Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2020 Apr 9;10:6145. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-63089-2

Season of birth and sugary beverages are predictors of Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices Scores in adolescents

Reem Al-Sabah 1, Abdullah Al-Taiar 2, Abdur Rahman 3,, Lemia Shaban 3, Anwar Al-Harbi 3, Olusegun Mojiminiyi 4
PMCID: PMC7145867  PMID: 32273542

Abstract

To investigate factors associated with cognitive functioning in healthy adolescents, a school-based cross-sectional study was conducted on 1370 adolescents aged 11–16 years that were randomly selected from all governorates of Kuwait. Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices (SPM), a non-verbal test of intelligence, was used to measure cognitive functioning of the study participants. Data on predictors of cognitive functioning were collected from parents and adolescents. Weight and height of the participants were measured in a standardized manner and blood samples were tested in an accredited laboratory under strict measures of quality control. In multivariable linear regression analysis, factors that showed significant association with the SPM score were gender (p = 0.002), season of birth (p = 0.009), place of residence (p < 0.001), father’s (p < 0.001) and mother’s (p = 0.025) educational level, type of housing (p < 0.001), passive smoking at home (p = 0.031), sleeping hours during weekends (p = 0.017), students’ educational level (p < 0.001) and the frequency of consumption of sugary drinks (p < 0.001). The link between cognitive functioning and season of birth seems to be robust in various geographical locations including the Middle East. The association between sugary drinks and cognitive functioning highlights the importance of diet independently of obesity and support efforts to reduce consumption of sugary drinks among children.

Subject terms: Health sciences, Risk factors

Introduction

Cognitive function comprises various mental processes such as attention, memory, and language that are required to manage daily life activities in all age groups. Among children and adolescents, cognitive functions are essential for good school performance and educational achievements. More importantly, cognitive function during childhood and adolescence is strongly linked to various health outcomes in adulthood. As an example, in adulthood, children with poor cognitive functions have an increased risk of hypertension1, coronary heart diseases and stroke2, psychological disorders3, and all-cause mortality46. These associations seem to be strong, incremental across the whole spectrum of cognitive function and independent of socioeconomic conditions7.

Much of the recent public health research has focused on factors associated with the decline of cognitive function among the elderly. This is understandable because populations are aging in most countries, and the decline of cognitive function has become a major public health issue. Additionally, there has been substantial research on factors associated with the development of cognitive functions during early life, but cognitive abilities among healthy school-aged children have hardly been addressed in the literature. This holds true in spite of the fact that some areas of the human brain as well as higher cognitive functions continue to develop during adolescents8.

Predictors of cognitive function have also been thoroughly investigated among children and adolescents with various disease conditions such as children with specific syndromes9,10, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder11, sickle cell anemia12, beta-thalassemia13, HIV14,15, epilepsy16, type 1 diabetes17,18 and many other disease conditions. Among healthy adolescents, studies have focused on the role of poverty and undernutrition19, iron deficiency and parasitic infections20, parental education21, or overall socioeconomic status22. These studies were mainly in low-income countries and thus their findings cannot be extrapolated to high-income settings such as the oil-rich Arab countries in the Middle East. In these countries, there has been a dramatic change in socioeconomic status in the last few decades, which has eliminated poverty and undernutrition and led to significant improvements in education. Therefore, factors like poverty and undernutrition are unlikely to be a major predictor of cognitive function among children in this setting. In these settings, new factors may have emerged such as overweight and obesity in addition to sedentary lifestyle, which have been linked to poor cognitive function23. We have previously reported that vitamin D levels is not associated with cognitive function24. In this study, we aim to investigate factors associated with cognitive functioning as measured by non-verbal test of intelligence, including obesity, physical activity, and nutritional deficiencies such as low levels of vitamin B12, iron and ferritin.

Methods

Kuwait is a small country with a population of 4.2 million and is divided into six provinces (governorates). A school-based cross-sectional study was conducted on students in middle schools typically aged 11–16 years. The details of the project have been published previously24,25. In brief, we recruited students from 12 public middle schools that were randomly selected using stratified multistage cluster random sampling with a probability proportional to size from all governorates of Kuwait. In each governorate, schools with larger number of students were given a higher probability of being selected compared to school with small number of students. The sample allocation in each governorate was based on the relative size of that province as judged by the total number of students in the governorate. The study was funded by Kuwait University and approved by The Ethics Committee at the Ministry of Health in Kuwait (No: 2015/248) as well as The Ethics Committee at the Health Sciences Centre, Kuwait University (No: DR/EC/2338). The study was conducted in accordance with Declaration of Helsinki ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. An informed consent was taken from the parents of each study participant.

Collecting data on predictors of cognitive functioning

An informed consent was sent to the parents along with a self-administered questionnaire on parental level of education and income, type of housing, number of siblings of the index child, passive smoking in the household and the number of times per week the index child had a meal prepared outside the home during the last three months. After obtaining written informed consent from the parents and verbal assent from the school children, trained dedicated personnel carried out face-to-face interviews with the students using a structured questionnaire. The questionnaire was carefully developed after an extensive review of the literature and was pilot tested on 20 students who were not included in the study. We included questions on physical activity utilizing a questionnaire that was developed based on the Youth Physical Activity Questionnaire in UK26 and The Arab teens lifestyle study27. This questionnaire was validated among high school students and showed a strong correlation with data collected using accelerometers (Spearman correlation 0.92; p < 0.001 for total steps count) (not published). Anthropometric measurements, including standing height and body weight of the study subjects, were assessed using a digital weight and height scale in a standardized manner.

Blood sample of five ml of venous blood was drawn from each participant by a trained nurse. On the same day, the samples were centrifuged, and the serum was transferred to Eppendorf tubes and stored at −80 °C for analysis. In these blood samples vitamin D was measured using liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry (LC-MS/MS). Complete blood count (CBC), Parathyroid Hormone (PTH), vitamin B12, Iron, ferritin, transferrin and transferrin saturation were all measured in an accredited Teaching Hospital Clinical Biochemistry laboratory, where these tests are routinely conducted under strict quality control. Lead in whole blood (50 µL) was measured using ESA’s LeadCare II® Blood Lead Testing System (RNA Medical Bionostics, Inc. Devens, MA, USA) according the manufacturer’s instructions. Because this method was deemed to be not accurate, lead was evaluated again in all blood samples using Inductively Coupled Plasma Mass Spectrometry (ICP-MS).

Assessment of cognitive functioning

Comprehensive assessment of cognitive function requires using several tests that evaluate verbal cognition, non-verbal cognition, executive functioning and processing speed. However, conducting such comprehensive assessment in large epidemiological studies in school settings would be logistically difficult. Furthermore, many cognitive tests are heavily influenced by language and culture. We used Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices (SPM) to assess the cognitive functioning of the study participants. The test is culturally fair and non-verbal measure of intelligence (mainly fluid intelligence) and measures abstract reasoning, problem solving, perceptual awareness and reasoning by analogy. It measures participant’s eductive (to make meaning out of chaos), as opposed to reproductive (to understand and recall previously learned information) ability28. In SPM, participants are asked to identify the missing piece of an array of patterns by determining which rule or rules govern the patterns displayed in the rows and columns. SPM has been previously validated in our setting29. In fact, SPM is used routinely to measure cognitive function in schools as part of the psychological and social services provide by The Ministry of Education in Kuwait. Specialists from The Department of Psychological and Social Services at The Ministry of Education administered SPM in the schools. Raw scores were converted to standard scores as per the instruction manual30. We will use the term cognitive function or cognitive functioning to refer to results of the Raven’s SPM in our study.

Sample size estimation and power calculation

The required sample size was estimated using the Stata command powerreg to provide more than 90% power to detect a change of 0.03 in the coefficient of determination (R2) at 5% level of significance assuming that R2 in the full model is 0.20 taking into account the number of total predictors included in the study (29 predictors in total and 64 dummy variables). While the calculation showed that 1131 study subjects would be sufficient, an addition of 20% of the calculated sample size was added to compensate for missing data.

Statistical methods

Data were double entered into specifically designed database using Epidata Entry. Data analysis was conducted using Stata version 12 (StataCorp. College Station, TX, USA). BMI was calculated as weight (Kg) divided by height squared (m2), which was later categorized into normal, overweight and obese according to WHO growth charts. The main outcome in the analysis was SPM score, which was approximately normally distributed; hence we used linear regression models to investigate the predictors of SPM score. Guided by the conceptual frameworks in the literature31,32, variables were divided into groups. The first group included socio-demographic factors such as sex, nationality, parental education, total number of siblings, father’s income, mother’s employment, type of housing, having private room for the index child in the house, child rank and whether the child lives with both parents in addition to season of birth. The second group included educational level of the participant, hours of sleep during weekdays and weekends, whether the child takes a nap during the day at weekdays and weekends, whether the child is currently taking vitamins or supplements, and frequency of sugary drinks consumption. The third group included total time of physical activity and BMI categories as per WHO growth charts; while the fourth group included vitamin D, parathyroid hormone (PTH), hemoglobin (Hb), calcium, iron, ferritin, transferrin and transferrin saturation in addition to lead. Continuous variables were fitted either as continuous or categorized using acceptable cutoff points or using tertiles. In univariable analysis, variables that showed significant association with SPM score at a conservative α level < 0.2 were included in multivariate analysis. Then variables in each group were added sequentially to the model and those with p < 0.05 were retained in the model. Interaction terms were fitted in the model when there is a prior knowledge that support presence of the interaction. The assumptions for this analysis were checked including the linear relationship assumption using scatter plots and partial regression plots. Homoscedasticity assumption was checked by Breusch-Pagan test and by plotting the studentized residuals against the unstandardized predicted values. Multicollinearity was checked by the inspection of correlation coefficients matrix between independent variables and variance inflation factor. Normality of the residuals was checked by Q-Q plot. Finally, we used stepwise backward and forward selection approaches to identify factors that were independently associated with the cognitive function as a confirmatory method.

Results

Of the 1583 parents approached, 161 refused to participate (either child or parents refused). Another 6 adolescents had samples that were not sufficient to conduct blood analysis. Of the remaining 1416 adolescents, 1370 completed the SPM and their socio-demographic characteristics are shown in Table 1. Table 2 shows the association between various factors presumed to be associated with SPM scores in univariable analysis. Most socio-economic factors showed associations with SPM scores in univariable analysis. However, except for PTH and calcium, most of the laboratory measurements such as hemoglobin, iron, ferritin, transferrin, transferring saturation and vitamin D level were all not associated with SPM scores. In multivariable analysis, factors that showed significant associations with SPM scores were gender (p = 0.002), season of birth (p = 0.009), place of residence (province) (p < 0.001), father’s (p < 0.001) and mother’s (p = 0.025) educational level, type of housing (p < 0.001), passive smoking at home (p = 0.031), sleeping hours during weekends (p = 0.017), students’ educational level (grade) (p < 0.001) and the frequency of consumption of sugary drinks (p < 0.001) (Table 3). Both backward and forward stepwise regression selected the same variables and these variables were also the same as the manually developed model above. However, PTH as a continuous variable was significant in the stepwise selection (β = −0.27; p = 0.04) but not in the manually developed model (β = −0.23; p = 0.065). It is worth noting that PTH (when fitted as a categorical variable using quartiles or using clinically acceptable cutoff points) was not significantly associated with SPM neither in the stepwise selection nor in the manually developed model.

Table 1.

Socio-demographic characteristics of 1370 adolescents in public middle schools in Kuwait.

Characteristics
Age in years, Mean (SD) years 12.40 (0.93)
n (%)
Gender
   Male 674 (49.2)
Nationality
   Kuwaiti 1,047 (76.42)
   Non-Kuwait 323 (23.58)
Father’s Educationa
   No formal education 15 (1.12)
   Primary/Intermediate 215 (16.08)
   Secondary (high school) 330 (24.68)
   Diploma 251 (18.77)
   University & above 526 (39.34)
Mother’s Educationb
   No formal education 31 (2.30)
   Primary/Intermediate 145 (10.74)
   Secondary (high school) 294 (21.78)
   Diploma 293 (21.70)
University & above 587 (43.48)
Father’s Incomec (Kuwaiti Dinars)
   Less than 500 89 (6.72)
   500 to 1000 291 (21.98)
   1001 to 1500 414 (31.27)
   1501 to 2000 213 (16.09)
   More than 2000 164 (12.39)
   Do not wish to tell 153 (11.56)
Mother’s Employment Statusd
   Housewife 466 (34.72)
   Paid employment 664 (49.48)
   Others 212 (15.80)
Housinge
   Rented flat 499 (36.94)
   Rented house 159 (11.77)
   Owned flat 55 (4.07)
   Owned house 638 (47.22)

aMissing for 33 participants; bMissing for 20 participants; cMissing for 46 participants; dMissing for 28 participants; eMissing for 19 participants.

Table 2.

Association between Cognitive Function (Raven’s Progressive Matrices scores) and various factors among adolescents in univariable analysis.

Variables β (95% CI) p-valuea
Gender
   Male [Reference] 0.120
   Female 1.97 [−0.51,4.45]
Nationality
   Kuwaiti [Reference] 0.090
   Non-Kuwaiti 2.52 [−0.40,5.45]
Season of birth
   Summer (May, June, July) [Reference] 0.045
   Fall (August, September, October) -3.40 [−6.92,0.13]
   Winter (November, December, January) -4.70 [−8.18, −1.23]
   Spring (February, March, April) −3.94 [−7.52, −0.36]
Province
   Capital [Reference] <0.001
   Hawally −0.41 [−5.00,4.19]
   Farawanya −11.80 [−16.48, −7.12]
   Jahra −14.96 [−19.62, −10.30]
   Mubarak al-Kabeer −1.38 [−6.54,3.78]
   Ahmadi 0.18 [−4.11,4.48]
Father’s education
   Intermediate school or below [Reference] <0.001
   Secondary (high-school) 1.90 [−1.92,5.73]
   Diploma 7.05 [2.99,11.12]
   University and above 13.31 [9.79,16.83]
Mother’s education
   Intermediate school or below [Reference] <0.001
   Secondary (high-school) 4.37 [0.11,8.62]
   Diploma 8.23 [3.98,12.49]
   University and above 14.09 [10.26,17.93]
Number of siblings
   ≤2 [Reference] <0.001
   3–4 −3.99 [−7.30, −0.69]
   ≥5 −8.86 [−12.15, −5.57]
Father income
   Less than 500 KD [Reference]
   500–1000 KD −2.66 [−8.17,2.84] <0.001
   1001–1500 KD −8.35 [−13.66, −3.05]
   1501–2000 KD −2.50 [−8.24,3.23]
   More than 2000KD 0.27 [−5.71,6.25]
   Do not wish to tell −4.16 [−10.22,1.89]
Mother employment 0.010
   Housewife [Reference]
   Paid employment 4.25 [1.50,7.01]
   Other 2.56 [−1.21,6.34]
Type of housing <0.001
   Rented flat [Reference]
   Rented house −15.48 [−19.54, −11.41]
   Owned flat −4.44 [−10.80,1.90]
   Owned house −7.70 [−10.37, −5.03]
Having private room at home
   Yes [Reference] 0.027
   No 2.83 [0.32,5.34]
Child birth order (Rank)
   First [Reference] 0.007
   Second −1.71 [−5.37,1.97]
   Third −1.21 [−5.06,2.63]
   Fourth −3.29 [−7.29,0.71]
   Fifth or above −6.76 [−10.51, −3.02]
Passive smoking at home (Cigarettes)
   No [Reference] <0.001
   Yes −4.98 [−7.59, −2.37]
Passive smoking at home (Shesha)
   No [Reference] 0.081
   Yes −4.69 [−9.97,0.58]
Frequency of consumption of breakfast prepared outside home
   Never or almost never [Reference] 0.014
   1–2 times per week −2.19 [−4.89,0.52]
   3–4 times per week −7.22 [−11.95, −2.48]
   5 or more times per week −4.46 [−9.91,0.99]
Frequency of consumption of lunch prepared outside home
   Never or almost never [Reference] 0.002
   1–2 times per week 4.15 [1.23,7.08]
   3–4 times per week 0.96 [−3.98,5.90]
5 or more times per week −4.52 [−10.46,1.43]
Frequency of consumption of dinner prepared outside home
   Never or almost never [Reference] 0.067
   1–2 times per week −1.46 [−5.59,2.66]
   3–4 times per week −5.19 [−10.00, −0.38]
   5 or more times per week −4.63 [−10.68,1.43]
Educational level (grade in the school) 0.032
   Six [Reference]
   Seventh 2.50 [−0.46,5.45]
   Eights 3.90 [0.86,6.93]
Family member help in study subjectsb
   No [Reference] <0.001
   Yes, regularly −11.90 [−15.46, −8.35]
   Yes, sometimes −3.04 [−6.41,0.33]
   Yes, rarely −2.80 [−7.90,2.29]
Having personal tutor to help in any study subject b
   No [Reference] <0.001
   Yes −6.76 [−9.27, −4.29]
Sleep during the weekdays (self-reported)
   First tertile (<7.5 hours) [Reference] 0.474
   Second tertile (7.5 to <9 hours) 1.90 [−1.16,4.96]
   Third tertile (≥ 9 hours) 0.86 [−2.17,3.90]
Sleeping nap during the day at weekdays
   No [Reference] 0.168
   Yes −1.75 [−4.24,0.74]
Sleep during the weekends (self-reported)
   First tertile (<9 hours) [Reference] 0.042
   Second tertile (9 to <11 hours) 0.35 [−2.92,3.62]
   Third tertile (≥ 11 hours) −3.08 [−6.45,0.28]
Sleeping nap during the day at weekends
   No [Reference] 0.003
   Yes −6.8 [−11.22, −2.39]
Having medical condition that limits physical activities (self-reported)
   No [Reference] 0.022
   Yes −4.54 [−8.43, −0.66]
Time of the first meal during weekdays
   Before 7 am [Reference] 0.033
   7–8 am 3.06 [−1.62,7.75]
   8–9 am 11.97 [3.43,20.51]
   9–10 am 1.74 [−1.06,4.55]
   After 10 am 4.30 [−1.30,9.89]
Time of the first meal during weekends
   Before 7 am [Reference] 0.100
   7–8 am 8.91 [−2.38,20.20]
   8–9 am 9.29 [−0.97,19.55]
   9–10 am 10.22 [0.47,19.96]
   After 10 am 6.87 [−2.62,16.37]
Consumptions of sugary drinks per week
   Zero time [Reference] <0.001
   One per week times −7.77 [−12.32, −3.22]
   Two per week times −6.36 [−10.72, −2.00]
   Three per week times −9.47 [−14.19, −4.76]
   Four to six times −11.64 [−16.63, −6.65]
   Seven times or more −15.28 [−19.68, −10.88]
Currently taking vitamins or supplements
   No [Reference] 0.671
   Yes −0.86 [−4.84,3.12]
Total time of physical activity
   First tertile (Low) [Reference] 0.903
   Second tertile (Middle) 0.20 [−2.87,3.27]
   Third tertile (High) 0.68 [−2.39,3.75]
Body mass index as per WHO growth Charts
   Obesity [Reference] 0.100
   Overweight 2.65 [−0.73,6.03]
   Normal 1.32 [−1.55,4.19]
   Severe thinness/Thinness −8.69 [−18.71,1.32]
Hemoglobin level (as per WHO cutoffs)
   Not anemic [Reference]
   Anemic −1.02 [−5.65,3.61] 0.665
Iron (umol/L) −0.01 [−0.240,0.22] 0.935
Ferritin (ng/mL) 0.01 [−0.06,0.08] 0.827
Transferrin (g/L) −0.10 [−3.44,3.24] 0.953
Transferrin saturation (%) −0.02 [−0.18,0.13] 0.753
Vitamin D (nmol/L) 0.03 [−0.02,0.08] 0.234
Parathyroid Hormone (pmol/L) −0.33 [−0.59, −0.07] 0.011
Vitamin B12 (pmol/L) −0.03 [−0.08,0.02] 0.280
Calcium (mmol/L) 12.88 [−0.50,26.27] 0.059
Lead (5 ug/dL) 0.04 [−0.16–0.24] 0.684

ap-values were generated by partial F-test. bThese factors were excluded from multivariate analysis because they were deemed to be a result of low cognitive functioning.

Table 3.

Factors associated with cognitive function (Raven’s Progressive Matrices scores) in multivariable analysis.

Variables β (95% CI) p-valuea
Gender
   Male [Reference] 0.002
   Female 3.92 [1.46,6.38]
Season of birth
   Summer (May, June, July) [Reference] 0.009
   Fall (August, September, October) −3.81 [−7.16, −0.46]
   Winter (November, December, January) −4.87 [−8.19, −1.56]
   Spring (February, March, April) −5.24 [−8.64, −1.84]
Province
   Capital [Reference] <0.001
   Hawally 0.76 [−3.73,5.26]
   Farawanya −9.97 [−14.61, −5.32]
   Jahra −6.07 [−11.08, −1.06]
   Mubarak al-Kabeer 1.69 [−3.38,6.77]
   Ahmadi −0.83 [−5.18,3.52]
Father’s education
   Intermediate school or below [Reference] <0.001
   Secondary (high-school) −0.18 [−4.01,3.65]
   Diploma 1.93 [−2.27,6.13]
   University and above 6.27 [2.42,10.12]
Mother’s education
   Intermediate school or below [Reference] 0.025
   Secondary (high-school) 1.89 [−2.45,6.22]
   Diploma 1.37 [−3.13,5.87]
   University and above 5.19 [0.96,9.43]
   Type of housing <0.001
   Rented flat [Reference]
   Rented house −7.93 [−12.22, −3.63]
   Owned flat −1.94 [−8.24,4.36]
   Owned house −4.64 [−7.42, −1.85]
   Cigarettes smoking at home
   No [Reference]
   Yes −2.79 [−5.34, −0.25] 0.031
Sleep hours during the weekends
First tertile (<9 hours) [Reference] 0.017
Second tertile (9 to<11 hours) 0.10 [−2.94,3.33]
Third tertile (≥ 11 hours) −3.49 [−6.76, −0.22]
Grade level
Six [Reference] <0.001
Seventh 3.17 [0.32,6.03]
Eights 5.60 [2.68,8.54]
Consumptions of soft drinks per week
Zero time [Reference] <0.001
One per week times −6.46 [−10.81, −2.10]
Two per week times −3.60 [−7.77,0.56]
Three per week times −6.24 [−10.81, −1.67]
Four to six times −5.93 [10.87, −1.00]
Seven times or more −9.92 [−14.30, −5.54]
Parathyroid Hormone (pmol/L) −0.23 [−0.47,0.01] 0.065

ap-values were generated by partial F-test.

Discussion

Poor cognitive functioning in children and adolescents is extremely important not only because it affects school performance and academic achievements but also predicts various disease conditions in adulthood25. In this study we aimed to investigate factors associated with cognitive functioning, as measured by a non-verbal test of intelligence (Raven’s SPM), among adolescents in a high-income setting in Kuwait. Socio-demographic factors, season of birth and consumption of soft sugary drinks were the main predictors of cognitive functioning while obesity and laboratory markers such as Hb, iron, ferritin, calcium and vitamin B12 were all not significantly associated with cognitive functioning among adolescents.

With the ever-increasing world-wide prevalence of childhood obesity, the link between obesity and cognitive functioning has generated great interest. Kuwait Nutritional Surveillance System reported that 22% and 32% of adolescents are either overweight or obese, respectively33. We found no significant association between obesity and cognitive functioning, neither in univariable nor in multivariable analysis. This is different from that reported in other studies, which showed a significant association between cognitive function and obesity21,3436. Other studies have suggested that obese adolescents have slower cognitive processing speed while maintaining equivalent performance on executive functions compared to their healthy weight peers37. On the other hand, several studies reported no association between obesity and cognitive functioning among children or adolescents38,39. The mixed findings of these studies could be explained by using different tests of cognitive function. Liang et al. reviewed studies on the association between obesity and cognitive function and concluded that the association is evident only with certain aspects of executive functions40 that are not precisely measured by SPM. This could also explain the lack of association in our study. Although some studies suggested that obese adolescents have lower grey and white matter volume in the brain, particularly in areas responsible for learning and cognition4143 suggesting a direct link between obesity and cognitive function, it is possible that an indirect association also exists, whereby obesity may affect cognitive functioning through poor sleep quality, decreased school attendance and social isolation23. Furthermore, it is difficult to untangle the exact impact of obesity on cognitive functioning. That is, whether it is obesity proper (fat deposition and physiological effects on the brain), obesity-related behaviors (behavior that promote weight gain such as poor diet and lack of physical activity), or both, that have an effect on cognitive function. In our study, no association was found between the total time adolescents engaged in physical activity and their cognitive function. Although most studies have suggested that increased physical activity may improve cognitive function44, a recent literature review showed that physical activity interventions can lead only to small improvements in problem-solving skills23.

We found a strong inverse association between consumption of sugary drinks and SPM scores in both univariable and multivariable analyses. Consumption of sugary drinks is an indicator of poor diet and previous studies have shown that a diet high in fats and sugar is associated with decreased intelligence and school performance45,46. We did not collect extensive data on diet, but consumption of meals prepared outside the home was significant predictor of cognitive function in univariable analysis, which highlights the importance of diet in our setting. We also found that excessive sleep (≥ 11 hours per night) during weekends (not weekdays) is associated with low SPM scores. Previous studies have reported either modest or no association between duration of sleep and cognitive functioning47,48 attributing this to subjective measures of sleep such as self-report methods or the notion that brain immaturity in children may prevent them from experiencing low cognitive performance due to sleep deprivation. A recent review49 of studies that objectively measured duration of sleep also reported a modest association, which existed only with Full/Verbal Intelligence Quotient (IQ) but not with Fluid IQ, memory or executive functions, processing speed or attention. We divided the duration of sleep into tertiles and found that sleeping 11 or more hours per night during weekends is associated with a decrease in SPM scores. Also, having a nap during the daytime on weekends was associated with decreased SPM scores in univariable analysis. Sleeping more than 11 hours per night or having a nap during the day on weekends may reflect poor quality of sleep during school days. However, if we consider the recommended duration of sleep for children aged 6–13 years (between 9 and 11 hours of sleep per night)50, approximately half of the participants slept between 9 and 11 hours per night on weekends. When we categorized the duration of sleep as per these recommendations, there was no association between cognitive functioning and duration of sleep on weekends or weekdays.

Previous studies have shown season of birth to be significantly associated with cognitive function, particularly non-verbal IQ51,52. Most of these studies were conducted in Europe or North America; and our study is the first to report a strong association between season of birth and cognitive function in Middle Eastern settings (Fig. 1). Several plausible mechanisms have been proposed to explain this association including variations in vitamin D levels during pregnancy, exposure to viral infections such as influenza in the winter season and poor prenatal nutrition in some seasons in addition to educational policies regarding school entry and sociocultural influences on timing of conception and birth51. Generally, none of these mechanisms has offered a satisfactory explanation for the robust association between season of birth and cognitive function. In our study, we did not measure vitamin D during pregnancy but the current vitamin D levels of the adolescents was not associated with their cognitive function24. In our setting, there is no clear pattern in birth distribution that is likely to be related to sociocultural factors as has been described elsewhere53. It is worth noting that season of birth has also been linked to various health outcomes and conditions including low birth weight52, schizophrenia54, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder55 and many others.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Raven’s Progressive Matrices scores by month of birth.

In low-income settings, it is estimated that many children do not reach their full cognitive potential due to poverty, insufficient nutrition and lack of access to health care. It is not clear what is the relative contribution of these factors in high-income settings such as in the oil-rich Arab states in the Middle East, where poverty has been eliminated, food is abundant and healthcare services are easily accessible. We found parental education to be significant independent predictor of cognitive functioning. Of note is the significant inverse association between father’s income and SPM score of adolescents in univariable analysis (Table 2). Although this was not selected in the final model, it shows that parental education is far more important than income in our setting. Our findings are substantially different from that reported in low-income countries such as Uganda, where maternal education was not associated with cognitive outcomes56. In that study, however, the authors adjusted for home environment which was related to maternal education. The mechanisms by which socioeconomic status affects cognitive abilities is complex and has been debated extensively. There is a growing body of literature which suggests that socioeconomic status modifies the heritability of cognitive function probably by epigenetic mechanisms, with early poverty having a greater impact on cognitive functions compared to poverty at middle or late-childhood22.

In our study, none of the laboratory measurements including Hb, iron, ferritin, transferrin, vitamin B12 or calcium were found to be significant predictors of cognitive functioning as measured by Raven’s SPM. Iron deficiency with or without anemia has been reported to be associated with low cognitive function, although studies have acknowledged the difficulty of disentangling the impact of iron deficiency from the effect of low socioeconomic background where iron deficiency or iron deficiency anemia prevail57,58. Most studies that showed an association between iron deficiency and cognitive function used electrophysiological measures of neurocognitive processing rather than neurocognitive performance such as cognitive tests. Similar to our findings, previous studies that used SPM as a measure of cognitive ability have reported no association between iron status and SPM scores59. Furthermore, it has been suggested that there is a critical period during early childhood (infants and preschool children) during which iron deficiency might have long-term effects on cognitive functions6062. Therefore, iron status during adolescence may not yield clear associations with cognitive functions. Finally, it is worth noting that only <8% of the participants were anemic as per WHO cutoff points (Hb < 12 g/dl for children 12–14.99 years, Hb<13 g/dl for males 15+ years and Hb<12 g/dl for females 15+ years).

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate factors associated with cognitive functioning among healthy adolescents in the oil-rich Arab states in the Middle East, where socioeconomic status has improved substantially in recent decades. We selected a nationally representative sample of adolescents and used Raven’s SPM, which is a nonverbal and culture fair intelligence test in order to minimize the influence of these factors. SPM is the most widely used nonverbal intelligence test; and has been reported to provide reliable data on cognitive functioning, even among children with disabilities63. In our study, SPM scores were highly correlated with students’ academic performance (Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient=0.45; p < 0.001 with mathematics, Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient=0.34; p < 0.001 with science, and Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient =0.43; p < 0.001 with the overall school performance), which boosted our confidence in the test. However, SPM is not the only measure of cognitive abilities. In fact, cognitive ability has several domains some of which are not precisely measured by Raven’s SPM such as verbal cognition, executive functioning and processing speed. Also, the study was cross-sectional making it difficult to identify the temporal relationship between several factors and cognitive functions. In our analysis, we used an exploratory approach with the aim of finding the strongest independent predictors of cognitive function regardless of their position in the causal pathway. Finally, we did not collect data on home environment indicators (parental interaction, provision of stimulation and early education) or on depression among adolescents.

In the oil-rich countries in the Middle East, where socioeconomic status has improved materially in the last few decades, parental education (but not income) in addition to season of birth and consumption of sugary drinks, were the main predictors of cognitive functioning as measured by the Raven’s SPM while obesity and laboratory markers such as Hb, iron, ferritin and calcium were all not significantly associated with cognitive functioning among adolescents. The link between cognitive functioning and season of birth seems to be robust in various geographical locations including the Middle East. The association between sugary drinks and cognitive functioning highlights the importance of diet independently of obesity and support efforts to reduce consumption of sugary drinks among children.

Acknowledgements

The authors acknowledge the assistance of Nadien Sameeh Rushdi and the team of data collectors. We also acknowledge the cooperation of all participating schools and the facilitation of the project by the Ministry of Education. The study was supported and funded by Kuwait University, Research Sector (Project No. WF 02/13).

Author contributions

R.S. contributed to the study design and data collection, and drafted the paper. A.T. contributed to the study design and data collection, analyzed the data and writing up of the paper. He had full access to all the data and took responsibility for the integrity of the data. A.R. contributed to the design of the study and data collection in addition to revising the manuscript. He was also responsible for the overall supervision of the project. L.S. contributed to the design of the study and data collection in addition to revising the manuscript with significant intellectual input. A.H. contributed to data collection and discussion of the findings in addition to revising the paper with significant intellectual input. O.M. contributed to the laboratory analysis, discussion of the findings and revising the paper with significant intellectual input.

Data availability

The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Starr JM, et al. Childhood mental ability and blood pressure at midlife: linking the Scottish Mental Survey 1932 and the Midspan studies. Journal of hypertension. 2004;22:893–897. doi: 10.1097/00004872-200405000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lawlor DA, Batty GD, Clark H, McIntyre S, Leon DA. Association of childhood intelligence with risk of coronary heart disease and stroke: findings from the Aberdeen Children of the 1950s cohort study. European journal of epidemiology. 2008;23:695–706. doi: 10.1007/s10654-008-9281-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Osler M, Lawlor DA, Nordentoft M. Cognitive function in childhood and early adulthood and hospital admission for schizophrenia and bipolar disorders in Danish men born in 1953. Schizophrenia research. 2007;92:132–141. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Cukic I, Brett CE, Calvin CM, Batty GD, Deary IJ. Childhood IQ and survival to 79: Follow-up of 94% of the Scottish Mental Survey 1947. Intelligence. 2017;63:45–50. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2017.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Whalley LJ, Deary IJ. Longitudinal cohort study of childhood IQ and survival up to age 76. BMJ (Clinical research ed.) 2001;322:819. doi: 10.1136/bmj.322.7290.819. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hart CL, et al. Childhood IQ, social class, deprivation, and their relationships with mortality and morbidity risk in later life: prospective observational study linking the Scottish Mental Survey 1932 and the Midspan studies. Psychosomatic medicine. 2003;65:877–883. doi: 10.1097/01.PSY.0000088584.82822.86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Batty GD, Deary IJ. Early life intelligence and adult health. BMJ (Clinical research ed.) 2004;329:585–586. doi: 10.1136/bmj.329.7466.585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Blakemore SJ, Choudhury S. Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47:296–312. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01611.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bosnyak E, et al. Predictors of Cognitive Functions in Children With Sturge-Weber Syndrome: A Longitudinal Study. Pediatric neurology. 2016;61:38–45. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2016.05.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Steinberg T, et al. Tic-related cognition, sensory phenomena, and anxiety in children and adolescents with Tourette syndrome. Comprehensive psychiatry. 2013;54:462–466. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kashala E, Elgen I, Sommerfelt K, Tylleskar T, Lundervold A. Cognition in African children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pediatric neurology. 2005;33:357–364. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2005.05.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Burkhardt L, Lobitz S, Koustenis E, Rueckriegel SM, Hernaiz Driever P. Cognitive and fine motor deficits in a pediatric sickle cell disease cohort of mixed ethnic origin. Annals of hematology. 2017;96:199–213. doi: 10.1007/s00277-016-2861-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Raafat N, et al. Assessment of cognitive function in children with beta-thalassemia major: a cross-sectional study. Journal of child neurology. 2015;30:417–422. doi: 10.1177/0883073814550827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Boyede GO, Lesi FE, Ezeaka VC, Umeh CS. Impact of sociodemographic factors on cognitive function in school-aged HIV-infected Nigerian children. HIV/AIDS (Auckland, N.Z.) 2013;5:145–152. doi: 10.2147/hiv.s43260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.van Wyhe KS, van de Water T, Boivin MJ, Cotton MF, Thomas KG. Cross-cultural assessment of HIV-associated cognitive impairment using the Kaufman assessment battery for children: a systematic review. Journal of the International AIDS Society. 2017;20:21412. doi: 10.7448/ias.20.1.21412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Reilly C, et al. Cognition in school-aged children with “active” epilepsy: A population-based study. Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology. 2015;37:429–438. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2015.1024103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Li W, Huang E, Gao S. Type 1 Diabetes Mellitus and Cognitive Impairments: A Systematic Review. Journal of Alzheimer’s disease: JAD. 2017;57:29–36. doi: 10.3233/jad-161250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Broadley MM, White MJ, Andrew B. A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis of Executive Function Performance in Type 1 Diabetes Mellitus. Psychosomatic medicine. 2017;79:684–696. doi: 10.1097/psy.0000000000000460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Walker SP, et al. Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet (London, England) 2007;369:145–157. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(07)60076-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Perignon M, et al. Stunting, poor iron status and parasite infection are significant risk factors for lower cognitive performance in Cambodian school-aged children. PloS one. 2014;9:e112605. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0112605. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Vondrova D, et al. The impact of selected environmental, behavioral and psychosocial factors on schoolchildren’s somatic and mental health. Reviews on environmental health. 2017;32:189–192. doi: 10.1515/reveh-2016-0034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hackman DA, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2009;13:65–73. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Martin A, et al. Physical activity, diet and other behavioural interventions for improving cognition and school achievement in children and adolescents with obesity or overweight. The Cochrane database of systematic reviews. 2018;1:CD009728. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD009728.pub3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Rahman, A. et al. Plasma 25-Hydroxy Vitamin D Is Not Associated with Either Cognitive Function or Academic Performance in Adolescents. Nutrients10, 10.3390/nu10091197 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 25.Al-Taiar A, Rahman A, Al-Sabah R, Shaban L, Al-Harbi A. Vitamin D status among adolescents in Kuwait: a cross-sectional study. BMJ open. 2018;8:e021401. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2017-021401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.UEA. Youth Physical Activity Questionnaire, http://epi-meta.medschl.cam.ac.uk/includes/speedy/pdf/PhysicalActivity%20V4%20SPEEDY-3.pdf (2011).
  • 27.Al-Hazzaa HM, Al-Sobayel HI, Musaiger AO. Convergent validity of the Arab Teens Lifestyle Study (ATLS) physical activity questionnaire. International journal of environmental research and public health. 2011;8:3810–3820. doi: 10.3390/ijerph8093810. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Raven J. The Raven’s progressive matrices: change and stability over culture and time. Cognitive psychology. 2000;41:1–48. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Abdel-Khalek AM. Reliability and factorial validity of the standard progressive matrices among Kuwaiti children ages 8 to 15 years. Perceptual and motor skills. 2005;101:409–412. doi: 10.2466/pms.101.2.409-412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Raven, J. (Pearson Education, 2000).
  • 31.Xu H, et al. Migration and cognitive function: a conceptual framework for Global Health Research. Glob Health Res Policy. 2018;3:34. doi: 10.1186/s41256-018-0088-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Santos DN, et al. Determinants of cognitive function in childhood: a cohort study in a middle income context. BMC public health. 2008;8:202. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-8-202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.KNSS. Kuwait Nutrition Surveillance System: 2016 Annual Report.. (Ministry of Health., Kuwait, 2016).
  • 34.Li Y, Dai Q, Jackson JC, Zhang J. Overweight is associated with decreased cognitive functioning among school-age children and adolescents. Obesity (Silver Spring, Md.) 2008;16:1809–1815. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Davis CL, Cooper S. Fitness, fatness, cognition, behavior, and academic achievement among overweight children: do cross-sectional associations correspond to exercise trial outcomes? Preventive medicine. 2011;52(Suppl 1):S65–69. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2011.01.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Delgado-Rico E, Rio-Valle JS, Gonzalez-Jimenez E, Campoy C, Verdejo-Garcia A. BMI predicts emotion-driven impulsivity and cognitive inflexibility in adolescents with excess weight. Obesity (Silver Spring, Md.) 2012;20:1604–1610. doi: 10.1038/oby.2012.47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Sweat V, Yates KF, Migliaccio R, Convit A. Obese Adolescents Show Reduced Cognitive Processing Speed Compared with Healthy Weight Peers. Childhood obesity (Print) 2017;13:190–196. doi: 10.1089/chi.2016.0255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Mond JM, Stich H, Hay PJ, Kraemer A, Baune BT. Associations between obesity and developmental functioning in pre-school children: a population-based study. International journal of obesity (2005) 2007;31:1068–1073. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Gunstad J, et al. Body mass index and neuropsychological function in healthy children and adolescents. Appetite. 2008;50:246–251. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Liang J, Matheson BE, Kaye WH, Boutelle KN. Neurocognitive correlates of obesity and obesity-related behaviors in children and adolescents. International journal of obesity (2005) 2014;38:494–506. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Alarcon G, Ray S, Nagel BJ. Lower Working Memory Performance in Overweight and Obese Adolescents Is Mediated by White Matter Microstructure. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS. 2016;22:281–292. doi: 10.1017/s1355617715001265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Alosco ML, et al. Body mass index and brain structure in healthy children and adolescents. The International journal of neuroscience. 2014;124:49–55. doi: 10.3109/00207454.2013.817408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Kennedy JT, Collins PF, Luciana M. Higher Adolescent Body Mass Index Is Associated with Lower Regional Gray and White Matter Volumes and Lower Levels of Positive Emotionality. Frontiers in neuroscience. 2016;10:413. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Donnelly JE, et al. Physical Activity, Fitness, Cognitive Function, and Academic Achievement in Children: A Systematic Review. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 2016;48:1197–1222. doi: 10.1249/mss.0000000000000901. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Northstone K, Joinson C, Emmett P, Ness A, Paus T. Are dietary patterns in childhood associated with IQ at 8 years of age? A population-based cohort study. Journal of epidemiology and community health. 2012;66:624–628. doi: 10.1136/jech.2010.111955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Feinstein L, et al. Dietary patterns related to attainment in school: the importance of early eating patterns. Journal of epidemiology and community health. 2008;62:734–739. doi: 10.1136/jech.2007.068213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Astill RG, Van der Heijden KB, Van Ijzendoorn MH, Van Someren EJ. Sleep, cognition, and behavioral problems in school-age children: a century of research meta-analyzed. Psychological bulletin. 2012;138:1109–1138. doi: 10.1037/a0028204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.de Bruin EJ, van Run C, Staaks J, Meijer AM. Effects of sleep manipulation on cognitive functioning of adolescents: A systematic review. Sleep medicine reviews. 2017;32:45–57. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2016.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Short MA, et al. Cognition and objectively measured sleep duration in children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep health. 2018;4:292–300. doi: 10.1016/j.sleh.2018.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Paruthi S, et al. Consensus Statement of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine on the Recommended Amount of Sleep for Healthy Children: Methodology and Discussion. Journal of clinical sleep medicine: JCSM: official publication of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine. 2016;12:1549–1561. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.6288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Grootendorst-van Mil NH, et al. Brighter children? The association between seasonality of birth and child IQ in a population-based birth cohort. BMJ open. 2017;7:e012406. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-012406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.McGrath JJ, Saha S, Lieberman DE, Buka S. Season of birth is associated with anthropometric and neurocognitive outcomes during infancy and childhood in a general population birth cohort. Schizophrenia research. 2006;81:91–100. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.07.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Roenneberg T, Aschoff J. Annual rhythm of human reproduction: I. Biology, sociology, or both? Journal of biological rhythms. 1990;5:195–216. doi: 10.1177/074873049000500303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Davies G, Welham J, Chant D, Torrey EF, McGrath J. A systematic review and meta-analysis of Northern Hemisphere season of birth studies in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia bulletin. 2003;29:587–593. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Zhang C, Brook JS, Leukefeld CG, Rosa M, Brook DW. Season of birth: A predictor of ADHD symptoms in early midlife. Psychiatry research. 2018;267:243–248. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.05.056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Bangirana P, et al. Socioeconomic predictors of cognition in Ugandan children: implications for community interventions. PloS one. 2009;4:e7898. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0007898. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Grantham-McGregor, S. & Ani, C. A review of studies on the effect of iron deficiency on cognitive development in children. The Journal of nutrition131, 649S–666S; discussion 666S–668S, 10.1093/jn/131.2.649S (2001). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 58.Jauregui-Lobera I. Iron deficiency and cognitive functions. Neuropsychiatric disease and treatment. 2014;10:2087–2095. doi: 10.2147/ndt.s72491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Dissanayake DS, Kumarasiri PV, Nugegoda DB, Dissanayake DM. The association of iron status with educational performance and intelligence among adolescents. The Ceylon medical journal. 2009;54:75–79. doi: 10.4038/cmj.v54i3.1199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Armony-Sivan R, Kaplan-Estrin M, Jacobson SW, Lozoff B. Iron-deficiency anemia in infancy and mother-infant interaction during feeding. Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics: JDBP. 2010;31:326–332. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181dc525d. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Lozoff, B. et al. Long-lasting neural and behavioral effects of iron deficiency in infancy. Nutrition reviews64, S34–43; discussion S72–91 (2006). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 62.Yehuda, S. & Yehuda, M. Long lasting effects of infancy iron deficiency–preliminary results. Journal of neural transmission. Supplementum, 197–200 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 63.Ballester-Plane J, et al. Measuring intellectual ability in cerebral palsy: The comparison of three tests and their neuroimaging correlates. Research in developmental disabilities. 2016;56:83–98. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2016.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES