Abstract
Social cognition has become a topic of widespread interest in experimental and treatment research in schizophrenia over the past 15 years. This explosion of interest largely reflects the robust evidence that social cognition is among the strongest known correlates of poor community functioning throughout the course of schizophrenia. While progress has been impressive, we consider several fundamental questions about the scope, structure, and optimal measurement of social cognition that remain unanswered and point to the need for continued method development. We also consider more recently emerging questions about individual differences, ecological and cross-cultural validity, and intervention approaches, as well as broader technological changes that impact how we understand and use social cognition at a societal level. Continued efforts to creatively grapple with the complexities and challenges the field now faces hold great promise for helping us understand and more effectively treat a major source of functional disability in schizophrenia.
Keywords: schizophrenia, neuropsychology, cognition, social functioning
Introduction
Social cognition as a topic of interest in schizophrenia research gained momentum after two National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH)-sponsored workshops in the mid-2000s.1,2 Both workshops recommended agendas for future research. An early short-term goal was to achieve agreement on definitions and domains of social cognition,1 accomplished with the second workshop.2 Other proposed research goals included investigation of psychometrics and the refinement of existing measures, describing the social cognitive impairments in schizophrenia (timing, associations with symptoms and functioning, and comparisons with other mental disorders), examining the factor structure of social cognition, translational studies to identify neural substrates, and treatment development.1,2
Since then, the field has witnessed an enormous rise in scientific interest. Entering the search term “social cognition AND schizo*” in PubMed gives 38 hits for the year 2000 and 341 for the year 2018. This In Translation article briefly takes stock of the impressive accomplishments of social cognitive research in schizophrenia over the past 15 years, describes some key issues and challenges that this maturing field currently faces, and looks forward to the next generation of research in this vibrant area.
What We Know
Over the past 15 years, our understanding of social cognitive impairments in schizophrenia, including their magnitude and course, functional significance, and amenability to intervention, has dramatically expanded in several ways. First, it is now clear that people with schizophrenia demonstrate substantial impairments in three areas of social cognition defined by the NIMH workgroup.2 Dozens of studies show large impairments on performance measures of emotion perception/processing (d = 0.89) and mentalizing (d = 0.96), while a much smaller number of studies show a large impairment in social perception (n = 12; d = 1.04). A fourth area, attributional style, was also recognized by the workgroup but, as discussed below, impairment is not consistently found in this area.3 Neuroimaging studies show corresponding patterns of abnormal regional activation and connectivity during the performance of these social cognitive tasks in psychosis.4 More recent studies suggest that the impairments extend to many other social cognitive domains, such as empathic accuracy and self-referential processing.5,6
Second, converging evidence indicates that emotion perception/processing, mentalizing, and social perception impairments are trait-like features of schizophrenia. The impairments seen in chronically ill individuals are present at similar levels during the early postonset phase, are longitudinally stable, are not secondary to factors such as medication effects, and are detectable at attenuated levels in unaffected biological relatives.7 There is growing evidence that social cognitive impairments are present in prodromal individuals at heightened risk for psychosis and are possible predictors of conversion to psychosis.8,9
Third, social cognition is a key correlate and predictor of functional outcome. Similar to the well-established link between nonsocial cognitive deficits and functioning, social cognitive impairments consistently relate to how well-affected individuals function in their daily lives both cross-sectionally and longitudinally.10,11 In fact, a meta-analysis reported that social cognition shows an even stronger link to functioning, accounting for about 16% of the variance in community functioning compared to 6% for nonsocial cognition.12 Along these lines, there is now compelling evidence that social cognition mediates the relationship between nonsocial cognition and functional outcome, with approximately 25% of the variance in the functional outcome being explained by such multifactorial models.13
Fourth, social cognitive impairments detected on performance measures are amenable to treatment. A recent meta-analysis of 16 studies using social cognitive training programs14 reported medium-to-large improvements in the two most commonly assessed domains, facial affect identification (d = 0.84 in 12 studies) and mentalizing (d = 0.70 in 13 studies). Notably, the social cognitive treatment-related gains are not accompanied by improvements in nonsocial cognition14 and, alternatively, nonsocial cognitive remediation does not result in significant social cognitive improvements.15 These findings converge to suggest that social and nonsocial cognition are distinct treatment targets that require different types of interventions. Beyond improvements in social cognitive task performance, preliminary evidence indicates that these interventions produce detectable structural and functional brain changes.16
Current Challenges
Despite the rapid expansion of social cognition research in schizophrenia, some of the basic issues raised in the NIMH workgroups 15 years ago remain unresolved. Additionally, as in any maturing area of research, new issues and complexities have emerged. We focus on four unresolved basic issues and four more recently emerging questions.
Basic Issues
One unresolved basic issue concerns the scope and profile of social cognitive impairments within schizophrenia. Although we highlighted several areas of impairment, not all aspects of social cognition appear to be markedly impacted. For example, affective empathy, ie, relatively automatic experience sharing or “simulation” processes through which observed actions and social/emotional cues trigger a shared neural response in the observer,17–19 may be relatively intact. It has been proposed that the mirror neuron system is centrally involved in the affective component of empathy.20,21 A number of studies have found normal neural responses in schizophrenia across experience-sharing tasks that range from observing simple motor actions to more complex social-emotional behaviors.22–26 As another example, as a group, individuals with schizophrenia do not demonstrate consistent impairment in the area of attributional bias (particularly hostility bias),3,27 one of the main areas defined in the original NIMH workshops. Pronounced attributional biases may be more specifically linked to experiencing paranoid beliefs.
Second, it remains unclear if there is a distinctive social cognitive profile in schizophrenia compared to other neuropsychiatric conditions. It is now well documented that social cognitive impairments are present across dozens of psychiatric, neurological, and developmental,28 as well as genetic disorders.29 Direct comparisons between schizophrenia and other disorders thus far provide a mixed picture. For example, although impairments consistently appear to be more severe in schizophrenia than bipolar disorder, the magnitude of the group differences are modest.30 Further, meta-analytic evidence indicates that schizophrenia and autism do not differ for mentalizing but that autism has larger impairments for emotion perception.31 However, recent direct comparisons using validated measures found similar social cognitive impairments across these disorders,32 though the similar performance deficits may be associated with different neural mechanisms33 and/or error types, such as overmentalizing or undermentalizing.34
Third, the basic structure of social cognition in schizophrenia remains unsettled. Factor analytic studies of social cognitive measures have been plagued by a lack of replicability, yielding one, two, three, or even four factors.35 It remains unclear to what extent the number of factors identified reflects methodological issues (eg, the specific tasks included, the frequent use of a narrow range of social cognitive tasks, and variability in sample characteristics) vs the actual structure of social cognition. Most larger studies using measures from multiple social cognitive domains report multifactorial, rather than unitary, solutions but more large-scale studies that include measures from all of the theoretically proposed domains are needed to resolve this structural issue.
Fourth, most available social cognitive measures have substantial limitations for use in schizophrenia. Many commonly used measures were borrowed from other fields (eg, childhood neuropsychiatric disorders) and their validity and psychometric properties (eg, floor/ceiling effects and test–retest reliability) in schizophrenia are often inadequate or simply unknown. An impressive effort to address this measurement issue was the Social Cognition Psychometric Evaluation (SCOPE) study,36,37 which identified existing tests worthy of comprehensive evaluation, either “as is” or with modifications, for potential use as clinical trial endpoints. Of eight candidate measures, only two emotion processing tasks and one mentalizing task showed acceptable properties for clinical trial use though; even these measures are not without limitations. Further development was recommended for other tasks. Another initiative, the Social Cognition and Functioning in Schizophrenia (SCAF) study,38 sought to create new measures by adapting paradigms from social neuroscience for use in schizophrenia clinical trials. Of the four adapted paradigms examined, only one, an empathic accuracy task, showed acceptable properties,39,40 while the others require further development.
Overall, these unresolved basic questions have important practical consequences. The suboptimal test characteristics and unknown factor structure make it difficult to provide guidance for researchers or clinicians interested in selecting a battery of tests to adequately measure “social cognition” in schizophrenia. The paucity of measures with adequate properties for clinical trial use also hinders efforts to convincingly demonstrate the efficacy of new interventions. Although the SCOPE and SCAF initiatives demonstrate the substantial challenge of establishing measures that are suitable for use in schizophrenia, the development of new measures is a pressing need.
New Challenges
As the research base has rapidly accumulated over the past 15 years, several new challenges have arisen. Further, there have been major technological advances throughout the world that directly impact how we communicate with each other and use social cognition in daily life. Looking forward to the next 15 years of research, we conclude by highlighting four questions that have more recently emerged.
How Should We Deal With the Heterogeneity of Social Cognition in Psychosis?
It has become clear that there is considerable variability in social cognitive task performance among those with schizophrenia. One approach to addressing heterogeneity focuses on identifying subgroups with distinct profiles within schizophrenia.41,42 For example, some studies have reported on a subgroup seemingly without social cognitive impairments.43,44 Notably, in the area of nonsocial cognition, schizophrenia researchers have similarly debated for years about the possible existence of a “neuropsychologically normal” subgroup. At this stage, a number of studies using various designs suggest that virtually all individuals with schizophrenia, including those with superior intelligence,45 experience some degree of cognitive deterioration. For example, 98% of people with schizophrenia fail to reach the nonsocial cognitive level expected from estimated premorbid intellectual function or maternal education.46 This issue is much more challenging to address for social cognition because we do not have well-established indices of expected social cognitive ability, and the clinical value of subgrouping remains to be seen. In light of the considerable variability in social cognition among those with schizophrenia, it would be desirable to develop assessment batteries that can identify profiles of both weaknesses and strengths at the individual level. Such batteries could guide interventions that capitalize on an individual’s relative social cognitive strengths.
Another approach to heterogeneity focuses on conceptualizing social cognitive processes as broad transdiagnostic constructs. The NIHM workgroups occurred in a largely pre-Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) world. The RDoC initiative was intended to inspire researchers to look beyond conventional diagnostic categories and, instead, focus on integrating behavioral and biological components of basic dimensional constructs that span the full range of normal to abnormal functioning. Several social cognitive constructs are included within the social processes domain of the RDoC matrix.47 At this stage, only a few studies have examined social cognition from an RDoC perspective across the psychosis spectrum. While early results, primarily in the area of facial emotion processing48–50 show promise, the value of this approach remains to be determined.
How Should We Address the Ecological and Cross-Cultural Validity of Social Cognitive Measures?
Linking performance on social cognition tasks not to biology, but to real-life, is at the core of ecological validity. Despite the dynamic, interactive, and rapidly fluctuating nature of social cognition in daily life, most commonly used measures in schizophrenia, including those endorsed in the SCOPE project, involve viewing and responding to static stimuli (eg, labeling emotions in faces or eyes) or interpreting written social vignettes (eg, making inferences about a person’s true intention). These types of tasks have been criticized in schizophrenia research and beyond for their limited ability to capture social cognitive processes as they typically unfold in daily life.51,52 In addition to developing laboratory tasks using more representative dynamic stimuli,53 alternative methods, such as experience sampling and virtual reality are emerging in an effort to enhance ecological validity.54 Further, the global availability of smartphones opens up the possibility of new digital phenotyping metrics (eg, calls/texting and social media use) of social cognition and behavior.55,56
A related concern is the cross-cultural validity of social cognitive tasks. Research has been dominated by work in Western cultures, especially the United States, and remarkably few studies have conducted direct cross-cultural comparisons. Although numerous social cognitive tasks have been translated for use in other countries, it is quite possible that they measure somewhat different constructs when administered in different languages or contexts with different social norms. Several studies have found an impact of race, culture, and social class on social cognition.57–60 It is critical that we carefully attend to potential cultural biases when interpreting task scores and developing new measures.61
How Can We Develop More Effective Social Cognitive Interventions?
As treatment research has matured, early enthusiasm about training programs to improve social cognition and community functioning has been tempered. For example, a closer look at training interventions for social cognition reveals substantial methodological limitations (eg, frequent use of small sample sizes, nonexperimental designs, and nonactive control conditions62). Further, recent studies using larger samples and more rigorous designs have often shown notably weaker effects on social cognitive task performance and there is currently not much compelling evidence of generalization to meaningful improvements in functioning.63,64 Thus, the complexities of improving targets as complex as social cognition have come to the fore. To enhance the impact and generalizability of social cognitive training, initial creative efforts have attempted to incorporate new technologies (eg, virtual-reality-based exercises65,66) and implement novel bridging activities conducted outside of the clinic. Fully computerized interventions that can be independently completed using portable electronic devices have also been developed. While questions may be raised about the viability of these approaches—eg, can interacting with a computer training program truly generalize to improvements in real-world social interactions?—initial results have been encouraging.67,68
The picture has been similar for pharmacological treatment approaches, which has been dominated by a focus on oxytocin. Following a proliferation of single and repeated dose studies that built on early excitement about the benefits of oxytocin, enthusiasm has been tempered considerably by research literature in schizophrenia that is now decidedly mixed.69,70 Again, the complexities of oxytocin have come to fore, with current research focused on more fine-grained questions, such as identifying optimal social cognitive treatment targets, dosing, and administration parameters (eg, in combination with psychosocial treatment).71,72 Looking forward, we also anticipate an expansion of interest beyond oxytocin to other molecules as basic research into the pharmacology of social cognition continues to grow.73–77 There is also emerging interest in brain stimulation approaches to enhancing social cognition in schizophrenia, though findings thus far have been mixed.78–80
How Should We Investigate Social Cognition in an Increasingly Digital Society?
