Skip to main content
Journal of the Royal Society Interface logoLink to Journal of the Royal Society Interface
. 2020 Mar 25;17(164):20200018. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2020.0018

Environmental perturbations induce correlations in midge swarms

Kasper van der Vaart 1, Michael Sinhuber 1,, Andrew M Reynolds 2, Nicholas T Ouellette 1,
PMCID: PMC7115240  PMID: 32208820

Abstract

Although collectively behaving animal groups often show large-scale order (such as in bird flocks), they need not always (such as in insect swarms). It has been suggested that the signature of collective behaviour in disordered groups is a residual long-range correlation. However, results in the literature have reported contradictory results as to the presence of long-range correlation in insect swarms, with swarms in the wild displaying correlation but those in a controlled laboratory environment not. We resolve these apparently incompatible results by showing that the external perturbations generically induce the emergence of correlations. We apply a range of different external stimuli to laboratory swarms of the non-biting midge Chironomus riparius, and show that in all cases correlations appear when perturbations are introduced. We confirm the generic nature of these results by showing that they can be reproduced in a stochastic model of swarms. Given that swarms in the wild will always have to contend with environmental stimuli, our results thus harmonize previous findings. These findings emphasize that collective behaviour cannot be understood in isolation without considering its environmental context, and that new research is needed to disentangle the distinct roles of intrinsic dynamics and external stimuli.

Keywords: collective behaviour, perturbed insect swarms, correlations

1. Introduction

Microscopic interactions in many-body systems generically lead to emergent behaviour at the macroscale [1]. In the context of animal behaviour, social interactions within animal groups drive phenomena such as flocking [2,3], where potentially vast numbers of animals move in a directed, coordinated fashion even when executing group-level manoeuvres despite the lack of internal leadership or external direction [47]. This kind of ordering has been well studied and can be described, at least at a semi-quantitative level, by spin-like models [810]. In particular, the degree of order, and thus the effective strength of the collective behaviour, can be captured by a simple order parameter, namely the polarization of the group.

Not all collective animal groups, however, show macroscopic order. In particular, although mating swarms of flying insects are generally assumed to be collective, they lack positional or orientational order. Thus, no meaningful order parameter can be defined to described the degree of collectivity. It has been proposed that instead the relevant property to look for in swarms is long-range correlation of the velocity fluctuations [11], since such correlations are also observed in other collective animal groups such as bird flocks [12,13]. However, seemingly contradictory results for the presence of correlations in swarms have been reported: insect swarms observed in the wild appear to exhibit the anticipated long-range correlations [11,14], while those observed in a controlled laboratory environment do not [15].

Here, we reconcile this apparent contradiction by showing that externally applied perturbations stimulate the appearance of long-range correlation in laboratory midge swarms. The development of correlation is surprisingly insensitive to the type of perturbation applied: we find qualitatively similar results when we subject the swarms to air flows, motion cues, varying illumination or acoustic signals. This phenomenon is strikingly universal, arising from both behavioural and sensory cues and physical, force-based perturbations. We explain these results in the context of a generic stochastic swarming model [1618] that makes no assumptions about the details of how midges respond to specific stimuli. Since swarms in the wild unavoidably experience a dynamic external environment, our findings harmonize what has been reported previously in the literature. The picture that emerges from our results is that long-range correlation is not a result of the interaction between individuals per se but rather arises from the additional influence of the external environment on the swarm. Our results add further support to recent arguments that collective behaviour cannot be understood in isolation without considering its environmental context [19,20].

We addressed these questions by measuring the behaviour of laboratory mating swarms of the non-biting midge Chironomus riparius. Full details of our methods, along with datasets for the unperturbed swarms analysed here, are given in [21]; here, we only describe the laboratory set-up briefly. The midges spend their entire life cycle in an acrylic cube measuring 122 cm on a side. In their larval state, the midges live in nine tanks containing water and a cellulose substrate. Near the end of their life cycle, they emerge as flying adults. When not actively swarming, midges sit still on the walls or floor of the enclosure. At laboratory ‘dawn’ and ‘dusk’ (as controlled by a timed circadian overhead light source), male midges spontaneously form swarms, nucleating over a ground-based swarm marker. We image these swarms with three synchronized PointGrey Flea3 cameras positioned outside the enclosure, illuminating the midges with near-infrared LED lamps at a wavelength that they cannot see. Images (1280 × 1024 pixels) are acquired at a rate of 100 frames per second; these images are then processed with a predictive particle-tracking algorithm to produce three-dimensional trajectories along with time-resolved velocities and accelerations for each midge in the swarm [21]. Our swarms are composed of up to about 100 individuals. Although swarms of some insect species in the wild can be much larger, swarms in this size range are quite common; additionally, we have shown previously that many swarm statistics saturate and become independent of size once swarms reach sizes of about 10 individuals [22].

Given the velocities of all the individual midges, we can compute the single-time correlation function of the velocity fluctuations (sometimes called the connected correlation function) for our swarms in this controlled environment. Following previous work [11,14,15], we define this correlation function as

C(r)=ijNδviδvjδ(rrij)ijNδ(rrij). 1.1

Here, δv is the velocity fluctuation of midge i relative to the instantaneous centre-of-mass velocity of the swarm, rij is the distance between midges i and j, and N is the instantaneous number of midges in the swarm, which changes only slowly with respect to any dynamical time scales of the swarm. As we have reported before [15], correlation functions for these unperturbed swarms have small magnitudes and fall off very rapidly, with correlation lengths of the order of the body length of individual midges. Although perturbations can, in general, induce global modes such as net compression that would induce spurious correlations, we find that these modes are not present for all of our perturbations. For those perturbations where global modes are discernible, they have only a negligible effect on the correlation functions.

The length scale over which the correlation function decays has played a particularly central role in describing the physics of collective behaviour. In flocks of starlings [12] and jackdaws [13], for example, correlations have been reported to be both long range (persisting over the entire flock) and scale free (increasing proportionally with the flock size). Similar results have been reported for insect swarms in the wild [11,14]. Although there is no precise, general definition of a correlation length, we here follow previous work and take the distance r0 at which C(r) first crosses zero as a measure of this length scale. Although there is some increase of r0 with swarm size, even for our largest swarms it is still always smaller than the typical nearest-neighbour distance, and thus only a fraction of the total swarm size RS (defined as the standard deviation of the midge positions relative to the centre of mass of the swarm). This result is in agreement with our previous findings for laboratory swarms observed in a smaller enclosure [15].

Swarms in the wild and in the laboratory, however, need not be the same: in natural conditions, insects must contend with a host of externally imposed stimuli that are not present in the controlled laboratory environment. It is possible that at least some of these additional perturbations may induce the longer range correlations observed in wild swarms. To test this hypothesis, we introduced various types of externally imposed stimuli on our swarms. Midges are sensitive to many external factors, including light [23], sound [24] and motion [18], giving us a range of possible stimuli to test. Here, we report the results of five different applied stimuli.

Chironomids are quite sensitive to sound at frequencies of a few hundred hertz, near their natural wingbeat frequency. Previously, we showed that playing sinusoidally modulated sound signals to midge swarms induced coherent motion of the swarm centre of mass [24]. Here, we drove the swarms with an acoustic signal consisting of pulses of telegraph noise with varying length and amplitude. The telegraph noise was constructed by passing a white-noise signal through a low-pass 700 Hz filter. The pulse length ranged from 0.1 s to 0.3 s and the pause between pulses ranged from 0.25 s to 0.5 s. The noise amplitude was clearly audible over the ambient sound levels in the laboratory, and we varied it only slightly.

Midges are also sensitive to light signals, and dynamically varying illumination tends to change both the kinematics of individuals and the swarm density [23]. We applied illumination perturbations by exposing the swarms to an array of LEDs with a periodically varying brightness level controlled by a function generator. We varied both the period of the illumination changes over a wide range (with a median of 2.25 s) and the difference in brightness between bright and dim phases. Neither the period nor the brightness difference had a noticeable effect on the correlations, and so below we simply consolidate all of the data.

In the wild, midge swarms nucleate over ground-based visual features known as swarm markers [25]. In the laboratory, we position our swarms with an artificial swarm marker—in our case, a simple square of black felt. By mounting this marker on a movable stage, however, we have shown that we can dynamically move and manipulate the swarm by applying an effective shear stress to its base [18,26]. The marker was attached to a linear stage with a position accuracy of 14 μm (CS Series Belt Drive with NEMA 23 Brushless Servo Motor, Newmark Systems). We moved the marker in two distinct ways: a single abrupt movement, which we refer to as a step shear, and a continuous oscillatory shear. For the step shear experiments, the marker was displaced at a speed of 140.6 mm s−1 over a distance of 40 cm. After a 5 s pause, it was then displaced in the same fashion in the opposite direction. Data were only analysed during the movement of the marker and not during the pause. For the oscillatory shear experiments, the marker was moved sinusoidally with frequencies between 0.05 Hz and 0.45 Hz and amplitudes between 2 cm and 10 cm (see [18] for more details). The correlations were again not noticeably affected by the details of the oscillatory shear, and so again we average all of the data together in our analysis.

Finally, wild swarms must also contend with direct mechanical perturbations in the form of, for example, wind. Thus, we also subjected our laboratory swarms to wind produced by a commercially available fan. The fan was positioned outside of the midge enclosure (with one of the acrylic walls replaced by a plastic mesh) and was pointed directly at the centre of the swarm. The mean horizontal wind speed at the centre of the swarm was 280 mm s−1 with small angular and temporal variances, as measured by a two-dimensional sonic anemometer. Despite the wind applying an actual physical force to the swarm rather than a transduced behavioural response as with the other perturbations, the response of the swarm is strikingly similar and the centre of the swarm does not move.

In figure 1, we show correlation functions for swarms with comparable numbers of midges for all of these different stimuli. Although the details of these correlation functions are different for each stimulus, they show a number of similarities. In all cases, they fall off more slowly and have longer correlation lengths than the data for unperturbed swarms of a similar size, suggesting that the presence of the perturbations is inducing longer range correlation in the swarms. This effect is particularly noticeable for the wind-perturbed swarms, which also show a much stronger degree of correlation in addition to a longer correlation length.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Correlation functions (as defined in equation (1.1)) for unperturbed swarms and swarms subjected to various external stimuli. All swarms, perturbed and not, have N ≈ 45. Data are shown as a function of distance normalized by (a) the swarm radius Rs and (b) the typical body length of a midge, Rb = 7 mm, and statistical errors are smaller than the symbol sizes. Note that laboratory swarms are typically dilute, resulting in swarm radii that are often of comparable order to the typical nearest-neighbour distances. In all cases, correlation lengths are longer for perturbed swarms.

To make the trend of increased correlation length more apparent, we plot in figure 2 the correlation length r0 as a function of swarm size N for both unperturbed swarms and swarms subjected to each of the different external stimuli. In all cases, r0 is longer for perturbed swarms than for unperturbed swarms, and in some cases becomes comparable to the swarm size itself. In addition, for perturbed swarms, r0 appears to grow roughly linearly with N, suggesting that it will continue to increase for larger swarms and remain appreciable compared with the swarm size. By contrast, the correlation length for unperturbed swarms saturates at a value much smaller than the swarm size (though still several body lengths). This result reconciles previously reported findings for laboratory and wild swarms, suggesting that the unavoidable external perturbations in the natural environment drive the emergence of long-range correlation in swarms. That the emergence of correlations appears to be essentially independent of the type of perturbation strengthens this supposition even more.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Mean correlation lengths r0 as a function of swarm size N for unperturbed swarms and swarms subjected to external stimuli. The shading shows the standard error. r0 is shown normalized by (a) the swarm radius Rs and (b) the typical body length of a midge, Rb = 7 mm. In all cases, correlation lengths are longer for perturbed swarms, and grown roughly linearly with N. By contrast, r0 appears to saturate for unperturbed swarms.

Our assertion that long-range correlations are not the result of direct interactions between individuals but instead arise from perturbations finds strong support in the results of numerical simulations of the model of Reynolds et al. [27], which produces results that are consistent with numerous observations of laboratory swarms [16,18]. This model generates positions and velocities of individual midges as solutions of a Langevin equation with an additional restoring force, given by

dxi=vidt 1.2

and

dvi=viTdt+Ai|v,xdt+2σv2TdWi(t). 1.3

Here, vi are the Cartesian components of the midge velocity v, x is the midge position and Ai are the components of the midge acceleration. Given the simplicity of this model, we only seek a qualitative comparison between it and the experimental measurements; thus, we set the correlation time scale T and the root-mean-square speed σv to unity. dWi(t) is an incremental Wiener process that is δ-correlated in time. More details on the model can be found in Reynolds et al. [27] and van der Vaart et al. [18]. Note that interactions between the individuals are not explicitly modelled; rather, their net effect is subsumed into the restoring force term since, as we have argued previously [2729], this restoring force is an emergent property of swarms and is the primary macroscopic consequence of the microscopic interactions. All simulated individuals will therefore respond in a similar way following global displacement of the swarm, thus leading to the emergence of correlations in the system. Thus, to model the effect of perturbations on the swarm corresponding to the oscillatory shear experiments, we simulated how swarms relax back to their equilibrium positions (that is, just above their swarm markers) after being displaced simply by forcing the horizontal position of the swarm to oscillate along the horizontal axis.

On short distances the model produces strong positive correlation, indicating that simulated individuals have common velocity fluctuations (figure 3a). After some negative correlation at intermediate distances, the correlations relax to zero at large distances. The correlation length r0 is seen to extend across the entire core of the swarm. Such correlations are entirely absent at equilibrium in the model when no perturbations are applied.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Emergent correlations in a simulated swarm relaxing back to equilibrium after being instantaneously displaced horizontally from its equilibrium position by 5 a.u. Data were computed from simulations of the model of Reynolds et al. [27] with root-mean-square velocity σv = 1, root-mean-square size σr = 1, and velocity autocorrelation time scale T = 1, all in a.u. The (a) connected correlation function C(r) and (b) cumulative correlation function Q(r) are shown for swarms containing N = 50 individuals. (c) The susceptibility χ increases with swarm size N.

The collective response of the simulated swarm depends on both the span and intensity of the correlation. Attanasi et al. [11,14] combined these two factors into a single calculable property, namely the cumulative correlation up to a scale r. Defined as

Q(r)=1NijNδviδvjθ(rrij), 1.4

where θ(rrij) is the Heaviside function, Q(r) reaches a maximum at r0. The maximum χ = Q(r0) is a measure of the total amount of correlation present in the swarm, and, if the swarm satisfied the fluctuation–dissipation theorem, would be equivalent to a susceptibility. The large value of χ observed in the simulations (figure 3b) indicates that, despite the lack of collective order, large clusters of simulated midges move coherently when the swarms are displaced and recovering. Moreover, χ for the simulated swarms increases linearly with the swarm size (figure 3c). The simulated displaced swarms therefore exhibit a near-maximal degree of correlation at all sizes, mirroring observations of swarms in the wild [11,14].

The strong static correlations and critical-like behaviour of wild swarms reported by Attanasi et al. [11,14] prompted Cavagna et al. [30] to go one step further and examine fully spatio-temporal correlations. They presented empirical evidence for the emergence of the so-called dynamic scaling in wild swarms. We note that we find the same dynamic scaling in our simulations of displaced swarms with no direct interactions. Following Cavagna et al. [30], we tested for dynamic scaling in our model by computing the spatio-temporal correlation functions of velocity fluctuations in Fourier space, given by

C(k,t)=sin(kr(t0,t))kr(t0,t)δv(t0)δv(t0+t), 1.5

where r(t0, t) = |r(t0) − r(t)| and associated characteristic time scales τk were determined by

0sin(t/τk)C(k,t)tdt=π4. 1.6

For exponential correlation, τk is the exponential decay time; and even for more complex forms, τk is the most relevant time scale of the system. Cavagna et al. [30] evaluated C(k, t) at k = 1/ξ, where ξ is the correlation length. Here, the root-mean-square swarm size was used as a proxy for the correlation length. This is reasonable because correlation lengths of wild swarms grow linearly with swarm size [11,14]. The results (figure 4) do not change significantly when the proxy for ξ is increased or decreased by a factor of 2. In the simulations, the only other model parameters (the velocity autocorrelation time scale and the root-mean-square velocity) were kept constant across differently sized swarms because they are assumed to be characteristics of individual midges.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Predicted dynamical scaling and critical exponent. (a) Normalized temporal correlation functions C(k, t) evaluated at k = 1/ξ, where ξ = σr, the root-mean-square size of the swarm. The lines correspond to ξ = 1 (red), 1/2 (green), 1/4 (blue) and 1/8 (black). (b) C(k, t) as a function of the scaling variable kz t for the same cases shown in (a). (c) Characteristic time scale τk computed at k = 1/ξ as a function of k. Predictions are shown for the model of Reynolds et al. [27] with root-mean-square velocity σv = 1 and velocity autocorrelation time scale T = 1, and with root-mean-square sizes σr ranging from 1 to 1/64 a.u.

In accordance with the analysis of Cavagna et al. [30], we find that the spatio-temporal correlation functions collapse onto a single curve when plotted as functions of kzt and τkkz, where the dynamic critical exponent z = 1 (figure 4). This result suggests that the wild swarms studied by Cavagna et al. [30] and our simulated swarms belong to the same dynamic university class. Here, however, we attribute this dynamic scaling to the effects of perturbations rather than to interactions between the individuals per se.

Externally induced perturbations are inevitable for wild swarms that must contend with a host of environmental disturbances. Here, we have shown that such disturbances generically tend to induce the emergence of correlations in laboratory swarms that closely resemble those reported for wild swarms by Attanasi et al. [11,14]. Such correlations were interpreted as evidence that wild swarms are nearly critical, and tune themselves to be close to a phase transition from disorder to order (though always remaining on the disordered side). Our findings here suggest instead that, even when the intrinsic dynamics are always disordered with no hint of criticality, external perturbations can impose an ordered response—perhaps indeed poising swarms near a phase transition, though one different from that envisioned by Attanasi et al. In this regard, it is interesting to note that the emergent solid-like properties observed in laboratory swarms [18,26] have also been attributed to the presence of perturbations that move the swarm centre of mass [31]. More work thus remains to be done to disentangle the roles of intrinsic dynamics and external stimuli in producing correlations in collective animal groups, and thus to interpret such correlations properly.

Supplementary Material

Electronic supplementary material
rsif20200018supp1.zip (1.1GB, zip)

Data accessibility

The trajectory data are available as part of the electronic supplementary material.

Authors' contributions

K.v.d.V. and M.S. contributed equally to this work. A.M.R. conceived of the initial idea for unifying laboratory and wild swarm results, and undertook the numerical simulations. K.v.d.V. and M.S. ran the experiments and analysed the data. All authors interpreted the data and wrote the paper.

Competing interests

We declare we have no competing interest.

Funding

The research at Stanford was sponsored by the Army Research Laboratory and was accomplished under grant no. W911NF-16-1-0185. K.v.d.V. acknowledges support from an Early Postdoc.Mobility fellowship from the Swiss National Science Foundation, and M.S. acknowledges support from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft under grant no. 396632606. The work at Rothamsted forms part of the Smart Crop Protection (SCP) strategic programme (BBS/OS/CP/000001) funded through the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council’s Industrial Strategy Challenge Fund.

Disclaimer

The views and conclusions in this document are those of the authors and should not be interpreted as representing the official policies, either expressed or implied, of the Army Research Laboratory or the US government.

References

  • 1.Anderson PW. 1972. More is different. Science 177, 393–396. ( 10.1126/science.177.4047.393) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Sumpter DJT. 2006. The principles of collective animal behaviour. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 361, 5–22. ( 10.1098/rstb.2005.1733) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Cavagna A, Giardina I. 2014. Bird flocks as condensed matter. Annu. Rev. Condens. Matter Phys. 5, 183–207. ( 10.1146/annurev-conmatphys-031113-133834) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Pomeroy H, Heppner F. 1992. Structure of turning in airborne rock dove (Columba livia) flocks. Auk 109, 256–267. ( 10.2307/4088194) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Attanasi A. et al. 2014. Information transfer and behavioural inertia in starling flocks. Nat. Phys. 10, 691–696. ( 10.1038/nphys3035) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Attanasi A. et al. 2015. Emergence of collective changes in travel direction of starling flocks from individual birds’ fluctuations. J. R. Soc. Interface 12, 20150319 ( 10.1098/rsif.2015.0319) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ling H, McIvor GE, Westley J, van der Vaart K, Yin J, Vaughan RT, Thornton A, Ouellette NT. 2019. Collective turns in jackdaw flocks: kinematics and information transfer. J. R. Soc. Interface 31, 20190450 ( 10.1098/rsif.2019.0450) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Vicsek T, Czirók A, Ben-Jacob E, Cohen I, Shochet O. 1995. Novel type of phase transition in a system of self-driven particles. Phys. Rev. Lett. 75, 1226–1229. ( 10.1103/PhysRevLett.75.1226) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Couzin ID, Krause J, Franks NR, Levin SA. 2005. Effective leadership and decision-making in animal groups on the move. Nature 433, 513–516. ( 10.1038/nature03236) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Bialek W, Cavagna A, Giardina I, Mora T, Silvestri E, Viale M, Walczak AM. 2012. Statistical mechanics for natural flocks of birds. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 4786–4791. ( 10.1073/pnas.1118633109) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Attanasi A. et al. 2014. Collective behaviour without collective order in wild swarms of midges. PLoS Comput. Biol. 10, e1003697 ( 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003697) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Cavagna A, Cimarelli A, Giardina I, Parisi G, Santagati R, Stefanini F, Viale M. 2010. Scale-free correlations in starling flocks. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 11 865–11 870. ( 10.1073/pnas.1005766107) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Ling H, McIvor GE, van der Vaart K, Vaughan RT, Thornton A, Ouellette NT. 2019. Costs and benefits of social relationships in the collective motion of bird flocks. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 3, 943–948. ( 10.1038/s41559-019-0891-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Attanasi A. et al. 2014. Finite-size scaling as a way to probe near-criticality in natural swarms. Phys. Rev. Lett. 113, 238102 ( 10.1103/PhysRevLett.113.238102) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ni R, Ouellette NT. 2015. Velocity correlations in laboratory insect swarms. Eur. Phys. J. Spec. Top. 224, 3271–3277. ( 10.1140/epjst/e2015-50077-5) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Reynolds AM. 2018. Langevin dynamics encapsulate the microscopic and emergent macroscopic properties of midge swarms. J. R. Soc. Interface 15, 20170806 ( 10.1098/rsif.2017.0806) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Reynolds AM. 2018. Fluctuating environments drive insect swarms into a new state that is robust to perturbations. Europhys. Lett. 124, 38001 ( 10.1209/0295-5075/124/38001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.van der Vaart K, Sinhuber M, Reynolds AM, Ouellette NT. 2019. Mechanical spectroscopy of insect swarms. Sci. Adv. 5, eaaw9305 ( 10.1126/sciadv.aaw9305) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.King AJ, Fehlmann G, Biro D, Ward AJ, Fürtbauer I. 2018. Re-wilding collective behaviour: an ecological perspective. Trends Ecol. Evol. 33, 347–357. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2018.03.004) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Ling H, McIvor GE, Westley J, van der Vaart K, Vaughan RT, Thornton A, Ouellette NT. 2019. Behavioural plasticity and the transition to order in jackdaw flocks. Nat. Commun. 10, 5174 ( 10.1038/s41467-019-13281-4) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Sinhuber M, van der Vaart K, Ni R, Puckett JG, Kelley DH, Ouellette NT. 2019. Three-dimensional time-resolved trajectories from laboratory insect swarms. Sci. Data 6, 190036 ( 10.1038/sdata.2019.36) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Puckett JG, Ouellette NT. 2014. Determining asymptotically large population sizes in insect swarms. J. R. Soc. Interface 11, 20140710 ( 10.1098/rsif.2014.0710) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Sinhuber M, van der Vaart K, Ouellette NT. 2019. Response of insect swarms to dynamic illumination perturbations. J. R. Soc. Interface 16, 20180739 ( 10.1098/rsif.2018.0739) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Ni R, Puckett JG, Dufresne ER, Ouellette NT. 2015. Intrinsic fluctuations and driven response of insect swarms. Phys. Rev. Lett. 115, 118104 ( 10.1103/PhysRevLett.115.118104) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Downe AER, Caspary VG. 1973. The swarming behaviour of chironomus riparius (Diptera: Chironomidae) in the laboratory. Can. Entomol. 105, 165–171. ( 10.4039/Ent105165-1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ni R, Ouellette NT. 2016. On the tensile strength of insect swarms. Phys. Biol. 13, 045002 ( 10.1088/1478-3975/13/4/045002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Reynolds AM, Sinhuber M, Ouellette NT. 2017. Are midge swarms bound together by an effective velocity-dependent gravity? Eur. Phys. J. E 40, 46 ( 10.1140/epje/i2017-11531-7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Kelley DH, Ouellette NT. 2013. Emergent dynamics of laboratory insect swarms. Sci. Rep. 3, 1073 EP ( 10.1038/srep01073) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Gorbonos D, Ianconescu R, Puckett JG, Ni R, Ouellette NT, Gov NS. 2016. Long-range acoustic interactions in insect swarms: an adaptive gravity model. New J. Phys. 18, 073042 ( 10.1088/1367-2630/18/7/073042) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Cavagna A, Conti D, Creato C, Del Castello L, Giardina I, Grigera TS, Melillo S, Parisi L, Viale M. 2017. Dynamic scaling in wild swarms. Nat. Phys. 13, 914–918. ( 10.1038/nphys4153) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Reynolds AM. 2019. On the origin of the tensile strength of insect swarms. Phys. Biol. 16, 046002 ( 10.1088/1478-3975/ab12b9) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Electronic supplementary material
rsif20200018supp1.zip (1.1GB, zip)

Data Availability Statement

The trajectory data are available as part of the electronic supplementary material.


Articles from Journal of the Royal Society Interface are provided here courtesy of The Royal Society

RESOURCES