Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2019 Oct 3;51(6):1491–1503. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14574

Brain responses and approach bias to social alcohol cues and their association with drinking in a social setting in young adult males

Martine M Groefsema 1, Gabry W Mies 2,3, Janna Cousijn 3, Rutger C M E Engels 4, Guillaume Sescousse 5,6,7,, Maartje Luijten 1,†,
PMCID: PMC7155040  PMID: 31498505

Abstract

Alcohol is mainly consumed in social settings, in which people often adapt their drinking behaviour to that of others, also called imitation of drinking. Yet, it remains unclear what drives this drinking in a social setting. In this study, we expected to see stronger brain and behavioural responses to social compared to non‐social alcohol cues, and these responses to be associated with drinking in a social setting. The sample consisted of 153 beer‐drinking males, aged 18–25 years. Brain responses to social alcohol cues were measured during an alcohol cue‐exposure task performed in an fMRI scanner. Behavioural responses to social alcohol cues were measured using a stimulus‐response compatibility task, providing an index of approach bias towards these cues. Drinking in a social setting was measured in a laboratory mimicking a bar environment. Specific brain responses to social alcohol cues were observed in the bilateral superior temporal sulcus and the left inferior parietal lobe. There was no approach bias towards social alcohol cues specifically; however, we did find an approach bias towards alcohol (versus soda) cues in general. Brain responses and approach bias towards social alcohol cues were unrelated and not associated with actual drinking. Thus, we found no support for a relation between drinking in a social setting on the one hand, and brain cue‐reactivity or behavioural approach biases to social alcohol cues on the other hand. This suggests that, in contrast to our hypothesis, drinking in a social setting may not be driven by brain or behavioural responses to social alcohol cues.

Keywords: alcohol drinking, approach bias, cue‐reactivity, imitation, social


We found specific brain responses to social alcohol cues in the bilateral superior temporal sulcus and the left inferior parietal lobe. There was no approach bias towards social alcohol cues specifically; however, we did observe an approach bias towards alcohol (versus soda) cues in general. In contrast to our hypothesis, brain responses and behavioural approach biases towards social alcohol cues were unrelated with each other and not associated with actual drinking in a social setting.

graphic file with name EJN-51-1491-g005.jpg


Abbreviations

ACC

anterior cingulate cortex

AROMA

automatic removal of motion artifacts

AUDIT

Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test

BF

Bayes factor

BOLD

Blood oxygen level‐dependent

DSM‐IV

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders ‐ 4

fMRI

functional magnetic resonance imaging

FWE

family‐wise error

ICA

independent component analysis

IPL

inferior parietal lobe

MRI

magnetic resonance imaging

NA

non‐social alcohol

NS

non‐social soda

PCA

principal component analysis

SACE

social‐alcohol cue‐exposure

SA

social alcohol

SPSS

Statistical Package for Social Sciences

SRC

stimulus‐response compatibility

SS

social soda

STS

superior temporal sulcus

vmPFC

ventromedial prefrontal cortex

VS

ventral striatum

1. INTRODUCTION

Alcohol use is often initiated during adolescence and peaks in young adulthood (Chassin & Loeb, 2013; Dennis & Scott, 2007; Johnston, 2010). The health concerns associated with heavy alcohol use are manifold including violence, sexually transmitted diseases, accidents and increased mortality (Mokdad et al., 2018; White & Hingson, 2013). In order to prevent or reduce alcohol‐related health concerns in young adults, it is important to better understand the mechanisms underlying the motivation to drink alcohol.

Drinking behaviour in young and non‐dependent drinking adults is largely driven by social factors, and alcohol is usually consumed in the company of friends, during parties and in bars (Beck et al., 2008; Clapp & Shillington, 2001; Dallas et al., 2014). Social drinking motives are often indicated as the most important reasons to drink by young adults, followed by enhancement motives (i.e., enhancing positive mood) (Kuntsche, Knibbe, Gmel, & Engels, 2005; Kuntsche et al., 2014). In line with this, it has repeatedly been found that individuals tend to adjust their alcohol consumption to a drinking partner in social settings, a phenomenon called imitation of drinking (Bot, Engels, & Knibbe, 2005; Caudill & Marlatt, 1975; Larsen, Engels, Granic, & Overbeek, 2009; Larsen, Engels, Souren, Granic, & Overbeek, 2010; Larsen, Overbeek, Granic, & Engels, 2010; Larsen, Overbeek, et al., 2012; Larsen, Engels, Granic, & Huizink, 2013; Larsen, Lichtwarck‐Aschoff, Kuntsche, Granic, & Engels, 2013). Imitation of drinking can become problematic when individuals surround themselves with heavy drinkers as they might not be aware of it (Dallas et al., 2014). While there is extensive support for imitation of drinking, individual differences in the degree to which people imitate drinking behaviour are still elusive (Larsen, Engels, Wiers, Granic, & Spijkerman, 2012; Larsen, Overbeek, Vermulst, Granic, & Engels, 2010; Larsen, Engels, et al., 2013). Therefore, it is important to further examine the processes that contribute to drinking in social settings.

Neuroimaging studies using alcohol cue‐reactivity paradigms in heavy drinking and dependent samples have shown that alcohol cues elicit stronger responses than non‐alcohol cues in reward‐related brain regions such as the ventral striatum (VS), ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (for meta‐analysis, see Schacht, Anton, and Myrick (2013)). This sensitization of reward‐related regions by alcohol cues is in line with the idea that these cues carry a particularly strong incentive salience playing a role in the attractiveness of alcohol. Next to this process of sensitization, dual‐process models of addiction emphasize the role of implicit cognitive biases towards incentive cues (Everitt & Robbins, 2005; Gladwin, Figner, Crone, & Wiers, 2011; Lindgren et al., 2019; Stacy & Wiers, 2010) such as approach action tendencies that can trigger alcohol use (Wiers, Rinck, Kordts, Houben, & Strack, 2010; Wiers et al., 2007). When considering drinking in a social setting, a similar process of increased incentive salience attribution and approach tendency to socially relevant stimuli might play a role in explaining individual differences in imitation of drinking. In other words, cues that have repeatedly been paired with drinking in a social setting might carry more incentive salience (i.e., motivational value) for one individual compared with another, which could eventually result in differences in the level of imitation of drinking.

To measure the salience of alcohol stimuli for young adult drinkers, who mainly drink in social settings, we used a cue‐reactivity task and a behavioural approach bias task with pictures that include this social context, that is, pictures showing people having a beer/soda in a bar, in addition to plain alcohol and soda pictures. Based on our rationale that embedding the social setting into alcohol cues will increase their incentive value, we expect that social alcohol pictures elicit stronger brain responses than non‐social alcohol pictures in reward‐related regions (e.g., VS, vmPFC, ACC), as well as in brain regions known for their role in social processing (e.g., superior temporal sulcus, temporoparietal junction, (dorso)medial prefrontal cortex, ACC) (Amodio & Frith, 2006; Apps, Rushworth, & Chang, 2016; Cousijn, Luijten, & Feldstein Ewing, 2018; Ruff & Fehr, 2014; Witteman et al., 2015). We also expect a larger behavioural approach bias towards social compared with non‐social alcohol cues. Furthermore, we expect brain cue‐reactivity and behavioural approach biases towards social alcohol cues to be associated with each other and with actual drinking behaviour in a social setting. This drinking behaviour was examined in the unique and semi‐naturalistic environment of a Bar‐Lab. This environment has been shown to provide an ecologically valid and informative measure of drinking behaviour, leveraging the opportunity to experimentally manipulate social and contextual factors without causing social desirable behaviour among participants (Larsen et al., 2009; Larsen, Engels, et al., 2010; Larsen, Engels, et al., 2012; Larsen, Overbeek, et al., 2012; Larsen, Engels, et al., 2013; Larsen, Lichtwarck‐Aschoff, et al., 2013). In this study, we will look at (a) imitation of drinking (the degree to which an individual imitates the alcohol intake of his drinking partner—a confederate) and (b) social drinking in general (the individual's total amount of drinks in the presence of a drinking partner).

In sum, our aim was to examine social drinking in a large group of beer‐drinking young adults by triangulating three experimental measures: brain cue‐reactivity to social alcohol cues, behavioural approach biases to social alcohol cues, and drinking in a social setting. We expected that heightened brain cue‐reactivity and behavioural approach biases towards social alcohol cues (compared with soda cues and non‐social alcohol cues) would be associated with increased drinking in a social setting. More specifically, we hypothesized that (1a) social alcohol cues would elicit more activation in reward‐related and social brain regions than non‐social alcohol cues; (1b) a behavioural approach bias would be stronger towards social alcohol cues than non‐social alcohol cues; (2) brain cue‐reactivity and behavioural approach bias towards social alcohol cues would be correlated; and (3) both measures would be positively associated with drinking in a social setting.

2. MATERIALS AND METHODS

2.1. Participants

In the context of a larger project on alcohol use in young adults (see also Groefsema et al. (2019)), participants were recruited via flyers and online advertisement. Potential participants completed an online screening to assess their eligibility to participate (see detailed flow chart in Figure S1). Inclusion criteria were as follows: (a) age 18–25, (b) drinking beer and (c) being male. We decided to restrict our sample to male participants because of the higher prevalence of alcohol‐related problems among males (Nolen‐Hoeksema, 2004). Furthermore, focussing on one gender among participants and confederates minimized the possible confounding effect of attractiveness and relationship status on our experimental measure of drinking in a social setting (Karremans & Verwijmeren, 2008; van Straaten, Engels, Finkenauer, & Holland, 2008). Exclusion criteria were MRI contraindications and a history of brain injury. Originally, participants were further categorized into three groups—light, at‐risk and dependent drinkers—based on two self‐report measures collected during the initial online screening, as well as the DSM‐IV (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders‐IV) criteria for alcohol dependence assessed later during an onsite clinical interview (Sheehan et al., 1997). The self‐report measure assessed the level of alcohol‐related problems (Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT)) (Saunders, Aasland, Babor, de La Fuente, & Grant, 1993) and heaviness of drinking (number of alcoholic drinks per week). Yet, as we were interested in social drinking, and not heaviness of drinking, all participants were combined into one group for the current research question. For more details on our recruitment criteria, see Figure S1. All participants participated voluntarily, gave written informed consent and received a financial compensation of 50 euros (with an additional 10 euros for the individuals who underwent an interview). The study was approved by the regional ethics committee CMO‐Arnhem‐Nijmegen (#2014/043).

The initial sample consisted of 166 individuals. Seven individuals were incorrectly included, as they did not meet the group criteria set out for the purpose of the broader scope of this project. In addition, six participants dropped out of the study prematurely, as they did not complete all three laboratory visits (see Procedure). The data of these thirteen participants were not taken into account in any of the analyses. The final sample thus consisted of 153 young adult males with a mean age of 22.78 (SD = 1.84). They were mostly highly educated (3.3% low, 20.9% middle and 75.8% high, according to the Dutch education system), drank on average 18.09 (SD = 13.26) alcoholic drinks per week according to the Timeline Follow‐back (Sobell & Sobell, 1994), and had a mean AUDIT score of 12.69 (SD = 6.49). Among included individuals, 17% (= 26) were smokers. From the final sample, slightly different numbers of participants were included in the separate analyses (see flow chart in Figure S1; Bar‐Lab measures: = 144, cue‐reactivity task: = 150, and approach‐avoidance task: = 153).

2.2. Procedure

Following an online screening, participants completed two behavioural sessions in a Bar‐Lab, followed by a separate fMRI (functional magnetic resonance imaging) session, 1 week apart. All data collection took place between 4:00 and 10:00 pm, coinciding with typical drinking hours. Participants were asked to abstain from drinking alcohol in the 24 hrs preceding testing, and sobriety was verified using a breath analyzer.

2.3. Bar‐Lab sessions

The Bar‐Lab was designed to look like a real bar (see Figure 1), increasing the ecological validity of the (imitation of ) drinking measures (Larsen et al., 2009). To cover the real aim of the study, participants were told that they took part in a study on the evaluation of alcohol advertisements. Participants were fully debriefed after study completion. During both Bar‐Lab sessions, a confederate was present, acting as a participant to facilitate imitation of drinking. Confederates were 20 males aged between 18 and 25, similar to the participants.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Bar‐Lab; overview and close‐up on the bar. The participants and confederates were sitting on the barstools. [Colour figure can be viewed at http://www.wileyonlinelibrary.com]

After entering the Bar‐Lab, the participant and confederate were instructed to fill in online questionnaires on demographics, drinking habits and drinking motives, followed by the rating of several non‐alcoholic video advertisements in terms of attractiveness. Then, they were asked to sit at the bar, where peanuts and drinks were available, for a break lasting 30 min, before they had to rate video advertisements again. The experimenter offered a drink to the confederate first to set the norm and enable the examination of whether the participant would choose the same drink. Various soda drinks (200 ml) and two types of local beers (250 ml, 5%–5.2% alcohol) were offered. After providing the first drink, the experimenter left the Bar‐Lab after explaining to the participant and confederate that they were allowed to get more drinks if they wanted to. Just before the sessions, the confederates were told to either drink one alcoholic beer followed by one soda (hereafter referred to as the “light” condition) or three alcoholic beers (hereafter referred to as the “heavy” condition) during a 30‐min “break.” Importantly, the confederate was instructed to initiate the drinks, by informing the participant on what he was drinking, and asking the participant if he would like something to drink as well in a neutral tone. Video and audio recordings were made during the sessions to record the number of drinks consumed. Following the break, the participant and confederate were asked again to rate the alcohol advertisements. They were also asked how they felt during the experiment and what they thought the aim of the study was (suspicion check), as well as how they subjectively evaluated the confederate (see Figure S3b). These evaluations of the confederates were not correlated with the number of alcoholic drinks consumed by the participants (see Figure S3c), and across all confederates, similar drinking patterns were found (see Figure S3a). Each participant completed a “light” and a “heavy” session with two different confederates. Session order was counterbalanced across participants and confederates. This procedure allowed us to quantify both imitation of drinking and social drinking in an ecological setting. Imitation scores were calculated by computing the difference in the number of beers consumed by the participant versus the confederate in each session, and then summing the absolute values of these differences. Social drinking scores were calculated by summing the number of beers consumed across both sessions.

2.4. fMRI session

During the final test session, participants were instructed on the fMRI scanning procedures. The total scanning time was approximately 1 hr, during which they performed two tasks; the social‐alcohol cue‐exposure (SACE) task in which participants viewed alcohol‐related pictures (reported in this paper) and a Beer‐Incentive‐Delay task (reported in Groefsema et al., 2019), see Table S1 for a complete overview of all data collected).

2.4.1. Social‐alcohol cue‐exposure (SACE) task

We used a modified version of a passive viewing Cue‐Exposure task (Schacht et al., 2011), including four conditions of interest (SA: social alcohol, SS: social soda, NA: non‐social alcohol, NS: non‐social soda, similar to Groefsema, Engels, Kuntsche, Smit, and Luijten (2016)), and one control condition (animal pictures) to which participants had to respond by a button press to ensure they paid attention to the cues. The non‐social cues were pictures of beer or soda bottles without any human beings present, while the social pictures showed two or more male and/or female individuals drinking beer or soda while interacting with each other in a social setting, such as a bar or at home. Alcohol and soda pictures were matched one‐on‐one in terms of social setting, and the number and gender of people present. Twenty cues for each condition were presented in a block design: there were four epochs each consisting of four blocks with five consecutively presented pictures of the same condition (SA, SS, NA or NS). Each picture was presented once for 4.8 s. Blocks were presented in a randomized order which was the same for all participants. There was a 6 s delay (fixation cross) in between each block. Between the four epochs, participants had a 16 s break (fixation cross). Each epoch included one control cue—an animal picture presented for 4.8 s to which participants had to respond—either at the beginning or at the end of a block (except for the last epoch in which two control cues were presented). Total task duration (including 10 practice trials) was approximately 10 min.

The main outcome measure of this task was brain cue‐reactivity to social alcohol pictures (versus social soda pictures) compared with non‐social alcohol pictures (versus non‐social soda pictures), that is, the interaction contrast ((SA>SS)‐(NA>NS)).

2.4.2. fMRI data acquisition and analyses (SACE task)

Imaging was conducted on a PRISMA(Fit) 3T Siemens scanner, using a 32‐channel head coil. Blood oxygen level‐dependent (BOLD) sensitive functional images were acquired with a whole‐brain T2*‐weighted sequence using multi‐echo echoplanar imaging (EPI) (35 axial slices, matrix 64 × 64, voxel size = 3.5 × 3.5 × 3.0 mm, repetition time = 2,250 ms, echo times = [9.4 18.8 28.2 37.6 ms], flip angle = 90°). The BOLD data acquisition sequence was updated during the course of the study, due to the discovery of MRI noise artifacts. The sequence parameters remained identical, except for the slice order which changed from ascending to interleaved. We took some actions in our analyses to (a) remove the artifacts and (b) model the change in scanning sequence halfway through the study (see below). A high‐resolution T1 scan was acquired in each participant (192 sagittal slices, field of view 256 mm, voxel size = 1.0 × 1.0 × 1.0 mm, repetition time = 300 ms, echo time 3.03 ms).

Pre‐processing steps were conducted in SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). For each volume, the four echo images were combined into a single one, weighing all echoes equally. Standard pre‐processing steps were performed on the functional data: realignment to the first image of the time series, co‐registration to the structural image, normalization to MNI space based on the segmentation and normalization of the structural image, and spatial smoothing with an 8‐mm Gaussian kernel. In addition, two cleaning methods were incorporated into the pipeline to ensure optimal removal of artifacts and thorough de‐noising of the data: (a) a principal component analysis (PCA) to filter out slice‐specific noise components (Viviani, Gron, & Spitzer, 2005) before pre‐processing and (b) an independent component analysis (ICA)‐based automatic removal of motion artifacts using FSL (http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl) after pre‐processing (ICA‐AROMA; (Pruim, Mennes, Buitelaar, & Beckmann, 2015; Pruim et al., 2015)). This pipeline has previously been found to be efficient to take care of the MRI noise artifacts identified in the first half of our data (Nieuwhof et al., 2017).

After pre‐processing, the data were modelled using a general linear model. For each condition of interest (SA, SS, NA or NS), the various blocks of five consecutive cues were modelled as boxcars with a duration of 24s. The control condition (single animal picture) was modelled as a boxcar with a duration of 4.8s. Six motion parameters were included, and a temporal high‐pass filter with a cut‐off of 240s (i.e., twice the maximum length between two blocks of the same condition) was applied. Parameter estimates for all conditions (i.e., SA, SS, NA and NS) were obtained by restricted maximum‐likelihood estimation.

2.4.3. Stimulus‐response compatibility (SRC) task

After scanning, approach biases were measured outside of the scanner using a well‐validated stimulus‐response compatibility (SRC) task (Field, Caren, Fernie, & De Houwer, 2011; Groefsema et al., 2016). Participants were presented with the exact same pictures as in the SACE task (i.e., SA, SS, NA and NS) and were instructed to either approach or avoid each picture, based on the alcohol content of the picture: “Approach Alcohol” (and “Avoid Soda”) or “Avoid Alcohol” (and “Approach Soda”). Every picture was presented for 2,000 ms with a manikin randomly positioned above or below the picture. Participants could approach or avoid the picture by pressing the “up” or “down” keyboard button and thereby moving the manikin in the corresponding direction. After incorrect responses, a red cross was shown for 2,000 ms, and after omissions, “please respond faster” was also shown for 2,000 ms. Participants completed four blocks of 32 trials each: two blocks with only social pictures (one with an “Approach Alcohol” instruction, and one with an “Avoid Alcohol” instruction) and two blocks with only non‐social pictures (one with an “Approach Alcohol” instruction and one with an “Avoid Alcohol” instruction). All pictures were presented twice, once within the “Approach Alcohol” block and once within the “Avoid Alcohol” block. The order of task blocks was counterbalanced across participants, with the restriction that those who started with a social block completed both social blocks before proceeding to the non‐social blocks, and vice versa. Total task duration was approximately 10 min. The task was preceded by 16 practice trials in which participants were instructed to approach bird pictures and avoid pictures of other animals. The outcome measure of the SRC task is the approach bias in each of the four conditions (SA, NA, SS and NS). For each condition, this approach bias was calculated by subtracting the mean reaction time observed for the “Approach” instruction from the mean reaction time observed for the “Avoid” condition including successful trials only. Errors, omissions and outliers (responses < 200 ms and > 2,000 ms or 3 SD above the individual mean) were discarded from these calculations (Cousijn et al., 2012; Field et al., 2011). Additionally, we calculated an interaction score by subtracting the non‐social alcohol bias from the social alcohol bias (i.e., (SA‐bias>SS‐bias)‐(NA‐bias>NS‐bias)).

2.5. Statistical analyses

All unthresholded T‐maps resulting from the fMRI analyses can be accessed at https://identifiers.org/neurovault.collection:5182. To test our first hypothesis (hypothesis 1a) regarding brain cue‐reactivity to social alcohol pictures in the SACE task, a whole‐brain one‐sample t test was conducted in SPM8 on the interaction contrast ((SA>SS)‐(NA>NS)). Scanning sequence (i.e., pre‐ versus post‐discovery of artifacts) was added as a binary covariate of no interest in all fMRI analyses. All T‐maps were thresholded with a voxel‐level uncorrected threshold of p < .001, combined with a cluster‐level family‐wise error (FWE) corrected threshold of p < .05, accounting for multiple comparisons across the whole brain. The probabilistic atlas of Hammers et al. (2003) was used to label significant clusters, and for visualization purposes, we overlaid the output T‐maps on an average T1 map of all participants, using Mango (http://www.ric.uthscsa.edu/mango).

To examine whether the approach bias towards social alcohol pictures was larger than the approach bias towards non‐social alcohol pictures in the SRC task (hypothesis 1b), a 2 × 2 repeated‐measures ANOVA was conducted, using the type of drink (alcohol, soda) and context (social, non‐social) as within‐subject factors.

To examine the association between social alcohol cue‐reactivity and social alcohol approach bias (hypothesis 2), a simple regression analysis was performed in SPM8 on the brain cue‐reactivity interaction contrast ((SA>SS)‐(NA>NS)), including the interaction contrast (i.e., (SA‐bias>SS‐bias)‐(NA‐bias>NS‐bias)) from the approach bias score as a covariate of interest.

To examine the relationship between the above‐mentioned variables and drinking in a social setting (hypothesis 3), we first examined whether imitation of drinking occurred in the Bar‐Lab, thereby checking the validity of this measure. For this purpose, the number of drinks in the heavy and light drinking sessions was compared by means of a paired‐samples t test. Subsequently, we included the imitation of drinking score and social drinking score as covariates of interest in two separate whole‐brain analyses modelling social alcohol cue‐reactivity (i.e., the ((SA>SS)‐(NA>NS)) contrast). Correlation analyses were performed in SPSS (Statistical Package for Social Sciences) to examine the associations between the social alcohol approach bias (i.e., (SA‐bias>SS‐bias)‐(NA‐bias>NS‐bias)), the social alcohol cue‐reactivity ((SA>SS)‐(NA>NS)) and the imitation of drinking score as well as the social drinking score. These correlation analyses were performed using Kendall's tau, as the social drinking scores were not normally distributed.

Finally, we performed Bayesian statistics with default priors in JASP (Wagenmakers et al., 2018) in order to quantify the evidence supporting the null hypothesis. We report Bayes factors (BF) expressing the probability of the data under H0 relative to the probability of the data under H1 (i.e., BF01).

3. RESULTS

3.1. Brain cue‐reactivity to social alcohol pictures (SACE)

The whole‐brain one‐sample t test on the contrast (SA>SS)‐(NA>NS) revealed three significant clusters that survived the FWE = .05 cluster‐level correction: the bilateral superior temporal sulcus (STS) [x,y,z max = 54, ‐4, ‐11, T max = 4.75, k = 104 & x,y,z max  = −51, 8, ‐8, T max = 4.48, k = 103] and the left inferior parietal lobe (IPL) [x,y,z max = −60, ‐28, 25, T max = 4.16, k = 66] (see Figure 2a and Table S2). Both the main effect of alcohol cues versus soda cues (SA+NA) > (SS+NS) and the main effect of social context versus non‐social context (SA+SS) > (NA+NS) revealed activation patterns in the reward‐related brain network (e.g., ventral striatum and vmPFC cortex, see Figure 2b and Table S2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Brain responses during the Social Alcohol Cue‐Exposure task (a) Whole‐brain analysis of cue‐reactivity to social alcohol pictures, that is interaction contrast [(Social Alcohol (SA) > Social Soda (SS))‐ (Non‐social Alcohol (NA) > Non‐Social Soda (NS))]. Boxplots—reported for illustrative purposes—show the percent signal change (PSC) in the three functional clusters that show a significant interaction effect in the whole‐brain analysis. (b) Whole‐brain analysis of cue‐reactivity to alcohol pictures and social pictures, that is main effect contrasts [(Social Alcohol + Non‐Social Alcohol)‐(Non‐Social Soda + Social Soda)] in red and [(Social Alcohol + Social Soda) – (Non‐Social Alcohol + Non‐Social Soda)] in blue. Display threshold for panels A and B: voxel‐level uncorrected < .001 combined with cluster‐level FWE corrected < .05. ACC, Anterior Cingulate Cortex; Non‐Social, Non‐Social Alcohol‐Non‐Social Soda; Social, Social Alcohol‐Social Soda; vmPFC, ventral medial Prefrontal Cortex. [Colour figure can be viewed at http://www.wileyonlinelibrary.com]

3.2. Approach bias towards social alcohol pictures (SRC)

Approach biases (i.e., faster reaction times for Approach compared with Avoid condition) occurred for all picture types; that is, all approach bias scores were significantly larger than zero (SA: t(152) = 12.537, p = <.001, SS: t(152) = 5.678, p = <.001, NA: t(152) = 13.612, p = <.001, NS: t(152) = 8.379, p = <.001). We found a stronger approach bias towards alcohol pictures than towards soda pictures, reflected in a main effect of drink (F 1,152 = 10.639, = .001, ηp2 = .065 |BF01 = .002, with decisive evidence for alternative hypothesis). In contrast to our hypothesis, we did not find a main effect of context (F 1,152 = 1.311, = .254, ηp2 = .009 | BF01 = 7.746, with substantial evidence for null hypothesis) nor an interaction between drink and context (F 1,152 = .335, = .563, ηp2 = .002 | BF01 = 7.111, with substantial evidence for the null hypothesis). These results suggest that the approach bias towards alcohol pictures was similar for social and non‐social pictures (see Figure 3).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Boxplots of approach bias scores (reaction time for Avoid – Approach condition in ms) for the 4 main conditions. SA, social alcohol; SS, social soda, NA, non‐social alcohol, NS, non‐social soda. There is a significant approach bias in all conditions, as well as a main effect of drink (= .001), with a stronger approach bias towards alcohol compared with soda pictures. [Colour figure can be viewed at http://www.wileyonlinelibrary.com]

3.3. Association between social alcohol cue‐reactivity (SACE) and social alcohol approach bias (SRC)

The whole‐brain regression analysis revealed no significant clusters for the association between social alcohol cue‐reactivity and social alcohol approach bias. In addition, we performed an exploratory analysis to examine whether activation in the three regions that were originally found in the interaction effect (SA>SS)‐(NA>NS) was correlated with the social alcohol approach bias. We extracted the percent signal change from these three clusters—the left and right superior temporal sulcus and the left inferior parietal lobe—using the rfxplot toolbox (Glascher, 2009) and regressed these values against the social alcohol approach bias scores using SPSS. We found no significant correlations (right STS r τ = −.090, = .115 | left STS r τ = −.040, = .486 | IPL r τ = −.102, = .074), which was further supported by Bayesian statistics providing anecdotal to moderate evidence for the null hypothesis of no correlation between social alcohol cue‐reactivity and social alcohol approach bias (right STS BF01 = 2.639 | left STS BF01 = 7.135 | left IPL BF01 = 1.856).

3.4. Associations between cue‐reactivity or approach bias and drinking in a social setting

To check whether imitation of drinking occurred in our sample, we first compared the number of beers consumed by the participant in the light session with the number of beers consumed in the heavy session. As expected, participants imitated the confederate: they consumed more alcohol in the heavy session (M = 1.70, range = 0–5, SD = 1.26) than in the light session (M = 1.22, range = 0–4, SD = .96) (F 1,143 = 19.945, < .001, ηp2 = .122). The imitation score, reflecting how closely participants matched the drinking pattern of the confederate, ranged from 0 to 4, wherein 0 reflects that the participant and confederate consumed exactly the same amount of alcohol, and higher scores reflect less imitation. The mean imitation score was 2.15 (SD = 1.27). The social drinking score (consumed drinks in a social drinking setting) ranged from 0 to 8, in which a higher score reflects more drinking during the two Bar‐Lab sessions. The mean social drinking score was 2.92 (SD = 1.81).

Importantly, no significant correlations were found between brain reactivity to social alcohol cues and either imitation of drinking or social drinking, both at the whole‐brain level and within ROIs, with substantial evidence for the null hypothesis (right STS—imitation of drinking r τ = −.020, = .746 | BF01 = 8.527; social drinking r τ = −.001, = .981 | BF01 = 9.080; left STS—imitation of drinking r τ = −.039, = .537 | BF01 = 7.225; social drinking r τ = .043, = .480 | BF01 = 6.838; left IPL—imitation of drinking r τ = −.076, = .226| BF01 = 3.767; social drinking r τ = −.035, = .567 | BF01 = 7.537). Also, no significant correlations with substantial evidence for the null hypothesis were found between the social alcohol approach bias score and either imitation of drinking (r τ = .027, = .662 | BF01 = 8.184) or social drinking (r τ = −.022, = .716 | BF01 = 8.510).

Additionally, we performed exploratory analyses to examine whether alcohol cue‐reactivity and alcohol approach biases, independently of social context ((SA+NA)‐(SS+NS)), showed correlations with each other and with imitation of drinking or social drinking. Using a whole‐brain simple regression model, we found no significant correlation between alcohol cue‐reactivity and alcohol approach bias. Furthermore, we did not find any significant correlations between alcohol cue‐reactivity and imitation of drinking or social drinking at the whole‐brain level, or between the alcohol approach bias scores and imitation of drinking or social drinking. Lastly, we examined whether AUDIT scores were associated with drinking in a social setting, brain responses to social alcohol cues and approach biases to social alcohol cues. We found that individuals with a higher AUDIT score tended to drink more alcohol in a social drinking setting, but no evidence for an association of AUDIT scores with either brain or behavioral responses to social alcohol cues (see Figure S2).

4. DISCUSSION

This study examined the relationship between brain and behavioural responses to social alcohol cues, as well as how these measures relate to drinking in a social setting. We included a large sample of young adults and measured imitation of drinking using a semi‐naturalistic Bar‐Lab setting. First, we observed brain reactivity specifically towards social alcohol cues in the bilateral superior temporal sulcus and the left inferior parietal lobe, as indicated by stronger responses to social versus non‐social alcohol cues, compared with social versus non‐social soda cues. Second, we found no support for an approach bias towards social alcohol cues specifically; however, we did find an approach bias towards alcohol (versus soda) cues independently of social context. Third, brain reactivity and behavioural approach bias towards social alcohol cues were uncorrelated with each other and were neither related to imitation of alcohol use or social drinking (for an overview of all findings see Figure 4). Additional exploratory analyses showed that, regardless of social context, brain and behavioural responses to alcohol cues were similarly uncorrelated with each other and were also not correlated with drinking in a social setting.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Overview of the results. Broken lines reflect no significant associations between the variables. IPL, inferior parietal lobe; STS, superior temporal sulcus

Since the social drinking setting is thought to be highly salient for young adult drinkers, we were interested in the effect of social context on alcohol cue‐reactivity. At the brain level, against our expectations, we found no increased activation in reward‐related brain regions, such as the ventral striatum, in response to social versus non‐social alcohol cues. Instead, increased activation was found in the superior temporal sulcus (STS) and the left inferior parietal lobe (IPL). While these brain areas have not often been emphasized in individual cue‐reactivity studies, two meta‐analyses performing activation likelihood estimation (ALE) analyses across these studies revealed an association between parietal lobe activation and craving (Chase, Eickhoff, Laird, & Hogarth, 2011), as well as stronger brain responses in the superior temporal gyrus to alcohol versus neutral cues in alcohol‐dependent patients versus healthy controls (Schacht et al., 2013). Interestingly, the temporal pole has also been implicated in emotion processing (Olson, Plotzker, & Ezzyat, 2007) and social cognition (Adolphs, 1999; Allison, Puce, & McCarthy, 2000; Insel & Fernald, 2004; Wöhr & Soren, 2017) and is thought to integrate emotional and sensory cues (Olson et al., 2007; Pehrs et al., 2017). Therefore, we could speculate that increased activation in response to social alcohol cues in these areas might reflect increased motivation towards socially meaningful stimuli. Still, the evidence for the involvement of brain areas outside of the reward system in substance use needs further exploration. Regarding the absence of specific responses to social alcohol stimuli in reward‐related regions, one potential explanation is that alcohol and social cues elicited overlapping activations in these regions (such as the ventral striatum, ACC and vmPFC, see Figure 2b). This may have resulted in a ceiling effect, leaving little room for an additive effect of social context above and beyond alcohol content.

At the behavioural level, we again expected that the approach bias towards alcohol cues would be stronger in a social context compared with a non‐social context. Yet, the current results revealed a general approach bias towards alcohol cues, which was of similar magnitude for social and non‐social cues. This suggests that while alcohol cues elicit a stronger approach bias than soda cues, there might be no additive effect of social context in strengthening that bias. The fact that the task instructions were tailored to the alcohol content; that is, the participants had to either approach or avoid alcohol, might have amplified the focus on the drink and thus mitigated any additional effect of context. In our earlier study using similar social and non‐social alcohol stimuli (Groefsema et al., 2016), we observed a comparable main effect of alcohol in the absence of a clear interaction with social context. Our study adds to the literature by replicating our previous findings of a general approach bias towards alcohol (Groefsema et al., 2016) in a young sample of drinkers with varying levels of alcohol use.

Additionally and against our expectations, we found no support for an association between our brain cue‐reactivity measures and our behavioural approach measures. This could reflect the fact that the social alcohol cue‐reactivity and the stimulus‐response compatibility tasks engage partly different (brain) mechanisms. More specifically, whereas cue‐reactivity (e.g., based on the incentive sensitization model of addiction (Robinson & Berridge, 1993, 2001, 2008)) engages the salience and reward brain networks (Zilverstand, Huang, Alia‐Klein, & Goldstein, 2018), approach tendencies (e.g., seen as an automatic response in the dual‐process models of addiction (Wiers et al., 2007; Stacy & Wiers, 2010)) require a response and therefore also involve the executive network (Zilverstand et al., 2018). This difference between the two measures might explain why they are not correlated in the present data and further suggest that cue‐reactivity and approach biases may be independent mechanisms associated with drinking behaviour.

The present observation that drinking in a social setting was not related to either brain cue‐reactivity or behavioural approach biases is in line with previous studies which have failed to pinpoint the origin of individual differences in imitation of drinking. More specifically, these individual differences seem to be unaccounted for by induced stress levels (Larsen, Engels, et al., 2013), engagement between drinking partners (Larsen, Lichtwarck‐Aschoff, et al., 2013), or implicit alcohol‐related cognitions (Larsen, Engels, et al., 2012). One potential explanation is that environmental factors, such as having peers around, may be stronger predictors of social drinking than individual factors such as brain or behavioural responses to alcohol cues. Supporting this idea, a study by van Schoor, Bot, and Engels (2008) showed that while several personality traits were associated with self‐reported daily alcohol consumption or self‐reported alcohol‐related problems, these personality traits no longer predicted drinking behaviour when peers were around. So the company of peers and “unwritten” social norms (Jackson et al., 2014; Teunissen et al., 2012) may have a bigger impact on drinking behaviour than individual traits like the response to alcohol cues.

We believe that the results of this study highlight two further issues that surface if one does not specifically focus on the effect of the social alcohol cues but more on responses to alcohol cues in general. First, it can be questioned what predictive validity approach biases hold, since the approach bias was not related to a measure of real‐world drinking. Approach biases are thought to play an important role in (the transition to) heavy drinking (Lindgren et al., 2019; Robinson & Berridge, 2001). Moreover, after training dependent individuals to avoid alcohol cues instead of approaching them, relapse rates decreased (Eberl et al., 2013; Kakoschke, Kemps, & Tiggemann, 2017; Manning et al., 2016; Rinck, Wiers, Becker, & Lindenmeyer, 2018; Wiers, Eberl, Rinck, Becker, & Lindenmeyer, 2011; Wiers, Ludwig, et al., 2015; Wiers, Stelzel, et al., 2015). Yet, such a re‐training has not been successful in undergraduate students in terms of reducing approach biases and drinking behaviour (Lindgren et al., 2015). It may be that re‐training an approach bias is only be effective in heavy or dependent drinkers and/or among individuals with the motivation to change their behaviour. Despite that we did not re‐train approach biases or measure the motivation to change, it could be hypothesized that our sample had indeed a low motivation to change their behaviour, as they were mostly students that generally do not see their alcohol consumption as problematic (Vik, Culbertson, & Sellers, 2000) and were not seeking any help. Future studies in young adult drinkers should test the possible moderating role of motivation to change on the link between approach biases and drinking in the real world.

Second, our findings emphasize the difficulty—and the importance—of relating laboratory measures with drinking in the real world. Recent studies among adolescent drinkers have shown no relationship between alcohol approach biases and the levels of alcohol consumption measured at different time points with ecological momentary assessments (Janssen, Larsen, Vollebergh, & Wiers, 2015), or have found such relationships only in individuals with weak inhibition skills (Peeters et al., 2013). Moreover, we have previously shown that cognitive biases towards social alcohol cues are not directly related to drinking in the real world, but only moderate the association between the number of friends present and alcohol use (Groefsema et al., 2016). With regard to cue‐reactivity, this was the first study to directly relate brain cue‐reactivity with an ecologically valid measure of social drinking. Previous studies that correlated brain activation with a measure of alcohol use revealed a positive association with VS activation, but this was almost always with a self‐report measure of alcohol such as problematic drinking or drinking desire (see meta‐analysis Schacht et al. (2013)). Collectively, these studies illustrate the difficulty to identify reliable predictors of real drinking behaviour. Alcohol use is a very complex behaviour and can be affected by many different motivations, with each of them possibly explaining a small amount of variance. New analyses techniques such as machine learning can offer promising opportunities allowing researchers to test multiple different determinants of drinking at once. Indeed, it has previously been found that life experiences, neurobiological differences and personality appear to be the most important factors influencing binge drinking among adolescents (Whelan et al., 2014). Future research should expand this field of research to other samples and types of drinking behaviour.

One of the major strengths of this study is the large sample size which enhances the interpretability of the null findings, as further suggested by our Bayesian statical analyses which generally revealed support for the null hypotheses. Another strength is the ecological validity of our Bar‐Lab procedure to measure drinking in a social setting, along with a triangulation approach between brain responses, behavioural responses and actual drinking behaviour in a social setting. The integration of such findings within a large sample is still rather unique. A limitation of this study was that only males were included, and most of them were college students, making it difficult to generalize these findings to young adult drinkers in general. Yet, individuals in this age range are specifically known for drinking heavily in social settings and we, therefore, believe it is important to examine such a sample to reveal the underlying mechanisms of social drinking.

In conclusion, our findings show that social alcohol cues elicit specific responses in brain regions (STS and IPL) that have been associated with emotion processing and social cognition rather than reward processing per se. Given that these findings are not aligned with our predictions, replication is needed and we prefer to refrain from making strong claims about how these areas might contribute to social alcohol cue‐reactivity at the moment. In addition, we found that our young adult heterogenous drinking sample shows approach tendencies towards alcohol cues, but no evidence supporting a relationship between brain cue‐reactivity and behavioural approach biases towards (social) alcohol cues, nor between either of these measures and drinking behaviour in a social setting. Despite the fact that laboratory measures of cue‐reactivity and approach biases were not related to drinking in a social setting in the current study, we would like to encourage future studies to continue to include a measure of real‐world drinking in combination with, for example, machine learning analyses, in order to strengthen the predictive validity of research in the laboratory to drinking behaviour in the real world.

CONFLICTS OF INTEREST

There are no conflicts of interest to declare.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

All authors contributed to the design of the study. MG collected the data. MG, GS, GM and ML performed data analyses. MG, GM, GS and ML wrote the first draft of the manuscript. RE and JC edited the manuscript.

Supporting information

 

 

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial or not‐for‐profit sectors.

Groefsema MM, Mies GW, Cousijn J, Engels RCME, Sescousse G, Luijten M. Brain responses and approach bias to social alcohol cues and their association with drinking in a social setting in young adult males. Eur J Neurosci. 2020;51:1491–1503. 10.1111/ejn.14574

Edited by Prof. John Foxe.

The peer review history for this article is available athttps://publons.com/publon/10.1111/EJN.14574

DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT

All unthresholded T‐maps resulting from the fMRI analyses can be accessed at https://identifiers.org/neurovault.collection:5182. Data are available from the corresponding author upon request.

REFERENCES

  1. Adolphs, R. (1999). Social cognition and the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Science, 3, 469–479. 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01399-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Allison, T. , Puce, A. , & McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: Role of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 267–278. 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01501-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Amodio, D. M. , & Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 268–277. 10.1038/nrn1884 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Apps, M. A. , Rushworth, M. F. , & Chang, S. W. (2016). The anterior Cingulate gyrus and social cognition: Tracking the motivation of others. Neuron, 90, 692–707. 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.04.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Beck, K. H. , Arria, A. M. , Caldeira, K. M. , Vincent, K. B. , O'Grady, K. E. , & Wish, E. D. (2008). Social context of drinking and alcohol problems among college students. American Journal of Health Behavior, 32, 420–430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bot, S. M. , Engels, R. C. M. E. , & Knibbe, R. A. (2005). The effects of alcohol expectancies on drinking behaviour in peer groups: Observations in a naturalistic setting. Addiction, 100, 1270–1279. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.01152.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Caudill, B. D. , & Marlatt, G. A. (1975). Modeling influences in social drinking: An experimental analogue. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43, 405–415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chase, H. W. , Eickhoff, S. B. , Laird, A. R. , & Hogarth, L. (2011). The neural basis of drug stimulus processing and craving: An activation likelihood estimation meta‐analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 70, 785–793. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Chassin, M. R. , & Loeb, J. M. (2013). High‐reliability health care: Getting there from here. The Milbank Quarterly, 91, 459–490. 10.1111/1468-0009.12023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Clapp, J. D. , & Shillington, A. M. (2001). Environmental predictors of heavy episodic drinking. Americal Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 27, 301–313. 10.1081/ADA-100103711 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cousijn, J. , Goudriaan, A. E. , Ridderinkhof, K. R. , van den Brink, W. , Veltman, D. J. , & Wiers, R. W. (2012). Approach‐bias predicts development of cannabis problem severity in heavy cannabis users: Results from a prospective FMRI study. PLoS ONE, 7, e42394 10.1371/journal.pone.0042394 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cousijn, J. , Luijten, M. , & Feldstein Ewing, S. W. (2018). Adolescent resilience to addiction: A social plasticity hypothesis. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health, 2, 69–78. 10.1016/S2352-4642(17)30148-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dallas, R. , Field, M. , Jones, A. , Christensen, P. , Rose, A. , & Robinson, E. (2014). Influenced but unaware: Social influence on alcohol drinking among social acquaintances. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research, 38, 1448–1453. 10.1111/acer.12375 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dennis, M. , & Scott, C. (2007). Managing addiction as a chronic condition. Addiction Science & Clinical Practice, 4, 45 10.1151/ascp [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Eberl, C. , Wiers, R. W. , Pawelczack, S. , Rinck, M. , Becker, E. S. , & Lindenmeyer, J. (2013). Approach bias modification in alcohol dependence: Do clinical effects replicate and for whom does it work best? Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 38–51. 10.1016/j.dcn.2012.11.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Everitt, B. J. , & Robbins, T. W. (2005). Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: From actions to habits to compulsion. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1481–1489. 10.1038/nn1579 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Field, M. , Caren, R. , Fernie, G. , & De Houwer, J. (2011). Alcohol approach tendencies in heavy drinkers: Comparison of effects in a Relevant Stimulus‐Response Compatibility task and an approach/avoidance Simon task. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 25, 697–701. 10.1037/a0023285 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gladwin, T. E. , Figner, B. , Crone, E. A. , & Wiers, R. W. (2011). Addiction, adolescence, and the integration of control and motivation. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 364–376. 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.06.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Glascher, J. (2009). Visualization of group inference data in functional neuroimaging. Neuroinformatics, 7, 73–82. 10.1007/s12021-008-9042-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Groefsema, M. , Engels, R. , Kuntsche, E. , Smit, K. , & Luijten, M. (2016). Cognitive biases for social alcohol‐related pictures and alcohol use in specific social settings: An event‐level study. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research, 40, 2001–2010. 10.1111/acer.13165 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Groefsema, M. M. , Engels, R. C. , Voon, V. , Schellekens, A. F. , Luijten, M. , & Sescousse, G. (2019). Brain responses to anticipating and receiving beer: Comparing light, at‐risk, and dependent alcohol users. Addiction , e12766 10.1111/adb.12766 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hammers, A. , Allom, R. Koepp, M. J. , Free, S. L. , Myers, R. , Lemieux, L. , … Duncan, J. S. (2003). Three-dimensional maximum probability atlas of the human brain, with particular reference to the temporal lobe. Human Brain Mapping, 19, 224–247. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144148 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Insel, T. R. , & Fernald, R. D. (2004). How the brain processes social information: Searching for the social brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 697–722. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144148 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Jackson, K. M. , Roberts, M. E. , Colby, S. M. , Barnett, N. P. , Abar, C. C. , & Merrill, J. E. (2014). Willingness to drink as a function of peer offers and peer norms in early adolescence. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 75, 404–414. 10.15288/jsad.2014.75.404 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Janssen, T. , Larsen, H. , Vollebergh, W. A. , & Wiers, R. W. (2015). Longitudinal relations between cognitive bias and adolescent alcohol use. Addictive Behaviors, 44, 51–57. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.11.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Johnston, L. D. (2010). Overview of key findings.
  27. Kakoschke, N. , Kemps, E. , & Tiggemann, M. (2017). Approach bias modification training and consumption: A review of the literature. Addictive Behaviors, 64, 21–28. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.08.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Karremans, J. C. , & Verwijmeren, T. (2008). Mimicking attractive opposite‐sex others: The role of romantic relationship status. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 939–950. 10.1177/0146167208316693 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kuntsche, E. , Gabhnainn, S. N. , Roberts, C. , Windlin, B. , Vieno, A. , Bendtsen, P. , … Wicki, M. (2014). Drinking motives and links to alcohol use in 13 European countries. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 75, 428–437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kuntsche, E. , Knibbe, R. , Gmel, G. , & Engels, R. (2005). Why do young people drink? A review of drinking motives. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 841–861. 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Larsen, H. , Engels, R. C. , Granic, I. , & Huizink, A. C. (2013). Does stress increase imitation of drinking behavior? An experimental study in a (semi‐)naturalistic context. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research, 37, 477–483. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2012.01942.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Larsen, H. , Engels, R. C. , Granic, I. , & Overbeek, G. (2009). An experimental study on imitation of alcohol consumption in same‐sex dyads. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 44, 250–255. 10.1093/alcalc/agp002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Larsen, H. , Engels, R. C. , Souren, P. M. , Granic, I. , & Overbeek, G. (2010). Peer influence in a micro‐perspective: Imitation of alcoholic and non‐alcoholic beverages. Addictive Behaviors, 35, 49–52. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2009.08.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Larsen, H. , Engels, R. C. , Wiers, R. W. , Granic, I. , & Spijkerman, R. (2012). Implicit and explicit alcohol cognitions and observed alcohol consumption: Three studies in (semi)naturalistic drinking settings. Addiction, 107, 1420–1428. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03805.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Larsen, H. , Lichtwarck‐Aschoff, A. , Kuntsche, E. , Granic, I. , & Engels, R. (2013). Drinking in a micro‐perspective: The role of engagement in imitation of sips of alcoholic beverages. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 4, 226–238. 10.5127/jep.027312 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Larsen, H. , Overbeek, G. , Granic, I. , & Engels, R. C. (2010). Imitation of alcohol consumption in same‐sex and other‐sex dyads. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 45, 557–562. 10.1093/alcalc/agq053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Larsen, H. , Overbeek, G. , Granic, I. , & Engels, R. C. (2012). The strong effect of other people's drinking: Two experimental observational studies in a real bar. The American Journal on Addictions, 21, 168–175. 10.1111/j.1521-0391.2011.00200.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Larsen, H. , Overbeek, G. , Vermulst, A. A. , Granic, I. , & Engels, R. C. M. E. (2010). Initiation and continuation of best friends and adolescents’ alcohol consumption: Do self‐esteem and self‐control function as moderators? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 34, 406–416. 10.1177/0165025409350363 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lindgren, K. P. , Hendershot, C. S. , Ramirez, J. J. , Bernat, E. , Rangel‐Gomez, M. , Peterson, K. P. , & Murphy, J. G. (2019). A dual process perspective on advances in cognitive science and alcohol use disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 69, 83–96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lindgren, K. P. , Wiers, R. W. , Teachman, B. A. , Gasser, M. L. , Westgate, E. C. , Cousijn, J. , … Neighbors, C. (2015). Attempted training of alcohol approach and drinking identity associations in US undergraduate drinkers: Null results from two studies. PLoS ONE, 10, e0134642 10.1371/journal.pone.0134642 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Manning, V. , Staiger, P. K. , Hall, K. , Garfield, J. B. , Flaks, G. , Leung, D. , … Verdejo‐Garcia, A. (2016). Cognitive bias modification training during inpatient alcohol detoxification reduces early relapse: A randomized controlled trial. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research, 40, 2011–2019. 10.1111/acer.13163 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Mokdad, A. H. , Ballestros, K. , Echko, M. , Glenn, S. , Olsen, H. E. , Mullany, E. , … Ferrari, A. J. (2018). The state of US health, 1990‐2016: Burden of diseases, injuries, and risk factors among US states. JAMA, 319, 1444–1472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Nieuwhof, F. , Bloem, B. R. , Reelick, M. F. , Aarts, E. , Maidan, I. , Mirelman, A. , … Helmich, R. C. (2017). Impaired dual tasking in Parkinson's disease is associated with reduced focusing of cortico‐striatal activity. Brain, 140, 1384–1398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Nolen‐Hoeksema, S. (2004). Gender differences in risk factors and consequences for alcohol use and problems. Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 981–1010. 10.1016/j.cpr.2004.08.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Olson, I. R. , Plotzker, A. , & Ezzyat, Y. (2007). The Enigmatic temporal pole: A review of findings on social and emotional processing. Brain, 130, 1718–1731. 10.1093/brain/awm052 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Peeters, M. , Monshouwer, K. , van de Schoot, R. A. , Janssen, T. , Vollebergh, W. A. , & Wiers, R. W. (2013). Automatic processes and the drinking behavior in early adolescence: A prospective study. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research, 37, 1737–1744. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Pehrs, C. , Zaki, J. , Schlochtermeier, L. H. , Jacobs, A. M. , Kuchinke, L. , & Koelsch, S. (2017). The temporal pole top‐down modulates the ventral visual stream during social cognition. Cerebral Cortex, 27, 777–792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Pruim, R. H. , Mennes, M. , Buitelaar, J. K. , & Beckmann, C. F. (2015). Evaluation of ICA‐AROMA and alternative strategies for motion artifact removal in resting state fMRI. NeuroImage, 112, 278–287. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Pruim, R. H. , Mennes, M. , van Rooij, D. , Llera, A. , Buitelaar, J. K. , & Beckmann, C. F. (2015). ICA‐AROMA: A robust ICA‐based strategy for removing motion artifacts from fMRI data. NeuroImage, 112, 267–277. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.064 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Rinck, M. , Wiers, R. W. , Becker, E. S. , & Lindenmeyer, J. (2018). Relapse prevention in abstinent alcoholics by cognitive bias modification: Clinical effects of combining approach bias modification and attention bias modification. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 86, 1005–1016. 10.1037/ccp0000321 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Robinson, T. E. , & Berridge, K. C. (1993). The neural basis of drug craving: An incentive sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Research Reviews, 18, 247–291. 10.1016/0165-0173(93)90013-P [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Robinson, T. E. , & Berridge, K. C. (2001). Incentive‐sensitization and addiction. Addiction, 96, 103–114. 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.9611038.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Robinson, T. E. , & Berridge, K. C. (2008). Review. The incentive sensitization theory of addiction: Some current issues. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, Biological Sciences, 363, 3137–3146. 10.1098/rstb.2008.0093 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Ruff, C. C. , & Fehr, E. (2014). The neurobiology of rewards and values in social decision making. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15, 549–562. 10.1038/nrn3776 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Saunders, J. B. , Aasland, O. G. , Babor, T. F. , de La Fuente, J. R. , & Grant, M. (1993). Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption II. Addiction, 88, 791–804. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02093.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Schacht, J. P. , Anton, R. F. , & Myrick, H. (2013). Functional neuroimaging studies of alcohol cue reactivity: A quantitative meta‐analysis and systematic review. Addiction Biology, 18, 121–133. 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00464.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Schacht, J. P. , Anton, R. F. , Randall, P. K. , Li, X. , Henderson, S. , & Myrick, H. (2011). Stability of fMRI striatal response to alcohol cues: A hierarchical linear modeling approach. NeuroImage, 56, 61–68. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Sheehan, D. V. , Lecrubier, Y. , Sheehan, K. H. , Janavs, J. , Weiller, E. , Keskiner, A. , … Dunbar, G. C. (1997). The validity of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) according to the SCID‐P and its reliability. European Psychiatry, 12, 224–231. [Google Scholar]
  59. Sobell, L. , & Sobell, M. (1994). Timeline followback (TLFB) user's manual. Toronto, ON: Addiction Research Foundation. [Google Scholar]
  60. Stacy, A. W. , & Wiers, R. W. (2010). Implicit cognition and addiction: A tool for explaining paradoxical behavior. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 551–575. 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131444 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Teunissen, H. A. , Spijkerman, R. , Prinstein, M. J. , Cohen, G. L. , Engels, R. C. , & Scholte, R. H. (2012). Adolescents’ conformity to their peers’ pro‐alcohol and anti‐alcohol norms: The power of popularity. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research, 36, 1257–1267. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01728.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. van Schoor, G. , Bot, S. M. , & Engels, R. C. (2008). Alcohol drinking in young adults: The predictive value of personality when peers come around. European Addiction Research, 14, 125–133. 10.1159/000130416 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. van Straaten, I. , Engels, R. C. , Finkenauer, C. , & Holland, R. W. (2008). Sex differences in short‐term mate preferences and behavioral mimicry: A semi‐naturalistic experiment. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 902–911. 10.1007/s10508-007-9179-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Vik, P. W. , Culbertson, K. A. , & Sellers, K. (2000). Readiness to change drinking among heavy‐drinking college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 61, 674–680. 10.15288/jsa.2000.61.674 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Viviani, R. , Gron, G. , & Spitzer, M. (2005). Functional principal component analysis of fMRI data. Human Brain Mapping, 24, 109–129. 10.1002/hbm.20074 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Wagenmakers, E. J. , Love, J. , Marsman, M. , Jamil, T. , Ly, A. , Verhagen, J. , … Morey, R. D. (2018). Bayesian inference for psychology. Part II: Example applications with JASP. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25, 58–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Whelan, R. , Watts, R. , Orr, C. A. , Althoff, R. R. , Artiges, E. , Banaschewski, T. , Barker, G. J. , … Consortium, I. (2014). Neuropsychosocial profiles of current and future adolescent alcohol misusers. Nature, 512, 185–189. 10.1038/nature13402 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. White, A. M. , & Hingson, R. (2013). The burden of alcohol use: Excessive alcohol consumption and related consequences among college students. Alcohol Research, 35, 201–218. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Wiers, R. W. , Bartholow, B. D. , van den Wildenberg, E. , Thush, C. , Engels, R. C. , Sher, K. J. , … Stacy, A. W. (2007). Automatic and controlled processes and the development of addictive behaviors in adolescents: A review and a model. Pharmacology Biochemistry Behavior, 86, 263–283. 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.09.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Wiers, R. W. , Eberl, C. , Rinck, M. , Becker, E. S. , & Lindenmeyer, J. (2011). Retraining automatic action tendencies changes alcoholic patients’ approach bias for alcohol and improves treatment outcome. Psychological Science, 22, 490–497. 10.1177/0956797611400615 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Wiers, C. E. , Ludwig, V. U. , Gladwin, T. E. , Park, S. Q. , Heinz, A. , Wiers, R. W. , … Bermpohl, F. (2015). Effects of cognitive bias modification training on neural signatures of alcohol approach tendencies in male alcohol‐dependent patients. Addiction Biology, 20, 990–999. 10.1111/adb.12221 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Wiers, R. W. , Rinck, M. , Kordts, R. , Houben, K. , & Strack, F. (2010). Retraining automatic action‐tendencies to approach alcohol in hazardous drinkers. Addiction, 105, 279–287. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02775.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Wiers, C. E. , Stelzel, C. , Gladwin, T. E. , Park, S. Q. , Pawelczack, S. , Gawron, C. K. , … Bermpohl, F. (2015). Effects of cognitive bias modification training on neural alcohol cue reactivity in alcohol dependence. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 335–343. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13111495 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Witteman, J. , Post, H. , Tarvainen, M. , de Bruijn, A. , Perna Ede, S. , Ramaekers, J. G. , & Wiers, R. W. (2015). Cue reactivity and its relation to craving and relapse in alcohol dependence: A combined laboratory and field study. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 232, 3685–3696. 10.1007/s00213-015-4027-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Wöhr, M. , & Soren, K. (2017). Social behavior from rodents to humans. Berlin, Germany: Springer; 10.1007/978-3-319-47429-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  76. Zilverstand, A. , Huang, A. S. , Alia‐Klein, N. , & Goldstein, R. Z. (2018). Neuroimaging impaired response inhibition and salience attribution in human drug addiction: A systematic review. Neuron, 98, 886–903. 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

 

 

Data Availability Statement

All unthresholded T‐maps resulting from the fMRI analyses can be accessed at https://identifiers.org/neurovault.collection:5182. Data are available from the corresponding author upon request.


Articles from The European Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES