-R (F(3, 664)=1.53, p=.205, ηp2=.007) or BSQ scores (F(3,664)=1.06, p=.366, ηp2=.005) (Table 1). Follow-up paired t-tests indicated that the oldest group showed greater body appreciation (BAS) compared to each of the other groups; the younger three groups did not differ from one another.

Table 1.

Means, standard deviations, and frequencies of shape/weight concerns, body satisfaction, and body appreciation by age.

Shape and weight concerns (BSQ)
M (SD) None (%) Mild (%) Moderate (%) Marked (%)
Total sample 26.87 (9.75) 20.3 28.6 23.3 27.8
25–30 26.41 (9.98) 23.4 26.0 25.0 25.5
31–45 26.76 (10.03) 21.8 28 22.5 27.7
46–60 27.91 (9.10) 13.3 31.3 24.1 31.3
61+ 25.53 (9.61) 25.8 30.6 19.4 24.2
Body dissatisfaction (BPSS-R)
M (SD) Extremely satisfied/satisfied (%) Somewhat satisfied (%) Neutral (%) Somewhat dissatisfied (%) Extremely dissatisfied/dissatisfied (%)

Total sample 4.29 (1.11) 17.5 26.0 30.2 21.7 4.6
25–30 4.30 (1.16) 19.3 24.5 27.1 24 5.2
31–45 4.27 (1.11) 15.9 28.7 29.4 28.7 5.9
46–60 4.27 (1.09) 16.4 24.1 34.4 22.6 2.6
61+ 4.45 (1.03) 22.6 24.2 30.6 19.4 9.7
Body appreciation (BAS)
M (SD) Always (%) Often (%) Sometimes (%) Seldom (%) Never (%)
Total sample 3.48 (0.84) 11.4 40.7 34.4 11.8 1.8
25–30 3.37a (0.86)   8.9 37.5 38.5 l2 3.1
31–45 3.44a (0.84) 10.4 40.8 32.9 14.2 1.7
46–60 3.52a (0.78) 10.8 41.0 37.4 9.7 1.0
61+ 3.86b (0.83) 25.8 48.4 19.4 6.5 0.0

BSQ: Body Shape Questionnaire; BPSS-R: Body Parts Satisfaction Scale–Revised; BAS: Body Appreciation Scale; SD: standard deviation.

Subgroupings with different superscript differ significantly at p < .02. Subgroupings with the same superscripts did not differ. Higher scores indicate higher body satisfaction for BPSS-R and BAS, whereas lower scores indicate better body image on BSQ. Total N = 738; age = 25–30 years, n = 192; age = 31–45 years, n = 289; age = 45–60 years, n = 195; age = 61+ years, n = 62. Frequency scores on the left side of the table represent better body image relative to the right.

Responding to Fiske et al.’s (2014) call for sample prevalences for BI using multiple measures in different groups, we categorized BI concerns for each measure. We utilized existing short-form cutoffs to create BSQ categories (no concern = <19, mild concern = 19–25; moderate concern = 26–33; marked concern = >33; http://www.psyctc.org/tools/bsq/; Table 1). Approximately 50 percent of the total sample reported moderate or marked BI concerns, and this held for all groups with a slight decrease for the oldest group.

We could not identify established cutoffs for the BPSS-R or the BAS. Consistent with Mond et al. (2013), we used the actual item labels (e.g. extremely dissatisfied to extremely satisfied) to categorize participants using their average BPSS score. We created five groups: extremely dissatisfied/dissatisfied, somewhat dissatisfied, neutral, somewhat satisfied, and extremely satisfied/satisfied. We collapsed the ends of the 7-point scale for two reasons. First, few participants scored in the extreme ranges. Second, the other two measures yielded four and five groups; thus, a five-group breakdown for the BPSS-R makes it easier to compare across measures. We took a similar approach for the BAS such that participants whose average indicated that they “always” appreciated their body were placed in the “always” category and likewise for “often,” “sometimes,” “seldom,” and “never” (see Table 1). For the BPSS-R, approximately 25 percent of the sample reported being at least somewhat dissatisfied with their bodies. On the flip side, 40 percent or more of the total sample and each age group reported being at least somewhat satisfied. For the BAS, a similar pattern emerged. Whereas 14 percent of the total sample (7%–15% among the age groups) reported seldom or never engaging in body appreciation, 52 percent of the total sample (46%–73% in the age groups) endorsed often or always engaging in body appreciation.

Association between BI and indices of health and functioning

As noted above, the oldest age group differed from the younger three groups on the BAS (i.e. 17% of pairwise comparisons), but no other differences emerged. To simplify analyses, we collapsed age groups for subsequent correlational analyses. Of note, exploratory correlational analyses were largely the same for the oldest group as compared to the overall sample, thus supporting the decision to collapse groups.

Correlations indicated that the three BI measures were highly correlated (absolute value r range = . 710 to .774; all ps < .0001). Consistent with hypotheses, results indicated that increased BSQ scores correlated with worse scores on all health/wellness variables except physical activity level (Table 2). An identical pattern emerged for body dissatisfaction, although because of the BPSS-R scoring direction, actual correlations are inverse compared to the BSQ. Also consistent with hypotheses, the BAS was inversely correlated with worse scores on all health/wellness variables with the exception of physical activity.

Table 2.

Correlations between body image and indices of wellness/health and functioning.

Total sample
Controlling for negative affect
BSQ BPSS-R BAS BSQ BPSS-R BAS
Measures for which higher scores = increased dysfunction
PSQI .316*** −.337*** −.341*** .154*** −.170*** −.159***
Nutrition .179*** −.284*** −.299*** .072 −.200*** −.186***
Negative affect .520*** −.547*** −.619***
Impairment in functioning .688*** −.630*** −.746** .513*** −.389*** −.526***
Measures for which higher scores = decreased dysfunction
Total exercise    .024 .057 .043    .027 .077* .050
Enjoy: physical active −.191*** .349*** .341*** −.053 .253*** .217***
Physical health QOL −.363*** .497*** .469*** −.089* .280*** .198***
Psychological QOL −.544*** .639*** .749*** .256*** .398*** .534***
Environment QOL −.314*** .420*** .424*** −.004 1.52*** .112**
Relationship QOL −.301*** .420*** .408*** −.047 .205*** .179***

BSQ: Body Shape Questionnaire; BPSS-R: Body Parts Satisfaction Scale–Revised; BAS: Body Appreciation Scale; QOL: quality of life.

BSQ—higher scores indicate greater body shape/weight concerns; BAS and BPSS-R—higher scores indicate greater body appreciation/satisfaction.

*

p < .05;

**

p < .01;

***

p < .001.

Because there is a well-recognized bi-directional relationship between negative BI and negative affect in younger women (Stice et al., 2000; Wertheim et al., 2001), we conducted partial correlations controlling for negative affect (Table 2). Results indicated that all correlations remained significant for the BPSS-R and BAS. For the BSQ, the correlations with sleep, impairment in functioning, physical health, and psychological health QOL remained significant. These results suggest that the association of BI with health and wellness variables, health-related QOL, and functional impairment is not merely due to negative affect.

Functional impairment and distress frequencies

To provide easy-to-understand statistics about the self-reported functional impact of negative BI, we report frequencies for specific types of functional impact secondary to BI concerns using the CIAQ, which directly targets the functional outcomes of BI concerns. Because the BSQ targets weight/shape concerns, which best matches CIAQ language, and because the BSQ has established cutoffs, we used the BSQ instead of the BPSS-R or BAS to categorize participants.

Before running frequencies, we confirmed that overall CIAQ scores differed between BSQ groups controlling for negative affect (F(3, 733) = 101.88, p = .0001, ηp2=.294). Next, we explored which CIAQ items differed significantly between groups controlling for negative affect. Results indicated that all items differed across groups at p < .005. Thus, we concluded it was reasonable to provide descriptive statistics for individual items with the aim of providing a more digestible picture of self-reported BI-related functional impairment that could be used to communicate with stakeholders (see Table 3).

Table 3.

Frequencies of responses to individual items on the CIAQ by BSQ groups.

Over the past 28 days, to what extent have your feelings about your shape or weight …
Shape and weight concern Not at all (%) A little (%) Quite a bit or a lot (%)
Made it difficult to concentrate None/mild 88 11   1
Moderate/marked 52 34 14
Made you feel critical of yourself None/mild 31 58 11
Moderate/marked 3 30 67
Stopped you going out with others None/mild 93   6   1
Moderate/marked 62 24 14
Affected your work performance None/mild 96   3   1
Moderate/marked 81 12   7
Made you forgetful None/mild 96   4   0
Moderate/marked 75 17   8
Affected your ability to make everyday decisions None/mild 96   3   1
Moderate/marked 74 15 11
Interfered with meals with family/friends None/mild 89 10   2
Moderate/marked 60 22 17
Made you upset None/mild 61 37   2
Moderate/marked 17 40 43
Made you feel ashamed None/mild 74 24   2
Moderate/marked 20 39 41
Made it difficult to eat out with others None/mild 93   6   1
Moderate/marked 63 19 18
Made you feel guilty None/mild 70 26   4
Moderate/marked 20 37 43
Interfered with doing things you used to enjoy None/mild 91   8   1
Moderate/marked 52 24 24
Made you absent minded None/mild 97   3   0
Moderate/marked 76 15   9
Made you feel a failure None/mild 88 10   2
Moderate/marked 36 30 33
Interfered with your relationships with others None/mild 88 11   1
Moderate/marked 49 29 22
Made you worry None/mild 69 28   3
Moderate/marked 24 35 41

CIAQ: Clinical Impairment Assessment Questionnaire; BSQ: Body Shape Questionnaire.

BSQ groups collapsed from 4 (none, mild, moderate, marked) to 2 (none to mild, moderate to marked); ratings of “quite a bit” and “a lot” collapsed to one ranking.

Discussion

To date, limited research has explored the association between BI and health-related correlates in adult women (Kilpela et al., 2015). Significant limitations also exist in the basic literature aimed at documenting BI in adult women, and much of what exists is not easy for potential stakeholders to understand. This study sought to begin to address these areas.

With respect to prevalence of women’s BI concerns across the lifespan, results suggest few age differences. We found no BSQ or BPSS-R differences between age cohorts. Furthermore, only the oldest cohort (61+ years) reported a significant difference with respect to positive BI, despite the fact that this same group reported the highest level (i.e. almost 10%) of extreme body dissatisfaction. This seemingly contradictory finding is actually consistent with past research and may indicate that even as women continue to feel dissatisfied with their bodies, they are able to increasingly appreciate and respect their bodies with aging (Tiggemann and McCourt, 2013). Overall, however, adult women at different stages in life report relatively similar BI concerns.

With respect to sample prevalence of positive and negative BI, approximately 50 percent of participants reported at least moderate shape/weight concerns (BSQ). In contrast, only a quarter reported being at least somewhat dissatisfied with their bodies (BPSS-R), and less than 20 percent scored low on body appreciation (BAS). These results highlight the importance of assessing BI with multiple measures. Although measures correlated highly, when results were translated into a format that is (1) more common in public health (i.e. sample prevalence; Fiske et al., 2014) and (2) more useful for communicating with stakeholders (Becker, 2017), the degree to which different validated measures assess different aspects of BI became clear. Researchers have long known that BI is a multifactorial construct. Thus, women may respond differently to a measure investigating feelings and behaviors related to BI (e.g. feeling fat when naked or pinching body to assess fat) versus a measure asks how satisfied they are with specific body parts. Furthermore, measures of body parts satisfaction typically view all body parts as being equal, and sum or average responses across body parts. However, some women might deeply hate one or two areas and experience significant distress and associated behaviors while reporting high satisfaction with other body parts. In this case, an affective/behavioral measure may better capture negative BI compared to a body parts satisfaction measure. In summary, further research needs to identify best practices with regard to sample prevalence of negative and positive BI.

As hypothesized, negative BI significantly correlated with self-reported worsened sleep, decreased consumption of nutrient-dense foods, less physical activity enjoyment, increased negative affect, increased functional impairment, and lowered QOL. We found the inverse for positive BI. Thus, results for positive BI simply mirrored those for negative BI and did not change results. Future research should tease out when the addition of positive BI measures help tell a more nuanced picture and when they do not.

Even after controlling for negative affect, all correlations remained for the BAS and BPSS-R, and half remained for the BSQ. Because this study is correlational, we do not know whether BI has a causal relationship with the other variables explored. For instance, it is likely that additional variables not included in this study play a role in worsening multiple dimensions of BI and QOL. This highlights why we need more research. Furthermore, we need to understand when variables have a bi-directional or even more complex relationship. For instance, lack of sleep may worsen appearance (Sundelin et al., 2013), which may increase negative BI, which in turn may worsen negative affect, which could worsen sleep. In summary, results suggest that further research is needed to understand adult women’s BI and the variables examined in this study.

