This cohort study assesses whether blood pressure levels are associated with racial differences in cognitive decline.
Key Points
Question
Do black individuals’ higher cumulative blood pressure levels contribute to their greater risk of cognitive impairment and dementia compared with white individuals?
Findings
In this pooled cohort analysis of 19 378 participants, black individuals, compared with white individuals, had significantly faster declines in global cognition. Differences between black and white individuals in global cognition decline were no longer statistically significant after adjusting for cumulative mean systolic blood pressure.
Meaning
Black individuals’ higher cumulative blood pressure levels may explain racial disparities in cognitive decline.
Abstract
Importance
Black individuals are more likely than white individuals to develop dementia. Whether higher blood pressure (BP) levels in black individuals explain differences between black and white individuals in dementia risk is uncertain.
Objective
To determine whether cumulative BP levels explain racial differences in cognitive decline.
Design, Setting, and Participants
Individual participant data from 5 cohorts (January 1971 to December 2017) were pooled from the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study, Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults Study, Cardiovascular Health Study, Framingham Offspring Study, and Northern Manhattan Study. Outcomes were standardized as t scores (mean [SD], 50 [10]); a 1-point difference represented a 0.1-SD difference in cognition. The median (interquartile range) follow-up was 12.4 (5.9-21.0) years. Analysis began September 2018.
Main Outcomes and Measures
The primary outcome was change in global cognition, and secondary outcomes were change in memory and executive function.
Exposures
Race (black vs white).
Results
Among 34 349 participants, 19 378 individuals who were free of stroke and dementia and had longitudinal BP, cognitive, and covariate data were included in the analysis. The mean (SD) age at first cognitive assessment was 59.8 (10.4) years and ranged from 5 to 95 years. Of 19 378 individuals, 10 724 (55.3%) were female and 15 526 (80.1%) were white. Compared with white individuals, black individuals had significantly faster declines in global cognition (−0.03 points per year faster [95% CI, −0.05 to −0.01]; P = .004) and memory (−0.08 points per year faster [95% CI, −0.11 to −0.06]; P < .001) but significantly slower declines in executive function (0.09 points per year slower [95% CI, 0.08-0.10]; P < .001). Time-dependent cumulative mean systolic BP level was associated with significantly faster declines in global cognition (−0.018 points per year faster per each 10–mm Hg increase [95% CI, −0.023 to −0.014]; P < .001), memory (−0.028 points per year faster per each 10–mm Hg increase [95% CI, −0.035 to −0.021]; P < .001), and executive function (−0.01 points per year faster per each 10–mm Hg increase [95% CI, −0.014 to −0.007]; P < .001). After adjusting for cumulative mean systolic BP, differences between black and white individuals in cognitive slopes were attenuated for global cognition (−0.01 points per year [95% CI, −0.03 to 0.01]; P = .56) and memory (−0.06 points per year [95% CI, −0.08 to −0.03]; P < .001) but not executive function (0.10 points per year [95% CI, 0.09-0.11]; P < .001).
Conclusions and Relevance
These results suggest that black individuals’ higher cumulative BP levels may contribute to racial differences in later-life cognitive decline.
Introduction
Understanding and reducing black individuals’ higher rate of cognitive impairment and dementia (CID) is an urgent public health priority.1,2 Older black individuals are 2 times more likely than older white individuals to have CID, including Alzheimer disease and related dementias.3,4,5,6 Preventing or delaying CID can lead to better survival, less disability, less nursing home use and family burden, lower health care costs, and better quality of life, all of which are worse in black individuals than in white individuals in the US.5
High blood pressure (BP) level, particularly in midlife, is associated with increased risk for CID.7,8 Black individuals have an earlier average age of onset and a greater severity of high BP levels than white individuals,9 making BP a potential leading contributor to racial disparities in health.10,11 Black individuals are also more likely than white individuals to have detrimental brain effects associated with high BP levels,12 including stroke13 and increased white matter hyperintensity volume.14,15 Evidence is growing that the effect of high BP on cognition16,17 and racial disparities in BP control10 might begin in young adulthood. The extent to which the cumulative effects of elevated BP levels explain the greater risk for CID in black individuals is uncertain.
Leveraging 5 population-based cohorts of individuals aged 5 to 95 years at cohort baseline with repeated objective measures of BP and cognition, we conducted a pooled cohort study to determine the extent to which differences between black and white individuals in cognitive decline are explained by black individuals’ higher cumulative BP levels. We hypothesized that differences between black and white individuals in cumulative BP levels contribute to racial disparities in cognitive decline.
Methods
Study Design, Participants, and Measurements
This pooled cohort analysis examined individual participant data from 5 well-characterized US prospective cohort studies with repeated measures of BP and cognition: Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study (ARIC),18 Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults Study (CARDIA),19 Cardiovascular Health Study (CHS),20 Framingham Offspring Study (FOS),21 and Northern Manhattan Study (NOMAS)22 for 1971 to 2017 (eMethods in the Supplement). The University of Michigan institutional review board approved this study. Participating institutions approved the cohort studies. Participants provided written informed consent, and minors provided assent. Data were collected from January 1971 to December 2017.
We required 2 or more measurements of cognition and 1 or more measurement of BP at or before the first cognition measurement. We excluded participants reporting a baseline history of stroke and those with incident stroke or cohort-defined incident dementia at or before the first cognitive measurement.
Cognitive Function Assessments
Trained cohort staff administered cognitive function tests longitudinally in person to participants using cognitive tests validated in black and white individuals23,24 and consistent with the Vascular Cognitive Impairment Harmonization Standards.25 In 3 cohorts (ARIC,18 CHS,20 and NOMAS22), trained staff administered global cognitive function tests (but not memory or executive function tests) by telephone for participants unable to attend some examination visits in person. Cognitive tests can be measured reliably and precisely over the telephone in adults with comparable results.26
To make inferences about cognitive domains instead of individual cognitive test items and to resolve the challenge of different cognitive tests administered across the cohorts, we cocalibrated available cognitive test items into factors representing global cognition (global cognitive performance), memory (learning and delayed recall/recognition), and executive function (complex and/or speeded cognitive functions) using item response theory methods that leverage all available cognitive information in common across cohorts and test items unique to particular cohorts.27 Cognitive outcomes were set to a t score metric (mean [SD], 50 [10]) at a participant’s first cognitive assessment; a 1-point difference represented a 0.1-SD difference in the distribution of cognition across the 5 cohorts. Higher cognitive scores indicate better performance (eMethods in the Supplement). The primary outcome was change in global cognition. Secondary outcomes were change in memory and executive function.
Measurement of Race
Participants self-reported race. We excluded participants who reported race other than black or white and those reporting Hispanic ethnicity from NOMAS.22
Measurement of Blood Pressure
Each cohort study measured BP at in-person visits using standard protocols and equipment. We summarized systolic BP (SBP) as the time-dependent cumulative mean of all BPs before each cognitive measurement. Long-term cumulative mean SBP has improved prediction of clinical outcomes compared with single BP measurements28 or mean BP over discrete intervals (eg, ≤1 year, 1-5 years) before outcome measurement.29 We used SBP because it tends to be a stronger predictor of BP-related outcomes than diastolic BP.8,29,30
Covariates
We used covariates measured closest to, but not after, the first cognitive assessment. Demographics included age, sex, years of school, and cohort study. Vascular risk factors included alcohol use, cigarette smoking, body mass index, waist circumference, physical activity, fasting glucose level, low-density lipoprotein cholesterol, and history of atrial fibrillation. Cohorts measured current hypertension medication use by evidence of medication bottles and self-report (eMethods in the Supplement).
Statistical Analysis
Following a prespecified analysis plan, we compared participant characteristics by race using a 2-sample t test with equal variance, Wilcoxon rank sum test, or χ2 test as appropriate. Linear mixed-effects models measured changes in each continuous cognitive outcome over time by race. The models included covariates in Table 1, interaction terms for age at the time of first cognitive assessment × follow-up time, sex × follow-up time, and race × follow-up time, as well as participant-specific random effects for intercepts and slopes. All continuous variables were centered at the overall median, except cumulative mean SBP, which was centered at 120 mm Hg. Glucose, low-density lipoprotein cholesterol, and SBP values were divided by 10 so that the parameter estimates reflect a 10-unit change in the variables. Time was treated as a continuous measure defined as years since first measurement of each cognitive outcome.
Table 1. Characteristics of Participants at First Cognitive Assessment by Race: 1971 to 2017.
Variable | No. (%) | |
---|---|---|
Black individuals (n = 3852) | White individuals (n = 15 526) | |
Age at first SBP measurement, y | ||
Range | 17-94 | 5-95 |
Median (IQR) | 51.0 (29.0-63.0) | 54.0 (45.0-64.0) |
Age at first cognitive assessment, y | ||
Range | 41-95 | 25-96 |
Median (IQR) | 54.8 (50.1-65.8) | 59.0 (52.0-67.6) |
Female | 2433 (63.2) | 8291 (53.4) |
Cohort | ||
ARIC18 | 1857 (48.2) | 7313 (47.1) |
CARDIA19 | 1051 (27.3) | 1285 (8.3) |
CHS20 | 562 (14.6) | 3599 (23.2) |
FOS21 | 0 (0.0) | 3016 (19.4) |
NOMAS22 | 382 (9.9) | 316 (2.0) |
Education | ||
≤8th Grade | 424 (11.0) | 849 (5.5) |
9th -11th Grade | 605 (15.7) | 1325 (8.5) |
Completed high school | 937 (24.3) | 5003 (32.2) |
Some college but no degree | 736 (19.1) | 2639 (17.0) |
≥College graduate | 1150 (29.9) | 5710 (36.8) |
Alcoholic drinks/wk | ||
0 | 2596 (67.4) | 7561 (48.7) |
1-6 | 834 (21.7) | 4909 (31.6) |
7-13 | 231 (6.0) | 1674 (10.8) |
≥14 | 191 (5.0) | 1382 (8.9) |
Current cigarette smoking | 816 (21.2) | 2535 (16.3) |
Any physical activity | 2582 (67.0) | 12907 (83.1) |
BMI, median (IQR) | 29.1 (25.8-33.4) | 26.4 (23.7-29.5) |
Waist circumference, median (IQR), cm | 97.0 (88.0-107.0) | 94.0 (85.0-103.0) |
History of atrial fibrillation | 50 (1.3) | 264 (1.7) |
Fasting glucose, median (IQR), mg/dL | 97.5 (90.0-108.0) | 98.0 (91.5-105.9) |
LDL cholesterol, median (IQR), mg/dL | 123.0 (101.0-148.8) | 127.0 (105.6-150.8) |
Antihypertensive medication use | 1631 (42.3) | 4087 (26.3) |
Follow-up time from first cognitive assessment, median (IQR), y | 6.0 (5.3-20.8) | 14.9 (6.0-21.0) |
SBP at first cognitive assessment, cumulative mean (SD), mm Hg | 141.3 (18.3) | 137.0 (18.8) |
No. of SBP measurements, median (IQR) | ||
Total | 4.0 (3.0-7.0) | 5.0 (4.0-8.0) |
Before first cognitive assessment | 1.0 (1.0-5.0) | 1.0 (1.0-3.0) |
Time from first SBP measurement to first cognitive assessment, median (IQR), y | 2.9 (2.7-24.3) | 2.9 (2.7-19.1) |
Cognitive scores at first assessment, median (IQR) | ||
General cognitive performance | 47.6 (42.0-52.8) | 53.3 (48.7-58.3) |
Executive function | 47.6 (41.6-52.9) | 53.0 (47.9-58.0) |
Memory | 48.9 (45.7-52.4) | 52.4 (48.9-55.9) |
Cognitive scores at first assessment, mean (SD) | ||
General cognitive performance | 47.5 (8.0) | 53.3 (7.0) |
Executive function | 47.4 (7.9) | 53.0 (7.2) |
Memory | 49.1 (6.5) | 52.1 (5.4) |
Abbreviations: ARIC, Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study; BMI, body mass index (calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared); CARDIA, Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults Study; CHS, Cardiovascular Health Study; FOS, Framingham Offspring Study; IQR, interquartile range; LDL, low-density lipoprotein; NOMAS, Northern Manhattan Study; SBP, systolic blood pressure.
SI conversion factors: To convert glucose to mmol/L, multiply by 0.0555; LDL cholesterol to mmol/L, multiply by 0.0259.
For each outcome, all available cognitive observations were used in the primary analysis except observations after the time of first cohort-adjudicated incident stroke during follow-up because incident stroke alters the cognitive trajectory.31 We evaluated model assumptions by inspecting residual plots. There was no evidence of nonlinear effects of SBP or other covariates on cognitive trajectories, or a significant interaction term for race × SBP × follow-up time in models (P > .05).
To estimate differences between black and white individuals in cognitive decline, model A included a black race × follow-up time interaction term. To examine whether time-dependent cumulative mean SBP explained the racial differences in cognitive decline, model B added SBP and an SBP × follow-up time interaction term to model A. To investigate whether hypertension treatment explained the racial differences in cognitive decline, model C added hypertension treatment and a hypertension treatment at first cognitive assessment × follow-up time interaction term to model B.
Given the large sample size, we put aside a 15% random subsample of participants within each cohort for model validation. We present results from the derivation cohort. We performed a complete case analysis excluding a small number of participants (n = 501) from the analytical data set owing to missing values in covariates. The Figure shows the derivation of the cohort. Statistical significance for all analyses was set as P < .05 (2-sided). All analyses were performed using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute). Analysis began September 2018.
Figure. Participant Cohort.
BP indicates blood pressure.
aCategories for missing data on covariates are not mutually exclusive. Missing data for covariates included glucose (n = 240), alcohol use (n = 18), body mass index (n = 24), waist circumference (n = 77), smoking (n = 2), physical activity (n = 18), low-density lipoprotein cholesterol (n = 259), antihypertensive medication use (n = 14), and education (n = 128). No participants were missing history of atrial fibrillation.
Sensitivity Analyses
We repeated analyses including participants’ cognitive observations after the time of incident stroke and also after adding kidney function (glomerular filtration rate32) and history of myocardial infarction because they may be on the causal pathway. We repeated analyses within cohorts to assess heterogeneity in the associations between race and BP with cognitive decline.
Results
The study sample included 19 378 participants (10 724 [55.3%] were female and 15 526 [80.1%] were white). Table 1 presents characteristics of participants by race. During a median (interquartile range) follow-up of 12.4 (5.9-21.0) years, the median (interquartile range) number of global cognition and memory assessments was 2 (2-4) for black individuals and 4 (2-7) for white individuals. The median (interquartile range) number of executive function assessments was 2 (2-3) for black individuals and 3 (2-3) for white individuals. eTable 1 in the Supplement shows characteristics of study participants by cohort. Because the secondary outcome measures were performed less frequently, the memory analysis included 13 145 participants, and the executive function analysis included 17 254 participants.
Change in Global Cognition
Black individuals, compared with white individuals, had significantly faster declines in global cognition (model A, Table 2). White men at a median age of 56 years experienced a decline in global cognition of −0.23 points per year (95% CI, −0.24 to −0.21; P < .001). Black men of similar age experienced a decline in global cognition of −0.26 points per year (95% CI, −0.29 to −0.22) (adjusted difference in slope between black individuals and white individuals: −0.03 points per year faster [95% CI, −0.05 to −0.01]; P = .004).
Table 2. Association of Global Cognition Decline With Race and Systolic Blood Pressure Over Time, 1971 to 2017a.
Coefficient | Model A: basic | Model B: cumulative mean SBP added as a time-varying covariate to model A | Model C: hypertension treatment added as a covariate to model B | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Estimate (95% CI) | P value | Estimate (95% CI) | P value | Estimate (95% CI) | P value | |
Change in intercept per 10-y increase in age at first cognitive assessment | −2.22 (−2.35 to −2.09) | <.001 | −2.01 (−2.15 to −1.88) | <.001 | −2.01 (−2.14 to −1.88) | <.001 |
Difference in intercept between black and white individuals at first cognitive assessment | −5.74 (−5.98 to −5.51) | <.001 | −5.68 (−5.92 to −5.44) | <.001 | −5.68 (−5.92 to −5.44) | <.001 |
Change in slope per 10-y increase in age at first cognitive assessment per y | −0.123 (−0.131 to −0.115) | <.001 | −0.125 (−0.133 to −0.116) | <.001 | −0.13 (−0.14 to −0.12) | <.001 |
Slope in white men at median age per y | −0.23 (−0.24 to −0.21) | <.001 | −0.20 (−0.21 to −0.19) | <.001 | −0.20 (−0.22 to −0.19) | <.001 |
Difference in slope between black and white individuals per y | −0.03 (−0.05 to −0.01) | .004 | −0.01 (−0.03 to 0.01) | .56 | −0.01 (−0.03 to 0.01) | .41 |
Change in slope per 10–mm Hg in cumulative mean SBP per y | NA | NA | −0.018 (−0.023 to −0.014) | <.001 | −0.020 (−0.024 to −0.015) | <.001 |
Change in slope associated with hypertension treatment per y | NA | NA | NA | NA | 0.02 (0.002 to 0.04) | .03 |
Abbreviations: NA, not applicable; SBP, systolic blood pressure.
N = 19 378. Global cognition measures global cognitive performance. All cognitive measures are set to a t score metric (mean [SD], 50 [10]) at a participant’s first cognitive assessment; a 1-point difference represents a 0.1-SD difference in the distribution of cognition across the 5 cohorts. Higher cognitive scores indicate better performance. Linear mixed-effects models included time since first cognitive assessment and baseline values (measured before or at time of first cognitive assessment) of race (black vs white), age, sex, cohort study, years of school, alcohol use, cigarette smoking, body mass index, waist circumference, physical activity, fasting glucose, low-density lipoprotein (LDL) cholesterol, history of atrial fibrillation, age × time, sex × time, and race × time. To take into account correlation between longitudinal cognitive measures, we included random intercept and slope effects associated with participants. All continuous variables were centered at the overall median, except cumulative mean SBP, which was centered at 120 mm Hg. Glucose, LDL cholesterol, and SBP values were divided by 10 so that the parameter estimates refer to a 10-unit change in the variables. Systolic blood pressure was the time-dependent mean of all SBPs before the measurement of cognition. To estimate differences between black and white individuals in cognitive decline, model A included a black race × follow-up time interaction term. To examine whether cumulative mean SBP, a time-varying covariate, explained the racial differences in cognitive decline, model B added SBP and an SBP × follow-up time interaction term to model A. To investigate whether hypertension treatment explained the racial differences in cognitive decline, model C added a hypertension treatment and hypertension treatment at first cognitive assessment × follow-up time interaction term to model B.
After adjusting for cumulative mean SBP, differences between black and white individuals in the decline in global cognition were no longer statistically significant (adjusted difference in slope between black and white individuals: −0.01 points [95% CI, −0.03 to 0.01]; P = .56) (model B, Table 2). Cumulative mean SBP was associated with faster declines in global cognition (−0.018 points per year faster per each 10–mm Hg increase [95% CI, −0.023 to −0.014]; P < .001). Further adjustment for antihypertensive medication use did not change the racial difference in decline in global cognition, although antihypertensive medication use was associated with slower declines in global cognition (0.02 points per year slower [95% CI, 0.002-0.04]; P = .03) (model C, Table 2).
Change in Memory
Black individuals, compared with white individuals, had significantly faster declines in memory (adjusted difference in slope between black and white individuals: −0.08 points per year faster [95% CI, −0.11 to −0.06]; P < .001) (model A, Table 3). After adjusting for cumulative mean SBP, differences between black and white individuals in memory decline were partially attenuated (adjusted difference in slope between black and white individuals: −0.06 points [95% CI, −0.08 to −0.03]; P < .001) (model B, Table 3). Cumulative mean SBP was associated with faster declines in memory (−0.028 points per year faster per each 10–mm Hg increase [95% CI, −0.035 to −0.021]; P < .001). Further adjustment for antihypertensive medication use did not change race differences in memory decline, and antihypertensive medication use was not associated with memory declines (change in slope associated with hypertension treatment, −0.003 points per year faster [95% CI, −0.03 to 0.02]; P = .84) (model C, Table 3).
Table 3. Association of Memory Decline With Race and Systolic Blood Pressure Over Time, 1971 to 2017a.
Coefficient | Model A: basic | Model B: cumulative mean SBP added as a time-varying covariate to model A | Model C: hypertension treatment added as a covariate to model B | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Estimate (95% CI) | P value | Estimate (95% CI) | P value | Estimate (95% CI) | P value | |
Change in intercept per 10-y increase in age at first cognitive assessment | −0.99 (−1.16 to −0.81) | <.001 | −1.05 (−1.23 to −0.88) | <.001 | −1.04 (−1.22 to −0.86) | <.001 |
Difference in intercept between black and white individuals at first cognitive assessment | −2.97 (−3.22 to −2.72) | <.001 | −3.04 (−3.30 to −2.79) | <.001 | −3.01 (−3.27 to −2.76) | <.001 |
Change in slope per 10-y increase in age at first cognitive assessment per y | −0.20 (−0.22 to −0.19) | <.001 | −0.18 (−0.20 to −0.16) | <.001 | −0.18 (−0.20 to −0.16) | <.001 |
Slope in white men at median age per y | −0.25 (−0.26 to −0.23) | <.001 | −0.19 (−0.21 to −0.17) | <.001 | −0.19 (−0.21 to −0.17) | <.001 |
Difference in slope between black and white individuals per y | −0.08 (−0.11 to −0.06) | <.001 | −0.06 (−0.08 to −0.03) | <.001 | −0.06 (−0.08 to −0.03) | <.001 |
Change in slope per 10–mm Hg in cumulative mean SBP per y | NA | NA | −0.028 (−0.035 to −0.021) | <.001 | −0.028 (−0.035 to −0.020) | <.001 |
Change in slope associated with hypertension treatment per y | NA | NA | NA | NA | −0.003 (−0.03 to 0.02) | .84 |
Abbreviations: NA, not applicable; SBP, systolic blood pressure.
n = 13 145. Memory measures learning and delayed recall/recognition. All cognitive measures are set to a t score metric (mean [SD], 50 [10]) at a participant’s first cognitive assessment; a 1-point difference represents a 0.1-SD difference in the distribution of cognition across the 5 cohorts. Higher cognitive scores indicate better performance. Linear mixed-effects models included time since first cognitive assessment and baseline values (measured before or at time of first cognitive assessment) of race (black vs white), age, sex, cohort study, years of school, alcohol use, cigarette smoking, body mass index, waist circumference, physical activity, fasting glucose, low-density lipoprotein (LDL) cholesterol, history of atrial fibrillation, age × time, sex × time, and race × time. To take into account correlation between longitudinal cognitive measures, we included random intercept and slope effects associated with participants. All continuous variables were centered at the overall median, except cumulative mean SBP, which was centered at 120 mm Hg. Glucose, LDL cholesterol, and SBP values were divided by 10 so that the parameter estimates refer to a 10-unit change in the variables. Systolic blood pressure was the time-dependent mean of all SBPs before the measurement of cognition. To estimate differences between black and white individuals in cognitive decline, model A included a black race × follow-up time interaction term. To examine whether cumulative mean SBP, a time-varying covariate, explained the racial differences in cognitive decline, model B added SBP and an SBP × follow-up time interaction term to model A. To investigate whether hypertension treatment explained the racial differences in cognitive decline, model C added a hypertension treatment and hypertension treatment at first cognitive assessment × follow-up time interaction term to model B.
Change in Executive Function
Black individuals, compared with white individuals, had significantly slower executive function declines (adjusted difference in slope between black and white individuals: 0.09 points per year slower [95% CI, 0.08-0.10]; P < .001) (model A, Table 4). After adjusting for cumulative mean SBP, differences between black and white individuals in executive function decline slightly increased (adjusted difference in slope between black and white individuals: 0.10 points [95% CI, 0.09-0.11]; P < .001) (model B, Table 4). Cumulative mean SBP was associated with faster declines in executive function (−0.01 points per year faster per each 10–mm Hg increase [95% CI, −0.014 to −0.007]; P < .001). Further adjustment for antihypertensive medication use did not change the racial differences in executive function decline, and antihypertensive medication use was not associated with declines in executive function (change in slope associated with hypertension treatment, 0.01 points per year slower [95% CI, −0.01 to 0.02]; P = .25) (model C, Table 4).
Table 4. Association of Executive Function Decline With Race and Systolic Blood Pressure Over Time, 1971 to 2017a.
Coefficient | Model A: basic | Model B: cumulative mean SBP added as a time-varying covariate to model A | Model C: hypertension treatment added as a covariate to model B | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Estimate (95% CI) | P value | Estimate (95% CI) | P value | Estimate (95% CI) | P value | |
Change in intercept per 10-y increase in age at first cognitive assessment | −2.72 (−2.86 to −2.58) | <.001 | −2.49 (−2.63 to −2.34) | <.001 | −2.48 (−2.62 to −2.33) | <.001 |
Difference in intercept between black and white individuals at first cognitive assessment | −6.28 (−6.51 to −6.04) | <.001 | −6.22 (−6.45 to −5.98) | <.001 | −6.19 (−6.43 to −5.95) | <.001 |
Change in slope per 10-y increase in age at first cognitive assessment per y | −0.011 (−0.018 to −0.005) | <.001 | −0.017 (−0.023 to −0.010) | <.001 | −0.017 (−0.024 to −0.011) | <.001 |
Slope in white men at median age per y | −0.35 (−0.36 to −0.34) | <.001 | −0.33 (−0.34 to −0.32) | <.001 | −0.33 (−0.34 to −0.32) | <.001 |
Difference in slope between black and white individuals per y | 0.09 (0.08 to 0.10) | <.001 | 0.10 (0.09 to 0.11) | <.001 | 0.10 (0.09 to 0.11) | <.001 |
Change in slope per 10–mm Hg in cumulative mean SBP per y | NA | NA | −0.01 (−0.014 to −0.007) | <.001 | −0.01 (−0.015 to −0.008) | <.001 |
Change in slope associated with hypertension treatment per y | NA | NA | NA | NA | 0.01 (−0.01 to 0.02) | .25 |
Abbreviations: NA, not applicable; SBP, systolic blood pressure.
n = 17 254. Executive function measures complex and/or speeded cognitive functions. All cognitive measures are set to a t score metric (mean [SD], 50 [10]) at a participant’s first cognitive assessment; a 1-point difference represents a 0.1-SD difference in the distribution of cognition across the 5 cohorts. Higher cognitive scores indicate better performance. Linear mixed-effects models included time since first cognitive assessment and baseline values (measured before or at time of first cognitive assessment) of race (black vs white), age, sex, cohort study, years of school, alcohol use, cigarette smoking, body mass index, waist circumference, physical activity, fasting glucose, low-density lipoprotein (LDL) cholesterol, history of atrial fibrillation, age × time, sex × time, and race × time. To take into account correlation between longitudinal cognitive measures, we included random intercept and slope effects associated with participants. All continuous variables were centered at the overall median, except cumulative mean SBP, which was centered at 120 mm Hg. Glucose, LDL cholesterol, and SBP values were divided by 10 so that the parameter estimates refer to a 10-unit change in the variables. Systolic blood pressure was the time-dependent mean of all SBPs before the measurement of cognition. To estimate differences between black and white individuals in cognitive decline, model A included a black race × follow-up time interaction term. To examine whether cumulative mean SBP, a time-varying covariate, explained the racial differences in cognitive decline, model B added SBP and an SBP × follow-up time interaction term to model A. To investigate whether hypertension treatment explained the racial differences in cognitive decline, model C added a hypertension treatment and hypertension treatment at first cognitive assessment × follow-up time interaction term to model B.
Sensitivity Analyses
Results were similar in analyses including participants’ cognitive observations after the time of incident stroke and adding glomerular filtration rate and history of myocardial infarction as covariates (eTables 2 and 3 in the Supplement). There was heterogeneity in the associations between race and BP with cognitive decline across cohorts (eTable 4 in the Supplement).
Discussion
Among 19 378 individuals pooled from 5 prospective cohort studies, black individuals’ higher BP levels were associated with faster decline, compared with white individuals, in global cognition and memory but not executive function. Antihypertensive medication use did not change the differences between black and white individuals in cognitive decline independent from their association with BP reduction. Systolic BP had a linear effect on cognitive decline, so lower SBP levels are associated with slower cognitive decline. We found no evidence that the magnitude of the association of SBP with cognitive decline differed between white and black individuals.
Our results, when combined with previous research,8 provide evidence suggesting that black individuals’ higher cumulative BP levels contribute to differences between black and white individuals in cognitive decline. A previous study33 suggested that black individuals’ lower socioeconomic status (eg, education and literacy) largely explain their higher rates of CID. We found that black individuals’ faster declines in global cognition and memory than white individuals persisted after adjusting for years of school and vascular risk factors and were partially attenuated after adjusting for cumulative mean BP. Our results might differ because we included young and middle-aged adults, included repeated BP measurements, measured cognitive trajectories (not incident dementia33) and controlled for age and sex differences in cognitive decline.
If causal, the differences between black and white individuals in the declines in global cognition and memory due to long-term BP levels we observed, equivalent to 2.5 to 4 years of cognitive aging, would undoubtedly be clinically significant. The faster declines in mean cognitive scores associated with black race can be associated with approximate equivalent changes in years of brain or cognitive aging by calculating the ratio of slope coefficients for black race and age on cognition. Declines in global cognition and memory substantially increase the risk of death, dementia, and functional disability.34,35,36 The combination of black individuals’ lower initial cognitive scores and faster declines in global cognition and memory mean they would reach a threshold of clinical dementia earlier in adulthood than white individuals.
Black individuals might be more likely than white individuals to experience deleterious brain effects from elevated BP levels owing to earlier age of onset, longer duration, greater severity, and worse BP control over the life course. Black individuals’ disproportionate burden of elevated BP levels across the life span might lead to greater severity of arterial stiffness, vascular brain injury, small vessel disease, and atherosclerosis12,13,14,15 that in turn cause cognitive decline through mechanisms of inflammation, oxidative stress, and damage to white matter integrity37,38,39 as well as neurodegeneration.40,41 There is a compelling need to study the mechanisms by which age of onset, duration, and severity of BP levels over the life course contribute to racial differences in the risk of cognitive decline.
The associations of race and BP with cognitive decline are modest, and pooling individual participant data from multiple cohorts enhanced the detection of these associations. We found a significant average difference in cognitive decline by race for 2 of 3 outcomes (global cognition and memory) and a significant average difference in cognitive decline by SBP for all cognitive outcomes. We were surprised that black individuals had slower declines in executive function than white individuals because executive function has been associated with vascular disease.8 Causes of persistent racial differences in decline in memory and executive function are uncertain and might include socioeconomic factors not captured by adjustment for years of education (eg, literacy,33 quality of education,42 childhood socioeconomic status, wealth) as well as dietary or lifestyle factors and early-life exposures. It is also possible that the memory and executive function measures are more sensitive than the global cognition measure to detect racial differences in cognitive decline.
Our findings of heterogeneity of white race, black race, and BP-related differences in cognitive decline across cognitive domains and cohorts are consistent with prior evidence.7,8,43 Some studies,43,44 but not all,7 have shown differences between black and white individuals in cognitive decline similar to those we observed in the CARDIA19 and NOMAS22 cohorts. Race categories can be a proxy for differences in social determinants of health that vary across populations. The potential reasons for the heterogeneity of the associations of race and BP with cognitive trajectories are unclear and include unmeasured socioeconomic, geographic, environmental, and early-life factors as well as cohort differences in sampling strategies, eligibility criteria, and cognitive tests. It is plausible that the racial differences in cognitive decline in the present study are driven by observations in younger adults where differences between black and white individuals’ BP levels and incident hypertension are largest.9,10,45 Indeed, in the youngest cohort, CARDIA,19 racial differences in cognitive decline were present and attenuated after adjustment for cumulative mean SBP for global cognition, memory, and executive function. High and increasing BP from early adulthood into midlife is associated with increased white matter hyperintensity volume and smaller brain volumes later in life.46
Strengths and Limitations
Our study has several strengths. We had longitudinal BP and cognitive assessments in a large number of young, middle-aged, and older adults to estimate the effects of cumulative BP levels on cognitive decline in black and white individuals. The cohort studies systematically measured cognitive domains commonly affected by vascular factors such as hypertension, global cognition, memory, and executive function.25 We had repeated cognitive measures during up to 21 years of follow-up.
Our study has potential limitations. We adjusted for educational years but not for other socioeconomic factors33,42 or depressive symptoms because they were unavailable for all cohorts at or before the first cognitive assessment. Studies suggest that socioeconomic factors tend to influence baseline cognition (intercept) rather than the change in cognition over time (slopes).47,48 Selective attrition of participants with cognitive impairments could underestimate the rate of cognitive decline49 or not.50 Estimating the potential clinical effect of racial differences in cognitive decline by correlating with decline owing to aging is a common approach, but it does not directly measure clinical impact, and a clinically meaningful change might vary by an individual’s age, educational quality, sex, and baseline cognition.51 Black individuals were more likely to be excluded than white individuals because of stroke or dementia before first cognitive assessment; however, this would reduce racial differences in cognitive decline. We did not study incident dementia because some cohort studies lacked these data. By study design, we did not adjust for baseline cognition.52 We did not study any particular age interval or BP level associated with greatest risk of BP-related cognitive decline. Race was self-reported. Heterogeneity of the effect between cohorts might affect the statistical validity of the summary estimate of the effect in the pooled cohort. Smaller sample size and fewer cognitive assessments might reduce precision of estimates of cognitive decline in black individuals.
Conclusions
Our study has policy implications. Although prominent organizations53 recommend improved BP treatment and control in black individuals to reduce cardiovascular disease risk and disparities between black and white individuals in cardiovascular outcomes, our results suggest that improved BP control may also reduce black individuals’ risk of accelerated cognitive decline and racial disparities in cognitive outcomes. Between 2014 and 2060, the number of black individuals in the United States is expected to increase from 42 million to 60 million.54 Along with early life interventions to improve racial differences in baseline cognitive performance,55,56,57 our results suggest that effective interventions58 to improve BP control in black individuals may also be strategies to reduce racial disparities in cognitive decline.59 The critical need is that adults, especially black individuals, receive standard and low-cost treatments to control high BP levels. Our results also suggest that cognitive decline and CID are potential outcomes for health care systems and payers to evaluate the effectiveness and costs of BP-lowering interventions. These results suggest that black individuals’ higher cumulative BP levels and worse control contribute to differences between black and white individuals in later-life cognitive decline.
eMethods.
eTable 1. Characteristics of Participants at First Cognitive Assessment by Race in Pooled Cohort Sample by Cohort, 1971 to 2017
eTable 2. Sensitivity Analysis of Association of Global Cognition Decline with Race and Systolic Blood Pressure over Time Including Cognitive Observations after Incident Stroke, 1971 to 2017
eTable 3. Sensitivity Analysis of Association of Global Cognition Decline with Race and Systolic Blood Pressure over Time Including Kidney Function and History of Myocardial Infarction as Covariates, 1971 to 2017
eTable 4. Association of Cognitive Decline with Race and Systolic Blood Pressure over Time by Cohort, 1971 to 2017
eReferences.
References
- 1.Alzheimer’s Association 2019 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement. 2019;15(3):321-387. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2019.01.010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Alzheimer’s Disease-Related Dementias Conference and recommendations report to the NINDS council. Published September 12, 2013. Accessed July 1, 2019. https://www.ninds.nih.gov/sites/default/files/ADRD_2013_Report-and-Memorandum_508comp_0.pdf
- 3.Gurland BJ, Wilder DE, Lantigua R, et al. Rates of dementia in three ethnoracial groups. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 1999;14(6):481-493. doi: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kuller LH, Lopez OL, Jagust WJ, et al. Determinants of vascular dementia in the Cardiovascular Health Cognition Study. Neurology. 2005;64(9):1548-1552. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000160115.55756.DE [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Plassman BL, Langa KM, Fisher GG, et al. Prevalence of cognitive impairment without dementia in the United States. Ann Intern Med. 2008;148(6):427-434. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-148-6-200803180-00005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Tang MX, Cross P, Andrews H, et al. Incidence of AD in African-Americans, Caribbean Hispanics, and Caucasians in northern Manhattan. Neurology. 2001;56(1):49-56. doi: 10.1212/WNL.56.1.49 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Gottesman RF, Schneider AL, Albert M, et al. Midlife hypertension and 20-year cognitive change: the atherosclerosis risk in communities neurocognitive study. JAMA Neurol. 2014;71(10):1218-1227. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.1646 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Levine DA, Galecki AT, Langa KM, et al. Blood pressure and cognitive decline over 8 years in middle-aged and older black and white Americans. Hypertension. 2019;73(2):310-318. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.118.12062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Flack JM, Sica DA, Bakris G, et al. ; International Society on Hypertension in Blacks . Management of high blood pressure in Blacks: an update of the International Society on Hypertension in Blacks consensus statement. Hypertension. 2010;56(5):780-800. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.110.152892 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Fiscella K, Holt K. Racial disparity in hypertension control: tallying the death toll. Ann Fam Med. 2008;6(6):497-502. doi: 10.1370/afm.873 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Wong MD, Shapiro MF, Boscardin WJ, Ettner SL. Contribution of major diseases to disparities in mortality. N Engl J Med. 2002;347(20):1585-1592. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa012979 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Birns J, Morris R, Jarosz J, Markus H, Kalra L. Ethnic differences in the cerebrovascular impact of hypertension. Cerebrovasc Dis. 2008;25(5):408-416. doi: 10.1159/000121341 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Howard G, Lackland DT, Kleindorfer DO, et al. Racial differences in the impact of elevated systolic blood pressure on stroke risk. JAMA Intern Med. 2013;173(1):46-51. doi: 10.1001/2013.jamainternmed.857 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Brickman AM, Schupf N, Manly JJ, et al. Brain morphology in older African Americans, Caribbean Hispanics, and whites from northern Manhattan. Arch Neurol. 2008;65(8):1053-1061. doi: 10.1001/archneur.65.8.1053 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Marcus J, Gardener H, Rundek T, et al. Baseline and longitudinal increases in diastolic blood pressure are associated with greater white matter hyperintensity volume: the northern Manhattan study. Stroke. 2011;42(9):2639-2641. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.111.617571 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Lande MB, Kupferman JC. Blood pressure and cognitive function in children and adolescents. Hypertension. 2019;73(3):532-540. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.118.11686 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Yaffe K, Vittinghoff E, Pletcher MJ, et al. Early adult to midlife cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive function. Circulation. 2014;129(15):1560-1567. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.113.004798 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.The ARIC investigators The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study: design and objectives. Am J Epidemiol. 1989;129(4):687-702. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a115184 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Friedman GD, Cutter GR, Donahue RP, et al. CARDIA: study design, recruitment, and some characteristics of the examined subjects. J Clin Epidemiol. 1988;41(11):1105-1116. doi: 10.1016/0895-4356(88)90080-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Fried LP, Borhani NO, Enright P, et al. The Cardiovascular Health Study: design and rationale. Ann Epidemiol. 1991;1(3):263-276. doi: 10.1016/1047-2797(91)90005-W [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Feinleib M, Kannel WB, Garrison RJ, McNamara PM, Castelli WP. The Framingham Offspring Study: design and preliminary data. Prev Med. 1975;4(4):518-525. doi: 10.1016/0091-7435(75)90037-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Sacco RL, Boden-Albala B, Gan R, et al. Stroke incidence among white, black, and Hispanic residents of an urban community: the Northern Manhattan Stroke Study. Am J Epidemiol. 1998;147(3):259-268. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009445 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ferraro FR, ed. Minority and Cross-Cultural Aspects of Neuropsychological Assessment: Enduring and Emerging Trends. Swets & Zeitlinger; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Lucas JA, Ivnik RJ, Smith GE, et al. Mayo’s older African Americans normative studies: norms for Boston Naming Test, Controlled Oral Word Association, Category Fluency, Animal Naming, Token Test, WRAT-3 Reading, Trail Making Test, Stroop Test, and Judgment of Line Orientation. Clin Neuropsychol. 2005;19(2):243-269. doi: 10.1080/13854040590945337 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hachinski V, Iadecola C, Petersen RC, et al. National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke-Canadian Stroke Network vascular cognitive impairment harmonization standards. Stroke. 2006;37(9):2220-2241. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000237236.88823.47 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Manly JJ, Schupf N, Stern Y, Brickman AM, Tang MX, Mayeux R. Telephone-based identification of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in a multicultural cohort. Arch Neurol. 2011;68(5):607-614. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2011.88 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Samejima F. Estimation of Latent Ability Using a Response Pattern of Graded Scores (Psychometric Monograph No. 17). Psychometric Society; 1969.. Accessed July 1, 2019. https://www.psychometricsociety.org/sites/main/files/file-attachments/mn17.pdf?1576606975 [Google Scholar]
- 28.Pool LR, Ning H, Wilkins J, Lloyd-Jones DM, Allen NB. Use of long-term cumulative blood pressure in cardiovascular risk prediction models. JAMA Cardiol. 2018;3(11):1096-1100. doi: 10.1001/jamacardio.2018.2763 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Lau KK, Li L, Simoni M, Mehta Z, Küker W, Rothwell PM; Oxford Vascular Study . Long-term premorbid blood pressure and cerebral small vessel disease burden on imaging in transient ischemic attack and ischemic stroke. Stroke. 2018;49(9):2053-2060. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.118.021578 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Franklin SS, Jacobs MJ, Wong ND, L’Italien GJ, Lapuerta P. Predominance of isolated systolic hypertension among middle-aged and elderly US hypertensives: analysis based on National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) III. Hypertension. 2001;37(3):869-874. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.37.3.869 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Levine DA, Galecki AT, Langa KM, et al. Trajectory of cognitive decline after incident stroke. JAMA. 2015;314(1):41-51. doi: 10.1001/jama.2015.6968 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Levey AS, Stevens LA, Schmid CH, et al. ; CKD-EPI (Chronic Kidney Disease Epidemiology Collaboration) . A new equation to estimate glomerular filtration rate. Ann Intern Med. 2009;150(9):604-612. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-150-9-200905050-00006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Yaffe K, Falvey C, Harris TB, et al. ; Health ABC Study . Effect of socioeconomic disparities on incidence of dementia among biracial older adults: prospective study. BMJ. 2013;347:f7051. doi: 10.1136/bmj.f7051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Bennett HP, Corbett AJ, Gaden S, Grayson DA, Kril JJ, Broe GA. Subcortical vascular disease and functional decline: a 6-year predictor study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2002;50(12):1969-1977. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50608.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Clark LJ, Gatz M, Zheng L, Chen YL, McCleary C, Mack WJ. Longitudinal verbal fluency in normal aging, preclinical, and prevalent Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2009;24(6):461-468. doi: 10.1177/1533317509345154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Cosentino S, Scarmeas N, Albert SM, Stern Y. Verbal fluency predicts mortality in Alzheimer disease. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2006;19(3):123-129. doi: 10.1097/01.wnn.0000213912.87642.3d [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Coleman CG, Wang G, Faraco G, et al. Membrane trafficking of NADPH oxidase p47(phox) in paraventricular hypothalamic neurons parallels local free radical production in angiotensin II slow-pressor hypertension. J Neurosci. 2013;33(10):4308-4316. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3061-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Faraco G, Iadecola C. Hypertension: a harbinger of stroke and dementia. Hypertension. 2013;62(5):810-817. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.113.01063 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Wang R, Fratiglioni L, Laukka EJ, et al. Effects of vascular risk factors and APOE ε4 on white matter integrity and cognitive decline. Neurology. 2015;84(11):1128-1135. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001379 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Sparks DL, Scheff SW, Liu H, Landers TM, Coyne CM, Hunsaker JC III. Increased incidence of neurofibrillary tangles (NFT) in non-demented individuals with hypertension. J Neurol Sci. 1995;131(2):162-169. doi: 10.1016/0022-510X(95)00105-B [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Launer LJ, Ross GW, Petrovitch H, et al. Midlife blood pressure and dementia: the Honolulu-Asia aging study. Neurobiol Aging. 2000;21(1):49-55. doi: 10.1016/S0197-4580(00)00096-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Sisco S, Gross AL, Shih RA, et al. The role of early-life educational quality and literacy in explaining racial disparities in cognition in late life. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2015;70(4):557-567. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt133 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Hajjar I, Rosenberger KJ, Kulshreshtha A, Ayonayon HN, Yaffe K, Goldstein FC. Association of JNC-8 and SPRINT systolic blood pressure levels With cognitive function and related racial disparity. JAMA Neurol. 2017;74(10):1199-1205. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.1863 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Levine DA, Kabeto M, Langa KM, Lisabeth LD, Rogers MA, Galecki AT. Does stroke contribute to racial differences in cognitive decline? Stroke. 2015;46(7):1897-1902. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.114.008156 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Levine DA, Lewis CE, Williams OD, et al. Geographic and demographic variability in 20-year hypertension incidence: the CARDIA study. Hypertension. 2011;57(1):39-47. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.110.160341 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Lane CA, Barnes J, Nicholas JM, et al. Associations between blood pressure across adulthood and late-life brain structure and pathology in the neuroscience substudy of the 1946 British birth cohort (insight 46): an epidemiological study. Lancet Neurol. 2019;18(10):942-952. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(19)30228-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Jorm AF, Rodgers B, Henderson AS, et al. Occupation type as a predictor of cognitive decline and dementia in old age. Age Ageing. 1998;27(4):477-483. doi: 10.1093/ageing/27.4.477 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Zahodne LB, Glymour MM, Sparks C, et al. Education does not slow cognitive decline with aging: 12-year evidence from the Victoria longitudinal study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17(6):1039-1046. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711001044 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Euser SM, Schram MT, Hofman A, Westendorp RG, Breteler MM. Measuring cognitive function with age: the influence of selection by health and survival. Epidemiology. 2008;19(3):440-447. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e31816a1d31 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Salthouse TA. Selectivity of attrition in longitudinal studies of cognitive functioning. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2014;69(4):567-574. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Stein J, Luppa M, Luck T, et al. The assessment of changes in cognitive functioning: age-, education-, and gender-specific reliable change indices for older adults tested on the CERAD-NP battery: results of the German Study on Ageing, Cognition, and Dementia in Primary Care Patients (AgeCoDe). Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2012;20(1):84-97. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e318209dd08 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Glymour MM, Weuve J, Berkman LF, Kawachi I, Robins JM. When is baseline adjustment useful in analyses of change? an example with education and cognitive change. Am J Epidemiol. 2005;162(3):267-278. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwi187 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Whelton PK, Einhorn PT, Muntner P, et al. ; National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute Working Group on Research Needs to Improve Hypertension Treatment and Control in African Americans . Research needs to improve hypertension treatment and control in African Americans. Hypertension. 2016;68(5):1066-1072. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.116.07905 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Colby SL, Ortman JM. Projections of the size and composition of the U.S. population: 2014 to 2060. Census Bureau; March 2015. Accessed March 19, 2019. https://www.census.gov/content/dam/Census/library/publications/2015/demo/p25-1143.pdf
- 55.Snowdon DA, Kemper SJ, Mortimer JA, Greiner LH, Wekstein DR, Markesbery WR. Linguistic ability in early life and cognitive function and Alzheimer’s disease in late life. Findings from the Nun Study. JAMA. 1996;275(7):528-532. doi: 10.1001/jama.1996.03530310034029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Hair NL, Hanson JL, Wolfe BL, Pollak SD. Association of child poverty, brain development, and academic achievement. JAMA Pediatr. 2015;169(9):822-829. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.1475 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Luby J, Belden A, Botteron K, et al. The effects of poverty on childhood brain development: the mediating effect of caregiving and stressful life events. JAMA Pediatr. 2013;167(12):1135-1142. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.3139 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Victor RG, Lynch K, Li N, et al. A cluster-randomized trial of blood-pressure reduction in black barbershops. N Engl J Med. 2018;378(14):1291-1301. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1717250 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Williamson JD, Pajewski NM, Auchus AP, et al. ; SPRINT MIND Investigators for the SPRINT Research Group . Effect of intensive vs standard blood pressure control on probable dementia: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA. 2019;321(6):553-561. doi: 10.1001/jama.2018.21442 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
eMethods.
eTable 1. Characteristics of Participants at First Cognitive Assessment by Race in Pooled Cohort Sample by Cohort, 1971 to 2017
eTable 2. Sensitivity Analysis of Association of Global Cognition Decline with Race and Systolic Blood Pressure over Time Including Cognitive Observations after Incident Stroke, 1971 to 2017
eTable 3. Sensitivity Analysis of Association of Global Cognition Decline with Race and Systolic Blood Pressure over Time Including Kidney Function and History of Myocardial Infarction as Covariates, 1971 to 2017
eTable 4. Association of Cognitive Decline with Race and Systolic Blood Pressure over Time by Cohort, 1971 to 2017
eReferences.