Abstract
Imagine smelling a novel perfume with only one nostril and then smelling it again with the other nostril. Clearly, you can tell that it is the same perfume both times. This simple experiment demonstrates that odor information is shared across both hemispheres to enable perceptual unity. In many sensory systems, perceptual unity is believed to be mediated by inter-hemispheric connections between iso-functional cortical regions. However, in the olfactory system, the underlying neural mechanisms that enable this coordination are unclear because the two olfactory cortices are not topographically organized and do not seem to have homotypic inter-hemispheric mapping. This review presents recent advances in determining which aspects of odor information are processed unilaterally or bilaterally, and how odor information is shared across the two hemispheres. We argue that understanding the mechanisms of inter-hemispheric coordination can provide valuable insights that are hard to achieve when focusing on one hemisphere alone.
Subject terms: Neural circuits, Olfactory system, Learning and memory
Dalal et al. provide a thorough review of bilateral hemispheric interactions in the context of odor perception and memory. They summarize reports on anatomy, physiology and behavior related to olfaction. This review provides insights on how odor information can be processed unilaterally and/or bilaterally despite the lack of topographical organization.
Introduction: inter-hemispheric communication and perceptual unity
In the early 1940s, patients suffering from severe untreatable epileptic seizures underwent a pioneering operation in which their corpus callosum was severed. While this mitigated the epilepsy syndromes, it generated strange phenomena: each hemisphere seemed to have its own separate perception, concepts, and even actions. When such split-brain patients were shown an image of an object in their left visual field, they could not vocally name what they had seen. This is because information about the image seen in the left visual field is sent only to the right side of the brain, while the speech-control center is usually on the left side of the brain. Nonetheless, patients could pick up the correct object with their left hand because it is controlled by the right hemisphere1. Some of the surgery-induced deficits improved over time, but they illustrate one important principle: the fibers that interconnect the hemispheres are responsible for transferring information from one side to the other. These connecting fibers may be required to achieve perceptual unity, at least in some sensory modalities.
But how is perceptual unity achieved? There are several possible ways to achieve perceptual unity in the healthy brain. One way is to ensure that sensory information is transmitted to both hemispheres directly from the sensory organs. This is how auditory information is unified across hemispheres: information from both ears converges to the left and the right Superior Olivary Complexes2 such that signals sent to the auditory cortices contain integrated information from both ears. However, not all sensory information is projected bilaterally. For example, ganglion cells activated by the left visual field project to the right hemisphere only and vice versa3. By contrast tactile and noxious sensory information is projected unilaterally4–6. To achieve perceptual unity in these sensory modalities, downstream neurons must be interconnected across hemispheres. This is accomplished by axonal connections between the two sides of the nervous system, known as commissures.
Commissural systems are present throughout vertebrate and invertebrate species. The commissure fibers include the corpus callosum, the anterior and posterior commissures and the hippocampal commissure. The corpus callosum, which is exclusive to eutherian (placental) mammals, is the largest commissural track. The anterior commissure (AC) connects the temporal lobes, in which the olfactory cortex resides. There are also commissure tracks that interconnect subcortical regions.
In general, commissural systems connect homotypic regions in each hemisphere7,8. Hence, information sharing between hemispheres is greatly simplified when the sensory information is topographically organized in the cortex according to some physical property, as is the case for almost all sensory systems9. However, there is no known topographical organization of odor space in the main olfactory cortices (OCs)10–15. This raises the intriguing question of how odor information is shared between the two olfactory cortices.
Studying how sensory information is shared through interconnected cortical regions provides a unique window into the processes involved in learning and memory, information coding, and perception. For example, learning-related plastic changes could occur in upstream neurons, before the sensory information is shared between the hemispheres. This would result in unilateral memories, whose analysis could shed light on how memories are encoded, where they are formed and how they are accessed. Consider another example: When conflicting stimuli are provided to the left and the right hemispheres, a perceptual conflict sometimes occurs known as binocular rivalry or binaural rivalry, in which only one of the two stimuli is perceived at a time16,17. This phenomenon can be used to reveal which hemisphere is more dominant and how dominance is represented and achieved.
Here we review recent advances in research on the unilateral and bilateral processing of odor information and their implications for learning, memory and perception. We start with a short review of the olfactory system with a focus on the bilateral connections. We then discuss what can be learned from delivering odors either unilaterally or bilaterally. We then discuss results from experiments on rodents that are beginning to reveal the underlying neural circuits.
An overview of the olfactory system
In mammals, smelling begins with the inhalation of odorized air into the nasal cavity. Inhalation is followed by exhalation to form a respiration cycle. The typical awake freely breathing respiration rates are ~0.1–0.3 Hz in humans and ~1–3 Hz in mice18–20. The flow of air in one nostril is greater than the other because there is a slight turbinate swelling in one nostril that switches between the nostrils every few hours21–25. The nasal cavity is a complex organ containing several passages and turbinates. In the upper part of the nasal cavity, a dense sheet of neurons forms the olfactory epithelium. The epithelium contains millions of olfactory sensory neurons (OSNs), each expressing just one olfactory receptor (OR) out of a large repertoire estimated to include ~1000 different ORs in mice26. The left and the right olfactory epithelia are separated by a septum that prevents direct communication between them. The interaction between an odorant molecule and an OR triggers a cascade of events that transduces this interaction into an equivalent neural signal27. An OR can respond to several odorants. The set of all such odorants can be considered the OR’s receptive field. Each OSN sends out only one unbranched axon unilaterally to a structure called the glomerulus, which is located on the olfactory bulb’s (OB) surface28. Glomeruli are spheroid or ellipsoid conglomerates of neuropil in which synapses are formed between the OSNs’ axons and OB neurons. OSNs projecting to one glomerulus all express the same OR29,30. Thus, each odor can be uniquely represented by the set of activated glomeruli, which in turn represents the set of ORs to which that odorant binds. The time of glomeruli activation is believed to be part of the odor code20,31–34 (and see ref. 35). Glomeruli in the left and right OBs that receive input from the same OR type are located in mirror-symmetric locations conserved within the species36–38. While each OSN projects to only one glomerulus, OSNs expressing the same OR project to one of two glomeruli; one on the medial side of the bulb and one on the lateral side of the bulb, forming two mirror-symmetric maps30,39,40.
Each glomerulus is innervated by a few dozen mitral and tufted (M/T) cells5. M/T cells interact with several inhibitory neurons in the OB41–52 and carry the processed olfactory information from the OB to several olfactory cortical areas including the anterior olfactory nucleus (AON), piriform cortex (PC), amygdala, entorhinal cortex (Ent), tenia tecta (TT), and the olfactory tubercle (OT)53. The AON is located caudally to the OB and rostrally to the PC, and is therefore the most anterior region receiving direct information from the OB. The AON is divided into four main regions corresponding to their positions (pars dorsalis, medialis, lateralis and ventralis; together termed pars principalis) and another external region called pars externa (AONpE). The AONpE is a band of neurons circumscribing the lateral and dorsal parts of the anterior AON. Early anatomical studies showed that the projections of M/T cells to the AONpE maintain a rough topography of the glomerulus positions in the lateral-medial and ventral-dorsal axes54. A more recent study showed that it also preserves the anterior–posterior axis55,56
The PC is the most highly investigated olfactory cortex and is believed to serve as an association cortex57. The PC is divided into anterior and posterior portions (aPC and pPC). The boundary between the two subdivisions is marked by a dramatic decrease in the thickness of the lateral olfactory tract58. The aPC receives more afferent inputs from the OB, whereas the pPC receives mainly associational inputs59. With the exception of the AONpE, OC neurons are innervated by several M/T cells11,12,60,61. The number of OB inputs that each OC neuron integrates is estimated to range between 4 and 100 and is likely to differ between OC regions11,60 (Fig. 2a). An OC neuron typically fires only if several of these inputs are active60–62. The exact number of M/T cell connections to a single OC cell, the fraction of these connections that must be active to drive an OC cell, and the different computations employed by different OC cells on these inputs are still open questions.
While the left and right olfactory systems exhibit anatomical symmetry, there are also some noteworthy differences. Several studies in humans have shown that odor recognition involves the right piriform and orbital frontal cortices, while pleasantness rating is predominated in the left PC63. Odor discrimination is better when using the left nostril64,65. Recent studies in rodents have also revealed lateralization in performing odor discrimination66,67 and valence68. For a comprehensive review of olfactory lateralization see ref. 69.
Anatomy of bilateral connectivity
The bilateral connectivity between the two olfactory cortices is mediated by the anterior commissure (AC). Most bilateral connections are mediated by the AON which has extensive projections to the contralateral AON, PC, and OB (Fig. 1). Anatomical studies utilizing anterograde and retrograde tracing to investigate the projections to the contralateral hemisphere have found that the AON pars dorsalis and pars lateralis receive dense projections from every subregion of the contralateral AON including the AONpE70,71 (Fig. 1a–b). The AON constitutes the largest source of centrifugal inputs to the OB72–74 (Fig. 1). The pars dorsalis & lateralis were shown to project bilaterally to the OBs but asymmetrically (ipsilateral projections are more dense), whereas projections to the OB from the posterior part of the pars ventralis (referred to as the pars ventroposterior) are bilateral and symmetric75–78. The pars ventroposterior together with pars lateralis, dorsalis & medialis project to the contralateral aPC79,80 (Fig. 1a).
In addition, the AONpE receives large inputs from the contralateral pars dorsalis and additional minor inputs from the contralateral pars ventralis70 (Fig. 1b). The AONpE projects topographically to the contralateral granule cell layer (GCL) underneath the iso-functional glomeruli54,56,81–83 (Fig. 1b).
PC neurons are also interconnected. Anterograde injections to the aPC on one side terminate in the contralateral pPC. Retrograde injection to the contralateral pPC revealed that the input originates in the ipsilateral aPC in layer IIb71. In addition, aPC receives input from the contralateral nucleus of the lateral olfactory tract80. According to Haberly and Price, the TT and the OT are not involved in inter-hemispheric communication71. All the currently known inter-hemispheric connections between the OBs, AONs and PCs are summarized in Fig. 1. To summarize, there are many commissural connections in the olfactory cortex. These intricate bilateral connections are likely to play a central role in the exchange of olfactory information and the formation of a unified odor percept.
Functional bilateral connectivity
The ways in which the contralateral projections affect odor responses has been much less investigated. One study showed that AON back-projections to the contra-OB is coupled to inhalation, and is higher in the awake state than in the anesthetized state76. AON-to-contra-OB inhibitory and excitatory back-projections were shown to be odor dependent76. Optogenetic activation of AON back-projections to the contra-OB have been found to elicit inhibition in M/T cells and excite local inhibitory neurons75. A more recent study showed that optogenetic activation of AONpE back-projections to the contra-OB can also excite M/T cells81.
As mentioned above, each OC neuron receives direct inputs from multiple ipsilateral M/T cells and responds only if several of these neurons are active. However, an OC neuron can also respond to an odor delivered to the contra-nostril84,85. There are at least two possible pathways by which OC neurons can respond to a contra-nostril odor stimulus. In the direct pathway, an OC neuron receives input from the contra-OC via the AC (Fig. 2a). OC neurons can also receive olfactory input indirectly. A recent study found that optogenetically activated contra-M/T neurons can activate mirror-symmetric ipsi-M/T neurons via the contra-AONpE81. About one-third of the recorded M/T cells were activated by optogenetically activating contra-M/T cells and these activations were relatively weaker than ipsi-activated M/T cells (Fig. 2c–e). Connected M/T cells responded to similar odors, although odor delivered to the contra-nostril typically elicited weaker responses. A recent study found similar bilateral symmetric excitatory link between zebrfish M/T cells86. An OC neuron can thus be activated by a contra-odor indirectly through the contralateral M/T cells that responded to the odor and then activate the corresponding ipsilateral M/T cells via the AONpE. The indirect pathway is summarized in Fig. 2b.
The direct pathway predicts that an OC neuron will have different ipsilateral and contralateral odor receptive fields. This is because OC neurons receive their input from a random set of M/T cells. Thus, an ipsi-OC and a contra-OC neurons are likely to receive their inputs from a different set of glomeruli (Fig. 2a). The indirect pathway predicts that an OC neuron will have similar odor receptive fields for ipsilateral and contralateral odors because the neuron is either directly activated by the ipsi-M/T cells it is connected to, or indirectly through the symmetric set of contra-M/T cells that also activate the mirror-symmetric ipsi-M/T cells. Since only approximately one-third of the M/T cells are connected by the indirect pathway to the contra-M/T cells, and those that are connected have relatively weak connections81, the indirect pathway also predicts that only a subset of OC neurons will respond bilaterally and that these responses will be weaker than ipsi-odor responses (Fig. 2b). A study that examined the response of AON pars principalis neurons to a large set of odors found that 60% of the neurons responded to ipsilateral and contralateral odors85. Responses to contra-odors were generally weaker. This result is inline with the indirect pathway. However, a very recent study found that a substential number of AON and aPC neurons responded to ipsi- and contralateral odors and that these responses are of similar magnitute87. This similarity in bilateral odor receptive fields suggests that AON and aPC neurons receive contra-input through the direct pathway. Another study found that some aPC neurons responded only to contra-odor or only to ipsi-odor, but a few responded only when the odor was delivered bilaterally84. This finding might suggest that some aPC neurons receive direct input from the contra-OC. However, since only one odor was used in this study, a comprehensive comparison of odor receptive fields could not be made. Future experiments that will examine the ipsi- and contra- odor receptive fields of neurons located in different OC regions to a battery of odor stimuli delivered at different concentrations to each nostril will shed new light on the bilateral functional connectivity in the olfactory system and the role of the indirect and the direct pathways.
How is perceptual unity achieved in olfaction?
The indirect pathway suggests a simple solution to the perceptual unity problem in olfaction. Assuming that an odor inhaled through one nostril is encoded by a small set of strongly activated glomeruli ipsilateral to that nostril, the M/T cells innervating these glomeruli will activate their mirror-symmetric contra-M/T cells, which will result in sharing the odor code across both bulbs (Fig. 2b–e). OC neurons in each hemisphere can then read out the same glomerular pattern from their corresponding OB. These neurons will be activated regardless of the odor entry via the left, the right, or both nostrils. Consistent with this model, it was found that the neural response elicited by contra-odors is sufficient to decode the odor identity by a classifier trained on the neural responses elicited by ipsi-odors81. Thus, the indirect pathway is sufficient to share odor identity across bulbs. This circuit enables sharing of odor information across hemispheres in the absence of a cortical topographical organization and homotypic projections, suggesting that olfactory glomerular maps are the equivalent of cortical sensory maps found in other senses. Thus, although there is no known topographical organization of OC neurons, the glomerular organization in the OB together with the indirect pathway can maintain the continuity of sensory maps across hemispheres as in other sensory systems. Alternatively, it is possible that odors are shared across hemispheres via the direct pathway. In this scheme, the AC must preferably interconnect neurons that have similar odor receptive fields despite the lack of topographical organization (Fig. 2a). This can be achieved in two steps. The first would form bilateral connections in an activity-independent manner to produce a rough map characterized by excessive inter-hemispheric connections. The second would involve activity-dependent pruning leaving behind mostly bilateral connections of similar receptive fields. It may be possible to have a similar set of neurons activated by ipsi- and contra-odors, even if the activity-dependent inter-OC connections are not reciprocal (Fig. 3). The exisitance of extensive bilateral connections suggests that the direct pathway plays an important role in bilateral information transfer87.
Random connectivity between OB and OC neurons10–12 makes the task of achieving perceptual unity more difficult. One way of overcoming this randomness in connections has been suggested in recent modeling studies88,89. The idea is that two neurons, each located downstream of PC neurons in a different hemisphere, can have similar odor receptive fields, despite the variability in the OB–OC connections in the two hemispheres, if they integrate odor information from a large number of OC neurons. This intriguing idea can explain how the mushroom body output neurons located in the two hemispheres of the Drosophila brain can have similar receptive fields90. Furthermore, it provides an explanation for how similar odor responses, and therefore olfactory behaviors, can be achieved across subjects. However, it does not explain how a unilateral odor memory is accessed bilaterally (see below and ref. 91).
How inter-nostril interactions affect odor perception
Since the brain must achieve perceptual unity for proper functioning, delivering different odors to the left and right nostrils and evaluating the resulting perception can reveal important sensory processing principles. A study examined the perceived odor intensity in humans when the same odor was delivered at different concentrations to the two nostrils, and found that the intensity perceived when inhaling the odor with both nostrils open was slightly lower than the sum of the intensities perceived when inhaled with the left nostril and the right nostril individually92. This result may suggest that the perceived odor intensity reflects the overall neural activity in both hemispheres summated sub-linearly or that there is some weak inter-hemispheric suppression that reduces the overall perceived intensity.
Another study examined how odors are perceived when each nostril receives a different odor16. The experimenters delivered Phenyl Ethyl Alcohol (PEA, a rose-like smell) to one nostril and n-butanol (a marker like -smell) to the other. Subjects were asked to report if the odor smelled was more like a “rose” or “marker”. They found that the subjects perceived either the marker or the rose odor16. The odor perception was constant within a sniff and could change between sniffs. This suggests that although each hemisphere presumably perceived a different odor, only one percept dominated rather than both contributing equally. Recording the neural activity of OC neurons during unilateral and bilateral stimulations could shed light on the possible mechanisms underlying this binaral rivalry phenomenon and the neural substrate of perception. One possibility is that there is contra-hemispheric inhibition among PC neurons resulting in a winner-takes-all arrangement, such that the left-odor dominates if the left-odor responding neurons inhibit the right-odor responding neurons, and vice versa.
Cross-nostril odor adaptation
In all sensory systems, prolonged exposure to a stimulus causes adaption such that the detection threshold of the adapted stimulus is increased. Olfaction is no exception: prolonged exposure to an odor increases the detection threshold to that odor93. Adaptation to one odorant does not substantially affect the detection threshold of another structurally dissimilar odorant93–95. Neural adaptation have been shown to occur at multiple levels from OSN to M/T and PC neurons96–101. Adaptation of aPC neurons seems to be odor specific98,99. This finding suggests that the adaptation is not a result of a global reduction in neuronal excitability or an increase in phasic recurrent inhibition. In vitro and in vivo experiments showed that the reduced response of aPC neurons during adaptation is due to synaptic depression of cortical afferents102. Thus, adaptation to one odor will cause adaptation to another odor only if they activate a similar set of M/T cells. Investigating cross-nostril adaptation can provide additional insights as to where adaptation occurs, the underlying mechanisms, and to what extent odor information is shared across hemispheres. Studies that investigated cross-nostril adaptation in humans found that an odorant stimulus presented to one nostril caused bilateral adaptation, although adaptation was stronger and longer in the ipsilateral nostril16,92. 12-day-old rat pups that were habituated to an odorant on one side were not habituated to the odorant on the other side when the AC was transected before habituation started103. This finding implies that the adaptation is transferred to the other side through the AC. If ipsilateral odor adaptation is a result of ipsi-M/T afferent synaptic depression, how does cross-nostril adaptation occur? One possible mechanism is that contra-M/T cells that were activated via the indirect pathway also undergo synaptic depression. Since only a subset of contra-M/T cells are active during ipsi-nostril odor stimulation81, cross-nostril adaptation will be weaker than ipsi-nostril adaptation. Alternatively, adaptation could be the result of reduced excitability caused by the direct pathway. An experiment in which contra-M/T neurons are silenced during ipsi-nostril adaptation could determine the exact mechanism.
Cross-nostril odor detection
While prolonged continuous exposure to an odorant may increase its detection threshold, short and repeated brief exposures to an odorant over days may have the opposite effect, enabling detection of odorants that were not detected previously94,104,105. About 30% of the adult population cannot smell the molecule androstenone but can be trained to detect it106–108. The mechanism underlying this phenomenon is unknown. One study found that people with specific anosmia to androstenone who learned to detect androstenone with one nostril plugged could detect it with their previously plugged nostril108. This may suggest that learning to detect androstenone occurs at a central location that has bilateral access to odorant stimuli or that the changes are shared bilaterally via the indirect pathway. Alternatively, it is possible that over the training days the number of OSNs expressing the ORs that are sensitive to androstenone in both epithelia slowly increased following some bilateral upregulation process. Consistent with this mechanism, changes in the activity of OSNs in the epithelium of mice trained to detect androstenone were reported using electro-olfactography107. Measuring changes in the activity of OSNs and M/T cells in the plugged nostril may demonstrate that these changes occur bilaterally.
Ipsilateral and contralateral odorant discrimination and generalization
As in the case of odorant detection thresholds, odor discrimination can also be improved with training109,110. Enantiomers are molecules that are mirror-reflections of each other. There are many enantiomers in nature. Some enantiomers elicit different smells and others elicit smells that are indistinguishable to humans111. However, people can be trained to discriminate between two previously undistinguishable enantiomers112,113. Subjects that were trained to discriminate between two previously undistinguishable enantiomers by exposing them to the odorants repeatedly over ~12 days with one nostril closed, failed to discriminate between the enantiomers with the previously closed nostril112. This finding suggests that changes occurring during the learning process were mostly unilateral. One possible mechanism to explain this result is that learning to discriminate the enantiomers involves changes in the interactions between M/T and local inhibitory neurons in the ipsi-OB which enhance the differences in the representations of the two previously indistinguishable odorants. Evidence of the involvement of local inhibitory neurons in learning to discriminate highly similar odorants has been reported114–117. These changes in local interneurons might only occur on the ipsi-side (perhaps because they might need high activation). Bilateral recording of M/T and OC cell responses in mice that learn to discriminate enantiomers with one nostril plugged could shed light on the exact locus of this learning process and its underlying mechanism.
It also remains unclear whether stimulus generalization is also nostril-specific. Generalization is the process of manifesting a conditioned response to a stimulus that is similar to a previously conditioned stimulus118–120. For example, pairing an odorant with a mild electrical shock results in freezing behavior to that odorant and similar odorants120. Generalization is a key process for survival. Determining whether generalization is nostril specific can further help reveal the mechanism underlying it.
Unilateral and bilateral odor memories
Adaptation is a simple form of memory that can be explained by synaptic depression. Learning to detect a previously undetected odorant or learning to discriminate between two very similar odorants requires mechanisms that involve changes in presumably central and peripheral circuits. Associative memory—learning to remember the relationship between two unrelated events—is an even higher level of memory and is likely to involve multiple brain regions. Can associative memory be confined to one hemisphere of an intact animal?
Kucharski and Hall investigated whether odorant association memory can be formed unilaterally. Their main training paradigm was to associate a milk reward with cedar odorant in rat pups. This was achieved by pairing the cedar odorant with intraoral infusions of milk while one nostril was occluded and then testing this odorant association by measuring the pups’ orientation towards cedar. 18-day-old pups trained with one naris closed showed increased preference for cedar odorant with both the trained and the untrained naris (Fig. 4a)121. This is expected, as odorant smelled with one nostril should be shared with the other hemisphere to enable perceptual unity. 6-day-old pups showed increased preference to cedar odorant only when tested with the trained naris but not with the untrained naris at the same age (Fig. 4b)122. This is also expected: as the AC is fully developed only after the age of 12 days123, 6-day-old pups are virtually split-brain rats with regard to their olfactory system and therefore odorant preference was not transmitted from one hemisphere to the other. Strikingly, these 6-day-old pups showed associative memory for the cedar odorant with the untrained naris when they reached the age of 18 days (Fig. 4b)91,121. This finding implies that the unilateral odorant association memory either became accessible from the contra-side or that it was transferred somehow to the contra-side. To determine which was correct, the authors dissected the AC of 18-day-old pups after they had learned the association for the cedar odorant with one nostril closed at the age of 6 days (Fig. 4c). The rats failed to show associative memory for the odorant with the untrained nostril although they continued to show the memory with the trained nostril. Hence, at least in this training paradigm, unilateral training forms a unilateral odor memory but this memory can be accessed from the untrained side through the AC. A more recent study showed that bilateral lesion of the AONpEs had the same effect as sectioning the AC56. A mouse placed in a maze with two arms received a mild electric shock following the presentation of odorant A or B in one of the arms. The shock was delivered in the same arm as the odorant when odorant A was delivered, and in the opposite arm when odorant B was delivered. The mice quickly learned to run to the other arm following the presentation of odorant A and to stay in the arm that they were in when odorant B was delivered. However, mice in which both AONpEs were lesioned before training (with one nostril) did not transfer this learning to the other hemisphere56. This finding suggests that AONpE is required for contralateral memory access or transfer. Since AONpE mainly projects topographically to the contralateral OB56,70,74,81 it is reasonable to assume that the indirect pathway is used for this memory retrieval. In contrast, one study found that mice could access a unilateral odorant association memory even after the olfactory peduncle was transected in the trained side124, which should block access to the trained side OB. This result implies that odorant association memory can be accessed without accessing the trained OB, lending weight to the direct pathway hypothesis for memory retrieval. It is possible that both pathways are involved in sharing odorant information across the hemispheres or that different memory types (e.g. fear conditioning versus odorant association) are accessed by different pathways.
Bilateral processing enables odorant localization
Although the distance between the two nostrils in most mammals is relatively small compared to the distance between the eyes or the ears, the existence of two symmetrical nostrils suggests that animals might use bilateral sampling to extract information about the location of an odorant source. Bilateral odorant sampling is used for odorant localization in many species. For example, Drosophila flies whose two antennae were exposed to air streams of different odorant concentrations turned toward the higher concentration125. Thus, flies utilize concentration differences between the two antennae to decide on the direction of the odorant source despite the fact that the OSNs in flies project to both sides of the brain. Flies are still able to distinguish the odorant contrast because the synaptic connections made by the sensory axons are stronger on the side where they receive their input, resulting in activation of the downstream neurons slightly earlier on the side with the higher odorant concentration126. Among vertebrates, hammer sharks (whose two nares are separated by several centimeters) have been shown to turn toward the side receiving the first stimulus rather than toward the side receiving the stronger stimulus127. This suggests that animals can also use bilateral time differences for odorant localization. Plugging one nostril in rats, or spatially reversing the antennae of a locust (by crossing and fixing the antennae), or switching between the left and right nostril inputs in blind moles or human subjects (by inserting plastic tubes into the nostrils and crossing them) hinders their ability to navigate toward the odorant source128–131. All these studies demonstrate that animals compare bilateral inputs to extract information about the odorant direction or location. Note that eliminating or crossing bilateral inputs generally disrupts but does not abolish odorant localization in freely behaving animals128,132,133. This suggests that animals can use unilateral odorant sampling strategies such as tracking odorant concentration gradients during movement or tracking wind direction to locate an odorant source. Nevertheless, they excel when both their nostrils are open.
The exact nature of the neural mechanism underlying odorant localization remains to be identified. Mice with AC transected or with their AON ablated failed to detect the direction of a novel odorant puff134. This strongly suggests that the AON is involved in comparing bilateral inputs. This is consistent with the observation that the majority of inter-hemispheric olfactory connections involve the AON74. One study found that neurons in the ventral part of the AONpE are excited by an odorant delivered to the ipsi-nostril and inhibited by the same odorant delivered to the contra-nostril135. Delivering different odorant concentrations to the left and the right nostrils showed that these AONpE neuron responses were correlated to the differences between the left and the right odorant concentrations. This finding suggests that ventral AONpE neurons are suitable candidates for comparing the bilateral inputs.
The recently discovered excitatory connections between sister-M/T cells across bulbs (the indirect pathway)81,86 might be involved in odorant localization (Fig. 2b). However, as discussed in Grobman et al. 81 excitatory inter-bulb connections decrease the difference in neural responses between the two sides generated by the difference in odorant concentrations across the two nostrils. This is because each M/T cell increases the response of its contralateral iso-functional M/T cell in a manner proportional to its activity. (Fig. 5a–b; see Grobman et al. for the model details). Inter-bulb inhibition is more likely to facilitate odorant localization since it enhances the difference in neural responses to different odorant concentrations between the two nostrils in a winner-takes-all fashion (Fig. 5c). The abundance of projections from the AONpE to the contra-OB granule cell layer strongly suggests that inter-hemispheric inhibition is involved in this circuit.
Conclusions
Inter-hemispheric neural interactions serve two major roles. They facilitate perceptual unity in sensory systems in which the sensory organs do not project bilaterally, and enable bilateral comparison of sensory information to extract additional information such as source localization. The neural circuits underlying these somewhat contradictory roles are now starting to be revealed. It is likely that several circuits achieve these goals. Understanding how the two hemispheres work together to achieve these goals is starting to provide valuable insights into long-standing questions in neuroscience: how neurons change during learning, how memories are formed, whether they are stored unilaterally or bilaterally, and how they are accessed. Future studies manipulating the bilateral stimuli or the bilateral information streams, together with bilateral recordings of neural activities in different brain regions, will help answer these fundamental questions.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the ISF-UGC joint research program, in which R.H. was supported by the Israel Science Foundation (2307/15) and N.G. was supported by the University Grants Commission (6-11/2016[IC]); and by the Wellcome Trust/DBT India Alliance Fellowship [grant number IA/I/15/2/502091] awarded to N.G.
Author contributions
T.D, N.G., and R.H wrote the manuscript. N.G. and R.H. secured funding.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Sperry RW. Hemisphere deconnection and unity in conscious awareness. Am. Psychol. 1968;23:723–733. doi: 10.1037/h0026839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Eldredge DH, Miller JD. Physiology of hearing. Annu Rev. Physiol. 1971;33:281–310. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ph.33.030171.001433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Boycott, B. B., J. D. Organization of the primate retina: Light microscopy, with an appendix: a second type of midget bipolar cell in the primate retina. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 255, 109–184 (1969).
- 4.Vallbo AB, Hagbarth KE, Torebjork HE, Wallin BG. Somatosensory, proprioceptive, and sympathetic activity in human peripheral nerves. Physiol. Rev. 1979;59:919–957. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1979.59.4.919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Shepherd, G. M. The Synaptic Organization of the Brain (Oxford University Press, 2004).
- 6.Applebaum AE, Beall JE, Foreman RD, Willis WD. Organization and receptive fields of primate spinothalamic tract neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 1975;38:572–586. doi: 10.1152/jn.1975.38.3.572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Zhou J, et al. Axon position within the corpus callosum determines contralateral cortical projection. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 2013;110:E2714–E2723. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1310233110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Hellige, J. B. Hemispheric Asymmetry: What’s Right and What’s Left (Harvard University Press, 2001).
- 9.Rothschild G, Mizrahi A. Global order and local disorder in brain maps. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2015;38:247–268. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ghosh S, et al. Sensory maps in the olfactory cortex defined by long-range viral tracing of single neurons. Nature. 2011;472:217–220. doi: 10.1038/nature09945. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Miyamichi K, et al. Cortical representations of olfactory input by trans-synaptic tracing. Nature. 2011;472:191–196. doi: 10.1038/nature09714. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Sosulski DL, Bloom ML, Cutforth T, Axel R, Datta SR. Distinct representations of olfactory information in different cortical centres. Nature. 2011;472:213–216. doi: 10.1038/nature09868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Stettler DD, Axel R. Representations of odor in the piriform cortex. Neuron. 2009;63:854–864. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Rennaker RL, Chen C-FF, Ruyle AM, Sloan AM, Wilson DA. Spatial and temporal distribution of odorant-evoked activity in the piriform cortex. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:1534–1542. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4072-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Roland B, Deneux T, Franks KM, Bathellier B, Fleischmann A. Odor identity coding by distributed ensembles of neurons in the mouse olfactory cortex. Elife. 2017;6:1–26. doi: 10.7554/eLife.26337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Zhou W, Chen D. Binaral rivalry between the nostrils and in the cortex. Curr. Biol. 2009;19:1561–1565. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Blake R, Logothetis NK. Visual competition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002;3:13–21. doi: 10.1038/nrn701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Russo MA, Santarelli DM, O’Rourke D. The physiological effects of slow breathing in the healthy human. Breathe. 2017;13:298–309. doi: 10.1183/20734735.009817. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Benchetrit G. Breathing pattern in humans: diversity and individuality. Respir. Physiol. 2000;122:123–129. doi: 10.1016/S0034-5687(00)00154-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Cury KM, Uchida N. Robust odor coding via inhalation-coupled transient activity in the mammalian olfactory bulb. Neuron. 2010;68:570–585. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Sobel N, Khan RM, Saltman A, Sullivan EV, Gabrieli JDE. The world smells different to each nostril. Nature. 1999;402:35–35. doi: 10.1038/46944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Pendolino AL, Lund VJ, Nardello E, Ottaviano G. The nasal cycle: a comprehensive review. Rhinol. Online. 2018;1:67–76. doi: 10.4193/RHINOL/18.021. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Parthasarathy K, Bhalla US. Laterality and symmetry in rat olfactory behavior and in physiology of olfactory input. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:5750–5760. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1781-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Porter J, Anand T, Johnson B, Khan RM, Sobel N. Brain mechanisms for extracting spatial information from smell. Neuron. 2005;47:581–592. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.06.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Bojsen-Moller F, Fahrenkrug J. Nasal swell-bodies and cyclic changes in the air passage of the rat and rabbit nose. J. Anat. 1971;110:25–37. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Buck L, Axel R. A novel multigene family may encode odorant receptors: a molecular basis for odor recognition. Cell. 1991;65:175–187. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(91)90418-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Reed, R. R. Signaling pathways in odorant detection. Neuron (1992). 10.1016/0896-6273(92)90287-N. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 28.Ressler KJ, Sullivan SL, Buck LB. A molecular dissection of spatial patterning in the olfactory system. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 1994;4:588–596. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(94)90061-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Vassar R, et al. Topographic organization of sensory projection to the olfactory bulb. Cell. 1994;79:981–991. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(94)90029-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Mombaerts P, et al. Visualizing an olfactory sensory map. Cell. 1996;87:675–686. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)81387-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Bathellier B, Buhl DL, Accolla R, Carleton A. Dynamic ensemble odor coding in the mammalian olfactory bulb: sensory information at different timescales. Neuron. 2008;57:586–598. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Sirotin YB, Shusterman R, Rinberg D. Neural coding of perceived odor intensity. eNeuro. 2015;2:1–16. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0083-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Bolding KA, Franks KM. Complementary codes for odor identity and intensity in olfactory cortex. Elife. 2017;6:1–26. doi: 10.7554/eLife.22630. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Uchida, N., Poo, C. & Haddad, R. Coding and transformations in the olfactory system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci.37, 363–385 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 35.Perl, O., Nahum, N., Belelovsky, K. & Haddad, R. The contribution of temporal coding to odor coding and odor perception in humans. Elife9, e49734 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 36.Oka Y, et al. Odorant receptor map in the mouse olfactory bulb: in vivo sensitivity and specificity of receptor-defined glomeruli. Neuron. 2006;52:857–869. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Soucy ER, Albeanu DF, Fantana AL, Murthy VN, Meister M. Precision and diversity in an odor map on the olfactory bulb. Nat. Neurosci. 2009;12:210–220. doi: 10.1038/nn.2262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Schaefer ML, Finger TE, Restrepo D. Variability of position of the P2 glomerulus within a map of the mouse olfactory bulb. J. Comp. Neurol. 2001;436:351–362. doi: 10.1002/cne.1072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Ressler KJ, Sullivan SL, Buck LB. Information coding in the olfactory system: evidence for a stereotyped and highly organized epitope map in the olfactory bulb. Cell. 1994;79:1245–1255. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(94)90015-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Schoenfeld TA, Cleland TA. Anatomical contributions to odorant sampling and representation in rodents: zoning in on sniffing behavior. Chem. Senses. 2006;31:131. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjj015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Yokoi M, Mori K, Nakanishi S. Refinement of odor molecule tuning by dendrodendritic synaptic inhibition in the olfactory bulb. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1995;92:3371–3375. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.8.3371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Fukunaga I, Herb JT, Kollo M, Boyden ES, Schaefer AT. Independent control of gamma and theta activity by distinct interneuron networks in the olfactory bulb. Nat. Neurosci. 2014;17:1208–1216. doi: 10.1038/nn.3760. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Liu S, Puche AC, Shipley MT. The interglomerular circuit potently inhibits olfactory bulb output neurons by both direct and indirect pathways. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:9604–9617. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1763-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Kim DH, Chang AY, McTavish TS, Patel HK, Willhite DC. Center-surround vs. distance-independent lateral connectivity in the olfactory bulb. Front. Neural Circuits. 2012;6:34. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2012.00034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Arevian AC, Kapoor V, Urban NN. Activity-dependent gating of lateral inhibition in the mouse olfactory bulb. Nat. Neurosci. 2008;11:80–87. doi: 10.1038/nn2030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Koulakov AA, Rinberg D. Sparse incomplete representations: a potential role of olfactory granule cells. Neuron. 2011;72:124–136. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.07.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Geramita MA, Burton SD, Urban NN. Distinct lateral inhibitory circuits drive parallel processing of sensory information in the mammalian olfactory bulb. Elife. 2016;5:1–22. doi: 10.7554/eLife.16039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Shmuel, R., Secundo, L. & Haddad, R. Strong, weak and neuron type dependent lateral inhibition in the olfactory bulb. Sci. Rep. 1–11 (2019). 10.1038/s41598-018-38151-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 49.Fukunaga I, Berning M, Kollo M, Schmaltz A, Schaefer AT. Two Distinct Channels of Olfactory Bulb Output. Neuron. 2012;75:320–329. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Uchida N, Eshel N, Watabe-Uchida M. Division of labor for division: inhibitory interneurons with different spatial landscapes in the olfactory system. Neuron. 2013;80:1106–1109. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.11.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Sugai T, Miyazawa T, Fukuda M, Yoshimura H, Onoda N. Odor-concentration coding in the guinea-pig piriform cortex. Neuroscience. 2005;130:769–781. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.09.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Gire DH, Schoppa NE. Control of on/off glomerular signaling by a local GABAergic microcircuit in the olfactory bulb. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:13454–13464. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2368-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Haberly LB, Price JL. The axonal projection patterns of the mitral and tufted cells of the olfactory bulb in the rat. Brain Res. 1977;129:152–157. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(77)90978-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Schoenfeld TA, Macrides F. Topographic organization of connections between the main olfactory bulb and pars externa of the anterior olfactory nucleus in the hamster. J. Comp. Neurol. 1984;227:121–135. doi: 10.1002/cne.902270113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Murthy VN. Olfactory maps in the brain. Annu Rev. Neurosci. 2011;34:233–258. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Yan Z, et al. Precise circuitry links bilaterally symmetric olfactory maps. Neuron. 2008;58:613–624. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Haberly LB. Parallel-distributed processing in olfactory cortex: new insights from morphological and physiological analysis of neuronal circuitry. Chem. Senses. 2001;26:551–576. doi: 10.1093/chemse/26.5.551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Hagiwara A, Pal SK, Sato TF, Wienisch M, Murthy VN. Optophysiological analysis of associational circuits in the olfactory cortex. Front. Neural Circuits. 2012;6:18. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2012.00018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Bekkers JM, Suzuki N. Neurons and circuits for odor processing in the piriform cortex. Trends Neurosci. 2013;36:429–438. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2013.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Davison IG, Ehlers MD. Neural circuit mechanisms for pattern detection and feature combination in olfactory cortex. Neuron. 2011;70:82–94. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Apicella a, Yuan Q, Scanziani M, Isaacson JS. Pyramidal cells in piriform cortex receive convergent input from distinct olfactory bulb glomeruli. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:14255–14260. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2747-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Haddad R, et al. Olfactory cortical neurons read out a relative time code in the olfactory bulb. Nat. Neurosci. 2013;16:949–957. doi: 10.1038/nn.3407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Royet JP, et al. Functional neuroanatomy of different olfactory judgments. Neuroimage. 2001;13:506–519. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Zatorre RJ, Jones-Gotman M, Evans AC, Meyer E. Functional localization and lateralization of human olfactory cortex. Nature. 1992;360:339–340. doi: 10.1038/360339a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Herz RS, McCall C, Cahill L. Hemispheric lateralization in the processing of odor pleasantness versus odor names. Chem. Senses. 1999;24:691–695. doi: 10.1093/chemse/24.6.691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Cohen Y, Wilson DA. Task-correlated cortical asymmetry and intra- and inter-hemispheric separation. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:1–9. doi: 10.1038/s41598-016-0028-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Cohen Y, Putrino D, Wilson DA. Dynamic cortical lateralization during olfactory discrimination learning. J. Physiol. 2015;593:1701–1714. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2014.288381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Jozet-Alves C, Percelay S, Bouet V. Olfactory laterality is valence-dependent in mice. Symmetry (Basel) 2019;11:1129. doi: 10.3390/sym11091129. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Royet J-P, Plailly J. Lateralization of olfactory processes. Chem. Senses. 2004;29:731–745. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjh067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Illig KR, Eudy JD. Contralateral projections of the rat anterior olfactory nucleus. J. Comp. Neurol. 2009;512:115–123. doi: 10.1002/cne.21900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Haberly LB, Price JL. Association and commissural fiber systems of the olfactory cortex of the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 1978;178:711–740. doi: 10.1002/cne.901780408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Shipley MT, Adamek G. D. the connections of the mouse olfactory bulb: a study using orthograde and retrograde transport of wheat germ agglutinin conjugated to horseradish peroxidase. Brain Res. Bull. 1984;12:669–688. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(84)90148-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Carson KA. Localisation of acetylcholinesterase positive neurons projecting to the mouse main olfactory bulb. Brain Res. Bull. 1984;12:635–639. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(84)90144-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Brunjes PC, Illig KR, Meyer EA. A field guide to the anterior olfactory nucleus (cortex) Brain Res. Rev. 2005;50:305–335. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Markopoulos F, Rokni D, Gire DH, Murthy VN. Functional properties of cortical feedback projections to the olfactory bulb. Neuron. 2012;76:1175–1188. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Rothermel M, Wachowiak M. Functional imaging of cortical feedback projections to the olfactory bulb. Front. Neural Circuits. 2014;8:1–14. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2014.00073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Reyher CKH, Schwerdtfeger WK, Baumgarten HG. Interbulbar axonal collateralization and morphology of anterior olfactory nucleus neurons in the rat. Brain Res. Bull. 1988;20:549–566. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(88)90214-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Wen P, et al. Cortical Organization of centrifugal afferents to the olfactory bulb: mono- and trans-synaptic tracing with recombinant neurotropic viral tracers. Neurosci. Bull. 2019;35:709–723. doi: 10.1007/s12264-019-00385-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Haberly LB, Price JL. Association and commissural fiber systems of the olfactory cortex of the rat. II. Systems originating in the olfactory peduncle. J. Comp. Neurol. 1978;181:711–740. doi: 10.1002/cne.901780408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Wang L, et al. Cell-type-specific whole-brain direct inputs to the anterior and posterior piriform cortex. Front. Neural Circuits. 2020;14:1–14. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2020.00004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Grobman M, et al. A mirror-symmetric excitatory link coordinates odor maps across olfactory bulbs and enables odor perceptual unity. Neuron. 2018;99:800–813.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Davis BJ, Macrides F. The organization of centrifugal projections from the anterior olfactory nucleus, ventral hippocampal rudiment, and piriform cortex to the main olfactory bulb in the hamster: an autoradiographic study. J. Comp. Neurol. 1981;203:475–493. doi: 10.1002/cne.902030310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Scott JW, Ranier EC, Pemberton JL, Orona E, Mouradian LE. Pattern of rat olfactory bulb mitral and tufted cell connections to the anterior olfactory nucleus pars externa. J. Comp. Neurol. 1985;242:415–424. doi: 10.1002/cne.902420309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Wilson DA. Binaral interactions in the rat piriform cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 1997;78:160–169. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.1.160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Kikuta S, Kashiwadani H, Mori K. Compensatory rapid switching of binasal inputs in the olfactory cortex. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:11989–11997. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3106-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Kermen, F. & Yaksi, E. Interhemispheric connections between olfactory bulbs improve odor detection. Preprint at bioRxiv. (2019). https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/767210v1.full.pdf+html. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 87.Grimaud, J., Dorrell, W., Pehlevan, C. N. & Murthy, V. N. Bilateral alignment of receptive fields in the olfactory cortex points to non-random connectivity. Preprint at bioRxiv. (2020). https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2020.02.24.960922v1.
- 88.Mittal A, Gupta D, Singh A, C Lin A, Gupta N. Multiple network properties overcome random connectivity to enable stereotypic sensory responses. Nat. Commun. 2020;11:1023. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-14836-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Schaffer ES, et al. Odor perception on the two sides of the brain: consistency despite randomness. Neuron. 2018;98:736–742.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Hige T, Aso Y, Rubin GM, Turner GC. Plasticity-driven individualization of olfactory coding in mushroom body output neurons. Nature. 2015;526:258–262. doi: 10.1038/nature15396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Kucharski D, Hall WG. New routes to early memories. Science. 1987;238:786–788. doi: 10.1126/science.3672125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Cain WS. Bilateral interaction in olfaction. Nature. 1977;268:50–52. doi: 10.1038/268050a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Moncrieff RW. Olfactory adaptation and odour likeness. J. Physiol. 1956;133:301–316. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1956.sp005587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Dalton P. Psychophysical and behavioral characteristics of olfactory adaptation. Chem. Senses. 2000;25:487–492. doi: 10.1093/chemse/25.4.487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Pierce JD, Wysocki CJ, Aronov EV, Webb JB, Boden RM. The role of perceptual and structural similarity in cross-adaptation. Chem. Senses. 1996;21:223–237. doi: 10.1093/chemse/21.2.223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Chaput MA, Panhuber H. Effects of long duration odor exposure on the unit activity of olfactory bulb cells in awake rabbits. Brain Res. 1982;250:41–52. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(82)90951-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Patterson MA, Lagier S, Carleton A. Odor representations in the olfactory bulb evolve after the first breath and persist as an odor afterimage. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2013;110:E3340–E3349. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1303873110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Wilson DA. Habituation of odor responses in the rat anterior piriform cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 1998;79:1425–1440. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.3.1425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Kadohisa M, Wilson DA. Olfactory cortical adaptation facilitates detection of odors against background. J. Neurophysiol. 2012;95:1888–1896. doi: 10.1152/jn.00812.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Chaudhury D, et al. Olfactory bulb habituation to odor stimuli. Behav. Neurosci. 2010;124:490–499. doi: 10.1037/a0020293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Kurahashi T, Menini A. Mechanism of odorant adaptation in the olfactory receptor cell. Nature. 1997;385:725–729. doi: 10.1038/385725a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Best AR, Wilson DA. Coordinate synaptic mechanisms contributing to olfactory cortical adaptation. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:652–660. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4220-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.King C, Hall WG. Developmental change in unilateral olfactory habituation is mediated by anterior commissure maturation. Behav. Neurosci. 1990;104:796–807. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.104.5.796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Dalton P, Wysocki CJ. The nature and duration of adaptation following long-term odor exposure. Percept. Psychophys. 1996;58:781–792. doi: 10.3758/BF03213109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Dalton P, Doolittle N, Breslin PAS. Gender-specific induction of enhanced sensitivity to odors. Nat. Neurosci. 2002;5:199–200. doi: 10.1038/nn803. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Wysocki, C. J., Dorries, K. M. & Beauchamp, G. K. Ability to perceive androstenone can be acquired by ostensibly anosmic people (molecular receptor/neuronal plasticity/olfaction/specific anosmia). Genetics86, 7976–7978 (1989). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 107.Wang H, Wysocki C, Gold G. Induction of olfactory receptor sensitivity in mice. Science. 1993;260:998–1000. doi: 10.1126/science.8493539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Mainland JD, et al. Olfactory plasticity: one nostril knows what the other learns. Nature. 2002;419:802. doi: 10.1038/419802a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Fletcher ML, Wilson DA. Experience modifies olfactory acuity: acetylcholine-dependent learning decreases behavioral generalization between similar odorants. J. Neurosci. 2002;22:RC201. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-02-j0005.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Rabin MD. Experience facilitates olfactory quality discrimination. Percept. Psychophys. 1988;44:532–540. doi: 10.3758/BF03207487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Laska M, Teubner P, Psychologie M, München L, München D- Olfactory discrimination ability of human subjects for ten pairs of enantiomers. Chem. Senses. 1999;24:161–170. doi: 10.1093/chemse/24.2.161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Feng G, Zhou W. Nostril-specific and structure-based olfactory learning of chiral discrimination in human adults. Elife. 2019;8:1–12. doi: 10.7554/eLife.41296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Li W, Howard JD, Parrish TB, Gottfried JA. Aversive learning enhances perceptual and cortical discrimination of indiscriminable odor cues. Science. 2008;319:1842–1845. doi: 10.1126/science.1152837. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Abraham NM, et al. Synaptic inhibition in the olfactory bulb accelerates odor discrimination in mice. Neuron. 2010;65:399–411. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Gschwend O, et al. Neuronal pattern separation in the olfactory bulb improves odor discrimination learning. Nat. Neurosci. 2015;18:1474–1482. doi: 10.1038/nn.4089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Nunes D, Kuner T. Disinhibition of olfactory bulb granule cells accelerates odour discrimination in mice. Nat. Commun. 2015;6:8950. doi: 10.1038/ncomms9950. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Nunes D, Kuner T. Axonal sodium channel NaV1.2 drives granule cell dendritic GABA release and rapid odor discrimination. PLOS Biol. 2018;16:e2003816. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2003816. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Barnes DC, Hofacer RD, Zaman AR, Rennaker RL, Wilson DA. Olfactory perceptual stability and discrimination. Nat. Neurosci. 2008;11:1378–1380. doi: 10.1038/nn.2217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Sahay A, Wilson DA, Hen R. Pattern separation: a common function for new neurons in hippocampus and olfactory bulb. Neuron. 2011;70:582–588. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Chen C-FF, Barnes DC, Wilson DA. Generalized vs. stimulus-specific learned fear differentially modifies stimulus encoding in primary sensory cortex of awake rats. J. Neurophysiol. 2011;106:3136–3144. doi: 10.1152/jn.00721.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Kucharski D, Hall WG. Developmental change in the access to olfactory memories. Behav. Neurosci. 1988;102:340–348. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.102.3.340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Kucharski D, Johanson IB, Hall WG. Unilateral olfactory conditioning in 6-day-old rat pups. Behav. Neural Biol. 1986;46:472–490. doi: 10.1016/S0163-1047(86)90506-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Schwob JE, Price JL. The development of axonal connections in the central olfactory system of rats. J. Comp. Neurol. 1984;223:177–202. doi: 10.1002/cne.902230204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Kucharski, D., Burka, N. & Hall, W. G. The anterior limb of the anterior commissure is an access route to contralaterally stored olfactory preference memories. Psychobiology18, 195–204 (1990).
- 125.Borst A, Heisenberg M. Osmotropotaxis in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Comp. Physiol. 1982;147:479–484. doi: 10.1007/BF00612013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Gaudry Q, Hong EJ, Kain J, de Bivort BL, Wilson RI. Asymmetric neurotransmitter release enables rapid odour lateralization in Drosophila. Nature. 2013;493:424–428. doi: 10.1038/nature11747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Gardiner JM, Atema J. The function of bilateral odor arrival time differences in olfactory orientation of sharks. Curr. Biol. 2010;20:1187–1191. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.04.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Catania KC. Stereo and serial sniffing guide navigation to an odour source in a mammal. Nat. Commun. 2013;4:1441. doi: 10.1038/ncomms2444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Porter J, et al. Mechanisms of scent-tracking in humans. Nat. Neurosci. 2007;10:27–29. doi: 10.1038/nn1819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Rajan R, Clement JP, Bhalla US. Rats smell in stereo. Science. 2006;311:666–670. doi: 10.1126/science.1122096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Kennedy JS, Moorhouse JE. Laboratory observation on locust responses to wind-borne grass odour. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 1969;12:487–503. doi: 10.1111/j.1570-7458.1969.tb02547.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Martin H. Osmotropotaxis in the Honey-Bee. Nature. 1965;208:59–63. doi: 10.1038/208059a0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Gomez-Marin A, Duistermars BJ, Frye MA, Louis M. Mechanisms of odor-tracking: multiple sensors for enhanced perception and behavior. Front Cell Neurosci. 2010;4:6. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2010.00006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Esquivelzeta Rabell J, Mutlu K, Noutel J, Martin del Olmo P, Haesler S. Spontaneous rapid odor source localization behavior requires interhemispheric communication. Curr. Biol. 2017;27:1542–1548.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Kikuta S, et al. From the cover: neurons in the anterior olfactory nucleus pars externa detect right or left localization of odor sources. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:12363–12368. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1003999107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]