Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2020 Apr 13;15(4):e0231592. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0231592

The neural influence of autobiographical memory related to the parent-child relationship on psychological health in adulthood

Eun Seong Kim 1,2,#, Hesun Erin Kim 1,3,#, Jae-Jin Kim 1,3,4,*
Editor: Luca Cerniglia5
PMCID: PMC7153857  PMID: 32282812

Abstract

The recollection of childhood memories is affected by the subjects involved, such as father and mother, and by the context. This study aimed to clarify the neural influence of autobiographical memory related to the parent-child relationship on psychological health in adulthood. Twenty-nine healthy volunteers participated in a functional magnetic resonance imaging experiment using a childhood memory recollection task, in which they appraised the emotion a parent would have provided in a given situation. Whole-brain univariate and psychophysiological interaction analyses were performed. Neuroimaging results indicated notable involvement of the caudal anterior cingulate cortex and precuneus in autobiographical memory related to the parent-child relationship, and their activities were closely associated with the level of depression and self-esteem, respectively. The functional connectivity results indicated increased connectivity between the caudal anterior cingulate cortex and fusiform gyrus for the father-positive condition compared to the mother-positive condition and there was a positive correlation between the strength of connectivity between the two regions and the anxiety level. Our findings suggest the processing of negative affect and the personalness of autobiographical memories are distinctly engaged depending on the parent in question and the situational valence. The present study illuminates the impact of autobiographical memory processes on various dimensions of psychological health.

Introduction

Like the saying, “no health without mental health,” promoted by the WHO, psychological health or psychological well-being is a fundamental factor contributing to quality of life [1]. There are many factors that influence psychological health in adulthood, and one of the most central determinants is the parent-child relationship [2]. The relationship between a parent and child becomes the template for other interpersonal relationships because attachment bonding allows a person to understand their environment and establish a sense of security [3,4]. An examination of the association between the retrospective perceptions of the parent-child relationship and emotional health in adulthood showed that daily emotional and psychological distress are closely related to how one perceives the quality of the parent-child relationship, and the retrospective perceptions of father-child and mother-child relationships contribute to different dimensions of emotional health [5]. Therefore, the perceptions of the childhood relationship with parents are important in the overall well-being, even as adults.

The retrospective perceptions of the parent-child relationship are based on an individual’s autobiographical memory. The development of autobiographical memory is a social process that is influenced by the reminiscing style of parents, and thus an integral part of the developing sense of self and interpersonal relatedness [6,7]. Autobiographical memory represents the self and shapes emotional states of our everyday lives [8,9]. In general, memories are highly intertwined with features like self-referential processing and emotion [9]. Functional neuroimaging studies have examined autobiographical memories in reference to other types of memories. For instance, a recent study that compared the difference between autobiographical memory and simple recognition memory found that when a task required retrieval of life memories, default-mode areas were engaged more, whereas a simple recollection of memories engaged the parietal memory network [10]. Since autobiographical memory retrieval relies heavily on self-reference and emotion, retrieval of autobiographical memories has been shown to employ various cortical and subcortical regions, including the prefrontal and parietal cortices, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), precuneus, medial temporal cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala [1114]. In particular, the amygdala has been implicated in the encoding of emotional memories [15] as well as retrieval of autobiographical memories [16]. Previous studies also found coactivation of the amygdala, hippocampus, and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) during the retrieval of autobiographical memories [17] and neural responses in the dorsal to caudal ACC and IFG elicited by the retrieval of painful autobiographical memories [18]. These reports consistently show a close relationship between autobiographical memory and emotional memory.

Psychological health is very closely related to memory encoding and retrieval, and there is evidence supporting its influence on neural activities related to autobiographical memory. Numerous studies have also shown the connection between psychological health and selective memory bias. The representation, recall, and maintenance of autobiographical memories have shown to be disturbed by individuals with poor psychological well-being, where they are systematically biased for negative information [11]. Memory bias and negative interpretations of events contribute to a vicious cycle, where one’s psychological health contributes to the tendency toward negative interpretation of memories, and this bias also maintains low self-esteem, depression, or anxiety [1921]. In fact, there is a consensus that self-esteem, anxiety, and depression are core elements of psychological health [2224]. The importance of these elements has spurred researchers to study the close associations between self-esteem, anxiety and depression, and quality of life in numerous fields [2528].

Self-esteem is a facet of personality that affects perception of social standing and modulates the salience of social interaction. Previous studies have shown that the midline cortical structures, such as the medial prefrontal cortex (PFC), ACC, posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), and precuneus that mediate self-referential processing are also closely involved in the expression of self-esteem [29]. Global self-esteem, which includes self-competence and self-liking, is related to selective memory, especially toward a negatively salient stimulus [19]. Individuals with lower self-esteem tend to be more concerned with how others perceive them, and this makes heightened negative events more memorable. Lower self-esteem predicts an increase in ventral ACC and medial PFC activity in response to positive versus negative social feedback, indicating the relationship between self-esteem and the salience processing [30]. On the other hand, a positivity bias for events that involve self-evaluations is stronger for individuals with higher self-esteem, suggesting that this bias affecting autobiographical memory is part of psychological mechanisms for maintaining a positive self-image [31]. The underlying neural substrates of a positive self-evaluation include the medial ventral and dorsolateral PFC and hippocampus, reflecting cognitive effort and emotional involvement for the positivity bias [32]. In addition, it has been reported that the lateral PFC, dorsal ACC, posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), precuneus, and caudate are involved in negative or positive character feedback associated with self-esteem [33].

The level of self-esteem may be related to the manifestation of depression through memory biases [34]. Many studies have reported that overall autobiographical memory predicts the onset or course of depression and thus is considered to be a risk factor for depression [35,36]. Depression contributes to the deviant reactivation of neural areas associated with autobiographical memory retrieval [37]. Neural correlates of autobiographical memory deficits in depressed patients have been known to involve a wide range of brain regions, such as the IFG, lateral orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), dorsomedial and ventrolateral PFC, ACC, precuneus, temporoparietal cortex, insula, hippocampus, and amygdala [3840].

Pathological anxiety has also been associated with negative memory bias [41]. Like depression, anxiety disorders are also characterized by maladaptive emotional responses and retrieval of memories [42]. In particular, negative autobiographical memory bias is prominent in individuals high in social anxiety [43]. Increased state anxiety decreases the ability to retrieve specific autobiographical memory [44]. Previous studies of patients with anxiety disorders have reported functional impairments of structures linked to the experience and regulation of emotion, such as the PFC, ACC, insula, and amygdala [4547].

Although behavioral and neurobiological studies indicate the impact of the psychological dimensions on the encoding and retrieval of autobiographical memories, the way in which psychological health affects memory processes in the general population is still unknown. The importance of the parent-child relationship in child development has been well documented. When looking at the way parents interact with a child, mothers are often more caring and nurturing, whereas fathers engage in more playful interactions [48]. Naturally, since the relationship between a father and child is different from the relationship between a mother and child, the autobiographical memories of parents in adulthood must be different. This difference is also observed in the brain response. For example, faces of mothers elicited more activity in the face processing network including the fusiform gyrus, whereas faces of fathers elicited more activity in the striatal region [49]. Despite the importance of the parent-child relationship and its difference between parents, no study has provided neurobiological evidence for how perceptions change depending on the parent and the valence of memories.

In this study of the general population, we sought to identify the neural substrates of the impression of parents while participants recalled childhood memories. For this purpose, we examined how fathers and mothers are perceived differently and whether the valence of memory influences this perception, and surveyed the effects of psychological health (self-esteem, depression, and anxiety) on the memory process. As childhood memories of parents triggered by various situations are inherently charged with emotions, we hypothesized that regions often involved in an autobiographical memory, such as the ACC, IFG, amygdala, and hippocampus, would be engaged differently depending on the parent and the valence of situation, and would be modulated by a person’s psychological health.

Methods

Participants

Twenty-nine normal volunteers were recruited through an advertisement at a local hospital and via the internet. All participants were right-handed, as assessed by the Annett Handedness Inventory [50]. Exclusion criteria included the presence of a neurological, psychiatric or significant medical illness, and a history of current or past substance abuse or dependence. Two participants who did not complete the behavior task were excluded, and three were excluded due to framewise displacement greater than 3 mm. A total of 24 participants (age: 28.0 ± 7.4 years old; 11 males and 13 females; education: 15.8 ± 4.0 years) were included in the final analysis. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Yonsei University Severance Hospital, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants before the study began.

Psychological assessment

We used the Parent-Adolescent Communication Inventory (PACI) [51], which consists of ten items assessing the openness of family communication and ten items measuring the negative aspects of interaction and communication among family members on a five-point scale (1 = not like that at all, 5 = always the case), to measure participants’ quality of communication with their parents. The ten-item Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES) [52] was also used to measure participants’ self-respect on a four-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 4 = strongly agree). We used the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) [53], which is comprised of seven items for anxiety and seven items for depression with each scored from 0 to 3 (0 to 21 for either anxiety or depression total scores), to determine the levels of anxiety and depression.

Task stimuli

Participants performed a task where they were asked to imagine what their parents’ facial expression looked like in a given situation during their childhood. Visual stimuli with a grey background included black words presenting a situation in the upper part and four black and white faces of the same person expressing different emotions (very happy, a little happy, very angry, and a little angry) in the lower part (Fig 1). The number of situations used in the task was 30, including ten positive (e.g., when I did errands), ten negative (e.g., when I did not do homework), and ten neutral (e.g., when I was on the phone). All actual situations used are shown in S1 Table.

Fig 1. Task stimuli and design.

Fig 1

The task imagining what one’s parents’ facial expression looked like in a certain situation of his/her childhood was built with mixed design, in which there were two experimental blocks for the father and mother conditions and six trials (two positive, two negative, and two neutral situations) in each block. The facial expressions presented were of four types: very happy (H), a little happy (h), a little angry (a), and very angry (A).

In order to evaluate the validity of the presented words, the vividness of memory associated with the situation, the intensity of reaction received from a parent in the situation, and the current degree of arousal caused by the recollection of the situation were assessed for each situation in 24 volunteers who did not participate in the fMRI experiment (27.3 ± 1.7 years old; 13 males and 11 females). Similar to the fMRI task, each situation was presented with either word “Father” or “Mother” on top of a page, then the volunteers were instructed to visualize an image of a parent during their childhood. All three items (vividness, response intensity, and arousal) were measured on a 9-point scale (0: not at all, 4: moderately, and 8: extremely). Analysis was conducted using repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) for parent (Father and Mother) and valence of situation (Positive, Negative, and Neutral) and post-hoc paired t-test.

Behavioral task and analysis

A total of ten blocks were presented in the task, and each block lasted for 30 sec with a 16 sec rest period between the blocks. Half of the blocks included a male face and the other half of the blocks included a female face. Participants were instructed to regard the man as their father and the woman as their mother. The 30 situations were presented in five blocks for the father condition and all of these situations were repeated in five blocks for the mother condition. Each block included two positive, two negative, and two neutral trials. Each trial lasted for 3 sec and the inter-stimulus interval was jittered between 0.5 sec and 3.0 sec. We randomly assigned the two block conditions in the whole sequence, the six situations in each block, and the four facial emotions in each trial. The timing and distribution of trials were optimized by optseq2 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/optseq/) to ensure that events in this mixed design could be statistically separated and to verify that the neural responses for each pair of stimuli were mutually orthogonal.

Participants responded by pressing one of four buttons corresponding to the location of the face that contained the closest imagined expression of their father or mother in the presented situation. The emotions of the selected faces were converted to scores (2 = very happy, 1 = a little happy, -1 = a little angry, and -2 = very angry) to represent the imagined emotion. In order to verify the validity of facial stimuli, we asked participants who finished scanning to rate the valence of each face on a 4-point Likert scale, anchored at 2 = very positive and -2 = very negative.

The imagined emotion score and reaction time (RT) were counted for behavioral analysis. The ANOVA was used to identify the main effect of parents (father and mother) and situation (positive, negative, and neutral) and the interaction effect between them. A paired t-test was subsequently performed to investigate the difference in imagined emotion scores between the father and mother conditions in each of the positive, negative, and neutral situations.

Imaging parameters and processing

MRI data were acquired on a 3T scanner (Achieva; Philips Medical System, Best, Netherlands). Functional images were acquired using a T2*-weighted gradient echo-planar imaging sequence (31 axial slices × 3 mm thickness and no gap; repetition time [TR] = 2,000 ms; echo time [TE] = 30 ms; flip angle [FA] = 90°; in-plane matrix = 124 × 124 pixels; and field of view [FOV] = 220 mm). Imaging slices were obtained at a tilted angle of 30° from the anterior commissure-posterior commissure line to minimize signal loss in the orbitofrontal cortex. An additional structural image using a 3D T1-weighted gradient echo sequence (TR = 9.692 ms; TE = 4.59 ms; FA = 8°; image matrix = 224 × 224; and FOV = 220 mm) was obtained from each participant after the functional runs.

