Abstract
During adolescence and early adulthood, learning when to avoid threats and when to pursue rewards, becomes crucial. Using a risky foraging task, we investigated individual differences in this dynamic across 781 individuals aged 14 to 24, which were split into a hypothesis-generating discovery sample and a hold-out. Sex was the most important predictor of cautious behaviour and performance. Males earned one standard deviation, or 20%, more reward than females, collected more reward when there was little to lose, and reduced foraging to the same level as females when potential losses became high. Other independent predictors of cautiousness and performance were self-reported daringness, IQ, and self-reported cognitive complexity. We found no evidence for an impact of age or maturation. Thus maleness, a high IQ or self-reported cognitive complexity, and self- reported daringness, predicted greater success in risky foraging, possibly due to better exploitation of low-risk opportunities in high-risk environments.
Introduction
Arbitrating risk and benefits is a challenge for all animals, including humans. Animals foraging for nutrition are often faced with fertile open spaces that expose them to potentially fatal predation 1. Many human situations echo this scenario. For example, driving entails an exposure to mortal dangers, but risk-taking taxi drivers earn more money on average 2. Adolescence and early adulthood is a critical period during which an ability to balance cautiousness versus daring emerges as a character trait 3, but is also associated with miscalculations that lead to harms such as traffic accidents 4, unwanted pregnancy, and substance-related morbidity 5. However, this does not affect all adolescents to the same extent. For example, sex differences in adolescent risk-taking are important 5.
Yet, there is a debate in relation to the cognitive and neurobiological mechanisms of youth risk taking and its predictors 6, 7, 8. This is likely, at least in part, to reflect a relative lack of suitable laboratory tasks to measure behavioural risk-taking. Adolescent risk miscalculations typically involve emotionally arousing and extended sequences of events with real consequences 3, where the latter are often not fully known 9. By contrast, laboratory tasks assessing risk taking in adolescents typically do not involve these features and fall into two broad classes 10: hypothetical or real monetary decisions (e.g. 11), usually involving economic lotteries (e.g. 12), and game-like tasks with intuitive cover stories such as the "balloon task" 13. In economic revealed preference tasks, all stakes and outcomes are fully described, unlike what pertains in real-life scenarios 9. Risk-seeking in these tasks appears to monotonically decrease from childhood into adulthood, and this fails to capture a reported characteristic mid-adolescence peak in real-life risk taking 14. When lotteries involve ambiguity or uncertainty 15, 16, adolescents avoid outcomes of unknown probability to a lesser extent, and invest less in information searching, compared to children or adults 9. A further discrepancy with real-life risk taking is the relative insignificance of potential (petty financial) outcomes in these tasks, which tends to obviate an element of emotional arousal 3. This also applies to the often-used, game-like, virtual Balloon task where the worst outcome in the cover story is bursting a party balloon 13.
Here, we sought to address these shortcomings in order to identify antecedents of individual differences in adolescent risk taking. To this end, we drew on ethological ideas of risky foraging for reward under threat of predation 17, 18, 19. Risky foraging is a common biological scenario, which is extended over time, and affords detection of relatively low-risk opportunities in high-risk environments 20, 21. We previously described a human risky foraging task, resembling rodent approach/avoidance conflict tests, which necessitates multiple sequential decisions and exploration of stakes and probabilities in real time. Within an ethological framing, which includes virtual 'death', we showed that cautious behaviour relies on similar neural circuits to that of rodent tasks where harm is looming 22, 23, 24, 25, 26. This task allows, in principle, a comparison to real-life scenarios, as well as an assessment of the "dynamic flow of decision-making" 27.
In this task, players weigh potential gains against two potential threat features. The first is the presence of one or other task scenario associated with a different overall threat probability, where these are learned by experience and signalled by frame colour. The second threat feature relates to foraging being extended over time. Early on, there is little to lose by getting caught, and participants typically forage vigorously, but reduce such foraging with the passage of time, as they accumulate gains. We have previously shown that this latter behavioural change over time is impacted by a range of anxiolytics as well as by hippocampal lesions, with no consistent effect reported in the case of threat probability 23, 24, 25. In a related task, we have provided neuroimaging and lesion evidence suggesting that distinct neural substrates account for a representation of threat probability and loss magnitude respectively 22, 26.
Here, we exploited a large dataset, involving an accelerated longitudinal design that spanned a limited age range (14-24 years), to probe how individual differences between adolescents and young adults, including sex, age, IQ, and a large variety of mental health measures, account for success in risky foraging. More specifically, we were interested in examining how these factors shape sensitivity to the presence of potential threat, differences in threat probability, and the passage of intra-epoch time. Importantly, the task provides a rich set of behavioural measures. By splitting our data into discovery and confirmation samples, we exploit these measures in an entirely data-driven way and form hypotheses that were first pre-registered before confirmation in the hold-out sample.
Results
Analysis strategy
N = 781 adolescents, drawn from the Neuroscience in Psychiatry Network (NSPN) 2400 cohort 28, completed 81 epochs (i.e. trials) of a pac-man style computer game in which they foraged for tokens under threat of virtual predation (see figure 1). The data curator (MM) randomised these data into a sex- and age-balanced discovery sample (N = 492) and a hold-out confirmation sample (N = 289, table 1). The data analyst (DRB) had access to the discovery sample alone. This sample was used in an exploratory analysis to derive 9 distinct pre-registered hypotheses (https://osf.io/hrce6/, registered on 28.07.2018, table 2) prior to accessing data for the confirmation sample. We then tested these hypotheses in the hold-out sample without parameter re-fitting. We report out-of-sample predictive performance (i.e. with parameters fixed from the discovery sample) from these tests, as well as in-sample predictive performance (i.e. with parameters fitted in-sample) for the combined sample 29. To put our findings into context, we report effect sizes (but no inference statistics) from post-hoc exploratory analyses.
Table 1. Age distribution, and psychometric measures predictive of behaviour, for the discovery and confirmation samples.
Discovery: Males | Discovery: Females | Confirmation: Males | Confirmation: Females | |
---|---|---|---|---|
14-15 | 47 | 54 | 24 | 27 |
16-17 | 48 | 51 | 21 | 27 |
18-19 | 47 | 50 | 19 | 27 |
20-21 | 48 | 49 | 28 | 33 |
22-24 | 47 | 51 | 28 | 32 |
Mean ± SD | Mean ± SD | Mean ± SD | Mean ± SD | |
IQ (WASI) | 111.95 ± 11.41 | 109.45 ± 11.18 | 111.89 ± 11.6 | 109.99 ± 11.05 |
CADS daringness | 2.67 ± 0.55 | 2.24 ± 0.61 | 2.61 ± 0.59 | 2.28 ± 0.61 |
BIS cogn. compl. | 2.18 ± 0.47 | 2.32 ± 0.45 | 2.22 ± 0.49 | 2.27 ± 0.48 |
See Supplementary Results 4 for performance (tokens retained) of the different age groups.
Table 2. Pre-registered hypotheses, and results of the confirmation analysis.
Hypothesis | Task variables in predictive model | Discovery sample: accuracy (bootstrapped 95%-CI); test statistic and significance level (uncorrected) from multiple (H1: logistic) regression; H2: LME results | Confirmation sample: accuracy (bootstrapped 95%-CI) of the discovery model; significance level (uncorrected) from non-parametric random permutation test; H2: LME results | Combined sample: accuracy (bootstrapped 95% CI) of the refitted joint predictive model | Non-confirmed bivariate relations included in the predictive model |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
H1: Male sex is associated with less cautious behavior and higher performance, as indexed by a weighted combination of 7 task measures | Decrease in distance from walls, Decrease in token collection rate, Decrease in speed when on grid, Average token collection rate, Average speed when on grid, Minimum distance from threat, Tokens retained | 69.3% (63.8%-72.6%) χ2(7) = 126.1 p < .001 |
69.2% (64.0%-74.7%) p < .001 |
69.7% (65.9%-73.0%) | Decrease in distance from walls |
H2: Male participants increase their performance (tokens retained) more over repeated epochs than females | N/A | F(1, 30982) = 9.66 p < .001 (see Supplementary Results 3) |
F(1, 1819) = 0.2 p = .65 (see Supplementary Results 3) |
N/A | N/A |
H3: Higher IQ is associated with less cautious behaviour and higher performance, as indexed by a weighted combination of 2 task measures | Decrease in token collection rate, Tokens retained | 4.0% (-0.2%-6.5%) F(2, 487) = 10.1 p < .001 |
6.7% (2.4%-12.2%) p < .001 |
4.9% (1.9%-7.7%) | All confirmed |
H4: Higher CADS subscale 'Daringness' is associated with less cautious behaviour and higher performance, as indexed by a weighted combination of 4 task measures | Decrease in token collection rate, Average token collection rate, Average speed, Tokens retained | 4.0% (-1.0%-6.2%) F(4, 456) = 4.8 p < .001 |
4.3% (0%-9.7%) p < .001 |
4.3% (4.0%-8.2%) | Decrease in token collection rate |
H5: Higher BIS factor 'Self-control' is associated with decrease in threat quadrant presence | Decrease in threat quadrant presence | 2.5% (-0.9%-4.5%) F(1, 453) = 11.7 p < .001 |
Not confirmed p >.99 |
Not confirmed | None confirmed |
H6: Higher BIS factor 'Cognitive complexity' is associated with less cautious behavior and higher performance, as indexed by a weighted combination of 2 task measures | Decrease in token collection rate, Tokens retained | 4.5% (-0.4%-7.4%) F(2, 459) = 10.8 p < .001 |
3.0% (-1.6%-8.5%) p = .002 |
3.8% (0%-6.1%) | All confirmed |
H7: CADS subscale 'Daringness' is associated with similarity of intra-epoch behavioral trajectory to trajectory of 20 highest CADS 'Daring' scorers in discovery sample (behavioral trajectory indexed by weighted combination of 3 time-dependent task variables) | Wall distance, Token collection rate, Speed | 5.4% (0.0%-8.3%) F(3, 435) = 8.3 p < .001 |
6.3% (1.6%-11.9%) p < .001 |
6.0 % (5.2%-10.7%) | All confirmed |
H8: RCMAS 'Anxiety' sum score quadratically predicts cautious behavior, as indexed by an average of 4 normalized and recoded task measures | Average cautiousness score, Average safe place presence, Average distance from walls, Time to reach safe place | 3.2% (-0.9%-5.4%) F(2, 460) = 7.5 p < .001 |
Not confirmed p >.99 |
Not confirmed | None confirmed |
H9: SPQ subscale ‘Odd or eccentric behaviour’ quadratically predicts cautious behavior, as indexed by an average of 2 normalized and recoded task measures | Average distance from threat, Average distance from walls | 3.2% (-0.9%-5.4%) F(2, 454) = 7.4 p < .001 |
Not confirmed p >.99 |
Not confirmed | None confirmed |
All predictive models and their coefficients were part of the pre-registration (https://osf.io/hrce6/, registered on 28.07.2018). Confirmatory analysis was based on a random permutation test of model predictions in the hold-out sample, without re-fitting. The table shows accuracy and 95% bootstrap confidence intervals of the discovery model together with test statistics, on which the pre-registration was based. It also shows accuracy of the discovery model and 95% bootstrap confidence intervals in the confirmation sample as estimate of out-of-sample predictive performance 29, and significance level of a random permutation test. As best estimate of in-sample predictive performance and to compare with the out-of-sample predictive performance from the confirmation sample, we also show accuracy and 95% bootstrap confidence intervals of a model fitted in the combined sample. Notably, the procedure of deriving confidence intervals is unrelated to the permutation test; they are included due to journal requirements and do not reflect the posterior plausibility of true parameter values 65. Accuracy is shown as percent correct for sex and percent variance explained for other variables (N/A for unconfirmed models). The last column highlights task variables contained in confirmed predictive models, for which the underlying bivariate relationship was not confirmed at p < .001 in the confirmation sample, even when not correcting for multiple comparison. See Supplementary Table 2 for a full list of bivariate relations in discovery and confirmation sample. See Extended Data Figures 2-4 for analysis of the predictive models separately in confirmation and discovery sample.
During data exploration in the discovery sample, we considered 38 task variables (see Supplementary Results 1 for a detailed list). These included 4 summary statistics (see Extended Data Figure 1 for their derivation) for each of 7 previously validated, (intra-epoch) time-dependent variables and their weighted "cautiousness" sum score 25, and 6 additional time-independent measures including overall performance (tokens retained after the epoch had finished), all averaged over the 81 epochs of the game. We analysed how these 38 task variables related to 32 predictor variables. The latter included sex, age, IQ, and self-report questionnaire data that covered a broad range of mental health symptoms and dispositions (see methods). All bivariate linear or quadratic relationships discovered in these 2432 tests formed part of our hypotheses summarised in table 2; any relationship not mentioned therein was not significant at an alpha level of p < .001 in the discovery sample.
Task properties
Test-retest reliability over two years for those task variables included in the confirmatory analysis exceeded rtt > .5 (see Supplementary Results 1 for test-retest variability of all task variables). These 38 variables were correlated (see Supplementary Results 1) and can be conceptually characterised as contributing to at least three interpretable and replicable factors; namely sensitivity to threat probability, sensitivity to intra-epoch time, and performance achieved (see Supplementary Results 1 for factor analysis). However, the factor scores explained less variance in predictor variables than some of the individual task variables. Therefore, in what follows we focused on the latter.
Task performance, ie. tokens retained after the predator wakes up, stands out among task variables. In a post-hoc analysis across the combined sample, average token collection during the epoch was the best predictor of performance (76.9% explained variance), decrease in wall distance over the epoch was the best predictor of remaining variance (12.4% explained variance), and average wall distance was the best predictor of still remaining variance (1.1% explained variance). At the suggestion of a reviewer, we investigated average survival rates. None of the 32 predictors explained more than 1.6% variance in survival rates in the discovery sample. Among task variables, survival was best explained by the average cautiousness sum score (67.1% explained variance in combined sample); increase in safe quadrant presence as the epoch progressed explained most of the remaining variance (3.8% explained variance); and average wall distance explained most of still remaining variance (1.7% explained variance). Another variable considered during the revision was average latency to initiate escape (defined as first movement away from the threat) once the predator woke up, which took on values between 100 ms (minimum latency defined by the maximum attainable speed) and 500 ms, explaining 1.4% variance in raw survival rate. Notably, the predator moved at 40 grid movements per second, while the maximum attainable speed of the human player was 10 grid movements per second. Thus, the player's sensorimotor performance during escape was by design less decisive for survival than the player's location on the grid when the predator woke up.
Sex was the strongest predictor of behavioural differences in the task
Amongst the 32 predictor variables, sex was the single best predictor of performance (tokens retained after the predator woke up), explaining 17.0% variance in this metric in the combined sample. Males earned 19.9%, or approximately 1 standard deviation, more tokens than females (see figure 2B, table 2, Extended Data Figure 2, Supplementary Table 2). In the discovery sample, sex was associated with 7 task variables (including task performance) at an alpha level of p < .001. Males collected more tokens per time unit, moved faster (see figure 2A), and approached closer to predator location (see figure 2B). Over time, they also decreased their distance to walls, token collection rate and overall speed, more rapidly than females. To collectively confirm these associations, we computed a multiple logistic regression model that predicted sex from these 7 task variables (H1, table 2). This model was confirmed without refitting, using a random permutation test in the hold-out sample.
Descriptively, male behaviour for most measures converged to that of females towards the end of an epoch, when they had collected a number of tokens, and the cost of predation was getting high. Consequently, post-hoc analyses across the entire sample revealed no evidence that males were less successful in avoiding a predator (sex difference in average survival rate 0.52%; -1.81%-2.84% 95% parametric confidence interval; LBF = 0.90 in favour of a model without sex difference). Figure 2C shows occupancy heat maps for the peak token collection period and illustrates how females, on average, maintained a closer proximity to the safe place and to the walls than did males.
Next, we asked if any of the other 6 task variables relating to sex mediated the observed performance difference between males and females (see figure 2E). In a post-hoc mediation analysis 30, over the combined sample, average token collection (82%) and decrease in token collection over the epoch (13%) mediated the largest proportion of the performance difference. For detailed results and further mediation analyses, see Supplementary Results 2.
Sex differences in the time people had spent with computer games in their daily lives could, in principle, affect performance in the present task. If, for example, males spent more play time and already performed better than females, then this could explain our results. In this case, one would expect a steeper performance increase over trials in female participants. To test this hypothesis in a pre-registered analysis, we analysed the epoch-by-epoch performance trajectory (H2, table 2). In our discovery sample, we found that males and females started at the same performance level, but males increased their performance more steeply over repeated epochs than females (see Supplementary Results 3). This is the opposite of what one would expect if females' overall worse performance were explained by less experience with computer games. However, this sex difference was not confirmed in the hold-out sample (see table 2 and Supplementary Results 3). Nevertheless, we did not find any evidence for a steeper epoch-by-epoch performance trajectory in females in the confirmation sample. Also, the individual slope of the epoch-by-epoch performance trajectory mediated only a negligible proportion (0%) of the sex effect on performance (see Supplementary Results 2).
Because of a strong effect of sex, we controlled for sex in all further analyses. All significant relationships reported in what follows remained significant when sex was modelled as covariate.
Self-reported daringness, IQ, and cognitive complexity predict better performance
We next investigated the relationship between 29 self-report measures, IQ, and age, and task variables. Self-reported daringness, measured with the CADS questionnaire, explained 3.9% of performance variance in the combined sample. In the discovery sample, participants with higher self-reported daringness collected, and retained, more tokens, and decreased their token collection more rapidly with the progression of time over an epoch (H4, see figure 3AC, table 2, Supplementary Table 2, Extended Data Figure 3). These associations were collectively confirmed in the hold-out.
In a post-hoc mediation analysis, over the combined sample, average token collection mediated 87% of the CADS effect on performance. For detailed results and further mediation analyses, see Supplementary Results 2.
Because daringness related to 3 summary-statistics of time-dependent measures, we assessed in a pre-registered analysis (H7, see table 2) whether daringness predicted continuous intra-epoch trajectories of these, or other, time-dependent measures. Thus, we computed the average intra-epoch trajectory for the 20 individuals with highest self-reported daringness in the discovery sample, for each of the 7 time-dependent measures (see figure 3B). We then projected each individual's trajectory onto this trajectory. This quantifies the extent to which an individual pursues the strategy that high CADS daringness scorers use (note that in this approach, an individual can use this strategy to a higher extent that the highest CADS daringness scorers). In the discovery sample, this metric was predicted by self-reported daringness for distance from walls, token collection rate, and speed on grid (see figure 3BC, table 2, Supplementary Table 2). These associations were collectively confirmed in the hold-out.
The next best predictor of performance was IQ, as measured with WASI-I, which explained 3.8% performance variance across the combined sample. In the discovery sample, participants with higher IQ decreased their token collection more as time progressed during an epoch, and retained more tokens (H3, see figure 4AB, table 2, Supplementary Table 2, Extended Data Figure 4). These associations were collectively confirmed in the hold-out.
In a post-hoc mediation analysis, over the combined sample, decrease in token collection mediated 80% of the IQ effect on performance. For detailed results and further mediation analyses, see Supplementary Results 2.
Finally, self-reported cognitive complexity as measured by the BIS explained 2.2% of the variance in performance across the combined sample. In the discovery sample, individuals with higher questionnaire scores (corresponding to lower cognitive complexity) reduced their token collection rate less over time, and retained fewer tokens (H6, see figure 4CD, table 2, Supplementary Table 2, Extended Data Figure 4). These associations were collectively confirmed in the hold-out.
In a post-hoc mediation analysis, over the combined sample, a decrease in token collection mediated 94% of the cognitive complexity effect on performance. For detailed results and further mediation analyses, see Supplementary Results 2.
Three associations found in the discovery sample were not confirmed in the hold-out (H5/8/9, see table 2). Furthermore, none of the underlying bivariate relationships replicated in the confirmation sample, even when we did not correct for multiple comparison (see Supplementary Table 2). In particular, the quadratic bivariate relationship of the anxiety questionnaire RCMAS with task variables was not confirmed (H8). Notably, there was also no linear relationship of self-reported anxiety with any task measure in the discovery sample at our alpha level. However, Bayes Factors were not strongly in favour of a model without RCMAS for any task measure (|LBF| < 3), such that we cannot firmly rule out a true effect of self-reported anxiety on task variables.
Absence of evidence for an impact of age or maturation on behaviour
Surprisingly in our cross-sectional analysis, age did not predict any task variable, including survival rates, either in a linear or quadratic manner, or when splitting the discovery sample as a function of sex. There was no age by sex interaction for any of the task variables. However, Bayes Factors were not strongly in favour of a model without age for any analysis (|LBF| < 3), such that we cannot rule out a population effect of age. All of these results replicated in the confirmation sample.
Our accelerated longitudinal design also enabled us to ask whether passage of time, as index of maturation at this age, had an impact on task measures. In a subsample of n = 63 participants, distributed across discovery and confirmation sample, who returned 6 months after the first visit (BSL) and played the game again (visit FU-R), the pattern of changes between BSL/FU-R and FU-R/FU-1 suggested an absence, or insignificant impact, of maturation (see Supplementary Results 4). Furthermore, in the larger number of participants (N = 567) who took part in BSL and FU-1 (after 11-32 months), but not necessarily in FU-R, any behavioural change between the two assessments was best explained by repetition and not by the time elapsed, for discovery, confirmation and combined sample, as evidenced by Bayesian model comparison (all LBF > 3 in favour of the simpler model without time, see Supplementary Results 4). The impact of age at BSL on the impact of repetition of the task is reported in Supplementary Results 4.
Relation of risk aversion and related economic preferences to risky foraging task
In a final post-hoc analysis, we examined how a pure economic risk aversion measure related to behavioural indices of cautiousness in our task, harnessing an economic risk preference paradigm in which participants made a choice between a sure amount and a lottery (see Extended Data Figure 5). A propensity to choosing the lottery was parameterised in a variant of an economic risk-return model 31, specifically the mean-variance-skewness (MVS) model 32, 33. This conceptualises choice as logistic function of the difference between the certain amount, and a weighted sum of the lottery's expected value, variance, and skewness. The relation between economic task parameters on the one hand, and the 7 task variables that related significantly to predictor variables on the other, is reported in Supplementary Results 5. None of these relations exceeded 1.7% explained variance. In keeping with previous research, we found that males were less averse to increasingly variable gambles than females (Cohen's d = 0.28; 0.13-0.42, 95% parametric confidence interval) without a pronounced difference in skewness preference (Cohen's d = -0.08; -0.20-0.07). Aversion to variable gambles explained 1.9% of the sex effect on performance (proportion of mediation 2%; - 0.1%-5.0%, 95% bootstrap confidence interval), preference for skewed gambles explained 0.1% (-0.4%-1.0%), and choice temperature mediated 2.1% (0.3%-5.0%).
Discussion
In this paper, we investigated antecedents of individual differences for risky foraging in adolescents and early adulthood. We explored a large number of relationships in a discovery sample, pre-registered 9 selected hypotheses, and then tested these in an independent hold-out sample. Our main finding is that sex was the best predictor of cautiousness as well as performance, with a 20% payment gap between the sexes. Independent of sex, self-reported daringness, cognitive complexity, and measured IQ, also predicted better task performance. At the same time, there was no evidence that internalizing measures such as clinical anxiety were associated with behaviour in the task. Neither did we find evidence that developmental time related to improved performance or reduced risk-taking, both in cross-sectional and accelerated longitudinal analyses.
An extensive literature suggests greater risk-taking for males than females, including many self-report and experimental broadly defined ‘risk’ measures 34. Our study reveals a more nuanced picture. Male adolescents took calculated risks that made them more successful in the long term. Male adolescents were less cautious (ie. collected on average more tokens and moved closer to the predator's location) when their potential losses were small early in an epoch, but adapted their behaviour to the same level as females (ie. decreased token collection to a greater extent over time) when a potential loss increased towards the end of the epoch. Our mediation analysis showed that higher initial, and more steeply decreasing, token collection explained most of the payment gap in our task. Therefore, male adolescents behaved daringly but not recklessly, in line with studies showing greater adjustment-to-risk in males in the Cambridge Gambling Task 35, 36. Males are often reported to prefer economic risk (higher variability in outcomes) more than females 34, 37. We replicated this finding here in an economic risk preference task. However, this preference did not mediate a large proportion of the sex effect on performance rendering it more likely to represent a separate propensity. Interestingly, our findings support a field study 2, which suggested a provision of economic bonuses for taking real-life risk in the ‘gig economy’ may disadvantage women, even in the absence of employer and customer discrimination.
What might account for sexual dimorphism in risky foraging behaviour? One explanation that we examined is that males are more practiced in computer games. While there is evidence for equivalent exposure to video games in both sexes 38, male adolescents engage more in the most violent-action-like video games 39, which provide for more intense sensorimotor training 40, 41. However, sensorimotor practice alone is unlikely to explain our results. First, as per design of the task, sensorimotor practice has only a very small impact on escape success, which is mostly determined by a player's location on the grid when the predator wakes up. We found that males moved closer to the sleeping predator but did not get caught more often, an observation not explained by better sensorimotor performance and which we speculate instead depends on meta-cognitive abilities. Furthermore, we found no evidence that females increased their performance more over repeated epochs (i.e. with increased sensorimotor training). On the contrary, performance increased more steeply for males with experience, at least under high threat probability, supporting an argument of greater metacognitive ability to learn based on observing one’s own performance. It is possible that training in violent-action-like games improves such ability, including habituation in perceiving ‘apparent death’ as an affordable outcome (in virtual reality), such that it can more easily be included into utility calculations 17.
A complementary explanation for females’ overall higher cautiousness might be that signals of potential threat presence weigh more negatively into their subjective perception of reward itself, possibly based on their life experience of potential threats. On the other hand, males may be more sensitive to quantitative threat features, particularly related to the way that losses vary over time. In our study, potential threat is signalled by a predator shadow looming in a corner, which stays constant over time. In a study of human avoidance learning, females engaged in avoidance behaviour more quickly and for longer during signalled threat periods than was the case for males 42, 43. Thus, in our study, threat signals may motivate females more against vigorous foraging despite a lack of actual hazard early in each epoch. Males may take into account the actual loss magnitude to a larger extent, which is variable over the course of an epoch. There was no difference between males and females in sensitivity to threat probability. Notably, the interpretation that decreased foraging over the epoch corresponds to increased cautiousness is plausible but there are alternative explanations, including a decreasing marginal utility of collecting additional tokens, or subjectively increasing hazard rate (which is objectively decreasing over time).
Two aspects of externalizing disposition were associated with task performance, but in opposite directions. Dispositional decision impulsivity, measured by ‘BIS cognitive complexity’, was anticorrelated with performance, while 'CADS daringness' correlated with performance and other task measures. These results provide an external validation for our in-task findings, albeit with small effect sizes. Furthermore, participants with high IQ performed better. Participants who did best were those that saw themselves as daring (high CADS daringness) and engaged in foraging to a greater extent, but were thoughtful as opposed to reckless (high IQ and low cognitive impulsivity), allowing them to decrease their foraging more steeply as potential loss increased. Mediation analysis suggested that although IQ and self-reported cognitive complexity were to some extent related, they represented for the most part separate influences on task behaviour. How these influences play out, and in particular how IQ leads to better performance, remains to be determined. We suggest a mediation is likely to reflect multiple influences, from faster reaction times through to reduced Pavlovian bias for losses 44.
We did not find evidence that self-reported anxiety predicted behaviour. Notably, approach/avoidance conflict paradigms such as the one we use here are designed to temporarily elicit cautious behaviour, not to distinguish among individuals based on self-reported anxiety. GABAergic anxiolytics consistently decrease cautiousness in rodent approach/avoidance conflict tasks 45, 46, 47, and in their human analogues 24, 25, 48, whereas other anxiolytic manipulations (such as chronic SSRI treatment) do not (or only inconsistently) reduce cautiousness in these tasks (see 47 for review). This suggests that cautiousness in this category of tasks (and possibly real-life cautiousness) is not directly related to, or determined by, feelings of anxiety, or their representation in questionnaire measures. More generally, while some models of human emotion make an implicit assumption that behaviour relates to concurrent subjective feeling, there is relatively little evidence for such a direct link 49, see for reviews e.g. 50, 51. This has motivated a view that regards reported feelings as representations inferred from both interoception and from mechanisms that generate behaviour 50, 52, presumably with considerable interindividual variability in this inference, as is the case for other meta-cognitive and interoceptive processes 53. In our view, this calls into question the viability of any straightforward mapping between cautious behaviour in approach/avoidance conflict tasks and self-reported anxiety, a mapping that has received limited empirical support to date. Interestingly, there was only a modest relation of anxiety and daringness in our sample (r = -.07), with some individuals expressing high values on both metrics. Thus, one may speculate that anxiety and daringness/cautiousness represent partly separate propensities that relate to different aspects of every-day behaviour on the one hand, and clinical symptoms on the other.
How reduced cautiousness in our task relates to catastrophic risk miscalculations, which can characterise the behaviour of male adolescents with a typical peak in mid-adolescence, remains to be determined. In our task, we did not detect any sex difference in terms of virtual survival. Furthermore, across the age range investigated here (14-24 years), we found no evidence for an effect of age or maturation on any task measure, including virtual survival. This raises a question as to which type of risk-taking our task measures. Reduced cautiousness in our task was linked to increased performance and thus may conceptually relate to adaptive risk-taking 7 where the latter has recently been suggested to be indexed by self-reported sensation-seeking 8. Sensation seeking is viewed to peak around 16 years of age 8, which is not the pattern we observe for cautious behaviour in our task. Impulsive risk-taking, thought to be maladaptive 7, might relate to task survival rates, but again we found no impact of age. To conclusively rule out a relation with age it will be desirable to investigate a wider age range of subjects, including children (see e.g. 9). However, we note that in a recent post-hoc analysis of adults between 18-57 years playing a similar game as ours, we observed a negative association of cautiousness with age. In other words, older participants were more risk-taking than younger ones by this metric 25. This implies our task might measure a type of risk-taking that is a relatively stable trait (as indexed by moderate test-retest reliability over 2 years) but one that is unrelated to the specific type of risk-taking that leads to adolescents' increased vulnerability to catastrophic outcomes.
In conclusion, we found that young people do not fully maximize returns in a setting where sensory features – but not actual consequences – approximate a prey situation. Attributes of male sex, a self-assessment as ‘daring’, high ‘cognitive complexity’ (i.e. low reflection impulsivity) and having higher IQ help maximize monetary returns. We found no evidence that age or maturation played a role in the 14-24 years age range. We speculate that specific subcategories of externalizing disposition, rather than internalizing features such as anxiety, dominate in determining better or worse performance under risky foraging in young people.
Methods
Ethics statement
This research complied will all relevant ethical regulations. Written informed consent was obtained for all participants over the age of 16, and written consent from a parent/legal guardian was obtained for younger participants together with their assent. Ethical approval was granted by the National Health Service Research Ethics Committee (project ID 97546). Participant compensation for this task was between £0.00 and £5.00 with an average of £2.50.
Participants and Design
Our sample consisted of individuals recruited from the Neuroscience in Psychiatry Network (NSPN) 2400 Cohort. This was a community-based sample of young people living in either Cambridgeshire or Greater London, UK (Kiddle et al., 2018). The sampled age range was chosen to capture a high-risk period for onset of a range of common mental health problems, including a period of peak incidence of adolescent risk taking. Using purposive sampling, we recruited approximately equally in 5 age and sex groups (14–15, 16–17, 18–19, 20–21, 22–24 years old), until 785 participants were tested as a ‘baseline cognition cohort’ between 2013 and 2016. All participants also filled several batteries of questionnaires, described below. Participants performed multiple tasks with different analysis methods and anticipated effect sizes. Thus, overall sample size was heuristically determined. Below we provide a post-hoc power analysis for the task reported here.
Study data were collected and managed using REDCap electronic data capture tools hosted at University College London. REDCap (Research Electronic Data Capture) is a secure, web-based application designed to support data capture for research studies 54. N = 781 participants provided complete data for the task reported here at baseline. Incomplete data were not analysed 28. An accelerated longitudinal design was then used, in which the baseline sample was invited for follow up testing. N = 568 (N = 567 complete data) participants attended after 11-32 months, the FU-1 sample. A small subset of participants was additionally tested 6 months after baseline testing (FU-R, N = 68 participants, N = 64 complete data) to test for task stability and help interpret the FU-1 results.
Human risky foraging task
This task was developed to reflect established rodent approach/avoidance conflict tests in a pac-man style computer game 23, 24, 25. The current study used a shortened version with reduced number of threat probability levels (2 instead of 3) and fewer trials per condition (20 instead of 40), and was presented using the MATLAB toolbox Cogent (www.vislab.ucl.ac.uk). The task included 80 ‘epochs’ (and one bonus epoch, see below), that is, time periods in which participants had the opportunity to accumulate monetary tokens. In each epoch, they collected tokens on a 24 × 16 grid while under threat of being chased by a predator. Being caught resulted in the loss of all tokens collected in that epoch (see Figure 1A). One corner of the grid was a location safe from predator attack. The safe place was either the player’s starting place or the opposite corner, randomly balanced over epochs. The 80 epochs were divided into five blocks of 16 epochs and approximately 5 minutes duration, with short self-paced breaks.
Tokens
At all times, ten tokens were uniformly distributed on the grid, and every 2 s one of the tokens changed its position randomly, in order to encourage uniform foraging across the grid. Collected tokens were replaced in a random position on the grid, and the number of collected tokens was displayed above the grid.
Predator
The predator was initially inactive in the corner diagonal to the safe place. Participants were instructed that the predator could become active and chase participants any time. Colour of the frame around the grid indicated two distinct predator wake-up probabilities (0.25 or 0.75), which participants learned to distinguish. Participants started either in the same place as the predator ('active') or in the safe place, ie. opposite the predator ('passive'). Notably, all epochs entailed going out onto the grid to collect tokens, and we have previously shown that over the course of an epoch, behaviour becomes comparable for the two starting positions 23, 24, 25, such that we averaged data over this factor, except for single-trial analysis of performance where such averaging was not possible.
Movements on the grid
Participants coordinated their movements by pressing the four computer keyboard arrow keys. No diagonal movements were possible. Participants could move at a maximum speed of 10 grid blocks per second if they held a key pressed. Both predators had the same speed of 40 grid blocks per second.
Epoch duration
Duration of the foraging phase was randomly drawn from 3 s, 6.5 s, 10 s, or 13.5 s. After the pre-determined foraging phase duration, the predator either woke up for a 5-second chase phase, or the next epoch started. Only the foraging phase was analysed. Before each epoch started, there was a 3 s countdown with a preview of the grid layout, during which the player could not move, to facilitate orientation on the grid.
Post-task questions
Participants rated on a visual analogue scale (ranging from 0% to 100%) the wake-up probability of the two predators. Finally, participants were given the choice to select the predator that they would like to face in a final bonus round. The majority of participants preferred the low-threat predator (discovery sample: 68%, confirmation sample 69%, both p < .001 in binomial test). They rated the wake-up rate of the low-threat predator as smaller than of the high-threat predator (mean ± standard deviation: discovery sample 47.5% ± 19.02% vs. 61.7% ± 17.84%; t491 = 10.7; p < .001; confirmation sample 46.1% ± 17.62% vs. 64.3% ± 17.69%; t324 = 11.9; p < .001).
Payment
At the end of the game, the average number of retained tokens over the whole task was transformed into a monetary reimbursement that was added to a constant fee for the whole testing day. Participants were truthfully told that average earnings from this task were expected to be £2.50 (and a maximum of £5.00) depending on performance.
Task measures
We took advantage of the substantial sample size to carry out extensive exploration in a ‘discovery’ sub-sample, and relied on an independent out-of-sample testing to validate our key hypotheses (see Analysis strategy below) Thus we analysed in total 38 task measures in the discovery sample.
First, we extracted seven previously reported continuous behavioural variables for each 1-second time bin within each epoch: (1) proportion of presence in safe place (the only grid block which the predator could not enter), (2) distance (as the crow flies) from threat (i.e., from the predator), (3) distance from nearest wall, (4) presence in safe quadrant (i.e. the quarter of the grid surrounding the safe place), (5) presence in threat quadrant (i.e. quarter of the grid surrounding the predator position), (6) token collection, and (7) speed when outside the safe place. Furthermore, we combined them into (8) a summary measure by weighting each measure by its theoretically possible range within the task, as reported previously 25. We then averaged these measures across trials for each task condition. In doing so, we had to account for the different duration of epochs. For analysis of trajectory similarity, we used mean imputation, which is the strategy we had used for trajectory analysis in previous publications 23, 24, 25. For the computation of epoch-summary scores (see below), due to a coding error that was detected after pre-registration, we imputed missing values with zeros. In supplementary results 6, we show that the resulting epoch-summary scores span the same space as scores computed with mean imputation and weighted least square regression. We further analyse the impact of this method on results, and show that all our key findings are replicated when using mean imputation before computing epoch-summary scores.
Normatively, as the number of collected tokens increases over an epoch within our task, so do potential losses, and participants should become more cautious by retreating to the safe place. We previously observed that the linear component of this intra-epoch adaptation of behaviour is reduced by the anxiolytics lorazepam, pregabalin and valproate, and by hippocampus and amygdala lesions 23, 24, 25. We have also observed lesion-induced overall changes in the average behaviour, and in the impact of threat probability 23. This motivated computing, for each of the 8 measures, the following 4 epoch-summary scores: (1) slope of a linear ordinary least squares regression on intra-epoch time, across both predators; (2) average over time and both predators; (3) difference in regression slope between the predators; (4) average difference over time, between the predators (see Extended Data Figure 1). We note that (2) corresponds to overall threat sensitivity, (1) to sensitivity to passage of time during an epoch, (4) to sensitivity to threat probability, and (3) to the interaction between time and threat probability. All measures were then recoded such that higher values mean higher cautiousness overall, or more cautiousness later (as opposed to earlier) during the epoch. Overall, this yielded 8 x 4 = 32 task measures. Furthermore, we analysed three summary statistics that did not depend on time: the number of tokens retained after the epoch ended (including predator chase phase) as main performance measure, the time until first (re-)entry into the safe place, and the minimum distance to the predator during foraging. For each of these three measures, we computed the average across conditions, and the difference between high and low threat probability, thus yielding another 6 task measures. Overall, 38 task measures were analysed.
Demographic and self-report measures
As demographic measures, we included sex and age on the day of the cognitive task. An estimate of total IQ was obtained using the 2-subtest version of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence – First Edition (WASI-I) 55. Self-report questionnaires were sent out in three waves not synchronised with the cognitive task battery. If more than one questionnaire pack was returned, we linearly interpolated questionnaire values from the two closest time points to the time point of the cognitive task. We measured symptoms with the Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (MFQ) 56, the Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale (RCMAS) 57, the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSE) 58, the Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (K10) 59, and the ‘Behaviour Checklist’. The latter was a new brief self-report instrument based on the DSM-IV criteria for conduct disorder 28. Dispositions were assessed by the sum scores for the 3 subscales of the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APD) 60, the 3 measures of the Child and Adolescent Disposition Scale (CADS, see Supplementary Table 3) 61, the 9 subscales of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ) 62, the 3 subscales of the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits (ICU) 63, and the 6 first-order factors of the Barratt Impulsive Scale (BIS, see Supplementary Table 4) 64. Overall, 32 demographic and self-report measures covering ‘internalizing’ characteristics that might be related to over-cautiousness and anxiety, and ‘externalizing’ measures that might relate to inability or unwillingness to exercise thoughtful caution, were included in the analysis.
Statistical analysis: exploration-confirmation analysis
The high dimensionality of the data set and the many possible ways of analysing it posed a formidable multiple comparison problem. This is why we opted for a rigorous out-of-sample validation approach. All analyses were performed in a ‘discovery’ sub-sample and selected hypotheses from this analysis were pre-registered. The hold-out sample was then used for confirmation. The data analyst (DRB), who was located outside the NSPN centres, had no access to the primary NSPN database. He was provided the two samples via the data curator (MM) sequentially, so as not to have access to the hold-out sample until after the confirmation analysis was pre-registered at the Open Science Framework on 28.07.2018 (https://osf.io/hrce6/). All models to be confirmed were included in this pre-registration as RData files. The discovery sample comprised around 2/3 of the data, randomly drawn for each sex and age group from the entire sample (N = 492), while the remaining cases constituted the confirmation sample (N = 289). In addition, the data analyst had access to all data from the N = 64 FU-R cases, which were distributed between discovery and confirmation samples. The size of discovery and confirmation sample was determined heuristically. At our chosen alpha level of α = .001, the discovery sample was sufficiently large to detect a bivariate correlation of R2 = .10 with > 99% power, a correlation of R2 = .05 with 96% power, and a correlation of R2 = .02 with 44% power.
All statistical tests (with the exception of random permutation tests) were two-tailed. All statistical models (with the exception of random permutation tests) assumed normality of the residuals, but this assumption was not formally tested.
First, we constructed quantitative hypotheses of how task measures related to demographic/psychometric variables. We computed 1216 bivariate regressions between each task measure on the one hand, and each demographic/self-report variable on the other. Only findings at an alpha level of p < .001 were retained. To minimise the number of confirmation tests, we then built multiple regression models to predict each psychometric/demographic measure simultaneously from all those task measures that had a significant bivariate relation. These models were then applied to the confirmation data set without refitting, and tested by randomly permuting the dependent variable 105 times. We present the ratio of explained variance (R2) in the hold-out sample as best estimate of the out-of-sample predictive performance, and the explained variance for a multiple regression model with the same predictors, fit on the combined sample, as best estimate of in-sample predictive performance 29. For all effect sizes, we computed 95% confidence intervals from the sampling distribution of 105 bootstrapped samples, taking into account the asymmetric distributions. Notably, confidence intervals are included due to journal requirements and do not reflect the posterior plausibility of true parameter values 65. We then estimated the relative contribution of each task measure in predicting the demographic/self-report measure in sequential procedure, by residualising on each step each task measure with respect to the measure that shared most variance with the predictor in the previous step.
Next, we were interested in quadratic relationships between demographic/self-report variables, and task variables. We computed 1216 multiple regression models to predict each task measure from a second order polynomial of each demographic/self-report variable (ie. from the variable and its square). Findings at an alpha-level of p < .001 were retained. We then build multiple regression models to relate the questionnaire variable to the several task measures with significant bivariate quadratic relations. To do so, we z-scored each relating task variable across participants, and then averaged over all relating task variables. We then build a multiple regression model to predict this task sum score from a second order polynomial of the questionnaire measure. This model was then applied to the confirmation data set without refitting and with the normalisation parameters established in the discovery sample, and subject to the aforementioned random permutation test. Notably, neither of the two hypotheses derived in this way was confirmed in the hold-out sample.
CADS daringness was the self-report variable with the highest number of relationships to task variables. We were thus interested whether CADS daringness did not only predict summary statistics from the task, but also the intra-epoch trajectories. To address this, we averaged each of the 7 time-dependent measures across the predator factor. We then created the average intra-epoch trajectory for the 20 individuals scoring highest on CADS daringness. For the remaining individuals, we calculated the scalar product between individual trajectory and high-daringness trajectory, separately for each measure. We then computed the bivariate relation between each trajectory similarity measure and CADS daringness. Findings at an alpha-level of p < .001 were retained. We then built a multiple regression model to predict CADS daringness from all trajectory similarity measures that significantly related to CADS daringness, similar to the approach described above. In the confirmation sample, we computed, for all participants, trajectory similarity with the high-daringness trajectories from the discovery sample. We then applied this model to the confirmation data set without refitting, in a random permutation test.
All bivariate relations and multiple regression models in the discovery sample were replicated after controlling for sex as a covariate. All linear and trajectory models that were significant in the confirmation sample were followed up to examine sex as a possible confound, by refitting the multiple regression model with sex as covariate, and all confirmed hypotheses were replicated in this procedure.
Some individual bivariate relationships were not replicated in the confirmation sample, although the joint predictive model was confirmed. To follow up on this discrepancy, all confirmed multiple regression models were refitted to the entire data set, to test for a dataset × predictor interaction (indicating different weights in discovery and confirmation sample); and no significant differences between the regression weights for discovery and confirmation sample were found with this approach.
In all cases where we interpret null results, we computed evidence for a linear model with the predictor in question, and a null model without the predictor. We approximated model evidence by extracting Akaike Information Criterion 66 with the R function "AIC". We computed log Bayes Factors (LBF) as LBF = 0.5 (AICpred - AICnull), where positive values are evidence in favour of the simpler null model.
We reasoned that performance differences between the sexes could reflect a differential prior experience with computer games, and this could lead to a different performance trajectory over trials. Thus, we analysed sex effects on the trajectory of task performance over trials, as indexed by the number of tokens retained (after catch phase). We only analysed the first 64 trials, as the duration (which strongly influences performance) of the remaining 16 trials was unbalanced. First we used Bayesian model selection to pinpoint the best model for the trial-by-trial trajectory across categories. We compared linear, quadratic, logarithmic and square root models by Akaike Information Criterion. We then tested the effect of sex in a 2 (sex) x 2 (predator) x 2 (task) ANCOVA with log(trl) as continuous covariate.
Developmental and practice effects on the task were tested by analysing data from the baseline and follow-up measures. We first tested, for each task measure, the change between BSL and FU-R, and between FU-R and FU-1. To disentangle the effect of maturation and practice in the FU-1 sample, we computed, for each task measure, a linear mixed-effects model with repetition as within-subject factor, and time between BSL and FU-1 as continuous predictor. We compared this with a model not including time, and extracted Akaike Information Criterion to approximate model evidence.
In our analysis, we combined weighted summary statistics of the seven time-dependent measures into cautiousness sum scores. We have previously suggested that linear change in cautiousness reflects a "loss adaptation" sum score 25. To assess the internal consistency of this metric, we report Cronbach's alpha of the 7 contributing statistics. We assessed test-retest reliability of all 38 task measures by computing the bivariate correlation between BSL and FU-R, or FU-1, respectively. Finally, we investigated the internal structure of 7 linear change coefficients, or all 34 task measures (excluding the collinear sum scores) by computing an exploratory factor analysis using maximum likelihood factorisation with varimax rotation. Parallel analysis suggested 2 or 6 factors, respectively. We computed exploratory factor analysis with these numbers of factors. We then split the discovery data set into random partitions and found that for the factor analysis of 34 task measures, only 3 factors robustly replicated between different partitions. These 3 factors had a cumulative factor loading of .71. This is why we chose to report and interpret the first 2, or 3, factors of the exploratory factor analysis. Because factor analysis was computed with a higher number of factors, this choice does not impact upon the factor solution. We then defined a confirmatory factor analysis by retaining all factor loadings above an absolute threshold of .2. The exploratory factor analysis was computed in the confirmation sample in the same way, using 2 or 6 factors as determined on the discovery sample, of which we retained 2 or 3 factors. The confirmatory factor analysis did not converge in the confirmation sample and is not reported here. To nevertheless confirm the exploratory factor analysis in the confirmation sample, we computed factor values in the confirmation sample using either the loadings derived in the discovery sample, or factor loadings from an exploratory factor analysis on the confirmation sample. We then assessed the correlation between the two ways of computing factor values.
Further exploratory analyses that did not yield additional insights within the discovery sample included predicting the change in task measures between BSL and FU-1 from demographic/self-report measures at BSL or from change in these measures between BSL and FU-1, and canonical correlation analysis between task and demographic/self-report measures.
Statistical analysis: post-hoc analysis in the combined sample
After gaining access to the complete data set, the following exploratory and non-planned analyses were performed. We only report parameter estimates and, where appropriate, confidence intervals, but provide no inference statistics.
First, we analysed how task performance related to all other task measures. Furthermore, at the suggestion of a reviewer, we analysed virtual survival rates, ie. the rate with which participants were caught when the predator woke up. We analysed how survival rate related to the 38 task measures in the entire sample, and to the 32 predictor measures in the discovery sample.
Second, we analysed mediation of the impact of predictors on task variables using the R package 'mediation', which uses a quasi-Bayesian Monte Carlo approximation to estimate causally mediated, and non-mediated direct, effects 30.
Third, we analysed a separate ‘lottery task’, administered to the same cohort (N = 781 available data sets, baseline only), in order to assess canonical economic risk preferences. This followed very closely the methodology of Symmonds et al 33, but the lotteries were simplified to four ‘slices’ (see Extended Data Figure 5), to reduce cognitive load and facilitate large scale testing. Please see 33 for further details.
The probability of choosing the roulette was modelled as:
Where the index r refers to the roulette and sure to sure amount, E is expectation (mean), VAR the variance, SKEW the skewness, τ the decision temperature parameter, wvar, wskew the parameters quantifying the taste of the individual for variable and skewed distributions of outcome respectively and z is a normalizing factor ensuring choice probabilities add to 1. A lower wvar was therefore hypothesized to statistically explain less avoidance to risk-related states (distance from predator, etc) in our main task. The model was fitted by finding, for each individual, the set of parameters that maximized the likelihood of the data.
Extended Data
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank the the NSPN management and research assistant teams. Cognitive experiments were realized using Cogent 2000 developed by the Cogent 2000 team at the FIL and the ICN, and Cogent Graphics developed by John Romaya at the Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience. The Wellcome Trust funded the ‘Neuroscience in Psychiatry Project’ (NSPN). All NSPN members (Supplementary Table 1) are supported by Wellcome Strategic Award (095844/7/11/Z). DRB is supported by funding from the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (Grant agreement No. ERC-2018 CoG-816564 ActionContraThreat). MM and DRB receive support from the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) UCLH Biomedical Research Centre. RJD is supported by a Wellcome Investigator Award (098362/Z/12/Z). Peter Fonagy (NSPN consortium, Supplementary Table 1) is in receipt of a NIHR Senior Investigator Award (NF-SI-0514-10157) and was in part supported by the NIHR Collaboration for Leadership in Applied Health Research and Care (CLAHRC) North Thames at Barts Health NHS Trust. The Max Planck – UCL Centre for Computational Psychiatry and Ageing is a joint initiative of the Max Planck Society and UCL. The Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging is funded by core funding from the Wellcome Trust (203147/Z/16/Z). The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.
Footnotes
Data availability
Anonymised data are available on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/mnbfy/) 67. Full data are available upon reasonable request from the corresponding authors or from OpenNSPN@medschl.cam.ac.uk.
Code availability
All custom code used for the analysis is available on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/mnbfy/) 67. After extracting task measures using Matlab 2017b, all discovery analyses were performed in R 3.4.1 (www.r-project.org), using the following toolboxes: R.matlab v 3.6.1, abind 1.4-5, reshape2 1.4.3, nlme 3.1-131.1, lme4 1.1-15, lmerTest 2.0-36, nFactors 2.3.3, sem 3.1-9. Confirmation and post-hoc analyses were performed in R 3.5.2, using the following toolboxes: R.matlab v 3.6.2, abind 1.4-5, reshape2 1.4.3, psych 1.8.12, lme4 1.1-21, lmerTest 3.1-0, sem 3.1-9, pracma 2.2.5, mediation 4.5.0, gvlma 1.0.0.3, DescTools 0.99.30, corrplot 0.84.
Author contributions
DRB, MM, NSPN consortium, and RJD contributed to conception and design of this work. MM, NSPN consortium, and RJD contributed to acquisition of the data. DRB and MM analysed the data. DRB, MM, AB and RJD contributed to interpretation of the data and to drafting and revising the manuscript.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
References
- 1.Lima SL, Dill LM. Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Canadian journal of zoology. 1990;68:619–640. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Cook C, Diamond R, Hall J, List JA, Oyer P. The gender earnings gap in the gig economy: Evidence from over a million rideshare drivers. National Bureau of Economic Research; 2018. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Steinberg L. Risk taking in adolescence: what changes, and why? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2004;1021:51–58. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Schwebel DC, Severson J, Ball KK, Rizzo M. Individual difference factors in risky driving: the roles of anger/hostility, conscientiousness, and sensation-seeking. Accid Anal Prev. 2006;38:801–810. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2006.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Eaton DK, et al. Youth risk behavior surveillance--United States, 2007. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2008;57:1–131. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Somerville LH, Casey BJ. Developmental neurobiology of cognitive control and motivational systems. Current opinion in neurobiology. 2010;20:236–241. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.01.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Romer D, Reyna VF, Satterthwaite TD. Beyond stereotypes of adolescent risk taking: Placing the adolescent brain in developmental context. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2017;27:19–34. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.07.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Khurana A, Romer D, Betancourt LM, Hurt H. Modeling Trajectories of Sensation Seeking and Impulsivity Dimensions from Early to Late Adolescence: Universal Trends or Distinct Sub-groups? J Youth Adolesc. 2018;47:1992–2005. doi: 10.1007/s10964-018-0891-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.van den Bos W, Hertwig R. Adolescents display distinctive tolerance to ambiguity and to uncertainty during risky decision making. Scientific reports. 2017;7 doi: 10.1038/srep40962. 40962. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Frey R, Pedroni A, Mata R, Rieskamp J, Hertwig R. Risk preference shares the psychometric structure of major psychological traits. Sci Adv. 2017;3:e1701381. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1701381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Overman WH, Frassrand K, Ansel S, Trawalter S, Bies B, Redmond A. Performance on the IOWA card task by adolescents and adults. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:1838–1851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Deakin J, Aitken M, Robbins T, Sahakian BJ. Risk taking during decision-making in normal volunteers changes with age. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10:590–598. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704104104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Lauriola M, Panno A, Levin IP, Lejuez CW. Individual Differences in Risky Decision Making: A Meta-analysis of Sensation Seeking and Impulsivity with the Balloon Analogue Risk Task. J Behav Decis Making. 2014;27:20–36. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Defoe IN, Dubas JS, Figner B, van Aken MA. A meta-analysis on age differences in risky decision making: adolescents versus children and adults. Psychol Bull. 2015;141:48–84. doi: 10.1037/a0038088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Bach DR, Hulme O, Penny WD, Dolan RJ. The known unknowns: neural representation of second-order uncertainty, and ambiguity. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:4811–4820. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1452-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bach DR, Dolan RJ. Knowing how much you don't know: a neural organization of uncertainty estimates. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13:572–586. doi: 10.1038/nrn3289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Korn CW, Bach DR. Maintaining homeostasis by decision-making. PLoS computational biology. 2015;11:e1004301. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Korn CW, Bach DR. Heuristic and optimal policy computations in the human brain during sequential decision-making. Nature Communications. 2018;9 doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-02750-3. 325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Korn CW, Bach DR. Minimizing threat via heuristic and optimal policies recruits hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex. Nat Hum Behav. 2019 doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0603-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Caraco T. Energy Budgets, Risk and Foraging Preferences in Dark-Eyed Juncos (Junco-Hyemalis) Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1981;8:213–217. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kolling N, Behrens TE, Mars RB, Rushworth MF. Neural mechanisms of foraging. Science. 2012;336:95–98. doi: 10.1126/science.1216930. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Khemka S, Barnes G, Dolan RJ, Bach DR. Dissecting the Function of Hippocampal Oscillations in a Human Anxiety Model. J Neurosci. 2017;37:6869–6876. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1834-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Bach DR, et al. Human Hippocampus Arbitrates Approach-Avoidance Conflict. Current Biology. 2014;24:541–547. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.01.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Bach DR, Korn CW, Vunder J, Bantel A. Effect of valproate and pregabalin on human anxiety-like behaviour in a randomised controlled trial. Transl Psychiatry. 2018;8:157. doi: 10.1038/s41398-018-0206-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Korn CW, Vunder J, Miro J, Fuentemilla L, Hurlemann R, Bach DR. Amygdala Lesions Reduce Anxiety-like Behavior in a Human Benzodiazepine-Sensitive Approach-Avoidance Conflict Test. Biological psychiatry. 2017;82:522–531. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.01.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bach DR, Hoffmann M, Finke C, Hurlemann R, Ploner CJ. Disentangling Hippocampal and Amygdala Contribution to Human Anxiety-Like Behavior. J Neurosci. 2019;39:8517–8526. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0412-19.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Mobbs D, Kim JJ. Neuroethological studies of fear, anxiety, and risky decision-making in rodents and humans. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2015;5:8–15. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kiddle B, et al. Cohort Profile: The NSPN 2400 Cohort: a developmental sample supporting the Wellcome Trust NeuroScience in Psychiatry Network. Int J Epidemiol. 2018;47:18–19g. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyx117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Yarkoni T, Westfall J. Choosing Prediction Over Explanation in Psychology: Lessons From Machine Learning. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2017;12:1100–1122. doi: 10.1177/1745691617693393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Tingley D, Yamamoto T, Hirose K, Keele L, Imai K. mediation: R Package for Causal Mediation Analysis. J Stat Softw. 2014;59 [Google Scholar]
- 31.Markowitz H. Portfolio selection. Journal of Finance. 1952;7:77–91. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Symmonds M, Bossaerts P, Dolan RJ. A behavioral and neural evaluation of prospective decision-making under risk. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:14380–14389. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1459-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Symmonds M, Wright ND, Bach DR, Dolan RJ. Deconstructing risk: Separable encoding of variance and skewness in the brain. Neuroimage. 2011 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Byrnes JP, Miller DC, Schafer WD. Gender differences in risk taking: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 1999;125:367–383. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Lewis G, Srinivasam R, Roiser JP, Blakemore SJ, Flouri E, Lewis G. Risk taking to obtain reward: gender differences and associations with emotional and depressive symptoms in a nationally representative cohort of UK adolescents. 2019 doi: 10.1101/644450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.van den Bos R, Taris R, Scheppink B, de Haan L, Verster JC. Salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase levels during an assessment procedure correlate differently with risk-taking measures in male and female police recruits. Front Behav Neurosci. 2013;7:219. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Fisher PJ, Yao R. Gender differences in financial risk tolerance. J Econ Psychol. 2017;61:191–202. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Stuart K. UK gamers: more women play games than men, report finds. The Guardian) 2014 [Google Scholar]
- 39.DeCamp W. Who plays violent video games? An exploratory analysis of predictors of playing violent games. Pers Indiv Differ. 2017;117:260–266. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Green CS, Bavelier D. Action video game modifies visual selective attention. Nature. 2003;423:534–537. doi: 10.1038/nature01647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Dye MW, Green CS, Bavelier D. Increasing Speed of Processing With Action Video Games. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2009;18:321–326. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01660.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Sheynin J, et al. Behaviourally inhibited temperament and female sex, two vulnerability factors for anxiety disorders, facilitate conditioned avoidance (also) in humans. Behav Processes. 2014;103:228–235. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.01.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Sheynin J, Moustafa AA, Beck KD, Servatius RJ, Myers CE. Testing the role of reward and punishment sensitivity in avoidance behavior: a computational modeling approach. Behav Brain Res. 2015;283:121–138. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.01.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Moutoussis M, et al. Change, stability, and instability in the Pavlovian guidance of behaviour from adolescence to young adulthood. PLoS computational biology. 2018;14:e1006679. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Calhoon GG, Tye KM. Resolving the neural circuits of anxiety. Nature Neuroscience. 2015;18:1394–1404. doi: 10.1038/nn.4101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Gray JA, McNaughton N. The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septohippocampal system. Oxford University Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kirlic N, Young J, Aupperle RL. Animal to human translational paradigms relevant for approach avoidance conflict decision making. Behav Res Ther. 2017;96:14–29. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2017.04.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Biedermann SV, et al. An elevated plus-maze in mixed reality for studying human anxiety-related behavior. BMC Biol. 2017;15:125. doi: 10.1186/s12915-017-0463-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.DeWall CN, Baumeister RF, Chester DS, Bushman BJ. How Often Does Currently Felt Emotion Predict Social Behavior and Judgment? A Meta-Analytic Test of Two Theories. Emot Rev. 2015 [Google Scholar]
- 50.Bach DR, Dayan P. Algorithms for survival: a comparative perspective on emotions. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2017;18:311–319. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.LeDoux JE. Semantics, Surplus Meaning, and the Science of Fear. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2017;21:303–306. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Barrett LF. The theory of constructed emotion: An active inference account of interoception and categorization. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2016 doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Rouault M, Seow T, Gillan CM, Fleming SM. Psychiatric Symptom Dimensions Are Associated With Dissociable Shifts in Metacognition but Not Task Performance. Biological psychiatry. 2018;84:443–451. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.12.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Harris PA, Taylor R, Thielke R, Payne J, Gonzalez N, Conde JG. Research electronic data capture (REDCap)--a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J Biomed Inform. 2009;42:377–381. doi: 10.1016/j.jbi.2008.08.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Wechsler D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. The Psychological Corporation: Harcourt Brace & Company; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Costello EJ, Angold A. Scales to assess child and adolescent depression: checklists, screens, and nets. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1988;27:726–737. doi: 10.1097/00004583-198811000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Reynolds CR, Richmond BO. What I Think and Feel: a revised measure of Children's Manifest Anxiety. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1997;25:15–20. doi: 10.1023/a:1025751206600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Rosenberg M. Conceiving the self. Basic Books; 1979. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Kessler R, Mroczek D. Final versions of our non-specific psychological distress scale. Memo dated March. 1994;10:1994. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Frick PH, Hare RD. The Antisocial Process Screening Device. Multi-Health Systems; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Lahey BB, Rathouz PJ, Applegate B, Tackett JL, Waldman ID. Psychometrics of a self-report version of the Child and Adolescent Dispositions Scale. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2010;39:351–361. doi: 10.1080/15374411003691784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Raine A. The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophr Bull. 1991;17:555–564. doi: 10.1093/schbul/17.4.555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Kimonis ER, et al. Assessing callous-unemotional traits in adolescent offenders: validation of the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits. Int J Law Psychiatry. 2008;31:241–252. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2008.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol. 1995;51:768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Morey RD, Hoekstra R, Rouder JN, Lee MD, Wagenmakers EJ. The fallacy of placing confidence in confidence intervals. Psychon Bull Rev. 2016;23:103–123. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0947-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Burnham KP, Anderson DR. Multimodel inference - understanding AIC and BIC in model selection. Sociol Method Res. 2004;33:261–304. [Google Scholar]
- 67.Bach DR, Moutoussis MM, NSPN consortium. Bowler A, Dolan RJ. NSPN_AAC. 2020 doi: 10.17605/OSF.IO/MNBFY. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.