Human communication has changed markedly since the mid-2000s. Personal smartphones are now extremely common across the globe and an increasingly large quantity of our social interactions occur online and through various forms of social media. As a consequence, the means through which we communicate and use our social cognitive abilities are dramatically shifting. As noted above, these technological advances open up exciting new vistas for assessing social cognition through digital phenotyping or delivering digital interventions.
In line with our increasingly computer-mediated interpersonal communication and relationships, the social cognitive skills that are most important in our daily lives are also rapidly shifting.81–83 For example, when we post on Facebook or Instagram, we consider how others at widely varying levels of closeness to us will react. When interpreting others’ intentions and beliefs from brief posts, emojis, or “likes”/absence of “likes,” we have far fewer social cues to draw upon than if we were directly interacting. We consider issues such as whether it is socially appropriate to use emojis or abbreviations (“tmi,” “btw,” and “lmk”) with, eg, a friend, an online friend, an acquaintance, or a supervisor. These new forms of communication can be just as important for adaptive functioning as face-to-face interactions, and the sociocultural norms that guide them are evolving. We anticipate an expansion of attention to digital social cognition in schizophrenia research in the next 15 years. This is particularly important for younger individuals, including those at risk for or with a relatively recent onset of psychosis because their generation has not experienced a world without digital technology and social media.
Acknowledgments
W.P.H. is a full-time employee of VeraSci Inc. A.V. reports no conflict of interest.
Funding
This work was supported by the Southern and Eastern Norway Regional Health Authority (grant number 2017069 to A.V.).
References
- 1. Green MF, Olivier B, Crawley JN, Penn DL, Silverstein S. Social cognition in schizophrenia: Recommendations from the MATRICS New Approaches Conference. Schizophr Bull. 2005;31:882–887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Green MF, Penn DL, Bentall R, et al. . Social cognition in schizophrenia: an NIMH workshop on definitions, assessment, and research opportunities. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34(6):1211–1220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Savla GN, Vella L, Armstrong CC, Penn DL, Twamley EW. Deficits in domains of social cognition in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of the emprical literature. Schizophr Bull. 2013;39:979–992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Green MF, Horan WP, Lee J. Social cognition in schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16(10):620–631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. van Donkersgoed RJM, de Jong S, Aan Het Rot M, et al. . Measuring empathy in schizophrenia: the Empathic Accuracy Task and its correlation with other empathy measures. Schizophr Res. 2019;208:153–159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Potvin S, Gamache L, Lungu O. A functional neuroimaging meta-analysis of self-related processing in schizophrenia. Front Neurol. 2019;10:990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. McCleery A, Horan WP, Green MF. Social cognition during the early phase of schizophrenia. In: Lysaker P, Dimaggio G, Brune M, eds. Metacognition and Social Cognition in Schizophrenia. London, UK: Elsevier Academic Press; 2014:49–57. [Google Scholar]
- 8. Lee TY, Hong SB, Shin NY, Kwon JS. Social cognitive functioning in prodromal psychosis: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2015;164(1-3):28–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. van Donkersgoed RJ, Wunderink L, Nieboer R, Aleman A, Pijnenborg GH. Social cognition in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis: a meta-analysis. PLoS One. 2015;10(10):e0141075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Horan WP, Green MF, DeGroot M, et al. . Social cognition in schizophrenia, part 2: 12-month stability and prediction of functional outcome in first-episode patients. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38(4):865–872. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. McCleery A, Lee J, Fiske AP, et al. . Longitudinal stability of social cognition in schizophrenia: a 5-year follow-up of social perception and emotion processing. Schizophr Res. 2016;176(2-3):467–472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Fett AK, Viechtbauer W, Dominguez MD, Penn DL, van Os J, Krabbendam L. The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35(3):573–588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Schmidt SJ, Mueller DR, Roder V. Social cognition as a mediator variable between neurocognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: empirical review and new results by structural equation modeling. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37(suppl 2):S41–S54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Kurtz MM, Gagen E, Rocha NB, Machado S, Penn DL. Comprehensive treatments for social cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: a critical review and effect-size analysis of controlled studies. Clin Psychol Rev. 2016;43:80–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Prikken M, Konings MJ, Lei WU, Begemann MJH, Sommer IEC. The efficacy of computerized cognitive drill and practice training for patients with a schizophrenia-spectrum disorder: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2019;204:368–374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Campos C, Santos S, Gagen E, et al. . Neuroplastic changes following social cognition training in schizophrenia: a systematic review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2016;26(3):310–328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Singer T, Lamm C. The social neuroscience of empathy. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2009;1156:81–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Kanske P, Böckler A, Singer T. Models, mechanisms and moderators dissociating empathy and theory of mind. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2017;30:193–206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Zaki J, Ochsner KN, Ochsner K. The neuroscience of empathy: progress, pitfalls and promise. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15(5):675–680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Iacoboni M. Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60:653–670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Rizzolatti G, Craighero L. The mirror-neuron system. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2004;27:169–192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Andrews SC, Enticott PG, Hoy KE, Thomson RH, Fitzgerald PB. No evidence for mirror system dysfunction in schizophrenia from a multimodal TMS/EEG study. Psychiatry Res. 2015;228(3):431–440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Simonsen A, Fusaroli R, Skewes JC, et al. . Enhanced automatic action imitation and intact imitation-inhibition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2019;45(1):87–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Horan WP, Jimenez AM, Lee J, Wynn JK, Eisenberger NI, Green MF. Pain empathy in schizophrenia: an fMRI study. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2016;11(5):783–792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Horan WP, Iacoboni M, Cross KA, et al. . Self-reported empathy and neural activity during action imitation and observation in schizophrenia. Neuroimage Clin. 2014;5:100–108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Horan WP, Pineda JA, Wynn JK, Iacoboni M, Green MF. Some markers of mirroring appear intact in schizophrenia: evidence from mu suppression. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2014;14(3):1049–1060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Healey KM, Bartholomeusz CF, Penn DL. Deficits in social cognition in first episode psychosis: a review of the literature. Clin Psychol Rev. 2016;50:108–137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Cotter J, Granger K, Backx R, Hobbs M, Looi CY, Barnett JH. Social cognitive dysfunction as a clinical marker: a systematic review of meta-analyses across 30 clinical conditions. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;84:92–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Abu-Akel A, Shamay-Tsoory S. Neuroanatomical and neurochemical bases of theory of mind. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(11):2971–2984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Bora E, Pantelis C. Social cognition in schizophrenia in comparison to bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2016;175(1–3):72–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Fernandes JM, Cajão R, Lopes R, Jerónimo R, Barahona-Corrêa JB. Social cognition in schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis of direct comparisons. Front Psychiatry. 2018;9:504. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Pinkham AE, Morrison KE, Penn DL, et al. . Comprehensive comparison of social cognitive performance in autism spectrum disorder and schizophrenia [published online ahead of print Oct 2 2019]. Psychol Med. Oct 2 2019:1–9. doi:10.1017/S0033291719002708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Eack SM, Wojtalik JA, Keshavan MS, Minshew NJ. Social-cognitive brain function and connectivity during visual perspective-taking in autism and schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2017;183:102–109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Vaskinn A, Abu-Akel A. The interactive effect of autism and psychosis severity on theory of mind and functioning in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology. 2019;33(2):195–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Etchepare A, Prouteau A. Toward a two-dimensional model of social cognition in clinical neuropsychology: a systematic review of factor structure studies. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2018;24(4):391–404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Pinkham AE, Harvey PD, Penn DL. Social cognition psychometric evaluation: results of the final validation study. Schizophr Bull. 2018;44(4):737–748. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Green MF, Buck B, Healey K, Harvey PD. The social cognition psychometric evaluation study: results of the expert survey and RAND panel. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40(4):813–823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Green MF, Penn DL. Going from social neuroscience to schizophrenia clinical trials. Schizophr Bull. 2013;39(6):1189–1191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Kern RS, Penn DL, Lee J, et al. . Adapting social neuroscience measures for schizophrenia clinical trials, part 2: trolling the depths of psychometric properties. Schizophr Bull. 2013;39(6):1201–1210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Olbert CM, Penn DL, Kern RS, et al. . Adapting social neuroscience measures for schizophrenia clinical trials, part 3: fathoming external validity. Schizophr Bull. 2013;39(6):1211–1218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Etchepare A, Roux S, Destaillats JM, et al. . What are the specificities of social cognition in schizophrenia? A cluster-analytic study comparing schizophrenia with the general population. Psychiatry Res. 2019;272:369–379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Bell MD, Corbera S, Johannesen JK, Fiszdon JM, Wexler BE. Social cognitive impairments and negative symptoms in schizophrenia: are there subtypes with distinct functional correlates? Schizophr Bull. 2013;39(1):186–196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Hajdúk M, Harvey PD, Penn DL, Pinkham AE. Social cognitive impairments in individuals with schizophrenia vary in severity. J Psychiatr Res. 2018;104:65–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Rocca P, Galderisi S, Rossi A, et al. ; Italian Network for Research on Psychoses Social cognition in people with schizophrenia: a cluster-analytic approach. Psychol Med. 2016;46(13):2717–2729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Vaskinn A, Ueland T, Melle I, Agartz I, Andreassen OA, Sundet K. Neurocognitive decrements are present in intellectually superior schizophrenia. Front Psychiatry. 2014;5:45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Keefe RS, Eesley CE, Poe MP. Defining a cognitive function decrement in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(6):688–691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Gur RC, Gur RE. Social cognition as an RDoC domain. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2016;171B(1):132–141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Ruocco AC, Reilly JL, Rubin LH, et al. . Emotion recognition deficits in schizophrenia-spectrum disorders and psychotic bipolar disorder: findings from the Bipolar-Schizophrenia Network on Intermediate Phenotypes (B-SNIP) study. Schizophr Res. 2014;158(1–3):105–112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Sabharwal A, Kotov R, Szekely A, Leung HC, Barch DM, Mohanty A. Neural markers of emotional face perception across psychotic disorders and general population. J Abnorm Psychol. 2017;126(5):663–678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Gur RE, Moore TM, Calkins ME, Ruparel K, Gur RC. Face processing measures of social cognition: a dimensional approach to developmental psychopathology. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2017;2(6):502–509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Zaki J, Ochsner K. The need for a cognitive neuroscience of naturalistic social cognition. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2009;1167:16–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Hermans K, Achterof R, Min-Germeys I, Kasanova Z, Kirtley O, Schneider M. Improving ecological validity in research on social cognition. In: Lewandowski KE, Moustafa AA, eds. Social Cognition in Psychosis: London, UK: Elsevier Academic Press; 2019:249–268. [Google Scholar]
- 53. Lee J, Zaki J, Harvey PO, Ochsner K, Green MF. Schizophrenia patients are impaired in empathic accuracy. Psychol Med. 2011;41:2297–2304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Myin-Germeys I, Kasanova Z, Vaessen T, et al. . Experience sampling methodology in mental health research: new insights and technical developments. World Psychiatry. 2018;17(2):123–132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Insel TR. Digital phenotyping: technology for a new science of behavior. JAMA. 2017;318(13):1215–1216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Torous J, Staples P, Barnett I, Sandoval LR, Keshavan M, Onnela JP. Characterizing the clinical relevance of digital phenotyping data quality with applications to a cohort with schizophrenia. NPJ Digit Med. 2018;1:15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Pinkham AE, Sasson NJ, Calkins ME, et al. . The other-race effect in face processing among African American and Caucasian individuals with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165(5):639–645. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Kessler K, Cao L, O’Shea KJ, Wang H. A cross-culture, cross-gender comparison of perspective taking mechanisms. Proc Biol Sci. 2014;281(1785):20140388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Wu S, Keysar B. The effect of culture on perspective taking. Psychol Sci. 2007;18(7):600–606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Dodell-Feder D, Ressler KJ, Germine LT. Social cognition or social class and culture? On the interpretation of differences in social cognitive performance. Psychol Med. 2020;50(1):133–145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Hajduk M, Achim AM, Brunet-Gouet E, Mehta UM, Pinkham AE. How to move forward in social cognition research? Put it into an international perspective. Schizophr Res. 2020;215:463–464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Horan WP, Green MF. Treatment of social cognition in schizophrenia: current status and future directions. Schizophr Res. 2019;203:3–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Horan WP, Dolinsky M, Lee J, et al. . Social cognitive skills training for psychosis with community-based training exercises: a randomized controlled trial. Schizophr Bull. 2018;44(6):1254–1266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Roberts DL, Combs DR, Willoughby M, et al. . A randomized, controlled trial of Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT) for outpatients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Br J Clin Psychol. 2014;53(3):281–298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Park MJ, Kim DJ, Lee U, Na EJ, Jeon HJ. A literature overview of virtual reality (VR) in treatment of psychiatric disorders: recent advances and limitations. Front Psychiatry. 2019;10:505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Nijman SA, Veling W, Greaves-Lord K, et al. . Dynamic Interactive Social Cognition Training in Virtual Reality (DiSCoVR) for social cognition and social functioning in people with a psychotic disorder: study protocol for a multicenter randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry. 2019;19(1):272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Nahum M, Fisher M, Loewy R, et al. . A novel, online social cognitive training program for young adults with schizophrenia: a pilot study. Schizophr Res Cogn. 2014;1(1):e11–e19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Maroño Souto Y, Vázquez Campo M, Díaz Llenderrozas F, Rodríguez Álvarez M, Mateos R, García Caballero A. Randomized clinical trial with e-MotionalTraining® 1.0 for social cognition rehabilitation in schizophrenia. Front Psychiatry. 2018;9:40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Zheng W, Zhu XM, Zhang QE, et al. . Adjunctive intranasal oxytocin for schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trials. Schizophr Res. 2019;206:13–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Ettinger U, Hurlemann R, Chan RCK. Oxytocin and schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2018;35:515–527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Wynn JK, Green MF, Hellemann G, Reavis EA, Marder SR. A dose-finding study of oxytocin using neurophysiological measures of social processing. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2019;44(2):289–294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Davis MC, Green MF, Lee J, et al. . Oxytocin-augmented social cognitive skills training in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39(9):2070–2077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Homberg JR, Olivier JD, VandenBroeke M, et al. . The role of the dopamine D1 receptor in social cognition: studies using a novel genetic rat model. Dis Model Mech. 2016;9(10):1147–1158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Fernández M, Mollinedo-Gajate I, Peñagarikano O. Neural circuits for social cognition: implications for autism. Neuroscience. 2018;370:148–162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Jones C. α7 Nicotinic acetylcholine receptor: a potential target in treating cognitive decline in schizophrenia. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2018;38(3):247–249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Lee J, Green MF. Social preference and glutamatergic dysfunction: underappreciated prerequisites for social dysfunction in schizophrenia. Trends Neurosci. 2016;39(9):587–596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Hong MP, Erickson CA. Investigational drugs in early-stage clinical trials for autism spectrum disorder. Expert Opin Investig Drugs. 2019;28(8):709–718. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Rassovsky Y, Dunn W, Wynn JK, et al. . Single transcranial direct current stimulation in schizophrenia: randomized, cross-over study of neurocognition, social cognition, ERPs, and side effects. PLoS One. 2018;13(5):e0197023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Rassovsky Y, Dunn W, Wynn J, et al. . The effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on social cognition in schizophrenia: a preliminary study. Schizophr Res. 2015;165(2-3):171–174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Gupta T, Kelley NJ, Pelletier-Baldelli A, Mittal VA. Transcranial direct current stimulation, symptomatology, and cognition in psychosis: a qualitative review. Front Behav Neurosci. 2018;12:94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Meshi D, Tamir DI, Heekeren HR. The emerging neuroscience of social media. Trends Cogn Sci. 2015;19(12):771–782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82. Wilmer HH, Hampton WH, Olino TM, Olson IR, Chein JM. Wired to be connected? Links between mobile technology engagement, intertemporal preference and frontostriatal white matter connectivity. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2019;14(4):367–379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83. McFarland LA, Ployhart RE. Social media: a contextual framework to guide research and practice. J Appl Psychol. 2015;100(6):1653–1677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]