Data preprocessing and analysis were performed using SPM12 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, University College London). Images remaining after discarding the first six images from the dummy scan were used for further preprocessing. Differences in the slice acquisition time were corrected, and realignment was performed to correct for head motion. These functional images were coregistered on the T1-weighted image. The T1-weighted images were spatially normalized to the standard template, and the resulting transformation matrices were applied to the coregistered functional images. The normalized images were smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 6 mm full-width at half-maximum.

Imaging data analysis

Preprocessed images were analyzed using a general linear model. A series of events as regressors of interest was modeled by convolving the event-train of stimulus onsets with the canonical hemodynamic response function. Trials were modeled based on the onset of stimulus presentation, specified as zero-duration events. With movement parameters as regressors of no-interest, left-hand and right-hand button pressing during the task period were also included as additional regressors. The positive and negative situations were contrasted with the neutral situations in each of the father and mother conditions for each participant on first-level analysis. At this first stage of analysis, the neutral situation was chosen as a control to subtract out general effects involving visual process and recall. Subsequently, four main contrasts for the positive and negative emotional situations in the father and mother conditions were created and used for the second-level analysis using a flexible factorial model and a post-hoc paired t-test to investigate the main and interaction effects of parents and situation. Statistical inferences were conducted at a threshold of a corrected p < 0.05, which corresponds to the family-wise error (FWE) corrected significance at the cluster level with a cluster-defining threshold of p < 0.001. In addition, correlation analysis was conducted between parameter estimates from the significant clusters showing the interaction effect and psychological scale scores. Parameter estimates were extracted from 6-mm spherical regions of interest (ROIs) centered on the peak voxel of significant clusters using the MarsBaR toolbox.

Functional connectivity analyses were implemented with psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analyses, in which the seed region was defined as a cluster showing the interaction effect in the second-level analysis. The time course from a 6-mm sphere in each seed region was extracted. Results were reported at a cluster-level FWE-corrected p < 0.05, with an initially voxel-level uncorrected p < 0.001. Finally, correlation analyses were conducted between the strength of functional coupling of the seed with the coupled region and the psychological scale scores. The MarsBaR toolbox was used to extract mean PPI coefficients from the first-level single-subject contrasts using the target area used for PPI analyses.

Results

Stimulus assessment results

Results from the assessment of the vividness, response intensity, and arousal of the 30 word stimuli are presented in Table 1. The vividness showed a significant result in a main effect of parent (F1,23 = 7.63, p = 0.011), but not in a main effect of situation (F2,46 = 0.46, p = 0.637) and an interaction effect (F2,46 = 1.08, p = 0.347). In post-hoc analysis, the vividness was significantly greater in the mother condition than in the father condition (t = 2.76, p = 0.011). The response intensity showed significant results in a main effect of parent (F1,23 = 24.23, p < 0.001) and a main effect of situation (F2,46 = 6.83, p = 0.003), but not in an interaction effect (F2,46 = 1.45, p = 0.246). Post-hoc analysis revealed that the response intensity was significantly higher in the mother condition than in the father condition (t = 4.92, p < 0.001) and significantly higher in the positive situation and negative condition than in the neutral situation (t = 3.24, p = 0.004; t = 3.95, p = 0.001, respectively). The arousal showed significant results in a main effect of parent (F1,23 = 6.66, p = 0.017) and a main effect of situation (F2,46 = 5.62, p = 0.007), but not in an interaction effect (F2,46 = 0.98, p = 0.384). In post-hoc analysis, the arousal was significantly higher in the mother condition than in the father condition (t = 2.58, p = 0.017), and in the positive and negative conditions than in the neutral condition (t = 3.35, p = 0.003; t = 3.25, p = 0.004, respectively).

Table 1. Results from the assessment of the vividness, response intensity, and arousal of the 30 word stimuli by the type of the parent and valence (mean ± standard deviation).

Positive Negative Neutral
Vividness
 Father 4.92 ± 2.03 4.95 ± 1.92 5.00 ± 1.75
 Mother 5.65 ± 1.66 5.83 ± 1.50 5.54 ± 1.60
Response intensity
 Father 4.57 ± 1.37 4.62 ± 1.11 4.23 ± 1.13
 Mother 5.48 ± 1.08 5.47 ± 1.24 4.78 ± 1.25
Arousal
 Father 3.92 ± 1.70 3.97 ± 1.70 3.64 ± 1.43
 Mother 4.71 ± 1.72 4.70 ± 1.63 4.15 ± 1.74

Behavioral results

Fig 2 illustrates the mean imagined emotion score and mean RT in each condition during the task. The imagined emotion scores showed a main effect of situation [F2,47 = 408.28, p < 0.001], but not a main effect of parents [F1,71 = 0.45, p = 0.504] and a significant parents × situation interaction [F2,23 = 0.80, p = 0.460]. Post-hoc analyses showed that the scores for the positive situations were significantly higher than those for the negative (t = 20.49, p < 0.01) and neutral situations (t = 14.53, p < 0.01), and the scores for the negative situations were significantly lower than those for the neutral situations (t = -15.86, p < 0.01).

Fig 2. The imagined emotion scores (A) and reaction times (B) in each situation and each block condition.

Fig 2

**p < 0.01.

The RTs also showed a main effect of situation [F2,47 = 45.42, p < 0.001] and a significant parents × situation interaction [F2,23 = 4.42, p = 0.024], but not a main effect of parents [F1,71 = 0.06, p = 0.813]. Post-hoc analyses showed that the RTs for the positive situations were significantly shorter than those for the negative situations (t = -6.82, p < 0.01) and the neutral situations (t = -6.77, p < 0.01), and there was no significant difference between the negative and neutral situations (t = 1.49, p = 0.151). The RTs in the father-negative condition were significantly longer than those in the father-neutral condition (t = 3.90, p < 0.01), whereas there was no significant difference between the mother-negative and mother-neutral conditions (t = 1.49, p = 0.453).

In the psychological assessments, the mean openness and problem scores for father in the PACI were 31.3 ± 7.9 and 25.4 ± 5.6, respectively, and those for mother were 38.3 ± 8.9 and 23.3 ± 5.6, respectively. The mean RSES score was 24.9 ± 2.4. The mean anxiety and depression scores in the HADS were 5.2 ± 3.3 and 5.1 ± 3.6, respectively. With regard to the relationships between the imagined emotion scores and PACI, the imagined emotion scores in the father-positive condition were positively correlated with the openness scores for father (r = 0.45, p = 0.028) and negatively correlated with the problem scores for father (r = -0.50, p = 0.013), and the imagined emotion scores in the mother-positive condition were also positively correlated with the openness scores for mother (r = 0.66, p < 0.001) and negatively correlated with the problem score for mother (r = -0.58, p < 0.01). With regard to relationships with the other scale scores, the only significant result was a positive correlation between the imagined emotion scores in the father-positive condition and the RSES scores (r = 0.44, p = 0.034).

Imaging results

As shown in Table 2, four main contrasts for the positive and negative emotional situations in the father and mother conditions showed significant main and interaction effects. The main effect of parents was evident in the left inferior frontal gyrus and left calcarine cortex, in which regional activity was higher in the mother condition than in the father condition. The main effect of situation was found in the left inferior frontal gyrus, left dorsal ACC, bilateral premotor cortex, and left thalamus, in which regional activity was higher in the positive situation than in the negative situation, and the right fusiform gyrus and left insula, in which regional activity was higher in the negative situation than in the positive situation.

Table 2. Brain regions showing the main and interaction effects among four main contrasts for the positive and negative situations in the father and mother conditions.

Region BA MNI coordinates Number of voxels F Post-hoc
x y z
Main effect of parents
 L. inferior frontal gyrus 44 -50 14 20 215 24.76 Fa < Mo
 L. calcarine cortex 17 -10 -90 0 749 31.76 Fa < Mo
Main effect of situation
 L. inferior frontal gyrus 47 -34 22 -24 121 22.02 Po > Ne
 L. dorsal ACC 32 -2 24 36 312 22.63 Po > Ne
 L. premotor cortex 6 -38 -4 58 1519 29.57 Po > Ne
 R. premotor cortex 6 48 8 34 697 26.48 Po > Ne
 R. fusiform gyrus 18 38 -74 -14 18460 147.75 Po < Ne
 L. insula -32 20 6 132 24.02 Po < Ne
 L. thalamus -2 -20 12 132 19.29 Po > Ne
Interaction: parents x situation
 R. caudal ACC 32 6 10 40 562 30.47 Fig 3A
 R. precuneus 7 12 -50 46 296 17.45 Fig 3B

BA, Brodmann area; Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI); L., left; R., right; Fa, father condition; Mo, mother condition; Po, positive emotional situation; Ne, negative emotional situation; and ACC, anterior cingulate cortex.

Significant clusters were obtained at voxel level Punc.< .001 and cluster level PFWE < .05.

There was an interaction effect in the right caudal ACC and right precuneus. As shown in Fig 3A, right caudal ACC activity in the father condition was significantly greater in the negative situation than in the positive situation (t = 5.09, p < 0.01), whereas that in the mother condition was not significant but greater in the positive situation than in the negative situation (t = 1.34, p = 0.193). Fig 3B also shows that right precuneus activity in the father condition was significantly greater in the negative situation than in the positive situation (t = 2.35, p < 0.05), whereas that in the mother condition was greater at a marginal level in the positive situation than in the negative situation (t = 1.94, p = 0.065). Correlation analysis between regional activity and psychological scale scores revealed a significant negative correlation between right caudal ACC activity in the mother-positive condition and depression scores (r = -0.45, p = 0.029) and between right precuneus activity in the father-negative condition and RSES scores (r = -0.44, p = 0.033).

Fig 3. Brain activity in the right (R.) caudal anterior cingulate cortex (A) and precuneus (B) showing the interaction effect between parents and emotions.

Fig 3

Post-hoc analysis showed that activity in both regions was significantly greater in the father condition than in the mother condition, and that in the father condition was significantly greater in the negative emotions than in the positive emotions (A-1 and B-1). In addition, R. caudal anterior cingulate cortex activity in the mother-positive condition was negatively correlated with the depression scores (A-2), whereas R. precuneus activity in the father-negative condition was negatively correlated with the Rosenberg self-esteem scale scores (B-2). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.

Fig 4 shows the results from PPI analysis, in which the seeds were located in clusters showing an interaction effect. Significantly increased functional connections with the right caudal ACC were found in the bilateral fusiform gyrus and postcentral gyrus for the father-positive condition compared to the mother-positive condition. No significant connections were found with the right precuneus. Correlation analysis between the connectivity strengths and psychological scale scores, the only significant result was positive correlation between the caudal ACC-fusiform gyrus connectivity strengths and anxiety scores (r = 0.48, p = 0.017).

Fig 4. The psychophysiological interaction effect showing functional connections of caudal anterior cingulate cortex (cACC) activity with bilateral fusiform gyrus activity for the father-positive condition compared to the mother-positive condition.

Fig 4

The connectivity strengths between the two regions showed positive correlation with the anxiety scores.

Discussion

The present study examined the neural relationship between the perception of a parent and the valence of an autobiographical memory, then explored how it is modulated by one’s psychological health. We measured perception by inciting imagination of the facial expression a parent had made in a given situation. The three seconds given for the task may have been too short to remember an autobiographical memory. However, since the vividness and reaction intensity in the stimulus assessments were rated as moderately or higher, we can say that the task was to evoke an autobiographical memory rather than an imagination or a simple impression. From our behavioral data, we saw a rank in imagined emotion scores among the valences of childhood memories, where positive situations elicited the highest imagined emotion score, followed by neutral situations and negative situations. In conjunction with the RT results, we could infer a sense of hesitation in choosing an appropriate expression especially for negative childhood situations compared to positive situations.

The quality of communication between parents and children is an excellent indicator of the quality of a parent-child relationship. Studies suggest that open communication with parents allows children to learn skills that are closely related to healthy coping skills and problem-solving strategies [54,55]. Thus, we assessed whether the quality of the relationship with parents affects the impression of parents during childhood. As expected, we found that more open communication with either father or mother was correlated with a greater emotion score in positive emotion situations for both parents. On the other hand, negative correlations were seen between communication problem scale scores (father/mother) and the imagined emotion ratings for the respective parent-positive conditions. Altogether, these findings convey that relationship quality directly affects the recollection of one’s parents in positive contexts, but not necessarily in negative situations. Moreover, the results also revealed that one’s level of self-esteem positively influences the perception of one’s father, particularly in positive situations. This finding further highlighted the influence of self-esteem on how positively one perceives his or her father for positive memories, rather than rumination on exaggerated negative affects seen in previous patient studies [19,41].

From the neuroimaging data, we saw intriguing neural responses to each condition from the right caudal ACC and precuneus, in particular. In the result of the caudal ACC, there was a particular bias toward father-negative and mother-positive, suggesting that in the autobiographical memory of emotional events, different forms of brain reactions occur depending on the parent’s gender. Attention on the caudal ACC has risen in recent years and studies have indicated that the region contributes significantly to negative affect and pain processing [5658]. This region receives an ample amount of emotion-related and voluntary movement signals [59,60], and has been suggested as a pivotal site for the emotional and voluntary interaction [58]. In a fascinating investigation on pain processing in self and others, it was seen that the caudal ACC was activated in response to self-experienced pain, but not when perceiving pain in others [56]. Furthermore, this region was shown to increase in response to negatively charged autobiographical memories [61]. On the other hand, its involvement in other-experienced pain processing has also been reported. A meta-analysis on empathy-related regions indicates that this region is part of the pain empathy network, and is engaged when pain was anticipated [62]. Regardless, it is clear that the caudal ACC plays a role in processing negative affect.

In light of these caudal ACC functions, our results may be linked to mothers taking on the role of primary caretaker more so than the fathers. In many cultures, traditional gender roles have determined the roles and expectations for parents, which usually assumes fathers as breadwinners and mothers as the primary caretaker in child rearing [63,64]. This point can be related to our results of the stimulus assessment. The analysis of the stimulus assessments indicated that the responses fathers gave were less intense in all situations than those given by mothers; and the imagination of fathers’ appearances were less vivid and evoked less arousal than that of the mothers. In these respects, it is possible that caudal ACC activity may be a response to the stoic and emotionally distant perception of fathers. Meanwhile, the importance of memory to the mother can be seen in our result of negative correlation between caudal ACC activity in the mother-positive condition and depression score. There was a report that pathological changes in this region may be related to major depressive disorder [65]. A study on the behavioral adaptation to unpleasant environment showed the involvement of the caudal ACC to be critical in the ability to regulate the behavior [66]. It has been suggested that the region might contribute to defending one from developing depression [67]. Taken together, our result of the negative correlation in the mother-positive condition may suggest that through the function of the caudal ACC, autobiographical memories of positive events with the mother may act as a factor that prevents depression.

The precuneus, a central hub of the default mode network, also exhibited a similar interaction effect between the parent and situation during the childhood memory recollection task. The precuneus has been thought to play a critical role in episodic memory [68,69], source monitoring [70], and self-referential processing [71]. Recently, a multivariate analysis method, representational similarity analysis, demonstrated that the precuneus is a key region engaged during the subjective experience of vivid autobiographical memory reminiscence [72]. Other experiments have confirmed its prominent involvement in the vividness of self-related memories [73,74]. In addition, right lateralization of the precuneus has been shown for highly personal and vivid reminiscence of autobiographical events [72]. Right lateralization is corroborated by the present study, which required participants not only to visualize a given circumstance, but also to delineate the facial expression by either parent. Our results indicate overall greater activation of the right precuneus during recollection of fathers than mothers, and more specifically, its activity was higher when recollecting fathers in negative compared to positive situations. It is interesting to note that despite lower vividness score in the stimulus assessments for fathers than mothers, these findings were observed, suggesting that the perceptions of fathers may be more personal than those of mothers, especially during reminiscence of negative situations. These may indicate the saliency of childhood memories of fathers and particularly how fathers responded during negative circumstances. In conjunction with activity of the caudal ACC, we cautiously conclude that memories of fathers in negative situations have a more adverse effect as the negative impression of fathers is more permanently embedded. This interpretation can also be linked to the results of correlation with self-esteem.

Studies report that the neural structure of personal semantic memories serves as a neural correlate of self-esteem [75,76]. It is postulated that due to its role in using autobiographical memories to choose the applicable self-relevant information, activation of the precuneus is closely related to self-esteem and how one processes given information [76,77]. The current study confirmed the established association between self-esteem and precuneus activity, as we found a negative relationship between one’s self-esteem and precuneus activity during autobiographical reminiscence of fathers in negative events. Recently, the precuneus was identified to correlate positively with evaluation sensitivity, measured by a change in state self-esteem [75]. Together with the evidence supporting the precuneus’ affiliation with evaluation sensitivity, as well as the tendency for individuals with lower self-esteem to be more affected by negative information [76], our result seems to suggest that the heightened activation observed in individuals with lower self-esteem may be due to the saliency of negative memories during recollection and their tendency to be more influenced by negative information.

Our PPI analysis revealed significant increases in functional connectivity of the caudal ACC with the fusiform gyrus and postcentral gyrus for the father-positive condition compared to the mother-positive condition. Given that the fusiform gyrus processes facial identity [78] and emotion [79], our finding may indicate that a greater sense of negative affect was involved while processing facial expressions of fathers than mothers in positive childhood events. This raises the possibility that the negative affect arisen from processing emotional face expressions may be particularly different between the parent in a positive context. Going back to the stimulus assessments, we saw lower vividness, response intensity, and arousal from fathers than mothers. Again, this may be since, generally, mothers are primary caregivers who spend more time with children; thus, one may have more readily available positive memories involving mothers than fathers to access on demand.

Previously, individuals with high trait anxiety have shown to hyperactivate the fusiform gyrus while processing uncertain cues and angry faces [80,81]. Consistent with the activation study, our study revealed that the connectivity strengths between the cingulate cortex and fusiform gyrus during the father-positive versus mother-positive condition correlated positively with the severity of anxiety symptoms. These results may provide the neurobiological influence of trait anxiety in the retrieval of memory and the perception of fathers. In addition to emotional face processing, greater connectivity between the cingulate area and postcentral gyrus was witnessed for the father-positive than mother-positive condition. Functional connectivity between these two regions has been implicated in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), where patients with PTSD showed stronger connectivity than the control group, and also was positively correlated with the severity of symptoms [82]. As part of the sensorimotor network [83], the coupling of the caudal ACC and postcentral or paracentral gyri has been shown to be involved in shock processing [84] and other emotional memory tasks [85]. Based on the evidence, we postulate that the caudal ACC may relay negatively valenced affective information to the sensorimotor area more for fathers than mothers when processing facial expressions.

There are some potential limitations in the current study. First, in the relationship with parents, the degree of intimacy or memory biases can vary between father and mother, depending on the individual’s sex. Because of the small sample size, however, the effect of sex variation was not analyzed. Second, since the correlation analysis was performed between three scale scores and regional activity under each condition, the possibility of false positives should be considered in interpreting the results. Third, as the present study required participants to engage in an autobiographical memory retrieval task regarding their childhood, it would have been interesting to investigate the quality of their parent-child relationship their pre-adulthood period. Fourth, it also would have been helpful to acquire current living situation data as research indicates some relationships between child and parent change depending on the child’s living situation [86]. Similarly, the current study did not survey parental divorce as it has been shown to influence various aspects of childhood development [87]. However, all participants in the current study reported having had relationships with both parents in childhood, regardless of parental divorce status. Nevertheless, in future studies, incorporating the status of parental divorce, quality of parent-child relationship in childhood in the study to examine various effects of divorce, age, sex, etc. are imperative to achieve a more comprehensive understanding of the perception of parents during autobiographical memory reminiscence.

In conclusion, the present study sheds light on neurobiological differences in the reminiscence of parents and situational valence. The caudal ACC and precuneus responded differently depending on which parent was the target as well as the valence of the situation in which participants were to recollect. Interestingly, both neural regions exhibited similar behavior, where activations were heightened for the father-negative and mother-positive conditions. Activities of the caudal ACC and precuneus negatively correlated with the severity of depressive symptoms as well as the level of self-esteem, respectively. Together, the negative relationships highlight the influence of depression severity and self-esteem on the negative affect processing and the personalness of memories. In addition, task-dependent functional connectivity between the caudal ACC and fusiform gyrus was greater for the father-positive than mother-positive condition. The positive correlation between anxiety level and the strength of connectivity between the caudal ACC and fusiform gyrus highlights the consequence of anxiety on negative emotions arisen from face processing. The findings successfully demonstrate the modulatory effect of psychological health on the perception of parents during autobiographical reminiscence.

Supporting information

S1 Table. The list of situations presented in the experiment.

(DOCX)

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) grant funded by the Korea government (MSIP) (No. NRF-2016R1A2A2A10921744).

References

  • 1.World Health Organization (WHO). Mental health: facing the challenges, building solutions. Report from the WHO European Ministerial Conference. Copenhagen: WHO Regional Office for Europe. 2005.
  • 2.An JS, Cooney TM. Psychological well-being in mid to late life: The role of generativity development and parent-child relationships across the lifespan. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2006; 30(5): 410–421. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Sroufe LA, Waters B. Attachment as an organizational construct. Child Development. 1977; 49: 1184–1199. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Bowlby J. The making & breaking of affectional bonds. London: Tavistock Publications; 1979. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Mallers MH, Charles ST, Neupert SD, Almeida DM. Perceptions of childhood relationships with mother and father: daily emotional and stressor experiences in adulthood. Developmental Psychology. 2010; 46(6): 1651–1661. 10.1037/a0021020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Fivush R, Haden C, Reese E. Remembering, recounting, and reminiscing: The development of autobiographical memory in social context In Rubin DC (Ed.), Remembering our past: Studies in autobiographical memory (p. 341–359). New York: NY, Cambridge University Press; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Cimino S, Cerniglia L, Tambelli R, Ballarotto G, Erriu M, Paciello M, et al. Dialogues about emotional events between mothers with anxiety, depression, anorexia nervosa, and no diagnosis and their children. Parenting, 2020; 20(1): 69–82. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Conway MA, Pleydell-Pearce CW. The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review. 2000; 107(2): 261–288. 10.1037/0033-295x.107.2.261 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bluck S, Li KZH. Predicting memory completeness and accuracy: Emotion and exposure in repeated autobiographical recall. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 2001; 15: 145–158. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Chen HY, Gilmore AW, Nelson SM, McDermott KB. Are there multiple kinds of episodic memory? An fMRI investigation comparing autobiographical and recognition memory tasks. Journal of Neuroscience. 2017; 37(10): 2764–2775. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1534-16.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Dalgleish T, Werner-Seidler A. Disruptions in autobiographical memory processing in depression and the emergence of memory therapeutics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2014; 18(11): 596–604. 10.1016/j.tics.2014.06.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R. Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2011; 15(2): 85–93. 10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kim H. A dual-subsystem model of the brain’s default network: Self-referential processing, memory retrieval processes, and autobiographical memory retrieval. Neuroimage. 2012; 61(4): 966–977. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.McGaugh JL. Emotional arousal regulation of memory consolidation. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2018; 19: 55–60. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Dolcos F, LaBar KS, Cabeza R. Interaction between the amygdala and the medial temporal lobe memory system predicts better memory for emotional events. Neuron. 2004; 42(5): 855–863. 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00289-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Markowitsch HJ, Thiel A, Reinkemeier M, Kessler J, Koyuncu A, Heiss WD. Right amygdalar and temporofrontal activation during autobiographic, but not during fictitious memory retrieval. Behavioural Neurology. 2000; 12(4): 181–190. 10.1155/2000/303651 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Greenberg DL, Rice HJ, Cooper JJ, Cabeza R, Rubin DC, Labar KS. Co-activation of the amygdala, hippocampus and inferior frontal gyrus during autobiographical memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia. 2005; 43(5): 659–674. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Kelly S, Lloyd D, Nurmikko T, Roberts N. Retrieving autobiographical memories of painful events activates the anterior cingulate cortex and inferior frontal gyrus. Journal of Pain. 2007; 8(4): 307–314. 10.1016/j.jpain.2006.08.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Tafarodi RW, Marshall TC, Milne AB. Self-esteem and memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003; 84(1): 29–45. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Dalgleish T, Watts FN. Biases of attention and memory in disorders of anxiety and depression. Clinical Psychology Review. 1990; 10(5): 589–604. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Hamilton JP, Gotlib IH. Neural substrates of increased memory sensitivity for negative stimuli in major depression. Biological Psychiatry. 2008; 63(12): 1155–1162. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.12.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Mann M, Hosman CM, Schaalma HP, de Vries NK. Self-esteem in a broad-spectrum approach for mental health promotion. Health Education Research. 2004; 19(4): 357–372. 10.1093/her/cyg041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Ryff CD. Psychological well-being in adult life. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 1995; 4(4): 99–104. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Tiller JWG. Depression and anxiety. Medical Journal of Australia. 2013; 199(6): 28–31.23829255 [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Diehr PH, Derleth AM, McKenna SP, Martin ML, Bushnell DM, Simon G, et al. Synchrony of change in depressive symptoms, health status, and quality of life in persons with clinical depression. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes. 2006; 4: 27–37. 10.1186/1477-7525-4-27 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Headey B, Kelley J, Wearing A. Dimensions of mental health: Life satisfaction, positive affect, anxiety and depression. Social Indicators Research. 1993; 29(1): 63–82. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Moksnes UK, Espnes GA. Self-esteem and life satisfaction in adolescents-gender and age as potential moderators. Quality of Life Research. 2013; 22(10): 2921–2928. 10.1007/s11136-013-0427-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Trzesniewski KH, Donnellan MB, Moffitt TE, Robins RW, Poulton R, Caspi A. Low self-esteem during adolescence predicts poor health, criminal behavior, and limited economic prospects during adulthood. Developmental Psychology. 2006; 42(2): 381–390. 10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.381 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Frewen PA, Lundberg E, Brimson-Théberge M, Théberge J. Neuroimaging self-esteem: A fMRI study of individual differences in women. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2013; 8(5): 546–555. 10.1093/scan/nss032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Somerville LH, Kelley WM, Heatherton TF. Self-esteem modulates medial prefrontal cortical responses to evaluative social feedback. Cerebral Cortex. 2010; 20(12): 3005–3013. 10.1093/cercor/bhq049 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.D’Argembeau A, van der Linden M. Remembering pride and shame: Self-enhancement and the phenomenology of autobiographical memory. Memory. 2008; 16(5): 538–547. 10.1080/09658210802010463 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Pauly K, Finkelmeyer A, Schneider F, Habel U. The neural correlates of positive self-evaluation and self-related memory. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2013; 8(8): 878–886. 10.1093/scan/nss086 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Peng M, Wu S, Shi Z, Jiang K, Shen Y, Dedovic K, et al. Brain regions in response to character feedback associated with the state self-esteem. Biological Psychology. 2019; 148: 107734 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2019.107734 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Romero N, Sanchez A, Vázquez C, Valiente C. Explicit self-esteem mediates the relationship between implicit self-esteem and memory biases in major depression. Psychiatry Research. 2016; 242: 336–344. 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Hermans D, Vandromme H, Debeer E, Raes F, Demyttenaere K, Brunfaut E, et al. Overgeneral autobiographical memory predicts diagnostic status in depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2008; 46(5): 668–677. 10.1016/j.brat.2008.01.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Rawal A, Rice F. Examining overgeneral autobiographical memory as a risk factor for adolescent depression. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2012; 51(5): 518–527. 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.02.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Whalley MG, Rugg MD, Brewin CR. Autobiographical memory in depression: an fMRI study. Psychiatry Research. 2012; 201(2): 98–106. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.08.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Young KD, Erickson K, Nugent AC, Fromm SJ, Mallinger AG, Furey ML, et al. Functional anatomy of autobiographical memory recall deficits in depression. Psychological Medicine. 2012; 42(2): 345–357. 10.1017/S0033291711001371 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Young KD, Bellgowan PS, Bodurka J, Drevets WC. Neurophysiological correlates of autobiographical memory deficits in currently and formerly depressed subjects. Psychological Medicine. 2014; 44(14): 2951–2963. 10.1017/S0033291714000464 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Young KD, Siegle GJ, Bodurka J, Drevets WC. Amygdala activity during autobiographical memory recall in depressed and vulnerable individuals: Association with symptom severity and autobiographical overgenerality. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2016; 173(1): 78–89. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15010119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Mogg K, Mathews A, Weinman J. Memory bias in clinical anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1987; 96(2): 94–98. 10.1037//0021-843x.96.2.94 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Mathews A, MacLeod C. Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2005; 1: 167–195. 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143916 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Krans J, de Bree J, Bryant RA. Autobiographical memory bias in social anxiety. Memory. 2014; 22(8): 890–897. 10.1080/09658211.2013.844261 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Hallford DJ, Mellor D, Bafit L, Devenish B, Bogeski T, Austin DW, et al. The effect of increasing state anxiety on autobiographical memory specificity and future thinking. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2019; 65: 101488 10.1016/j.jbtep.2019.101488 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Etkin A, Wager TD. Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: A meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007; 164(10): 1476–1488. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07030504 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Brohawn KH, Offringa R, Pfaff DL, Hughes KC, Shin LM. The neural correlates of emotional memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2010; 68(11): 1023–1030. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Dickie EW, Brunet A, Akerib V, Armony JL. Neural correlates of recovery from post-traumatic stress disorder: A longitudinal fMRI investigation of memory encoding. Neuropsychologia. 2011; 49(7): 1771–1778. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.DeHart GB, Sroufe LA, Cooper RG. Child development: Its nature and course. 5th ed New York, NY: McGraw-Hill; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Arsalidou M, Barbeau EJ, Bayless SJ, Taylor MJ. Brain responses differ to faces of mothers and fathers. Brain and Cognition. 2010; 74(1): 47–51. 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Annett M. A classification of hand preference by association analysis. British Journal of Psychology, 1970; 61(3): 303–321. 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1970.tb01248.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Barnes HL, Olson DH. Parent-adolescent communication scale Family inventories: Inventories used in a national survey of families across the family life cycle. St. Paul, MN: Family Social Science, University of Minnesota; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Rosenberg M. Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1965. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 1983; 67(6): 361–370. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Gentzler AL, Contreras‐Grau JM, Kerns KA, Weimer BL. Parent-child emotional communication and children’s coping in middle childhood. Social Development. 2005; 14(4): 591–612. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Offrey LD, Rinaldi CM. Parent–child communication and adolescents’ problem-solving strategies in hypothetical bullying situations. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth. 2017; 22(3): 251–267. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Singer T, Seymour B, O’Doherty J, Kaube H, Dolan RJ, Frith CD. Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science. 2004;303(5661):1157–1162. 10.1126/science.1093535 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Tolomeo S, Christmas D, Jentzsch I, Johnston B, Sprengelmeyer R, Matthews K, et al. A causal role for the anterior mid-cingulate cortex in negative affect and cognitive control. Brain. 2016; 139: 1844–1854. 10.1093/brain/aww069 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Pereira MG, de Oliveira L, Erthal FS, Joffily M, Mocaiber IF, Volchan E, et al. Emotion affects action: Midcingulate cortex as a pivotal node of interaction between negative emotion and motor signals. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2010; 10(1): 94–106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Morecraft RJ, Van Hoesen GW. Convergence of limbic input to the cingulate motor cortex in the rhesus monkey. Brain Research Bulletin. 1998; 45(2): 209–232. 10.1016/s0361-9230(97)00344-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Paus T. Primate anterior cingulate cortex: Where motor control, drive and cognition interface. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2001; 2(6): 417–424. 10.1038/35077500 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Cabeza R, St Jacques P. Functional neuroimaging of autobiographical memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2007; 11(5): 219–227. 10.1016/j.tics.2007.02.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Lamm C, Decety J, Singer T. Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain. Neuroimage. 2011; 54(3): 2492–2502. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Wilson KR, Prior MR. Father involvement and child well-being. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health. 2011; 47(7): 405–407. 10.1111/j.1440-1754.2010.01770.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Saracho ON, Spodek B. Fathers: The “invisible” parents. Early Child Development and Care. 2008; 178: 821–836. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.van Tol MJ, Li M, Metzger CD, Hailla N, Horn DI, Li W, et al. Local cortical thinning links to resting-state disconnectivity in major depressive disorder. Psychological Medicine. 2014; 44(10): 2053–2065. 10.1017/S0033291713002742 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Kalin NH, Shelton SE, Fox AS, Oakes TR, Davidson RJ. Brain regions associated with the expression and contextual regulation of anxiety in primates. Biological Psychiatry. 2005; 58(10): 796–804. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.05.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Chen CH, Ridler K, Suckling J, Williams S, Fu CHY, Merlo-Pich E, et al. Brain imaging correlates of depressive symptom severity and predictors of symptom improvement after antidepressant treatment. Biological Psychiatry. 2007; 62(5): 407–414. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.09.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Cabeza R, Ciaramelli E, Olson IR, Moscovitch M. The parietal cortex and episodic memory: an attentional account. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008; 9(8): 613–625. 10.1038/nrn2459 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Vannini P, O’Brien J, O’Keefe K, Pihlajamäki M, Laviolette P, Sperling RA. What goes down must come up: role of the posteromedial cortices in encoding and retrieval. Cerebral Cortex. 2011; 21(1): 22–34. 10.1093/cercor/bhq051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Lundstrom BN, Ingvar M, Petersson KM. The role of precuneus and left inferior frontal cortex during source memory episodic retrieval. Neuroimage. 2005; 27(4): 824–834. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Hebscher M, Levine B, Gilboa A. The precuneus and hippocampus contribute to individual differences in the unfolding of spatial representations during episodic autobiographical memory. Neuropsychologia. 2018; 110: 123–133. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.03.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Sreekumar V, Nielson DM, Smith TA, Dennis SJ, Sederberg PB. The experience of vivid autobiographical reminiscence is supported by subjective content representations in the precuneus. Scientific Reports. 2018; 8(1): 14899 10.1038/s41598-018-32879-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Bird CM, Keidel JL, Ing LP, Horner AJ, Burgess N. Consolidation of complex events via reinstatement in posterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015; 35(43): 14426–14434. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1774-15.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.St-Laurent M, Abdi H, Buchsbaum BR. Distributed patterns of reactivation predict vividness of recollection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2015; 27(10): 2000–2018. 10.1162/jocn_a_00839 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Kawamichi H, Sugawara SK, Hamano YH, Kitada R, Nakagawa E, Kochiyama T, et al. Neural correlates underlying change in state self-esteem. Scientific Reports. 2018; 8(1): 1798 10.1038/s41598-018-20074-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.van Schie CC, Chiu CD., Rombouts SARB, Heiser WJ, Elzinga BM. When compliments do not hit but critiques do: an fMRI study into self-esteem and self-knowledge in processing social feedback. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2018; 13(4): 404–417. 10.1093/scan/nsy014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Cavanna AE, Trimble MR. The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain. 2006; 129(Pt 3): 564–583. 10.1093/brain/awl004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM. The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. Journal of Neuroscience. 1997; 17(11): 4302–4311. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04302.1997 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Said CP, Haxby JV, Todorov A. Brain systems for assessing the affective value of faces. Philosophical Transactions of The Royal Society B Biological Sciences. 2011; 366(1571): 1660–1670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Straube T, Kolassa IT, Glauer M, Mentzel HJ, Miltner WH. Effect of task conditions on brain responses to threatening faces in social phobics: An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biological Psychiatry. 2004; 56(12): 921–930. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.09.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Zhang M, Ma C, Luo Y, Li J, Li Q, Liu Y, et al. Neural basis of uncertain cue processing in trait anxiety. Scientific Reports. 2016; 6: 21298 10.1038/srep21298 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Chen HJ, Zhang L, Ke J, Qi R, Xu Q, Zhong Y, et al. Altered resting-state dorsal anterior cingulate cortex functional connectivity in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 2019; 53(1): 68–79. 10.1177/0004867418812674 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Chenji S., Jha S., Lee D., Brown M., Seres P., Mah D., & Kalra S. (). Investigating default mode and sensorimotor network connectivity in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. PLoS One. 2016; 11(6): e0157443 10.1371/journal.pone.0157443 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Wiemer J, Pauli P. Enhanced functional connectivity between sensorimotor and visual cortex predicts covariation bias in spider phobia. Biological Psychology. 2016; 121: 128–137. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.01.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Krause-Utz A, Elzinga BM, Oei NY, Paret C, Niedtfeld I, Spinhoven P, et al. Amygdala and dorsal anterior cingulate connectivity during an emotional working memory task in borderline personality disorder patients with interpersonal trauma history. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2014; 8: 848 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00848 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Lefkowitz ES. "Things have gotten better": Developmental changes among emerging adults after the transition to university. Journal of Adolescent Research. 2005; 20(1): 40–63. [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Zill N, Morrison DR, Coiro MJ. Long-term effects of parental divorce on parent-child relationships, adjustment, and achievement in young adulthood. Journal of Family Psychology. 1993; 7(1): 91–103. [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Luca Cerniglia

18 Feb 2020

PONE-D-19-30869

The neural influence of autobiographical memory related to the parent-child relationship on psychological health in adulthood

PLOS ONE

Dear Authors,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by 3/17/20. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Luca Cerniglia, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (if provided):

For this manuscript one referee recommended rejections, another one recommended major revisions. In light of the fact that also the reviewer who recommended rejection found merit in the paper, my decision is to ask the authors to revise the article with major revision.

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements:

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at http://www.plosone.org/attachments/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and http://www.plosone.org/attachments/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

3. We note that Figure 1 includes an image of a participant in the study. 

As per the PLOS ONE policy (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-human-subjects-research) on papers that include identifying, or potentially identifying, information, the individual(s) or parent(s)/guardian(s) must be informed of the terms of the PLOS open-access (CC-BY) license and provide specific permission for publication of these details under the terms of this license. Please download the Consent Form for Publication in a PLOS Journal (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=8ce6/plos-consent-form-english.pdf). The signed consent form should not be submitted with the manuscript, but should be securely filed in the individual's case notes. Please amend the methods section and ethics statement of the manuscript to explicitly state that the patient/participant has provided consent for publication: “The individual in this manuscript has given written informed consent (as outlined in PLOS consent form) to publish these case details”.

If you are unable to obtain consent from the subject of the photograph, you will need to remove the figure and any other textual identifying information or case descriptions for this individual.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Review title: The neural influence of autobiographical memory related to the parent-child relationship on psychological health in adulthood

The current paper investigates the brain regions associated with the recall of autobiographical memories of peoples’ childhood memories of parental facial expressions in certain childhood situations. Specifically, looking into the association between current psychological health (self-esteem, anxiety, and depression), quality of parent-child-relationship, and specific brain regions activities.

Overall: The paper is a very interesting study and there is potential for novel information. The biggest criticisms are the characterization of the task as an autobiographical memory retrieval task, the lack of clear theory, and the major reverse inferencing due to a lack of supporting measures. Additional data is needed to support any of the claims the authors make.

Introduction: The introduction needs a rewrite. The paper is bit hard to follow. The authors in a single paragraph talk about several topics, e.g., emotion regulation, global-self-esteem, anxiety, depression, memory biases, etc. Each topic should be within its own paragraph/section. One section can be on self-esteem and memory, anxiety and memory, etc.

Also, explaining more in-depth of the specific brain regions associated with the recall of autobiographical memories, emotional memories, since this is the specific task participants will be engaging in.

Also, the hypothesis of this paper are also not clear. For example, “We predicted that emotional ratings would be correlated with psychological health facets.” What emotional ratings are being referenced? Rating of the specific parental facial expressions expressed by a parent during a given encounter or the valence of the memory in general. Furthermore, there was no justification for why they hypothesized different activity in memory regions based on parent or emotion.

Methods: A concern of the study is whether participants actually engaged in autobiographical memory recall rather than imagination or simply an impression. First, there is substantial research showing that retrieving specific autobiographical memories takes more time than 3 seconds in response to a cue word (see Conway, 2005)! In addition, participants were not actually instructed to retrieve a memory, but rather to imagine their parents’ facial expression. This type of task will be heavily influenced by current/recent experiences with their parents’ facial expression. Lastly, I’m not sure it is possible to both retrieve a memory and make the facial judgment within 3 seconds. Because participants were forced to respond so quickly (and did so looking at the RT’s), this task is best interpreted as an impression task. Do the authors have any evidence to support their interpretation of the task?

The authors also report using a linear mixed model to analyze the behavior. First, this model is quite complex and much more information is needed regarding how the model was set up (first level, second level factors, etc) and why this model was chosen over an ANOVA.

Results: The authors should report all results and all p-values e.g. …”(t = 2.77, p < 0.05)” (p. 9); “but showed no parent effect and a significant parents × situation interaction.” (p. 8) Can the authors also show figures of the main effects? The authors also do not appropriately describe the interactions (which are based on differences between conditions). The authors’ worded the interaction as main effects (“ACC activity was significantly greater in the father condition than in the mother condition”), which were inaccurate.

The authors should reconsider the names of their regions and interpretation. The ACC activity they found is better characterized as mid cingulate cortex. It is a bit too posterior to be considered dACC activity.

Motivation for the correlations do not make sense. For example, the authors found activity in the “ACC” that was driven by valence differences in the father condition, and yet found a correlation between depression and brain activity in this region in the mother-positive condition! Same with the other correlation. This implies that the authors assessed all possible correlations, regardless as to whether they made sense which would be at least 24 tests (3 scales, 4 conditions, 2 regions), thus inflating false positives. Not only should these correlations not be trusted (due to false positives), but they also were not hypothesized.

Discussion: The major issue in the discussion is that the authors over-interpret their findings and use a lot of reverse inferencing to do so. As one example, the authors claim that the dACC activity represents conflict processing. First, I am not convinced that the dACC was activated at all. However, even if this were to be the dACC, the dACC is one of the most active areas and does not indicate that conflict processing has occurred. The authors would benefit from reading Poldrack, R. A. (2011). Inferring mental states from neuroimaging data: from reverse inference to large-scale decoding. Neuron, 72(5), 692-697. Another example is that the authors claim that they can “therefore infer that the perceptions of fathers were more personal and vivid than those of mothers, especially during reminiscence of negative situations.” Again, none of this can actually be inferred without actual ratings of vividness or “personalness”. Because the Discussion is primarily comprised of entirely reverse inferencing and over-interpretation, it would have to be rewritten and I am convinced that nothing can be firmly concluded without additional data.

One source of evidence of over-interpretation is again the interpretation of conflict monitoring. Decades of research has shown that conflict comes with a cost in response times. And yet, this “conflict monitoring” that occurs only for fathers and not mothers is not borne out in the RT ratings.

Lastly, the authors do indicate one of the major limitations is not being able to analyze sex differences due to the small sample size (which I agree with), but the authors may not realize how much of a problem this actually is. Because the authors have already chosen to differentially investigate effect mother vs. father effects, the relationship and memory biases that one might have will surely depend on the sex of the individual. The net result is an average set of brain activity that may not represent either the men or women (e.g., the average of 0 and 10 is 5 and 5 is equally as far from representing men and women, nor represent either).

Recommendations: In the end, I would highly recommend testing a new set of participants behaviorally in the same task, but with additional probing to test whether they actually retrieved a memory, to rate the vividness of that memory, assess the degree of conflict they experienced, and any other rating to support the inferences the authors are making. Making claims of cognitive processing based on the neuroimaging findings alone without such support is a critical and known inferencing flaw in the neuroimaging community.

Reviewer #2: Thank you very much for the possibility to review the manuscript titled "The neural influence of autobiographical memory related to the parent-child relationship on psychological health in adulthood". I think that this study contributes a lot to the literature and should be published in this journal if the authors want to review some aspects of the article that are lacking. This study investigates the neural influence on autobiographical memories; however, the introductory section refers to several concepts not explored in this work: emotional regulation, parent-child attachment. I think it may be important to review the whole introductory section in order to better focus the present work. In fact, the authors are invited to review studies related to the parent-child relationship (rather than to the model of indivisual attachment) and to deepen the literature on autobiographical memory.

For example, the authors are recommended to deepen their knowledge of the following works, which would greatly enrich this manuscript:

Fivush, R., & Haden, C. A. (Eds.). (2003). Autobiographical memory and the construction of a narrative self: Developmental and cultural perspectives. Psychology Press.

Fivush, R. et al. (1996). Remembering, recounting, and reminiscing: The development of autobiographical memory in social context.

Cimino, S. et al. (2020). Dialogues about Emotional Events between Mothers with Anxiety, Depression, Anorexia Nervosa, and No Diagnosis and Their Children. Parenting, 20(1), 69-82.

Again with regard to the introductory section, the authors are invited to define more clearly the objectives of the work and the hypotheses, consistent with the results and discussions.

Discussions can also be reviewed in the light of the cited literature and authors are invited to use an editing service for English language.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2020 Apr 13;15(4):e0231592. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0231592.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


18 Mar 2020

Reviewer #1:

Review title: The neural influence of autobiographical memory related to the parent-child relationship on psychological health in adulthood

The current paper investigates the brain regions associated with the recall of autobiographical memories of peoples’ childhood memories of parental facial expressions in certain childhood situations. Specifically, looking into the association between current psychological health (self-esteem, anxiety, and depression), quality of parent-child-relationship, and specific brain regions activities.

Overall: The paper is a very interesting study and there is potential for novel information. The biggest criticisms are the characterization of the task as an autobiographical memory retrieval task, the lack of clear theory, and the major reverse inferencing due to a lack of supporting measures. Additional data is needed to support any of the claims the authors make.

Reply:

We greatly appreciate the positive evaluation and valuable comments on the manuscript revision. Problems pointed out were all reflected in the revision of the manuscript.

Introduction: The introduction needs a rewrite. The paper is bit hard to follow. The authors in a single paragraph talk about several topics, e.g., emotion regulation, global-self-esteem, anxiety, depression, memory biases, etc. Each topic should be within its own paragraph/section. One section can be on self-esteem and memory, anxiety and memory, etc.

Reply:

As suggested, the introduction was totally rewritten. Autobiographical memory, memory biases, self-esteem, depression, and anxiety were described as separate paragraphs and contents were supplemented. The emotion regulation-related content was deleted because it did not fit the subject of this paper. In addition, we added some additional background on the nature of parental roles and how processing fathers and mothers engage the brain differently.

Revision:

In ‘the first to 7th paragraphs’ of the Introduction section

Like the saying, “no health without mental health,” promoted by the WHO, psychological health or psychological well-being is a fundamental factor contributing to quality of life [1]. There are many factors that influence psychological health in adulthood, and one of the most central determinants is the parent-child relationship [2]. The relationship between a parent and child becomes the template for other interpersonal relationships because attachment bonding allows a person to understand their environment and establish a sense of security [3,4]. An examination of the association between the retrospective perceptions of the parent-child relationship and emotional health in adulthood showed that daily emotional and psychological distress are closely related to how one perceives the quality of the parent-child relationship, and the retrospective perceptions of father-child and mother-child relationships contribute to different dimensions of emotional health [5]. Therefore, the perceptions of the childhood relationship with parents are important in the overall well-being, even as adults.

The retrospective perceptions of the parent-child relationship are based on an individual’s autobiographical memory. The development of autobiographical memory is a social process that is influenced by the reminiscing style of parents, and thus an integral part of the developing sense of self and interpersonal relatedness [6,7]. Autobiographical memory represents the self and shapes emotional states of our everyday lives [8,9]. In general, memories are highly intertwined with features like self-referential processing and emotion [9]. Functional neuroimaging studies have examined autobiographical memories in reference to other types of memories. For instance, a recent study that compared the difference between autobiographical memory and simple recognition memory found that when a task required retrieval of life memories, default-mode areas were engaged more, whereas a simple recollection of memories engaged the parietal memory network [10]. Since autobiographical memory retrieval relies heavily on self-reference and emotion, retrieval of autobiographical memories has been shown to employ various cortical and subcortical regions, including the prefrontal and parietal cortices, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), precuneus, medial temporal cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala [11-14]. In particular, the amygdala has been implicated in the encoding of emotional memories [15] as well as retrieval of autobiographical memories [16]. Previous studies also found coactivation of the amygdala, hippocampus, and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) during the retrieval of autobiographical memories [17] and neural responses in the dorsal to caudal ACC and IFG elicited by the retrieval of painful autobiographical memories [18]. These reports consistently show a close relationship between autobiographical memory and emotional memory.

Psychological health is very closely related to memory encoding and retrieval, and there is evidence supporting its influence on neural activities related to autobiographical memory. Numerous studies have also shown the connection between psychological health and selective memory bias. The representation, recall, and maintenance of autobiographical memories have shown to be disturbed by individuals with poor psychological well-being, where they are systematically biased for negative information [11]. Memory bias and negative interpretations of events contribute to a vicious cycle, where one’s psychological health contributes to the tendency toward negative interpretation of memories, and this bias also maintains low self-esteem, depression, or anxiety [19-21]. In fact, there is a consensus that self-esteem, anxiety, and depression are core elements of psychological health [22-24]. The importance of these elements has spurred researchers to study the close associations between self-esteem, anxiety and depression, and quality of life in numerous fields [25-28].

Self-esteem is a facet of personality that affects perception of social standing and modulates the salience of social interaction. Previous studies have shown that the midline cortical structures, such as the medial prefrontal cortex (PFC), ACC, posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), and precuneus that mediate self-referential processing are also closely involved in the expression of self-esteem [29]. Global self-esteem, which includes self-competence and self-liking, is related to selective memory, especially toward a negatively salient stimulus [19]. Individuals with lower self-esteem tend to be more concerned with how others perceive them, and this makes heightened negative events more memorable. Lower self-esteem predicts an increase in ventral ACC and medial PFC activity in response to positive versus negative social feedback, indicating the relationship between self-esteem and the salience processing [30]. On the other hand, a positivity bias for events that involve self-evaluations is stronger for individuals with higher self-esteem, suggesting that this bias affecting autobiographical memory is part of psychological mechanisms for maintaining a positive self-image [31]. The underlying neural substrates of a positive self-evaluation include the medial ventral and dorsolateral PFC and hippocampus, reflecting cognitive effort and emotional involvement for the positivity bias [32]. In addition, it has been reported that the lateral PFC, dorsal ACC, posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), precuneus, and caudate are involved in negative or positive character feedback associated with self-esteem [33].

The level of self-esteem may be related to the manifestation of depression through memory biases [34]. Many studies have reported that overall autobiographical memory predicts the onset or course of depression and thus is considered to be a risk factor for depression [35,36]. Depression contributes to the deviant reactivation of neural areas associated with autobiographical memory retrieval [37]. Neural correlates of autobiographical memory deficits in depressed patients have been known to involve a wide range of brain regions, such as the IFG, lateral orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), dorsomedial and ventrolateral PFC, ACC, precuneus, temporoparietal cortex, insula, hippocampus, and amygdala [38-40].

Pathological anxiety has also been associated with negative memory bias [41]. Like depression, anxiety disorders are also characterized by maladaptive emotional responses and retrieval of memories [42]. In particular, negative autobiographical memory bias is prominent in individuals high in social anxiety [43]. Increased state anxiety decreases the ability to retrieve specific autobiographical memory [44]. Previous studies of patients with anxiety disorders have reported functional impairments of structures linked to the experience and regulation of emotion, such as the PFC, ACC, insula, and amygdala [45-47].

Although behavioral and neurobiological studies indicate the impact of the psychological dimensions on the encoding and retrieval of autobiographical memories, the way in which psychological health affects memory processes in the general population is still unknown. The importance of the parent-child relationship in child development has been well documented. When looking at the way parents interact with a child, mothers are often more caring and nurturing, whereas fathers engage in more playful interactions [48]. Naturally, since the relationship between a father and child is different from the relationship between a mother and child, the autobiographical memories of parents in adulthood must be different. This difference is also observed in the brain response. For example, faces of mothers elicited more activity in the face processing network including the fusiform gyrus, whereas faces of fathers elicited more activity in the striatal region [49]. Despite the importance of the parent-child relationship and its difference between parents, no study has provided neurobiological evidence for how perceptions change depending on the parent and the valence of memories.

Also, explaining more in-depth of the specific brain regions associated with the recall of autobiographical memories, emotional memories, since this is the specific task participants will be engaging in.

Reply:

As suggested, additional explanations regarding autobiographical memory and emotional memory were added to a separate paragraph.

Revision:

In ‘the second paragraph’ of the Introduction section

The retrospective perceptions of the parent-child relationship are based on an individual’s autobiographical memory. The development of autobiographical memory is a social process that is influenced by the reminiscing style of parents, and thus an integral part of the developing sense of self and interpersonal relatedness [6,7]. Autobiographical memory represents the self and shapes emotional states of our everyday lives [8,9]. In general, memories are highly intertwined with features like self-referential processing and emotion [9]. Functional neuroimaging studies have examined autobiographical memories in reference to other types of memories. For instance, a recent study that compared the difference between autobiographical memory and simple recognition memory found that when a task required retrieval of life memories, default-mode areas were engaged more, whereas a simple recollection of memories engaged the parietal memory network [10]. Since autobiographical memory retrieval relies heavily on self-reference and emotion, retrieval of autobiographical memories has been shown to employ various cortical and subcortical regions, including the prefrontal and parietal cortices, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), precuneus, medial temporal cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala [11-14]. In particular, the amygdala has been implicated in the encoding of emotional memories [15] as well as retrieval of autobiographical memories [16]. Previous studies also found coactivation of the amygdala, hippocampus, and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) during the retrieval of autobiographical memories [17] and neural responses in the dorsal to caudal ACC and IFG elicited by the retrieval of painful autobiographical memories [18]. These reports consistently show a close relationship between autobiographical memory and emotional memory.

Also, the hypothesis of this paper are also not clear. For example, “We predicted that emotional ratings would be correlated with psychological health facets.” What emotional ratings are being referenced? Rating of the specific parental facial expressions expressed by a parent during a given encounter or the valence of the memory in general. Furthermore, there was no justification for why they hypothesized different activity in memory regions based on parent or emotion.

Reply:

We sincerely apologize for the confusion. To answer the first part, the “emotional ratings” we mentioned in the hypothesis was the in-scan behavior data. In the revised manuscript, the hypothesis was updated to avoid the confusion.

Revision:

In ‘the last paragraph’ of the Introduction section

In this study of the general population, we sought to identify the neural substrates of the impression of parents while participants recalled childhood memories. For this purpose, we examined how fathers and mothers are perceived differently and whether the valence of memory influences this perception, and surveyed the effects of psychological health (self-esteem, depression, and anxiety) on the memory process. As childhood memories of parents triggered by various situations are inherently charged with emotions, we hypothesized that regions often involved in an autobiographical memory, such as the ACC, IFG, amygdala, and hippocampus, would be engaged differently depending on the parent and the valence of situation, and would be modulated by a person’s psychological health.

Methods: A concern of the study is whether participants actually engaged in autobiographical memory recall rather than imagination or simply an impression. First, there is substantial research showing that retrieving specific autobiographical memories takes more time than 3 seconds in response to a cue word (see Conway, 2005)! In addition, participants were not actually instructed to retrieve a memory, but rather to imagine their parents’ facial expression. This type of task will be heavily influenced by current/recent experiences with their parents’ facial expression. Lastly, I’m not sure it is possible to both retrieve a memory and make the facial judgment within 3 seconds. Because participants were forced to respond so quickly (and did so looking at the RT’s), this task is best interpreted as an impression task. Do the authors have any evidence to support their interpretation of the task?

Reply:

We are sincerely grateful for this particular point. As indicated, 3 seconds can actually be a very short time to remember an autobiographical memory. Our manuscript had no basis to properly answer this pointed problem. Now, following your recommendations at the end, we provided clarity data for task stimuli after the additional experiment. As shown in the newly added Table 1, the vividness of memory associated with the situation and the intensity of reaction received from a parent in all situations indicated 4 or greater, which means ‘above moderately.’ We think these results may explain to an extent to what you have expressed concerns. In the revised manuscript, we added this comment in the discussion.

Revision:

In ‘the first paragraph’ of the Discussion section

The three seconds given for the task may have been too short to remember an autobiographical memory. However, since the vividness and reaction intensity in the stimulus assessments were rated as moderately or higher, we can say that the task was to evoke an autobiographical memory rather than an imagination or a simple impression.

The authors also report using a linear mixed model to analyze the behavior. First, this model is quite complex and much more information is needed regarding how the model was set up (first level, second level factors, etc) and why this model was chosen over an ANOVA.

Reply:

As pointed out, it seems that LMM was not necessary, and thus ANOVA was applied to the revised manuscript. As the analysis method changed, it was adjusted accordingly to the statistical values of the results.

Revision:

In ‘Behavioral task and analysis’ of the Methods section

The ANOVA was used to identify the main effect of parents (father and mother) and situation (positive, negative, and neutral) and the interaction effect between them.

In ‘Behavioral results’ of the RESULTS section

Figure 2 illustrates the mean imagined emotion score and mean RT in each condition during the task. The imagined emotion scores showed a main effect of situation [F2,47 = 408.28, p < 0.001], but not a main effect of parents [F1,71 = 0.45, p = 0.504] and a significant parents × situation interaction [F2,23 = 0.80, p = 0.460]. Post-hoc analyses showed that the scores for the positive situations were significantly higher than those for the negative (t = 20.49, p < 0.01) and neutral situations (t = 14.53, p < 0.01), and the scores for the negative situations were significantly lower than those for the neutral situations (t = -15.86, p < 0.01).

The RTs also showed a main effect of situation [F2,47 = 45.42, p < 0.001] and a significant parents × situation interaction [F2,23 = 4.42, p = 0.024], but not a main effect of parents [F1,71 = 0.06, p = 0.813]. Post-hoc analyses showed that the RTs for the positive situations were significantly shorter than those for the negative situations (t = -6.82, p < 0.01) and the neutral situations (t = -6.77, p < 0.01), and there was no significant difference between the negative and neutral situations (t = 1.49, p = 0.151). The RTs in the father-negative condition were significantly longer than those in the father-neutral condition (t = 3.90, p < 0.01), whereas there was no significant difference between the mother-negative and mother-neutral conditions (t = 1.49, p = 0.453).

Results: The authors should report all results and all p-values e.g. …”(t = 2.77, p < 0.05)” (p. 9); “but showed no parent effect and a significant parents × situation interaction.” (p. 8) Can the authors also show figures of the main effects? The authors also do not appropriately describe the interactions (which are based on differences between conditions). The authors’ worded the interaction as main effects (“ACC activity was significantly greater in the father condition than in the mother condition”), which were inaccurate.

Reply:

As suggested, we added statistics for the results without significance. Since the differences among the positive, negative, and neutral conditions were so similar between the father and mother conditions, we did not add a graph of the main effect to avoid the duplication. In terms of the interaction effect, we agree with the comment that our reports were inaccurate. In the revised results, we changed them into an appropriate form.

Revision:

In ‘Behavioral results’ of the RESULTS section

Figure 2 illustrates the mean imagined emotion score and mean RT in each condition during the task. The imagined emotion scores showed a main effect of situation [F2,47 = 408.28, p < 0.001], but not a main effect of parents [F1,71 = 0.45, p = 0.504] and a significant parents × situation interaction [F2,23 = 0.80, p = 0.460]. Post-hoc analyses showed that the scores for the positive situations were significantly higher than those for the negative (t = 20.49, p < 0.01) and neutral situations (t = 14.53, p < 0.01), and the scores for the negative situations were significantly lower than those for the neutral situations (t = -15.86, p < 0.01).

The RTs also showed a main effect of situation [F2,47 = 45.42, p < 0.001] and a significant parents × situation interaction [F2,23 = 4.42, p = 0.024], but not a main effect of parents [F1,71 = 0.06, p = 0.813]. Post-hoc analyses showed that the RTs for the positive situations were significantly shorter than those for the negative situations (t = -6.82, p < 0.01) and the neutral situations (t = -6.77, p < 0.01), and there was no significant difference between the negative and neutral situations (t = 1.49, p = 0.151). The RTs in the father-negative condition were significantly longer than those in the father-neutral condition (t = 3.90, p < 0.01), whereas there was no significant difference between the mother-negative and mother-neutral conditions (t = 1.49, p = 0.453).

In ‘Imaging results’ of the RESULTS section

There was an interaction effect in the right caudal ACC and right precuneus. As shown in Figure 3A, right caudal ACC activity in the father condition was significantly greater in the negative situation than in the positive situation (t = 5.09, p < 0.01), whereas that in the mother condition was not significant but greater in the positive condition than in the negative condition (t = 1.34, p = 0.193). Figure 3B also shows that right precuneus activity in the father condition was significantly greater in the negative situation than in the positive situation (t = 2.35, p < 0.05), whereas that in the mother condition was not significant but greater in the positive condition than in the negative condition (t = 1.94, p = 0.065).

The authors should reconsider the names of their regions and interpretation. The ACC activity they found is better characterized as mid cingulate cortex. It is a bit too posterior to be considered dACC activity.

Reply:

We agree with this comment. I have two significant results in the ACC. In the revised manuscript, the first one in the main effect of situation was named the dorsal ACC, and the second one in the interaction effect was regarded as the caudal ACC. As a result of this separation, the interpretation in the discussion was changed accordingly.

Revision:

In ‘Imaging results’ of the Results section

There was an interaction effect in the right caudal ACC and right precuneus.

In ‘3rd and 4th paragraphs’ of the Discussion section

From the neuroimaging data, we saw intriguing neural responses to each condition from the right caudal ACC and precuneus, in particular. In the result of the caudal ACC, there was a particular bias toward father-negative and mother-positive, suggesting that in the autobiographical memory of emotional events, different forms of brain reactions occur depending on the parent's gender. Attention on the caudal ACC has risen in recent years and studies have indicated that the region contributes significantly to negative affect and pain processing [56-58]. This region receives an ample amount of emotion-related and voluntary movement signals [59,60], and has been suggested as a pivotal site for the emotional and voluntary interaction [58]. In a fascinating investigation on pain processing in self and others, it was seen that the caudal ACC was activated in response to self-experienced pain, but not when perceiving pain in others [56]. Furthermore, this region was shown to increase in response to negatively charged autobiographical memories [61]. On the other hand, its involvement in other-experienced pain processing has also been reported. A meta-analysis on empathy-related regions indicates that this region is part of the pain empathy network, and is engaged when pain was anticipated [62]. Regardless, it is clear that the caudal ACC plays a role in processing negative affect.

In light of these caudal ACC functions, our results may be linked to mothers taking on the role of primary caretaker more so than the fathers. In many cultures, traditional gender roles have determined the roles and expectations for parents, which usually assumes fathers as breadwinners and mothers as the primary caretaker in child rearing [63,64]. This point can be related to our results of the stimulus assessment. The analysis of the stimulus assessments indicated that the responses that fathers gave were less intense in all situations than those given by mothers; and the imagination of fathers’ appearances were less vivid and evoked less arousal than that of the mothers. In these respects, it is possible that caudal ACC activity may be a response to the stoic and emotionally distant perception of fathers. Meanwhile, the importance of memory to the mother can be seen in our result of negative correlation between caudal ACC activity in the mother-positive condition and depression score. There was a report that pathological changes in this region may be related to major depressive disorder [65]. A study on the behavioral adaptation to unpleasant environment showed the involvement of the caudal ACC to be critical in the ability to regulate the behavior [66]. It has been suggested that the region might contribute to defending one from developing depression [67]. Taken together, our result of the negative correlation in the mother-positive condition may suggest that through the function of the caudal ACC, autobiographical memories of positive events with the mother may act as a factor that prevents depression.

Motivation for the correlations do not make sense. For example, the authors found activity in the “ACC” that was driven by valence differences in the father condition, and yet found a correlation between depression and brain activity in this region in the mother-positive condition! Same with the other correlation. This implies that the authors assessed all possible correlations, regardless as to whether they made sense which would be at least 24 tests (3 scales, 4 conditions, 2 regions), thus inflating false positives. Not only should these correlations not be trusted (due to false positives), but they also were not hypothesized.

Reply:

As defined in the introduction, we hypothesized that a person’s psychological health (self-esteem, anxiety, and depression) would be modulated by neural regions including the ACC, IFG, hippocampus, amygdala, and other areas associated with autobiographical memory recall. Since the regions of interest (caudal ACC and precuneus) result from the interaction effect, we considered their activity meaningful in every condition. We saw that if there was a difference in the father condition, there was also a meaning of no difference in the mother condition, and vice versa. Of course, as pointed out, it is a problem that the likelihood of false positives increases. We added this problem in the limitation of the study in the discussion.

Revision:

In the 9th paragraph of the DISCUSSION section

Second, since the correlation analysis was performed between three scale scores and regional activity under each condition, the possibility of false positives should be considered in interpreting the results.

Discussion: The major issue in the discussion is that the authors over-interpret their findings and use a lot of reverse inferencing to do so. As one example, the authors claim that the dACC activity represents conflict processing. First, I am not convinced that the dACC was activated at all. However, even if this were to be the dACC, the dACC is one of the most active areas and does not indicate that conflict processing has occurred. The authors would benefit from reading Poldrack, R. A. (2011). Inferring mental states from neuroimaging data: from reverse inference to large-scale decoding. Neuron, 72(5), 692-697. Another example is that the authors claim that they can “therefore infer that the perceptions of fathers were more personal and vivid than those of mothers, especially during reminiscence of negative situations.” Again, none of this can actually be inferred without actual ratings of vividness or “personalness”. Because the Discussion is primarily comprised of entirely reverse inferencing and over-interpretation, it would have to be rewritten and I am convinced that nothing can be firmly concluded without additional data.

One source of evidence of over-interpretation is again the interpretation of conflict monitoring. Decades of research has shown that conflict comes with a cost in response times. And yet, this “conflict monitoring” that occurs only for fathers and not mothers is not borne out in the RT ratings.

Reply:

We agree with the comments that our discussion contained too much of over-interpretation. As suggested, we included additional behavioral data in order to clarify certain claims that we made. We also edited our claims to eliminate reverse inferencing and over-interpretation as much as possible. Additionally, we changed the dACC region significant from the interaction effect to the caudal ACC. Accordingly, the discussion section of the revised manuscript was rewritten entirely.

Revision:

In ‘the 3rd to 8th paragraphs’ of the Discussion section

From the neuroimaging data, we saw intriguing neural responses to each condition from the right caudal ACC and precuneus, in particular. In the result of the caudal ACC, there was a particular bias toward father-negative and mother-positive, suggesting that in the autobiographical memory of emotional events, different forms of brain reactions occur depending on the parent's gender. Attention on the caudal ACC has risen in recent years and studies have indicated that the region contributes significantly to negative affect and pain processing [56-58]. This region receives an ample amount of emotion-related and voluntary movement signals [59,60], and has been suggested as a pivotal site for the emotional and voluntary interaction [58]. In a fascinating investigation on pain processing in self and others, it was seen that the caudal ACC was activated in response to self-experienced pain, but not when perceiving pain in others [56]. Furthermore, this region was shown to increase in response to negatively charged autobiographical memories [61]. On the other hand, its involvement in other-experienced pain processing has also been reported. A meta-analysis on empathy-related regions indicates that this region is part of the pain empathy network, and is engaged when pain was anticipated [62]. Regardless, it is clear that the caudal ACC plays a role in processing negative affect.

In light of these caudal ACC functions, our results may be linked to mothers taking on the role of primary caretaker more so than the fathers. In many cultures, traditional gender roles have determined the roles and expectations for parents, which usually assumes fathers as breadwinners and mothers as the primary caretaker in child rearing [63,64]. This point can be related to our results of the stimulus assessment. The analysis of the stimulus assessments indicated that the responses that fathers gave were less intense in all situations than those given by mothers; and the imagination of fathers’ appearances were less vivid and evoked less arousal than that of the mothers. In these respects, it is possible that caudal ACC activity may be a response to the stoic and emotionally distant perception of fathers. Meanwhile, the importance of memory to the mother can be seen in our result of negative correlation between caudal ACC activity in the mother-positive condition and depression score. There was a report that pathological changes in this region may be related to major depressive disorder [65]. A study on the behavioral adaptation to unpleasant environment showed the involvement of the caudal ACC to be critical in the ability to regulate the behavior [66]. It has been suggested that the region might contribute to defending one from developing depression [67]. Taken together, our result of the negative correlation in the mother-positive condition may suggest that through the function of the caudal ACC, autobiographical memories of positive events with the mother may act as a factor that prevents depression.

The precuneus, a central hub of the default mode network, also exhibited a similar interaction effect between the parent and situation during the childhood memory recollection task. The precuneus has been thought to play a critical role in episodic memory [68,69], source monitoring [70], and self-referential processing [71]. Recently, a multivariate analysis method, representational similarity analysis, demonstrated that the precuneus is a key region engaged during the subjective experience of vivid autobiographical memory reminiscence [72]. Other experiments have confirmed its prominent involvement in the vividness of self-related memories [73,74]. In addition, right lateralization of the precuneus has been shown for highly personal and vivid reminiscence of autobiographical events [72]. Right lateralization is corroborated by the present study, which required participants not only to visualize a given circumstance, but also to delineate the facial expression by either parent. Our results indicate overall greater activation of the right precuneus during recollection of fathers than mothers, and more specifically, its activity was higher when recollecting fathers in negative compared to positive situations. It is interesting to note that despite lower vividness score in the stimulus assessments for fathers than mothers, these findings were observed, suggesting that the perceptions of fathers may be more personal than those of mothers, especially during reminiscence of negative situations. These may indicate the saliency of childhood memories of fathers and particularly how fathers responded during negative circumstances. In conjunction with activity of the caudal ACC, we cautiously conclude that memories of fathers in negative situations have a more adverse effect as the negative impression of fathers is more permanently embedded. This interpretation can also be linked to the results of correlation with self-esteem.

Studies report that the neural structure of personal semantic memories serves as a neural correlate of self-esteem [75,76]. It is postulated that due to its role in using autobiographical memories to choose the applicable self-relevant information, activation of the precuneus is closely related to self-esteem and how one processes given information [76,77]. The current study confirmed the established association between self-esteem and precuneus activity, as we found a negative relationship between one’s self-esteem and precuneus activity during autobiographical reminiscence of fathers in negative events. Recently, the precuneus was identified to correlate positively with evaluation sensitivity, measured by a change in state self-esteem [75]. Together with the evidence supporting the precuneus’ affiliation with evaluation sensitivity, as well as the tendency for individuals with lower self-esteem to be more affected by negative information [76], our result seems to suggest that the heightened activation observed in individuals with lower self-esteem may be due to the saliency of negative memories during recollection and their tendency to be more influenced by negative information.

Our PPI analysis revealed significant increases in functional connectivity of the caudal ACC with the fusiform gyrus and postcentral gyrus for the father-positive condition compared to the mother-positive condition. Given that the fusiform gyrus processes facial identity [78] and emotion [79], our finding may indicate that a greater sense of negative affect was involved while processing facial expressions of fathers than mothers in positive childhood events. This raises the possibility that the negative affect arisen from processing emotional face expressions may be particularly different between the parent in a positive context. Going back to the stimulus assessments, we saw lower vividness, response intensity, and arousal from fathers than mothers. Again, this may be since, generally, mothers are primary caregivers who spend more time with children; thus, one may have more readily available positive memories involving mothers than fathers to access on demand.

Previously, individuals with high trait anxiety have shown to hyperactivate the fusiform gyrus while processing uncertain cues and angry faces [80,81]. Consistent with the activation study, our study revealed that the connectivity strengths between the cingulate cortex and fusiform gyrus during the father-positive versus mother-positive condition correlated positively with the severity of anxiety symptoms. These results may provide the neurobiological influence of trait anxiety in the retrieval of memory and the perception of fathers. In addition to emotional face processing, greater connectivity between the cingulate area and postcentral gyrus was witnessed for the father-positive than mother-positive condition. Functional connectivity between these two regions has been implicated in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), where patients with PTSD showed stronger connectivity than the control group, and also was positively correlated with the severity of symptoms [82]. As part of the sensorimotor network [83], the coupling of the caudal ACC and postcentral or paracentral gyri has been shown to be involved in shock processing [84] and other emotional memory tasks [85]. Based on the evidence, we postulate that the caudal ACC may relay negatively valenced affective information to the sensorimotor area more for fathers than mothers when processing facial expressions.

Lastly, the authors do indicate one of the major limitations is not being able to analyze sex differences due to the small sample size (which I agree with), but the authors may not realize how much of a problem this actually is. Because the authors have already chosen to differentially investigate effect mother vs. father effects, the relationship and memory biases that one might have will surely depend on the sex of the individual. The net result is an average set of brain activity that may not represent either the men or women (e.g., the average of 0 and 10 is 5 and 5 is equally as far from representing men and women, nor represent either).

Reply:

We whole-heartedly agree with this comment on the importance of exploring the sex differences in this study. Because we couldn't do it because of the small number of samples, we added the reviewer's point as an additional description to the limitation of the study.

Revision:

In the 9th paragraph of the DISCUSSION section

First, in the relationship with parents, the degree of intimacy or memory biases can vary between father and mother, depending on the individual's sex. Because of the small sample size, however, the effect of sex variation was not analyzed.

Recommendations: In the end, I would highly recommend testing a new set of participants behaviorally in the same task, but with additional probing to test whether they actually retrieved a memory, to rate the vividness of that memory, assess the degree of conflict they experienced, and any other rating to support the inferences the authors are making. Making claims of cognitive processing based on the neuroimaging findings alone without such support is a critical and known inferencing flaw in the neuroimaging community.

Reply:

We greatly appreciate this suggestion on conducting additional tests. In fact, before the experiment, we had performed a preliminary test to survey the intensity of reaction given by a parent, the vividness of each stimulus, and the level of arousal to ensure the validity of the task. However, due to the small number of subjects, the results were not included in the manuscript. In response to the reviewer’s recommendation, we carried out additional experiment to increase the number of subjects, and the data has been included in the method and results to the revised manuscript. We believe the results helped to mitigate the over-interpretation and reverse-inference problem, and we thank you again for the comment.

Revision:

In ‘Task stimuli’ of the Methods section

Participants performed a task where they were asked to imagine what their parents’ facial expression looked like in a given situation during their childhood. Visual stimuli with a grey background included black words presenting a situation in the upper part and four black and white faces of the same person expressing different emotions (very happy, a little happy, very angry, and a little angry) in the lower part (Figure 1). The number of situations used in the task was 30, including ten positive (e.g., when I did errands), ten negative (e.g., when I did not do homework), and ten neutral (e.g., when I was on the phone). All actual situations used are shown in Supplementary Table S1.

In order to evaluate the validity of the presented words, the vividness of memory associated with the situation, the intensity of reaction received from a parent in the situation, and the current degree of arousal caused by the recollection of the situation were assessed for each situation in 24 volunteers who did not participate in the fMRI experiment (27.3 ± 1.7 years old; 11 females and 13 males). Similar to the fMRI task, each situation was presented with either word “Mother” or “Father” on top of a page, then the volunteers were instructed to visualize an image of a parent during their childhood. All three items (vividness, response intensity, and arousal) were measured on a 9-point scale (0: not at all, 4: moderately, and 8: extremely). Analysis was conducted using repeated measures ANOVA for parent (Mother and Father) and valence of situation (Positive, Negative, and Neutral) and post-hoc paired t-test.

In ‘Stimulus assessment results’ of the Results section

Results from the assessment of the vividness, response intensity, and arousal of the 30 word stimuli are presented in Table 1. The vividness showed a significant result in a main effect of parent (F1,23 = 7.63, p = 0.011), but not in a main effect of situation (F2,46 = 0.46, p = 0.637) and an interaction effect (F2,46 = 1.08, p = 0.347). In post-hoc analysis, the vividness was significantly greater in the mother condition than in the father condition (t = 2.76, p = 0.011). The response intensity showed significant results in a main effect of parent (F1,23 = 24.23, p < 0.001) and a main effect of situation (F2,46 = 6.83, p = 0.003), but not in an interaction effect (F2,46 = 1.45, p = 0.246). Post-hoc analysis revealed that the response intensity was significantly higher in the mother condition than in the father condition (t = 4.92, p < 0.001) and significantly higher in the positive situation and negative condition than in the neutral situation (t = 3.24, p = 0.004; t = 3.95, p = 0.001, respectively). The arousal showed significant results in a main effect of parent (F1,23 = 6.66, p = 0.017) and a main effect of situation (F2,46 = 5.62, p = 0.007), but not in an interaction effect (F2,46 = 0.98, p = 0.384). In post-hoc analysis, the arousal was significantly higher in the mother condition than in the father condition (t = 2.58, p = 0.017), and in the positive and negative conditions than in the neutral condition (t = 3.35, p = 0.003; t = 3.25, p = 0.004, respectively).

Reviewer #2:

Thank you very much for the possibility to review the manuscript titled "The neural influence of autobiographical memory related to the parent-child relationship on psychological health in adulthood". I think that this study contributes a lot to the literature and should be published in this journal if the authors want to review some aspects of the article that are lacking. This study investigates the neural influence on autobiographical memories; however, the introductory section refers to several concepts not explored in this work: emotional regulation, parent-child attachment. I think it may be important to review the whole introductory section in order to better focus the present work. In fact, the authors are invited to review studies related to the parent-child relationship (rather than to the model of indivisual attachment) and to deepen the literature on autobiographical memory.

For example, the authors are recommended to deepen their knowledge of the following works, which would greatly enrich this manuscript:

Fivush, R., & Haden, C. A. (Eds.). (2003). Autobiographical memory and the construction of a narrative self: Developmental and cultural perspectives. Psychology Press.

Fivush, R. et al. (1996). Remembering, recounting, and reminiscing: The development of autobiographical memory in social context.

Cimino, S. et al. (2020). Dialogues about Emotional Events between Mothers with Anxiety, Depression, Anorexia Nervosa, and No Diagnosis and Their Children. Parenting, 20(1), 69-82.

Reply:

We would like to express our gratitude for your optimistic evaluation of our research and for drawing our attention to an important point. In the revised manuscript, descriptions of emotion regulation and attachment were deleted because they were not directly related to the research purpose. Instead, we added additional information to the introduction to clarify autobiographical memory-related contents, including the references that you suggested. Consequently, the introduction was entirely rewritten.

Revision:

In ‘the 1st to 3rd paragraphs’ of the Introduction section

Like the saying, “no health without mental health,” promoted by the WHO, psychological health or psychological well-being is a fundamental factor contributing to quality of life [1]. There are many factors that influence psychological health in adulthood, and one of the most central determinants is the parent-child relationship [2]. The relationship between a parent and child becomes the template for other interpersonal relationships because attachment bonding allows a person to understand their environment and establish a sense of security [3,4]. An examination of the association between the retrospective perceptions of the parent-child relationship and emotional health in adulthood showed that daily emotional and psychological distress are closely related to how one perceives the quality of the parent-child relationship, and the retrospective perceptions of father-child and mother-child relationships contribute to different dimensions of emotional health [5]. Therefore, the perceptions of the childhood relationship with parents are important in the overall well-being, even as adults.

The retrospective perceptions of the parent-child relationship are based on an individual’s autobiographical memory. The development of autobiographical memory is a social process that is influenced by the reminiscing style of parents, and thus an integral part of the developing sense of self and interpersonal relatedness [6,7]. Autobiographical memory represents the self and shapes emotional states of our everyday lives [8,9]. In general, memories are highly intertwined with features like self-referential processing and emotion [9]. Functional neuroimaging studies have examined autobiographical memories in reference to other types of memories. For instance, a recent study that compared the difference between autobiographical memory and simple recognition memory found that when a task required retrieval of life memories, default-mode areas were engaged more, whereas a simple recollection of memories engaged the parietal memory network [10]. Since autobiographical memory retrieval relies heavily on self-reference and emotion, retrieval of autobiographical memories has been shown to employ various cortical and subcortical regions, including the prefrontal and parietal cortices, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), precuneus, medial temporal cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala [11-14]. In particular, the amygdala has been implicated in the encoding of emotional memories [15] as well as retrieval of autobiographical memories [16]. Previous studies also found coactivation of the amygdala, hippocampus, and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) during the retrieval of autobiographical memories [17] and neural responses in the dorsal to caudal ACC and IFG elicited by the retrieval of painful autobiographical memories [18]. These reports consistently show a close relationship between autobiographical memory and emotional memory.

Psychological health is very closely related to memory encoding and retrieval, and there is evidence supporting its influence on neural activities related to autobiographical memory. Numerous studies have also shown the connection between psychological health and selective memory bias. The representation, recall, and maintenance of autobiographical memories have shown to be disturbed by individuals with poor psychological well-being, where they are systematically biased for negative information [11]. Memory bias and negative interpretations of events contribute to a vicious cycle, where one’s psychological health contributes to the tendency toward negative interpretation of memories, and this bias also maintains low self-esteem, depression, or anxiety [19-21]. In fact, there is a consensus that self-esteem, anxiety, and depression are core elements of psychological health [22-24]. The importance of these elements has spurred researchers to study the close associations between self-esteem, anxiety and depression, and quality of life in numerous fields [25-28].

Again with regard to the introductory section, the authors are invited to define more clearly the objectives of the work and the hypotheses, consistent with the results and discussions.

Reply:

As suggested, we revised the objective and hypothesis.

Revision:

In ‘the last paragraph’ of the Introduction section

In this study of the general population, we sought to identify the neural substrates of the impression of parents while participants recalled childhood memories. For this purpose, we examined how fathers and mothers are perceived differently and whether the valence of memory influences this perception, and surveyed the effects of psychological health (self-esteem, depression, and anxiety) on the memory process. As childhood memories of parents triggered by various situations are inherently charged with emotions, we hypothesized that regions often involved in an autobiographical memory, such as the ACC, IFG, amygdala, and hippocampus, would be engaged differently depending on the parent and the valence of situation, and would be modulated by a person’s psychological health.

Discussions can also be reviewed in the light of the cited literature and authors are invited to use an editing service for English language.

Reply:

As suggested, we have completely revised the discussion and the manuscript was reviewed by a native speaker for English expression.

Revision:

In ‘the 1st to 8th paragraphs’ of the Discussion section

The present study examined the neural relationship between the perception of a parent and the valence of an autobiographical memory, then explored how it is modulated by one’s psychological health. We measured perception by inciting imagination of the facial expression a parent had made in a given situation. The three seconds given for the task may have been too short to remember an autobiographical memory. However, since the vividness and reaction intensity in the stimulus assessments were rated as moderately or higher, we can say that the task was to evoke an autobiographical memory rather than an imagination or a simple impression. From our behavioral data, we saw a rank in imagined emotion scores among the valences of childhood memories, where positive situations elicited the highest imagined emotion score, followed by neutral situations and negative situations. In conjunction with the RT results, we could infer a sense of hesitation in choosing an appropriate expression especially for negative childhood situations compared to positive situations.

The quality of communication between parents and children is an excellent indicator of the quality of a parent-child relationship. Studies suggest that open communication with parents allows children to learn skills that are closely related to healthy coping skills and problem-solving strategies [54,55]. Thus, we assessed whether the quality of the relationship with parents affects the impression of parents during childhood. As expected, we found that more open communication with either father or mother was correlated with a greater emotion score in positive emotion situations for both parents. On the other hand, negative correlations were seen between communication problem scale scores (father/mother) and the imagined emotion ratings for the respective parent-positive conditions. Altogether, these findings convey that relationship quality directly affects the recollection of one’s parents in positive contexts, but not necessarily in negative situations. Moreover, the results also revealed that one’s level of self-esteem positively influences the perception of one’s father, particularly in positive situations. This finding further highlighted the influence of self-esteem on how positively one perceives his or her father for positive memories, rather than rumination on exaggerated negative affects seen in previous patient studies [19,41].

From the neuroimaging data, we saw intriguing neural responses to each condition from the right caudal ACC and precuneus, in particular. In the result of the caudal ACC, there was a particular bias toward father-negative and mother-positive, suggesting that in the autobiographical memory of emotional events, different forms of brain reactions occur depending on the parent's gender. Attention on the caudal ACC has risen in recent years and studies have indicated that the region contributes significantly to negative affect and pain processing [56-58]. This region receives an ample amount of emotion-related and voluntary movement signals [59,60], and has been suggested as a pivotal site for the emotional and voluntary interaction [58]. In a fascinating investigation on pain processing in self and others, it was seen that the caudal ACC was activated in response to self-experienced pain, but not when perceiving pain in others [56]. Furthermore, this region was shown to increase in response to negatively charged autobiographical memories [61]. On the other hand, its involvement in other-experienced pain processing has also been reported. A meta-analysis on empathy-related regions indicates that this region is part of the pain empathy network, and is engaged when pain was anticipated [62]. Regardless, it is clear that the caudal ACC plays a role in processing negative affect.

In light of these caudal ACC functions, our results may be linked to mothers taking on the role of primary caretaker more so than the fathers. In many cultures, traditional gender roles have determined the roles and expectations for parents, which usually assumes fathers as breadwinners and mothers as the primary caretaker in child rearing [63,64]. This point can be related to our results of the stimulus assessment. The analysis of the stimulus assessments indicated that the responses that fathers gave were less intense in all situations than those given by mothers; and the imagination of fathers’ appearances were less vivid and evoked less arousal than that of the mothers. In these respects, it is possible that caudal ACC activity may be a response to the stoic and emotionally distant perception of fathers. Meanwhile, the importance of memory to the mother can be seen in our result of negative correlation between caudal ACC activity in the mother-positive condition and depression score. There was a report that pathological changes in this region may be related to major depressive disorder [65]. A study on the behavioral adaptation to unpleasant environment showed the involvement of the caudal ACC to be critical in the ability to regulate the behavior [66]. It has been suggested that the region might contribute to defending one from developing depression [67]. Taken together, our result of the negative correlation in the mother-positive condition may suggest that through the function of the caudal ACC, autobiographical memories of positive events with the mother may act as a factor that prevents depression.

The precuneus, a central hub of the default mode network, also exhibited a similar interaction effect between the parent and situation during the childhood memory recollection task. The precuneus has been thought to play a critical role in episodic memory [68,69], source monitoring [70], and self-referential processing [71]. Recently, a multivariate analysis method, representational similarity analysis, demonstrated that the precuneus is a key region engaged during the subjective experience of vivid autobiographical memory reminiscence [72]. Other experiments have confirmed its prominent involvement in the vividness of self-related memories [73,74]. In addition, right lateralization of the precuneus has been shown for highly personal and vivid reminiscence of autobiographical events [72]. Right lateralization is corroborated by the present study, which required participants not only to visualize a given circumstance, but also to delineate the facial expression by either parent. Our results indicate overall greater activation of the right precuneus during recollection of fathers than mothers, and more specifically, its activity was higher when recollecting fathers in negative compared to positive situations. It is interesting to note that despite lower vividness score in the stimulus assessments for fathers than mothers, these findings were observed, suggesting that the perceptions of fathers may be more personal than those of mothers, especially during reminiscence of negative situations. These may indicate the saliency of childhood memories of fathers and particularly how fathers responded during negative circumstances. In conjunction with activity of the caudal ACC, we cautiously conclude that memories of fathers in negative situations have a more adverse effect as the negative impression of fathers is more permanently embedded. This interpretation can also be linked to the results of correlation with self-esteem.

Studies report that the neural structure of personal semantic memories serves as a neural correlate of self-esteem [75,76]. It is postulated that due to its role in using autobiographical memories to choose the applicable self-relevant information, activation of the precuneus is closely related to self-esteem and how one processes given information [76,77]. The current study confirmed the established association between self-esteem and precuneus activity, as we found a negative relationship between one’s self-esteem and precuneus activity during autobiographical reminiscence of fathers in negative events. Recently, the precuneus was identified to correlate positively with evaluation sensitivity, measured by a change in state self-esteem [75]. Together with the evidence supporting the precuneus’ affiliation with evaluation sensitivity, as well as the tendency for individuals with lower self-esteem to be more affected by negative information [76], our result seems to suggest that the heightened activation observed in individuals with lower self-esteem may be due to the saliency of negative memories during recollection and their tendency to be more influenced by negative information.

Our PPI analysis revealed significant increases in functional connectivity of the caudal ACC with the fusiform gyrus and postcentral gyrus for the father-positive condition compared to the mother-positive condition. Given that the fusiform gyrus processes facial identity [78] and emotion [79], our finding may indicate that a greater sense of negative affect was involved while processing facial expressions of fathers than mothers in positive childhood events. This raises the possibility that the negative affect arisen from processing emotional face expressions may be particularly different between the parent in a positive context. Going back to the stimulus assessments, we saw lower vividness, response intensity, and arousal from fathers than mothers. Again, this may be since, generally, mothers are primary caregivers who spend more time with children; thus, one may have more readily available positive memories involving mothers than fathers to access on demand.

Previously, individuals with high trait anxiety have shown to hyperactivate the fusiform gyrus while processing uncertain cues and angry faces [80,81]. Consistent with the activation study, our study revealed that the connectivity strengths between the cingulate cortex and fusiform gyrus during the father-positive versus mother-positive condition correlated positively with the severity of anxiety symptoms. These results may provide the neurobiological influence of trait anxiety in the retrieval of memory and the perception of fathers. In addition to emotional face processing, greater connectivity between the cingulate area and postcentral gyrus was witnessed for the father-positive than mother-positive condition. Functional connectivity between these two regions has been implicated in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), where patients with PTSD showed stronger connectivity than the control group, and also was positively correlated with the severity of symptoms [82]. As part of the sensorimotor network [83], the coupling of the caudal ACC and postcentral or paracentral gyri has been shown to be involved in shock processing [84] and other emotional memory tasks [85]. Based on the evidence, we postulate that the caudal ACC may relay negatively valenced affective information to the sensorimotor area more for fathers than mothers when processing facial expressions.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Responses to Reviewers 0316_HEK.docx

Decision Letter 1

Luca Cerniglia

27 Mar 2020

The neural influence of autobiographical memory related to the parent-child relationship on psychological health in adulthood

PONE-D-19-30869R1

Dear Authors,

We are pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it complies with all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you will receive an e-mail containing information on the amendments required prior to publication. When all required modifications have been addressed, you will receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will proceed to our production department and be scheduled for publication.

Shortly after the formal acceptance letter is sent, an invoice for payment will follow. To ensure an efficient production and billing process, please log into Editorial Manager at https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the "Update My Information" link at the top of the page, and update your user information. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, you must inform our press team as soon as possible and no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

With kind regards,

Luca Cerniglia, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

The authors were responsive to all comments. I think the manuscript can be published in the present version.

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Luca Cerniglia

31 Mar 2020

PONE-D-19-30869R1

The neural influence of autobiographical memory related to the parent-child relationship on psychological health in adulthood

Dear Dr. Kim:

I am pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

For any other questions or concerns, please email plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE.

With kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Luca Cerniglia

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. The list of situations presented in the experiment.

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Responses to Reviewers 0316_HEK.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES