Abstract
This study examines the association of parent-reported and behaviorally assessed child self-regulation with their use of mobile devices, including mean weekly exposure and age at first use.
Young children’s use of mobile devices (eg, smartphones, tablets) is prevalent and increasing.1 Although television exposure can harm young children’s self-regulation,2 little research has investigated associations between children’s use of mobile devices and this consequential capacity. We examined associations between multimethod assessments of self-regulation and screen media device use in early childhood, comparing use of traditional (eg, television, computers) and mobile devices.
Methods
Data were collected from July 1, 2016, through January 11, 2019. The institutional review board at the University of California, Davis, approved this research, and we obtained written parental consent and verbal child assent. As part of a longitudinal study, we recruited a community sample of children aged 32 to 47 months. In our laboratory, parents completed demographic questions; the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire3 or Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire,4 per the child’s age; and the Screen Media Survey, a novel measure of duration, content, and context of children’s screen media use by device. For this study, we aggregated children’s mean durations of weekday and weekend or holiday use of devices into weekly weighted means; parents also reported the age at which children began using each device owned. In another room, children attempted an 11-task behavioral self-regulation battery adapted from Kochanska and colleagues5 (eg, Day/Night, Whisper, Gift Delay); scores were standardized, and a mean was calculated (κ = 0.9; α = .70), with higher scores indicating better self-regulation. Controlling for demographic covariates for children with complete screen media data, we estimated multivariable linear regressions explaining children’s parent-reported or behaviorally assessed self-regulation from their mean weekly use of screen media devices, their age at first use of screen media devices, and statistical interactions between the 2. We also used this analysis to test the reverse direction of outcome. We used a 2-tailed P value threshold of .05, for all analyses. Analyses were completed in SPSS Statistics version 26 (IBM).
Results
A total of 73 children were recruited, and 56 were included in analyses. Parents (48 women [85.7%]) had a mean (SD) age of 36.7 (3.8) years; children (26 girls [46.4%]) had a mean (SD) age of 37.4 (4.5) months (Table 1). Fifty children (89.3%) used traditional devices weekly (mean [SD] time, 10.8 [11.7] hours; range, 0-68 hours), and 40 (71.4%) used mobile devices weekly (mean [SD] time, 3.9 [3.9] hours; range, 0-14 hours). Children began using screen media devices at ages 3 to 36 months (mean [SD] age, 16.1 [7.0] months). Children’s mean (SD) Children’s Behavior Questionnaire or Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire self-regulation scores (effortful control broadband) were 5.2 (0.4; α = .70) and 5.0 (0.5; α = 0.8), respectively, on a scale of 0 to 7 points. Accounting for child age (β, 0.39 [95% CI, 0.01-0.06]; P = .004) and sex, family income, and parent education (with no significant differences between groups), children’s mean weekly use of mobile devices (β, −0.27 [95% CI, −0.057 to −0.002]; P = .04) and age at first use of any screen media device (β, 0.28 [95% CI, 0.002-0.03]; P = .03) each were associated with children’s behaviorally assessed self-regulation scores (F2,46 = 6.02; P = .003; R2 = 0.34), but not parent-reported self-regulation (Table 2). Use of traditional media and separate assessment of age at first use of traditional vs mobile devices did not explain self-regulation. No evidence emerged of a statistical interaction between current use of screen media devices and the age at first use or the reverse direction of association; children’s behaviorally assessed or parent-reported self-regulation also did not explain their mean weekly use of screen media devices.
Table 1. Demographics of Study Participantsa.
Variable | No. (%) |
---|---|
Child | |
Female | 26 (46.43) |
Racial/ethnic minority | 24 (42.86) |
Race, No. | |
White | 34 |
Biracial/multiracial | 16 |
Asian/Asian American | 5 |
Black/African American | 1 |
Hispanic ethnicity, No. | 8 |
Parent | |
Female | 48 (85.71) |
Employed | 39 (69.64) |
Racial/ethnic minorityb | 25 (44.64) |
Race, No.b | |
White | 33 |
Asian/Asian American | 11 |
Black/African American | 9 |
Biracial/multiracial | 1 |
Hispanic ethnicity, No. | 6 |
Family income, $b | |
20 000-40 000 | 1 (1.85) |
40 000-60 000 | 4 (7.41) |
60 000-80 000 | 8 (14.81) |
80 000-100 000 | 6 (11.11) |
>100 000 | 35 (64.81) |
Included 56 individuals, unless otherwise noted.
Included 54 individuals.
Table 2. Summary of Multivariable Linear Regressions Explaining Children’s Behaviorally Assessed Self-regulation (N = 56).
Model | B (95% CI) | SE | β | P value |
---|---|---|---|---|
Parent education | ||||
1 | −0.07 (−0.17 to 0.04) | 0.05 | −0.17 | .20 |
2 | −0.07 (−0.18 to 0.03) | 0.05 | −0.18 | .17 |
3 | −0.06 (−0.16 to 0.05) | 0.05 | −0.14 | .26 |
4 | −0.07 (−0.17 to 0.04) | 0.05 | −0.16 | .20 |
Family income | ||||
1 | −0.02 (−0.12 to 0.08) | 0.05 | −0.05 | .72 |
2 | −0.02 (−0.12 to 0.08) | 0.05 | −0.04 | .74 |
3 | −0.00 (−0.10 to 0.10) | 0.05 | −0.01 | .97 |
4 | −0.01 (−0.10 to 0.09) | 0.05 | −0.01 | .92 |
Child’s age | ||||
1 | 0.03 (0.01 to 0.06) | 0.01 | 0.36 | .009 |
2 | 0.04 (0.01 to 0.06) | 0.01 | 0.36 | .008 |
3 | 0.04 (0.01 to 0.06) | 0.01 | 0.40 | .004 |
4 | 0.04 (0.01 to 0.06) | 0.01 | 0.39 | .004 |
Child’s sex | ||||
1 | 0.08 (−0.15 to 0.30) | 0.12 | 0.09 | .50 |
2 | 0.11 (−0.11 to 0.33) | 0.11 | 0.13 | .33 |
3 | 0.09 (−0.15 to 0.31) | 0.11 | 0.10 | .43 |
4 | 0.12 (−0.10 to 0.34) | 0.11 | 0.14 | .27 |
Child’s mean weekly use, h | ||||
All screen media devicesa | ||||
1 | −0.15 (−0.29 to −0.02) | 0.07 | −0.29 | .03 |
Mobile devices | ||||
2 | −0.04 (−0.06 to −0.01) | 0.01 | −0.34 | .009 |
4 | −0.03 (−0.06 to −0.002) | 0.01 | −0.27 | .04 |
Traditional devicesa | ||||
2 | −0.09 (−0.19 to 0.02) | 0.05 | −0.20 | .11 |
Child’s age at first use of any screen media device, mo | ||||
3 | 0.02 (0.01 to 0.04) | 0.01 | 0.33 | .01 |
4 | 0.02 (0.002 to 0.03) | 0.01 | 0.28 | .03 |
R2 (F for change in R2)b | ||||
1 | 0.25 (5.18) | .03 | ||
2 | 0.31 (4.70) | .01 | ||
3 | 0.27 (6.83) | .01 | ||
4 | 0.34 (6.02) | .005 |
Log transformed to address positive skew.
The F value for change in R2 indicates improvement over the model with demographic covariates only.
Discussion
Although cross-sectional designs cannot determine causality, our findings indicate that young children who began using screen media devices earlier or who spent more time engaging with mobile devices displayed lower self-regulation in a behavioral battery. In contrast with previous research,6 we found no evidence that self-regulation explained children’s use of screen media devices in this sample. Sample size and composition limit the generalizability of these findings beyond middle-class groups. Longitudinal studies, investigations of screen media content, and more rigorous assessments of screen media device use are needed to elucidate relations to self-regulation development.
References
- 1.Rideout V. The Common Sense census: media use by kids age zero to eight. Published 2017. Accessed February 21, 2020. https://www.commonsensemedia.org/sites/default/files/uploads/research/csm_zerotoeight_fullreport_release_2.pdf
- 2.Lillard AS, Li H, Boguszewski K. Television and children’s executive function. Adv Child Dev Behav. 2015;48:219-248. doi: 10.1016/bs.acdb.2014.11.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Putnam SP, Rothbart MK. Development of short and very short forms of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. J Pers Assess. 2006;87(1):102-112. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa8701_09 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Putnam SP, Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK. Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler temperament: the early childhood behavior questionnaire. Infant Behav Dev. 2006;29(3):386-401. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.01.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kochanska G, Murray K, Jacques TY, Koenig AL, Vandegeest KA. Inhibitory control in young children and its role in emerging internalization. Child Dev. 1996;67(2):490-507. doi: 10.2307/1131828 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Radesky JS, Peacock-Chambers E, Zuckerman B, Silverstein M. Use of mobile technology to calm upset children: associations with social-emotional development. JAMA Pediatr. 2016;170(4):397-399. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.4260 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]