Although the correlational nature of the study severely limits ability to propose causal relationships, one measure (i.e. CIAQ) directly asked participants to report to what degree shape/weight concerns caused daily-life impairment. Results indicated that those who reported moderate/marked BI concerns reported frequently experiencing the following problems as a result of their shape/weight concerns, feeling critical about themselves (67%), upset (43%), ashamed (41%), guilty (43%), worry (41%), and failure (33%). Additionally, 24 percent reported interference with doing things formerly enjoyed and 22 percent interference with relationships. These results suggest that BI concerns result in significant negative outcomes for many adult women who experience them and that increased research is needed to determine the best way to address BI in adult women.

There are various limitations in this study. First, we used a convenience snowball sample. Second, due to the online survey, we utilized only self-report measures. Additionally, use of the Internet may have resulted in a selection bias. For instance, this sample skewed toward a higher socioeconomic status (SES) than is representative of community samples; it also skewed more homogeneous and White/Caucasian than the overall population for the United States (https://www.census.gov/quickfacts/table/PST045216/00). Future research should utilize more inclusive sampling methodologies and alternative measures, including structured interviews.

Acknowledgments

Funding

The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This research was supported by MH094448-01A1

Footnotes

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

References

  1. Avalos L, Tylka TL, Wood-Barcalow NS, et al. (2005) The body appreciation scale: Development and psychometric evaluation. Body Image 2(3): 285–297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Barker ET and Galambos NL (2007) Body dissatisfaction, living away from parents, and poor social adjustment predict binge eating symptoms in young women making the transition to university. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 36(7): 904–911. [Google Scholar]
  3. Becker CB (2017) From efficacy to global impact: Lessons learned about what-not-to-do in translating our research to reach. Behavior Therapy 48(5): 718–730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Becker CB, Diedrichs PC, Jankowski G, et al. (2013) I’m not just fat, I’m old: Has the study of body image overlooked “old talk.” Journal of Eating Disorders 1(1): 6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Ben-Tovim DI and Walker MK (1991) Some body-related attitudes in women smokers and nonsmokers. British Journal of Addiction 86: 1129–1131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bohn K, Doll HA, Cooper Z, et al. (2008) The measurement of impairment due to eating disorder psychopathology. Behaviour Research and Therapy 46(10): 1105–1110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brownell KD and Roberto CA (2015). Strategic science with policy impact. The Lancet, 385(9986): 2445–2446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, et al. (1989) The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Research 28(2): 193–213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Clark MM, Croghan IT, Reading S, et al. (2005) The relationship of body image dissatisfaction to cigarette smoking in college students. Body Image 2(3): 263–270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cooper PJ, Taylor MJ, Cooper Z, et al. (1987) The development and validation of the body shape questionnaire. International Journal of Eating Disorders 6(4): 485–494. [Google Scholar]
  11. Craig CL, Marshall AL, Sjöström M, et al. (2003) International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise 35(13): 81–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Evans C and Dolan B (1993) Body shape questionnaire: Derivation of shortened “alternate forms”. International Journal of Eating Disorders 13(3): 315–321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fiske L, Fallon EA, Blissmer B, et al. (2014) Prevalence of body dissatisfaction among United States adults: Review and recommendations for future research. Eating Behaviors 15(3): 357–365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gagne DA, Von Holle A, Brownley KA, et al. (2012) Eating disorder symptoms and weight and shape concerns in a large web-based convenience sample of women ages 50 and above: Results of gender and body image (GABI) study. International Journal of Eating Disorders 45(7): 832–844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Kilpela LS, Becker CB, Wesley N, et al. (2015) Body image in adult women: Moving beyond the younger years. Advances in Eating Disorders: Theory, Research, and Practice 3(2): 144–164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. King TK, Matacin M, Marcus BH, et al. (2000) Body image evaluations in women smokers. Addictive Behaviors 25(4): 613–618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. King TK, Matacin M, White KS, et al. (2005) A prospective examination of body image and smoking cessation in women. Body Image 2(1): 19–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Levinson DJ (1985) Seasons of a Man’s Life. New York: Knopf. [Google Scholar]
  19. Liechty T, Freeman PA and Zabriskie RB (2006) Body image and beliefs about appearance: Constraints on the leisure of college-age and middle-age women. Leisure Sciences 28(4): 311–330. [Google Scholar]
  20. Mond J, Mitchison D, Latner J, et al. (2013) Quality of life impairment associated with body dissatisfaction in a general population sample of women. BMC Public Health 13: 920. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Muennig P, Haomiao J, Lee R, et al. (2008) I think therefore I am: Perceived ideal weight as a determinant of health. American Journal of Public Health 98(3): 501–506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Mullen SP, Olson EA, Phillips SM, et al. (2011) Measuring enjoyment of physical activity in older adults: Invariance of the physical activity enjoyment scale (paces) across groups and time. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity 8(1): 1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Neumark-Sztainer D, Paxton SJ, Hannan PJ, et al. (2006) Does body satisfaction matter? Five-year longitudinal associations between body satisfaction and health behaviors in adolescent females and males. Journal of Adolescent Health 39(2): 244–251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Patterson E (2002) International Physical Activity Questionnaire. Available at: https://docs.google.com/viewer?a=v&pid=sites&srcid=ZGVmYXVsdGRvbWFpbnx0aGVpcGFxfGd4OjFlYjJlZmJmOWY1YWMwNDk
  25. Perkins KA, Marcus MD, Levine MD, et al. (2001) Cognitive-behavioral therapy to reduce weight concerns improves smoking cessation outcome in weight-concerned women. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 69(4): 604–613. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Petrie TA, Trip MM and Harvey P (2002) Factorial and construct validity of the body parts satisfaction scale-revised: An examination of minority and non minority women. Psychology of Women Quarterly 26(3): 213–221. [Google Scholar]
  27. Prichard I and Tiggemann M (2008) Relations among exercise type, self-objectification, and body image in the fitness centre environment: The role of reasons for exercise. Psychology of Sport and Exercise 9: 855–866. [Google Scholar]
  28. Rosen JC, Jones A, Ramirez E, et al. (1996) Body shape questionnaire: Studies of validity and reliability. International Journal of Eating Disorders 20(3): 315–319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Segar M, Spruijt-Metz D and Nolen-Hoeksema S (2006) Go figure? Body-shape motives are associated with decreased physical activity participation among midlife women. Sex Roles 54(3–4): 175–187. [Google Scholar]
  30. Skevington SM, Lofty M and O’Connell KA (2004) The world health organization’s WHOQOL-BREF quality of life assessment: Psychometric properties and results of the international field trial a report from the WHOQOL group. Quality of Life Research 13(2): 299–310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Slevec JH and Tiggemann M (2011) Predictors of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating in middle-aged women. Clinical Psychology Review 31: 515–524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Stice E, Hayward C, Cameron RP, et al. (2000) Body-image and eating disturbances predict onset of depression among female adolescents: A longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109(3): 438–444. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Sundelin T, Lekander M, Kecklund G, et al. (2013) Cues of fatigue: Effects of sleep deprivation on facial appearance. Sleep 36(9): 1355–1360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Tiggemann M (2004) Body image across the adult life span: Stability and change. Body Image 1(1): 29–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Tiggemann M and McCourt A (2013) Body appreciation in adult women: Relationship with age and body dissatisfaction. Body Image 10: 624–627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Tylka TL and Wood-Barcalow NL (2015) What is and is not positive body image? Conceptual foundations and construct definition. Body Image 14: 118–129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Van Zutven K, Mond J, Latner J, et al. (2015) Obesity and psychosocial impairment: Mediating roles of health status, weight/shape concerns and binge eating in a community sample of women and men. International Journal of Obesity 39(2): 346–352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Watson D and Clark LA (1999) “The PANAS-X: Manual for the positive and negative affect schedule-expanded form. Available at: http://ir.uiowa.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1011&context=psychology_pubs
  39. Watson D, Clark LA and Tellegen A (1988) Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54(6): 1063–1070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Webster J and Tiggemann M (2003) The relationship between women’s body satisfaction and self-image across the life span: The role of cognitive control. The Journal of Genetic Psychology 164(2): 241–252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Wertheim EH, Koerner J and Paxton SJ (2001) Longitudinal predictors of restrictive eating and bulimic tendencies in three different age groups of adolescent girls. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 30(1): 69–81. [Google Scholar]
  42. Wilson RE, Latner JD and Hayashi K (2013) More than just body weight: The role of body image in psychological and physical functioning. Body Image 10(4): 644–647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES