Skip to main content
JAMA Network logoLink to JAMA Network
. 2020 Mar 25;77(8):1–12. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.0284

Association of Magnetoencephalographically Measured High-Frequency Oscillations in Visual Cortex With Circuit Dysfunctions in Local and Large-scale Networks During Emerging Psychosis

Tineke Grent-‘t-Jong 1,2, Ruchika Gajwani 3, Joachim Gross 1,4, Andrew I Gumley 3, Rajeev Krishnadas 1, Stephen M Lawrie 5, Matthias Schwannauer 6, Frauke Schultze-Lutter 7, Peter J Uhlhaas 1,2,
PMCID: PMC7097849  PMID: 32211834

Key Points

Question

Are high-frequency oscillations in visual cortex impaired during early stages of psychosis?

Findings

In this cross-sectional study, there were significant impairments in the variability, power, and connectivity of neural oscillations during visual processing in clinical high-risk participants and patients with first-episode psychosis that were associated with impaired functioning and cognitive deficits. Moreover, the increased variability of γ-band oscillations in visual cortex was also associated with the persistence of subthreshold psychotic symptoms in clinical high-risk participants.

Meaning

Impaired high-frequency oscillations in visual cortex are an important aspect of circuit dysfunction, which could constitute a biomarker for clinical staging of emerging psychosis.

Abstract

Importance

Psychotic disorders are characterized by impairments in neural oscillations, but the nature of the deficit, the trajectory across illness stages, and functional relevance remain unclear.

Objectives

To examine whether changes in spectral power, phase locking, and functional connectivity in visual cortex are present during emerging psychosis and whether these abnormalities are associated with clinical outcomes.

Design, Setting, and Participants

In this cross-sectional study, participants meeting clinical high-risk criteria for psychosis, participants with first-episode psychosis, participants with affective disorders and substance abuse, and a group of control participants were recruited. Participants underwent measurements with magnetoencephalography and magnetic resonance imaging. Data analysis was carried out between 2018 and 2019.

Main Outcomes and Measures

Magnetoencephalographical activity was examined in the 1- to 90-Hz frequency range in combination with source reconstruction during a visual grating task. Event-related fields, power modulation, intertrial phase consistency, and connectivity measures in visual and frontal cortices were associated with neuropsychological scores, psychosocial functioning, and clinical symptoms as well as persistence of subthreshold psychotic symptoms at 12 months.

Results

The study participants included those meeting clinical high-risk criteria for psychosis (n = 119; mean [SD] age, 22 [4.4] years; 32 men), 26 patients with first-episode psychosis (mean [SD] age, 24 [4.2] years; 16 men), 38 participants with affective disorders and substance abuse (mean [SD] age, 23 [4.7] years; 11 men), and 49 control participants (mean age [SD], 23 [3.6] years; 16 men). Clinical high-risk participants and patients with first-episode psychosis were characterized by reduced phase consistency of β/γ-band oscillations in visual cortex (d = 0.63/d = 0.93). Moreover, the first-episode psychosis group was also characterized by reduced occipital γ-band power (d = 1.14) and altered visual cortex connectivity (d = 0.74-0.84). Impaired fronto-occipital connectivity was present in both clinical high-risk participants (d = 0.54) and patients with first-episode psychosis (d = 0.84). Importantly, reductions in intertrial phase coherence predicted persistence of subthreshold psychosis in clinical high-risk participants (receiver operating characteristic area under curve = 0.728; 95% CI, 0.612-0.841; P = .001).

Conclusions and Relevance

High-frequency oscillations are impaired in the visual cortex during emerging psychosis and may be linked to behavioral and clinical impairments. Impaired phase consistency of γ-band oscillations was also associated with the persistence of subthreshold psychosis, suggesting that magnetoencephalographical measured neural oscillations could constitute a biomarker for clinical staging of emerging psychosis.


This study examines whether changes in spectral power, phase locking, and functional connectivity in visual cortex are present during emerging psychosis and whether these abnormalities are associated with clinical outcomes.

Introduction

Neural oscillations are a crucial aspect of normal brain functioning owing to their role in facilitating communication between neuronal populations,1 a process that is closely linked to the integrity of sensory and cognitive processes.2,3 There is emerging evidence that psychotic disorders with pronounced cognitive impairments, such as schizophrenia, involve aberrant neuronal oscillations,4 but the nature of the impairment, the onset of deficits, and clinical relevance remain unclear.

β/γ-Band oscillations5,6,7 but also lower frequencies are impaired during sensory8 and cognitive tasks9 in schizophrenia.10,11 During normal brain functioning, inhibition of excitatory pyramidal cells through different classes of aminobutyric acid (GABA)ergic interneurons lead to the emergence of neural oscillations.12,13,14,15 Converging evidence from genetics,16 postmortem data,17,18 and brain imaging have19 highlighted that GABAergic as well as glutamatergic neurotransmission is impaired in schizophrenia, supporting the possibility that measurements with electro/magnetoencephalography (EEG/MEG) could be important for translational research aimed at identifying circuit mechanisms in the disorder.10

Critical questions concerning the role of neural oscillations in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia are the onset of abnormalities, the nature of the deficit, and functional relevance. Early signs of psychosis as well as associated cognitive deficits are already present several years prior to the full emergence of schizophrenia,20 and thus, research efforts have shifted the focus toward identifying circuit abnormalities and biomarkers in participants who are at risk for the development of psychotic disorders that could allow for early intervention and clinical staging.21,22

There is only limited evidence available on alterations of neural oscillations in individuals meeting clinical high-risk criteria for psychosis (CHR-P).23,24 To address this fundamental question, we applied a state-of-the-art MEG approach to examine low-frequency and high-frequency oscillations during a visual paradigm in CHR-P participants, patients with first-episode psychosis (FEP), and participants with substance-related and affective disorders. Magnetoencephalography is characterized by an improved signal-to-noise ratio for measurements of high-frequency oscillations compared with EEG25,26 and is ideally suited for source reconstruction, allowing the identification of anatomical layout of generators with high spatial resolution.27

Based on models of developing psychosis that have highlighted the central role of visual deficits during the early stages of psychosis28,29 that predict transition to psychosis30 as well as the importance of high-frequency oscillations for the integrity of visual perception,1,31,32 we predicted that CHR-P participants would be characterized by a circumscribed dysfunction of β/γ-band oscillations in visual cortex that would be linked to clinical outcomes. Specifically, we focused on the persistence of attenuated psychotic symptoms (APS) because there is evidence to suggest that persistent APS are associated with poor outcomes33 and cognitive deficits in CHR-P populations.34 Patients with FEP, on the other hand, would involve large-scale dysfunctions of induced oscillations and effective connectivity between frontal and visual areas, consistent with a disconnection syndrome.35,36

Methods

Participants

Four groups of participants (total n = 232) were recruited: (1) participants meeting CHR-P criteria (n = 119) from the ongoing Youth Mental Health Risk and Resilience (YouR) Study37; (2) 38 participants who did not meet CHR-P criteria (CHR-N) and were characterized by nonpsychotic disorders, such as affective disorders (n = 11), anxiety disorders (n = 16), eating disorders (n = 1), and/or substance abuse (n = 10); (3) 26 patients with FEP (13 antipsychotic-naive); and (4) 49 healthy control individuals (HC) without an axis I diagnosis or family history of psychotic disorders. Data from 10 patients with FEP and 10 HC have been published previously.38

The CHR-P status was confirmed by ultrahigh-risk criteria according to the Comprehensive Assessment of At Risk Mental States (CAARMS) interview39 and the Cognitive Disturbances and Cognitive-Perceptive Basic Symptoms criteria according to the Schizophrenia Proneness Instrument, Adult version (SPI-A)40 (see Uhlhaas et al37). Patients with FEP were assessed with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (Table)41 and with the Positive and Negative Symptom Scale.42 For all groups except patients with FEP, neurocognition was assessed with the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS).43 The study was approved by the ethical committees of University of Glasgow and the National Health Services Research Ethical Committee Glasgow and Greater Clyde. All participants provided written informed consent.

Table. Demographics, Clinical Data, and Task Performance.

Demographic HC CHR-N CHR-P FEP Group effecta Pairwise comparisons
No. of participants 49 38 119 26 NA NA
Age, (SD), y 23 (3.6) 23 (4.7) 22 (4.4) 24 (4.2) NA NA
Male/female sex, No. (% male) 16/33 (32.7) 11/27 (28.9) 32/87 (26.9) 16/10 (61.5) χ23 = 11.9; P = .008 FEP to HC: P = .016
; FEP to CHR-N: P = .001; FEP to CHR-P: P = .01
Education, mean (SD), y 17 (3.0) 16 (3.5) 15 (3.1) 15 (3.0) F3,76 = 3.5; P = .02 CHR-P to HC: P = .03
BACS,b mean (SD)
Verbal memory 52 (8.7) 0.01 (1.1) −0.36 (1.3) NA NA NA
Digit sequencing 21 (2.1) 0.14 (1.2) −0.15 (1.5) NA NA NA
Token motor 81 (11.6) −0.66 (1.1) −0.98 (1.3) - F2,93 = 13.8; P < .001 CHR-N to HC: P = .01; CHR-P - HC: P < .001
Verbal fluency 59 (13.9) −0.22 (1.0) 0.05 (1.3) NA NA NA
Symbol coding 74 (11.8) 0.00 (1.3) −0.58 (1.1) NA F2,84 = 6.8; P = .002 CHR-P - HC: P = .004; CHR-P – CHR-N: P = .04
Tower of London 19 (1.7) 0.15 (1.3) −0.21 (1.5) NA NA NA
Composite score 304 (24.2) −0.15 (1.2) −0.63 (1.4) NA F2,93 = 5.8; P = .004 CHR-P - HC: P = .004
CAARMS, mean (SD)
Unusual thought content NA 1 (1.2) 2 (1.9) NA NA NA
Nonbizarre ideas NA 1 (1.1) 3 (1.8) NA NA NA
Perceptual abnormalities NA 1 (1.3) 3 (1.6) NA NA NA
Disorganized speech NA 1 (0.9) 1 (1.4) NA NA NA
Total severity score NA 6 (6.1) 29 (17.8) NA NA NA
GAF, mean (SD) 88 (6.4) 70 (12.8) 57 (13.4) 41 (16.9) F3,75 = 167; P < .001 All contrasts P < .001
GF-role, mean (SD) 8.6 (0.8) 8.1 (0.8) 7.4 (1.2) NA F2,99 = 29.6; P < .001 CHR-N - HC: P = .037; CHR-P to HC: P < .001; CHR-P to CHR-N: P < .001
GF-social, mean (SD) 8.8 (0.4) 8.2 (0.8) 7.5 (1.2) NA F2,94 = 59.5, P < .001 CHR-N - HC: P < .001; CHR-P to HC: P < .001; CHR-P to CHR-N: P < .001
PANSS, mean (SD)
Positive NA NA NA 18 (7.2) NA NA
Negative NA NA NA 15 (9.3) NA NA
Cognitive NA NA NA 20 (9.2) NA NA
Excitement NA NA NA 9 (4.3) NA NA
Depression NA NA NA 12 (5.9) NA NA
Total score NA NA NA 74 (28.4) NA NA
Medication, No. (%)c
None 48 27 61 6 NA NA
Antidepressants 0 11 47 13 NA NA
Mood stabilizers 0 0 5 0 NA NA
Antipsychotics 0 0 3 13 NA NA
Other (unknown) 1 (0) 2 (0) 21 (0) 5 (0) NA NA
CHR-P categories
SPI-A (COGDIS/COPER/both items) NA NA 30 (4/15/11) NA NA NA
CAARMS (APS/vulnerability criteria) NA NA 89 (87/2) NA NA NA
CAARMS plus SPI-A (COGDIS/COPER/both items) NA NA 55 (9/22/24) NA NA NA
MINI categories
Depressive/mood disorders NA 11 75 NA NA NA
Anxiety disorders/posttraumatic stress disorder/obsessive-compulsive disorder NA 16 87 NA NA NA
Drug/alcohol abuse/dependence NA 10 42 NA NA NA
Eating disorders NA 1 10 NA NA NA
DSM-IV/Structured Clinical Interview
Schizophrenia NA NA NA 9 NA NA
Schizophreniform disorder NA NA NA 3 NA NA
Schizoaffective disorder NA NA NA 1 NA NA
Psychotic disorder NOS NA NA NA 8 NA NA
Brief psychotic disorder NA NA NA 1 NA NA
Mood disorders with psychotic features NA NA NA 4 NA NA
Trial No., total included (SD) 197 (16.3) 194 (16.2) 185 (26.5) 181 (29.2) F3,80 = 5.9; P = .001 CHR-P to HC: P = .002
Task performance
Accuracy, % correct (SD) 92.2 (6.9) 92.0 (5.6) 88.0 (9.8) 85.9 (12.8) F3,79 = 5.5, P = .002 CHR-P to HC: P = .01; CHR-P vs CHR-N: P = .01
Reaction time, mean (SD), ms 528 (68.4) 524 (80.1) 545 (84.9) 577 (100.1) NA NA
Response variance,d mean (SD), ms 151.7 (37.5) 154.3 (34.9) 164.3 (42.2) 179.1 (40.8) F3,79 = 3.3; P = .02 FEP to HC: P = .03

Abbreviations: APS, attenuated psychotic symptoms; BACS, Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia; CAARMS, Comprehensive Assessment of At Risk Mental States; CHR-N, clinical high risk negative; CHR-P, clinical high risk positive; COGDIS/COPER, Cognitive Disturbances and Cognitive-Perceptive Basic Symptoms criteria; FEP, first-episode psychosis; GAF, global assessment of functioning; GF, global functioning; HC, healthy control individual; MINI, Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview; PANSS, Positive and Negative Symptom Scale; SPI-A, Schizophrenia Proneness Instrument, Adult version.

a

All F tests are Welch based; α = .05, 2-sided, 1000 samples bootstrapping, post hoc Games-Howell correction for type I errors.

b

BACS scores for clinical groups were standardized to control group data, controlled for sex category.

c

If multiple medications were reported, they were scored separately in the different categories listed.

d

Response variance equals standard deviation of response times across trials.

Clinical Follow-up

Participants meeting CHR-P and CHR-N criteria were reassessed at 3-, 6-, 9-, 12-, 18-, 24-, 30-, and 36-month intervals to examine persistence of CHR-P criteria and transition to psychosis (eMethods in the Supplement).

Stimuli and Task

Participants were presented with 3 blocks of 80 trials, with each trial consisting of a circular sinewave grating that contracted toward central fixation.44 The task of the participants was to detect and respond by button press to a velocity increase of the stimulus, randomly occurring between 750 and 3000 milliseconds (Figure 1).

Figure 1. Paradigm and Task Performance.

Figure 1.

A, Inward moving grating task: participants report, by button press, the onset of a change in velocity of inward motion of the visual stimulus (correct response window, 200-1200 milliseconds). Feedback on performance was provided on every trial, shortly after the response onset terminated stimulus presentation. B, Histograms of group means and standard errors for accuracy (% correct), mean reaction times (RTs), and behavioral variability (intraindividual standard deviation of RTs). CHR-N indicates clinical high risk negative; CHR-P, clinical high risk positive; FEP, first-episode psychosis; HC, healthy control individuals.

aIndicates significant group differences (Welch F tests, α = .05, 2-sided, 1000 samples bootstrapping, Games-Howell corrected for multiple comparisons).

Neuroimaging

Magnetoencephalography data were acquired using a 248-channel 4D-BTI magnetometer system (MAGNES 3600 WH, 4-dimensional neuroimaging; Bio-Medicine), recorded with 1017.25-Hz sampling rate and DC-400 Hz online filtered. T1 anatomical scans (3-dimensional magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo sequences) were collected for patient-specific source localization of MEG activity (eMethods in the Supplement).

Magnetoencephalography Data Analysis

Magnetoencephalography data were analyzed with MATLAB using the open-source Fieldtrip Toolbox.45 Preprocessing included correct trials only with nonoverlapping 3.8-second segments (1-second baseline), time locked to the onset of the visual grating. Line noise was attenuated with a discrete 50-Hz Fourier transform filter, and faulty sensors with large signal variance or flat signals were removed. Data were denoised relative to MEG reference channels and downsampled to 300 Hz. Artifact-free data were created by removing trials with excessive transient muscle activity, slow drift, or superconducting quantum interference device jumps using visual inspection, and independent component analysis–based removal of eye blink, eye movement, and electrocardiographic artifacts. Data were then submitted to time-frequency (TFR) analyses (1-90 Hz, stepsize 50 milliseconds, 450 milliseconds sliding-window fast-fourier transformed [FFT]; Hanning tapered), computed for planar-orientation transformed MEG data.46

Whole-brain source estimation of γ-band power (57-67 Hz) between 250 and 750 milliseconds was computed using the Dynamic Imaging of Coherent Sources beamforming approach47 (eMethods in the Supplement). γ-Band source data were statistically tested across groups to determine the location of main effects (Figure 2). These were then used to guide selection of the main regions of interest (ROIs) for more fine-grained virtual channel analyses (Figure 2 and Figure 3C) (eMethods in the Supplement). Virtual channel time series were used for the analysis of event-related fields (ERF), TFR, inter-trial phase coherence (ITPC),48 baseline FFT, and Granger causality (GC).

Figure 2. Sensor and Source-Power Magnetoencephalography (MEG) Data.

Figure 2.

A, Sensor-level MEG data: left topographical distribution plot shows grand average–induced γ power (n = 232) changes from baseline, with white dots marking the sensors for which the time frequency response (TFR) plot in the middle is plotted. In the TFR plot, the outlined (black box) window indicates the window of statistical testing for group differences in γ power. The TFR plot shows evoked activity from stimulus onset (time zero) to approximately 250 milliseconds, from which latency-induced activity is shown up to 1500 milliseconds. Right topographical distribution plot shows F values of significant (marked with white dots) sensors showing a main group effect on γ power. B, Beamformer-localized main group effect on γ power. Lighter-blue and light-orange values mark areas of significant effects uncorrected for multiple comparisons, whereas the darker colors display false discovery rate–corrected areas (α = .05; 2-sided). C, Locations across visual cortical areas of significant γ activity from which virtual channel time-series MEG data were reconstructed. The 10 occipital regions of interest (ROIs) included 3 subregions covering the primary visual cortex. CAL indicates calcarine; CUN, cuneus; IOG, inferior occipital gyrus; MOG, middle occipital gyrus; SOG, superior occipital gyrus.

Figure 3. Virtual Channel Time Frequency Response (TFR) and Intertrial Phase Coherence (ITPC) Analyses.

Figure 3.

A, Top 4 panels show per group the TFR, averaged over all virtual-channel regions of interest (ROIs) shown in Figure 2C. Bottom right panels: TFR plot with statistical results (nonparametric, Monte Carlo–based permutation independent t tests) of group differences in time-frequency clusters between 0 and 750 milliseconds, with significant clusters outlined and the remaining nonsignificant time-frequency bins masked out (opacity, 0.45). The line graph on the left shows the γ (57-67 Hz) response over time per group, with error bars representing standard error of the mean. B, Top panels show the ITPC responses per group and bottom panels show the significant group differences (between 0-350 milliseconds), with significant time-frequency bins outlined. The line graph on the left shows γ (30-50 Hz) range ITPC responses per group, with error bars representing standard error of the mean. CHR-N indicates clinical high risk negative; CHR-P, clinical high risk positive; FEP, first-episode psychosis; HC, healthy control individuals.

Granger-causality estimates were computed using a nonparametric approach, including spectral density matrices estimated directly from FFT-data (250-750 milliseconds; DC-149 Hz; Hanning tapered; 5-Hz frequency smoothing; 1-Hz resolution; data zero-padded to 4 seconds), followed by matrix factorization and variance decomposition. Epochs were split into 2 × 250-millisecond segments to increase trial numbers (see Michalareas et al32). The middle occipital gyrus and cuneus ROIs were not used in the GC analyses to minimize overlap between primary visual, dorsal, and ventral stream connectivity estimates. Granger-causality data from each pair were averaged over hemisphere to create 4 main ROI pairs for statistical testing. To determine the alterations in feedforward (FF) vs feedback (FB) GC activity, we also computed the directed asymmetry index (DAI; see Michalareas et al,32 Bastos et al,49 and eMethods in the Supplement).

Statistical Analysis

Group differences in trial numbers, γ-band peak frequency, behavioral performance, demographic, and clinical data were assessed with 1-way Welch analysis of variance (ANOVA); 2-sided α level of .05. Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia data were first z-normalized to the HC data. Bootstrapping (n = 1000) and Games-Howell correction were used to control type I errors in post hoc pairwise group comparisons.

Statistical testing of group differences in MEG virtual-channel data included nonparametric Monte-Carlo–based permutation (n = 2000) independent F test (main group effect) and post hoc t test statistics45 for ERFs (0-750 milliseconds); TFRs and ITPC power (1-90 Hz, 0-750 milliseconds for TFR power, 0-350 milliseconds for ITPC, and dB change from a 500-millisecond baseline); baseline FFT spectra (1-90 Hz); and GC data. Type I errors were controlled by cluster statistics across time and/or frequency (eMethods in the Supplement). Finally, binary logistic regression and receiver operating characteristic curve (ROC) analyses were used to examine the association between MEG parameters and clinical outcomes in CHR-P participants (eMethods in the Supplement).

Results

Demographic Data

The FEP group included significantly more men than the HC (χ21 = 5.8; P = .02), CHR-N (χ21 = 6.7; P = .01), and CHR-P (χ21 = 11.6, P = .001) groups (Table). The BACS composite score was significantly reduced in CHR-P participants compared with HC (−0.84; 95% CI, −1.43 to −0.25; P = .004). All clinical groups differed from HC in global assessment of functioning (GAF) scores (CHR-N, 17.7; 95% CI, 11.7 to 23.8; P < .001; CHR-P, 30.3; 95% CI, 26.3 to 34.2; P < .001; FEP, 46.6, 95% CI, 37.2 to 56.0; P < .001). Both CHR-P and CHR-N groups also differed from HC in global role and social functioning (CHR-N, −0.63; 95% CI, −0.28 to −0.99; P < .001; CHR-P,  −1.35; 95% CI, −1.05 to −1.66; P < .001).

Follow-up Outcomes

We examined persistence of APS up to 12 months in CHR-P participants who met APS criteria at baseline (n = 84). For 75 CHR-P participants, at least 1 follow-up assessment was available. Thirty-nine CHR-P participants continued to meet APS criteria (APS-persistent group) while 36 CHR-P participants were characterized by a remission of APS-criteria (eResults in the Supplement). Moreover, 9 of 119 CHR-P participants made a transition to psychosis (mean follow-up period, 17.3 months). Eight transitions occurred in the APS-persistent group.

Task Performance

The CHR-P group was characterized by reduced response accuracy (−4.2%; 95% CI, −7.6 to −0.7; P = .01), while the patients with FEP were significantly more variable in reaction times (RTs) (27 milliseconds; 95% CI, 2 to 53; P = .03) compared with HC (Table).

Sensor-Level Analysis

Modulation of spectral power was characterized by early evoked activity (<approximately 250 milliseconds), which is phase locked and time locked to the onset of the stimulus and sustained induced activity that represents non–phase-locked oscillations (>250 milliseconds) (Figure 2). Task-induced γ peak frequency across participants was approximately 62 Hz. A main group effect (cluster F3,228 = 341.7; P < .001; 95% CI range, −0.0004 to 0.002) for 57- to 67-Hz power was found over occipital and parietal-temporal regions (Figure 2A), with no differences in any other frequency range. Post hoc test results revealed significantly increased γ power for CHR-N vs HC over superior occipital-parietal regions (cluster t85 = 48.4; P = .03; 95% CI range, 0.0147-0.0453) and significantly decreased γ power (cluster t73 = −50.7; P = .02; 95% CI range = 0.0143-0.0257) over inferior occipital regions for FEP compared with HC (eFigure 1 in the Supplement).

Virtual Channel Analyses: TFR and ITPC Analyses

A cluster of sustained γ-band power decreases across all visual cortex ROIs (Figure 3A) for the FEP group compared with HC (TFR cluster approximately 50-75 Hz; approximately 0-750 milliseconds; cluster t73 = −791.8; P = .007; 95% CI range, 0.0033-0.0107) were observed in primary visual cortex as well as in ventral stream areas (eFigure 2A in the Supplement). The CHR-N and CHR-P groups did not show spectral power changes in any frequency range.

Differences in β/γ-band ITPC values were found for both CHR-P and FEP participants compared with HC (CHR-P: TFR-cluster approximately 21-68 Hz; approximately 125-275 milliseconds; cluster t166 = −509.1; P = .005; 95% CI range, 0.0022 to 0.0078; FEP: TFR-cluster approximately 11-57 Hz; approximately 75-325 milliseconds; cluster t73 = −633.1; P = .002; 95% CI, 0.0018-0.0022) (Figure 3B) that involved primary visual as well as ventral stream regions and that extended to dorsal stream areas in the patients with FEP (eFigure 2B in the Supplement). The CHR-N group showed intact ITPC spectral power across all visual ROIs and frequencies.

Behavioral and Magnetoencephalographical Parameters Associated With APS Persistence in the CHR-P Group

Intertrial phase coherence data (30-50 Hz; 125-200 milliseconds) from 10 occipital ROIs, accuracy, RTs, and RT variability were entered into a regression model to predict persistence of APS criteria in the CHR-P group. Only γ-band ITPC (30-50 Hz) activity contributed significantly to the model. Specifically, ITPC data from the left/right cuneus and left middle occipital gyrus ROIs led to a significant model (χ23 = 14.4; P = .002) that explained 22.2% of the variance (Nagelkerke R2 = 0.222). The associated ROC curve was also significant (Figure 4A: area under the curve, 0.728; 95% CI, 0.612-0.841; P = .001) (eMethods and eResults in the Supplement).

Figure 4. Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) Curve Analysis and Granger Causality (GC) Functional Connectivity.

Figure 4.

A, On the right, ROC curve computed from prediction probabilities associated with a significant logistic regression model for predicting 12 months Comprehensive Assessment of At Risk Mental States (CAARMS; attenuated psychotic symptoms) persistence status from baseline magnetoencephalography (MEG) recordings of intertrial phase coherence (ITPC) of visual cortex responses (left and right cuneus and left middle occipital gyrus; locations shown in left panel). B, Results of cluster-based statistics on GC data showing range of significant effects in first-episode psychosis (FEP), clinical high-risk positive (CHR-P), and clinical high risk negative (CHR-N) groups, compared with healthy control individuals (HC). The main connections tested are plotted on a smoothed surface of a standard Montreal Neurological Institute brain, with red lines representing increased and blue lines decreased GC values, compared with HC. For each significant connection, GC values are plotted across the frequency spectrum, separately per group (with error bars indicating standard error of the mean), and a horizontal line indicating the frequency range of significant group effects. The GC was computed for data between 250 to 750 milliseconds after stimulus onset. The directed asymmetry indices were all positive in the significant contrasts, indicating feed-forward flow of information between the nodes. AUC indicates area under curve.

Regions of Interest: Baseline Power Spectra and ERF Responses

No group differences in baseline spectral power (1-90 Hz) or ERF amplitudes were observed in any visual cortex ROI (eFigures 3 and 4 in the Supplement).

GC Connectivity

A main group effect was found for 2 connections in visual cortex (Figure 4B: calcarine [CAL] to superior occipital gyrus; approximately 50-77 Hz; cluster P = .004; 95% CI range, 0.0014-0.0066; inferior occipital gyrus [IOG] to CAL: approximately 1-14 Hz; cluster P = .02; 95% CI range, 0.0163-0.0237) and a fronto-occipital connection (medial-superior frontal [mSFG] to IOG: approximately 24-41 Hz; cluster P = .007; 95% CI range, 0.0033 to 0.0107). Post hoc comparisons revealed decreased connectivity in the FEP group in visual cortex (CAL to superior occipital gyrus: DAI = 0.04; t73 = −3.0; P = .006; 95% CI range, 0.0026 to 0.0094; IOG to CAL: DAI = 0.11; t73= −3.5; P = .004; 95% CI range, 0.0012-0.0068) but increased fronto-occipital connectivity (mSFG-to-IOG connection: DAI = 0.11; t73 = 4.5; P < .001; 95% CI range, −0.0004 to 0.002). Comparable long-range connectivity changes were seen in the CHR-P group (mSFG-to-IOG: DAI = 0.07; t166 = 3.19; P = .003; 95% CI range, 0.0028-0.0032). The CHR-N group showed decreased IOG-to-CAL connectivity (DAI = 0.11; t85 = −3.5; P = .002; 95% CI range, 0.0002-0.0038).

Correlations

Correlations were tested using linear regression models (α <.05; 2-sided; 1000-sample bootstrapping), with occipital γ power (57-67 Hz; 250-750 milliseconds) and occipital 30- to 50-Hz ITPC (125-200 milliseconds) as dependent variables. Across all groups, γ-band power changes were positively correlated with ITPC values (unstandardized B  = 0.077; 95% CI, 0.041-0.113; standardized β coefficient = 0.274; t228 = 4.3; P < .001) and RT variance (B = 0.003; 95% CI, 0.001-0.006; β = 0.257; t228 = 2.7; P = .008), and negatively with RTs (B = −0.002; 95% CI, −0.004 to −0.001; β = −0.349; t228 = −3.6; P < .001), together explaining 13.2% of variance (R2 = 0.132). Thirty- to 50-Hz ITPC was positively associated with accuracy (B = 0.041; 95% CI, 0.018-0.065; β = 0.198; t228 = 3.4; P = .001) and GAF scores (B = 0.036; 95% CI, 0.024-0.047; β = 0.347; t228 = 5.9; P < .001) but negatively correlated with occipital β-band (16-22 Hz) power (B = −0.301; 95% CI, −0.562 to −0.040; β = −0.128, t228 = −2.3; P = .02) and CAARMS severity (B = −0.025; 95% CI, −0.036 to −0.013; β = −0.281; t204 = −4.2; P < .001) and explained 28.7% of variance (R2 = 0.287).

Local and Long-range Oscillations in CHR-P Subgroups

We examined differences between CHR-P subgroups (CAARMS n = 34; SPI-A n = 30; CAARMS/SPI-A n = 55) in MEG activity and behavioral and clinical parameters (eResults, eFigure 6, and eTables 1-3 in the Supplement). Only the combined CAARMS/SPI-A group showed a significant ITPC-deficit relative to HC (TF-cluster approximately 24-72 Hz; approximately 0-300 milliseconds; cluster P < .001; 95% CI range, −0.0004 to 0.002). The effect size (d = 1.20) was comparable with the FEP group (d = 0.93). The CHR-P groups showed no difference in spectral power, while CHR-P individuals with CAARMS and CAARMS/SPI-A criteria showed a selective deficit in long-range connectivity between frontal and occipital cortex (for mSFG-to-IOG connection, see eResults and eFigure 7 in the Supplement).

Discussion

This study examined neural oscillations during visual processing with a state-of-the art MEG approach to investigate whether emerging psychosis is associated with aberrant oscillatory activity in visual cortex as well as the functional relevance of impaired neural oscillations. Specifically, our data highlight a reduction of phase locking of high-frequency oscillations, a measure of the variability of an ongoing oscillation across trials,48 in visual cortices as well as impaired long-range connectivity in CHR-P participants. Importantly, ITPC deficits were also associated with persistent APS, providing important evidence for the role of high-frequency oscillations in clinical staging of emerging psychosis.

Further evidence for the functional relevance of β/γ-band phase locking are significant correlations with RTs, severity of APS, and the combination of SPI-A/CAARMS criteria as well as GAF-scores across participants. In addition, β/γ-band ITPC was associated with induced γ-band power, highlighting the importance of the integrity of early visual processing for large-scale cognition and functioning. These data are consistent with previous findings that have identified associations between compromised sensory processing, impaired functioning, and cognitive deficits in schizophrenia.50,51,52

Comparisons between FEP and CHR-P groups revealed overlapping and distinct oscillatory signatures. Induced γ-band oscillations were prominently impaired in the FEP group in visual areas, which was not observed in CHR-P participants. Both groups were characterized by impaired long-range connectivity between visual and frontal cortices while the FEP group also showed reduced visual cortex connectivity. An influential model in schizophrenia has been the disconnectivity hypothesis36 as well as the notion of reduced cognitive control mediated by frontal cortices.53 Our GC data are consistent with these hypotheses, suggesting a shared feature of both FEP and at-risk participants is the presence of impaired connectivity between sensory regions and frontal cortices.

Impairments in high-frequency oscillations showed a considerable degree of specificity. First, ITPC impairments were only found for activity in the β/γ-band range but not for lower frequencies. Together with the large reductions in induced γ-band activity in the FEP group, these data highlight the unique contribution of high-frequency oscillations toward circuit impairments in emerging psychosis. Second, the CHR-N group showed intact behavioral task parameters as well normal power and phase of high-frequency oscillations in visual cortices and long-range connectivity with only evidence for a circumscribed impairment in local connectivity in visual cortex.

Thus, EEG/MEG readouts could potentially inform clinical decision-making and search for novel treatment opportunities. The search for biomarkers that have prognostic utility and could guide treatments in emerging psychosis is an important objective of research.22 This study highlights that impaired γ-band ITPC differentiates between CHR-P individuals who have a high likelihood of persistent APS and transition to psychosis vs CHR-P individuals who show more benign APS. On the other hand, reductions in induced γ-band power emerged as a specific signature of FEP, suggesting that impaired γ-band oscillations could serve as a biomarker for established psychosis that warrants more aggressive treatments, such as antipsychotic medications.

Our observations of increased variability in the timing of β/γ-band oscillations is consistent with formulations that have implicated aberrant glutamatergic and GABAergic neurotransmission as key mechanism for circuit dysfunctions in psychotic disorders.17,54,55 Specifically, an increase in variability of neuronal responses can be elicited by N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor hypofunction,56 suggesting that elevated excitability in sensory regions during the early stages of psychosis may lead to favorable conditions for altered network dynamics to emerge. Moreover, deficits in high-frequency oscillations highlight the contribution of specific GABAergic interneurons, such as parvalbumin or somatostatin-expressing interneurons13,57 that are impaired in visual areas in schizophrenia.58 In addition to the pharmacologic correction of aberrant circuit dynamics, it is also conceivable that interventions that improve the fidelity of sensory processing through cognitive remediation59 or brain stimulation60 could potentially prevent the progression of circuit dysfunctions from sensory areas to more extended networks.

Limitations

This study has several limitations. Although we can predict the persistence of subthreshold psychotic symptoms through MEG data in our CHR-P cohort, further follow-up data are required to test whether abnormalities in high-frequency oscillations can predict transition to psychosis as well as the persistence of Cognitive Disturbances and Cognitive-Perceptive Basic Symptoms criteria. Moreover, these data are only cross-sectional. Accordingly, further studies are required to examine the longitudinal course of oscillatory deficits during emerging psychosis.

Conclusions

In summary, this advanced MEG analysis provides, to our knowledge, the first comprehensive investigation into the oscillatory signatures during different stages of early psychosis. Specifically, we can show that the timing of high-frequency oscillations in visual cortices is the first impairment to emerge in CHR-P participants in combination with abnormal long-range connectivity. Patients with FEP were characterized by a pronounced reduction in the power of induced γ-band oscillations in combination with reduced β/γ-band ITPC as well as local and long-range connectivity. Importantly, impaired γ-band IPTC-values were associated with the persistence of subthreshold psychotic experiences, suggesting that γ-band oscillations could constitute a possible biomarker for clinical staging of emerging psychosis. Future studies and preclinical research should therefore focus on the circuit-mechanisms mediating precise coordinated neural responses that could offer targets for preventive approaches.

Supplement.

eMethods. Supplementary Methods

eResults. Supplementary Results

eTable 1. Overview of Effect Sizes (Cohen’s d)

eTable 2. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of CHR-P Subgroups

eTable 3. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics for Persistent-APS vs Non-Persistent APS CHR-P Subgroups

eFigure 1. Sensor Data Statistics: Post-Hoc Comparisons

eFigure 2. Virtual channel TFR/ITPC Group Differences in ROIs

eFigure 3. Virtual Channel Baseline FFT Spectra over Visual Cortex

eFigure 4. Virtual Channel ERF Data

eFigure 5. Correlations MEG data, Behavior and Clinical Parameters

eFigure 6. Time-Frequency and Inter-Trial-Phase-Coherence in CHR-P subgroups

eFigure 7. Granger-Causality (GC) connectivity changes in main and CHR-P subgroups

References

  • 1.Gray CM, König P, Engel AK, Singer W. Oscillatory responses in cat visual cortex exhibit inter-columnar synchronization which reflects global stimulus properties. Nature. 1989;338(6213):334-337. doi: 10.1038/338334a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Womelsdorf T, Fries P, Mitra PP, Desimone R. Gamma-band synchronization in visual cortex predicts speed of change detection. Nature. 2006;439(7077):733-736. doi: 10.1038/nature04258 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kim H, Ährlund-Richter S, Wang X, Deisseroth K, Carlén M. Prefrontal parvalbumin neurons in control of attention. Cell. 2016;164(1-2):208-218. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.11.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W. Neural synchrony in brain disorders: relevance for cognitive dysfunctions and pathophysiology. Neuron. 2006;52(1):155-168. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.09.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Spencer KM, Nestor PG, Niznikiewicz MA, Salisbury DF, Shenton ME, McCarley RW. Abnormal neural synchrony in schizophrenia. J Neurosci. 2003;23(19):7407-7411. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-19-07407.2003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Kwon JS, O’Donnell BF, Wallenstein GV, et al. . Gamma frequency-range abnormalities to auditory stimulation in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56(11):1001-1005. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.11.1001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Grent-’t-Jong T, Rivolta D, Sauer A, et al. . MEG-measured visually induced gamma-band oscillations in chronic schizophrenia: Evidence for impaired generation of rhythmic activity in ventral stream regions. Schizophr Res. 2016;176(2-3):177-185. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2016.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hamm JP, Gilmore CS, Picchetti NA, Sponheim SR, Clementz BA. Abnormalities of neuronal oscillations and temporal integration to low- and high-frequency auditory stimulation in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69(10):989-996. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.11.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ryman SG, Cavanagh JF, Wertz CJ, et al. . Impaired midline theta power and connectivity during proactive cognitive control in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2018;84(9):675-683. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.04.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W. Neuronal dynamics and neuropsychiatric disorders: toward a translational paradigm for dysfunctional large-scale networks. Neuron. 2012;75(6):963-980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Lisman J. Excitation, inhibition, local oscillations, or large-scale loops: what causes the symptoms of schizophrenia? Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2012;22(3):537-544. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.10.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Börgers C, Kopell N. Synchronization in networks of excitatory and inhibitory neurons with sparse, random connectivity. Neural Comput. 2003;15(3):509-538. doi: 10.1162/089976603321192059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Sohal VS, Zhang F, Yizhar O, Deisseroth K. Parvalbumin neurons and gamma rhythms enhance cortical circuit performance. Nature. 2009;459(7247):698-702. doi: 10.1038/nature07991 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Womelsdorf T, Valiante TA, Sahin NT, Miller KJ, Tiesinga P. Dynamic circuit motifs underlying rhythmic gain control, gating and integration. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17(8):1031-1039. doi: 10.1038/nn.3764 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Wang XJ. Neurophysiological and computational principles of cortical rhythms in cognition. Physiol Rev. 2010;90(3):1195-1268. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00035.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Pocklington AJ, Rees E, Walters JT, et al. . Novel findings from CNVs implicate inhibitory and excitatory signaling complexes in schizophrenia. Neuron. 2015;86(5):1203-1214. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.04.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lewis DA, Curley AA, Glausier JR, Volk DW. Cortical parvalbumin interneurons and cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Trends Neurosci. 2012;35(1):57-67. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.10.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Benes FM, Berretta S. GABAergic interneurons: implications for understanding schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;25(1):1-27. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00225-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Kegeles LS, Mao X, Stanford AD, et al. . Elevated prefrontal cortex γ-aminobutyric acid and glutamate-glutamine levels in schizophrenia measured in vivo with proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(5):449-459. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1519 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Fusar-Poli P, Borgwardt S, Bechdolf A, et al. . The psychosis high-risk state: a comprehensive state-of-the-art review. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(1):107-120. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.269 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Heinssen R, Insel TR. Prevention of mental disorders. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2012;8:269-289. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.McGorry P, Keshavan M, Goldstone S, et al. . Biomarkers and clinical staging in psychiatry. World Psychiatry. 2014;13(3):211-223. doi: 10.1002/wps.20144 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Tada M, Nagai T, Kirihara K, et al. . Differential alterations of auditory gamma oscillatory responses between pre-onset high-risk individuals and first-episode schizophrenia. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26(3):1027-1035. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu278 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Perez VB, Roach BJ, Woods SW, et al. . Early auditory gamma-band responses in patients at clinical high risk for schizophrenia. Suppl Clin Neurophysiol. 2013;62:147-162. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-7020-5307-8.00010-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Muthukumaraswamy SD, Singh KD. Visual gamma oscillations: the effects of stimulus type, visual field coverage and stimulus motion on MEG and EEG recordings. Neuroimage. 2013;69:223-230. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Siems M, Pape AA, Hipp JF, Siegel M. Measuring the cortical correlation structure of spontaneous oscillatory activity with EEG and MEG. Neuroimage. 2016;129:345-355. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Brookes MJ, Woolrich M, Luckhoo H, et al. . Investigating the electrophysiological basis of resting state networks using magnetoencephalography. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(40):16783-16788. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112685108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.McGhie A, Chapman J. Disorders of attention and perception in early schizophrenia. Br J Med Psychol. 1961;34:103-116. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1961.tb00936.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Uhlhaas PJ, Mishara AL. Perceptual anomalies in schizophrenia: integrating phenomenology and cognitive neuroscience. Schizophr Bull. 2007;33(1):142-156. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl047 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Klosterkötter J, Hellmich M, Steinmeyer EM, Schultze-Lutter F. Diagnosing schizophrenia in the initial prodromal phase. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58(2):158-164. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.2.158 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Fries P, Neuenschwander S, Engel AK, Goebel R, Singer W. Rapid feature selective neuronal synchronization through correlated latency shifting. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4(2):194-200. doi: 10.1038/84032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Michalareas G, Vezoli J, van Pelt S, Schoffelen JM, Kennedy H, Fries P. Alpha-beta and gamma rhythms subserve feedback and feedforward influences among human visual cortical areas. Neuron. 2016;89(2):384-397. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Rutigliano G, Valmaggia L, Landi P, et al. . Persistence or recurrence of non-psychotic comorbid mental disorders associated with 6-year poor functional outcomes in patients at ultra high risk for psychosis. J Affect Disord. 2016;203:101-110. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.05.053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Lee TY, Shin YS, Shin NY, et al. . Neurocognitive function as a possible marker for remission from clinical high risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2014;153(1-3):48-53. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.01.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Friston KJ, Frith CD. Schizophrenia: a disconnection syndrome? Clin Neurosci. 1995;3(2):89-97. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Stephan KE, Friston KJ, Frith CD. Dysconnection in schizophrenia: from abnormal synaptic plasticity to failures of self-monitoring. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35(3):509-527. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn176 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Uhlhaas PJ, Gajwani R, Gross J, Gumley AI, Lawrie SM, Schwannauer M. The Youth Mental Health Risk and Resilience Study (YouR-Study). BMC Psychiatry. 2017;17(1):43. doi: 10.1186/s12888-017-1206-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Grent-’t-Jong T, Rivolta D, Gross J, et al. . Acute ketamine dysregulates task-related gamma-band oscillations in thalamo-cortical circuits in schizophrenia. Brain. 2018;141(8):2511-2526. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy175 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Yung AR, Yuen HP, McGorry PD, et al. . Mapping the onset of psychosis: the Comprehensive Assessment of At-Risk Mental States. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2005;39(11-12):964-971. doi: 10.1080/j.1440-1614.2005.01714.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Schultze-Lutter F, Addington J, Ruhrmann S, Klosterkötter K. Schizophrenia Proneness Instrument, Adult Version. (SPI-A). Giovanni Fioriti Editore; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.First MB, Spitzer R. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM IV Axis I Disorders—Patient Edition (SCID-I/P Version 2.0). Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1987;13(2):261-276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Keefe RS, Goldberg TE, Harvey PD, Gold JM, Poe MP, Coughenour L. The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery. Schizophr Res. 2004;68(2-3):283-297. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Hoogenboom N, Schoffelen JM, Oostenveld R, Parkes LM, Fries P. Localizing human visual gamma-band activity in frequency, time and space. Neuroimage. 2006;29(3):764-773. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, Schoffelen JM. FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput Intell Neurosci. 2011;2011:156869. doi: 10.1155/2011/156869 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Bastiaansen MC, Knösche TR. Tangential derivative mapping of axial MEG applied to event-related desynchronization research. Clin Neurophysiol. 2000;111(7):1300-1305. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00272-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Gross J, Kujala J, Hamalainen M, Timmermann L, Schnitzler A, Salmelin R. Dynamic imaging of coherent sources: Studying neural interactions in the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98(2):694-699. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.694 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Tallon-Baudry C, Bertrand O, Delpuech C, Pernier J. Stimulus specificity of phase-locked and non-phase-locked 40 Hz visual responses in human. J Neurosci. 1996;16(13):4240-4249. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-13-04240.1996 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Bastos AM, Vezoli J, Bosman CA, et al. . Visual areas exert feedforward and feedback influences through distinct frequency channels. Neuron. 2015;85(2):390-401. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Dias EC, Butler PD, Hoptman MJ, Javitt DC. Early sensory contributions to contextual encoding deficits in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(7):654-664. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.17 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Haenschel C, Bittner RA, Haertling F, et al. . Contribution of impaired early-stage visual processing to working memory dysfunction in adolescents with schizophrenia: a study with event-related potentials and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(11):1229-1240. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.11.1229 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Sergi MJ, Rassovsky Y, Nuechterlein KH, Green MF. Social perception as a mediator of the influence of early visual processing on functional status in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(3):448-454. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.3.448 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Fornito A, Yoon J, Zalesky A, Bullmore ET, Carter CS. General and specific functional connectivity disturbances in first-episode schizophrenia during cognitive control performance. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70(1):64-72. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Murray JD, Anticevic A, Gancsos M, et al. . Linking microcircuit dysfunction to cognitive impairment: effects of disinhibition associated with schizophrenia in a cortical working memory model. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24(4):859-872. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Lisman JE, Coyle JT, Green RW, et al. . Circuit-based framework for understanding neurotransmitter and risk gene interactions in schizophrenia. Trends Neurosci. 2008;31(5):234-242. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.02.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Saunders JA, Gandal MJ, Siegel SJ. NMDA antagonists recreate signal-to-noise ratio and timing perturbations present in schizophrenia. Neurobiol Dis. 2012;46(1):93-100. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2011.12.049 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Veit J, Hakim R, Jadi MP, Sejnowski TJ, Adesnik H. Cortical gamma band synchronization through somatostatin interneurons. Nat Neurosci. 2017;20(7):951-959. doi: 10.1038/nn.4562 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Hashimoto T, Bazmi HH, Mirnics K, Wu Q, Sampson AR, Lewis DA. Conserved regional patterns of GABA-related transcript expression in the neocortex of subjects with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165(4):479-489. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07081223 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Fisher M, Loewy R, Hardy K, Schlosser D, Vinogradov S. Cognitive interventions targeting brain plasticity in the prodromal and early phases of schizophrenia. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2013;9:435-463. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143134 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Hadar R, Bikovski L, Soto-Montenegro ML, et al. . Early neuromodulation prevents the development of brain and behavioral abnormalities in a rodent model of schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry. 2018;23(4):943-951. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.52 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplement.

eMethods. Supplementary Methods

eResults. Supplementary Results

eTable 1. Overview of Effect Sizes (Cohen’s d)

eTable 2. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of CHR-P Subgroups

eTable 3. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics for Persistent-APS vs Non-Persistent APS CHR-P Subgroups

eFigure 1. Sensor Data Statistics: Post-Hoc Comparisons

eFigure 2. Virtual channel TFR/ITPC Group Differences in ROIs

eFigure 3. Virtual Channel Baseline FFT Spectra over Visual Cortex

eFigure 4. Virtual Channel ERF Data

eFigure 5. Correlations MEG data, Behavior and Clinical Parameters

eFigure 6. Time-Frequency and Inter-Trial-Phase-Coherence in CHR-P subgroups

eFigure 7. Granger-Causality (GC) connectivity changes in main and CHR-P subgroups


Articles from JAMA Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of American Medical Association

RESOURCES