Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Am Geriatr Soc. 2019 Dec 27;68(4):803–808. doi: 10.1111/jgs.16282

Personality and Motoric Cognitive Risk Syndrome

Yannick Stephan 1,*, Angelina R Sutin 2, Brice Canada 3, Antonio Terracciano 4
PMCID: PMC7156302  NIHMSID: NIHMS1559563  PMID: 31880326

Abstract

Objectives:

To examine whether five major personality traits are related to the motoric cognitive risk (MCR) syndrome, a pre-dementia syndrome characterized by cognitive complaints and slow gait speed.

Design:

Cross-sectional

Setting:

Health and Retirement Study (HRS) and the National Health and Aging Trends Survey (NHATS).

Participants:

Dementia-free older adults aged from 65 to 107 years (N> 8,000).

Measurements:

In both samples, participants provided data on personality, cognitive complaints and measures of gait speed, as well as on demographic factors, physical activity, depressive symptoms and body mass index (BMI).

Results.

Across the two samples and a meta-analysis, higher neuroticism was related to higher risk of MCR (Combined OR= 1.32; 95% CI=1.21–1.45; p<.001), whereas higher extraversion (Combined OR= .71; 95% CI=.65-.79; p<.001) and conscientiousness (Combined OR=.70; 95% CI=.62-.78; p<.001) were associated with a lower likelihood of MCR. Higher openness was also related to a lower risk of MCR in the HRS and the meta-analysis (Combined OR= .77; 95% CI= .70-.85; p<.001), whereas agreeableness was protective only in the HRS (OR= .83; 95% CI= .74-.92; p<.001). Additional analyses indicated that physical activity, depressive symptoms and BMI partially accounted for these associations.

Conclusion.

This study adds to existing research on the factors related to the risk of MCR by showing an association with personality traits. Personality assessment may help to identify individuals that may be targeted by interventions focused on reducing the risk of MCR, and ultimately of dementia.

Keywords: Personality, motoric cognitive risk, cognitive complaint, walking speed


The motoric cognitive risk (MCR), a predementia syndrome defined by cognitive complaints and slower walking speed1,2,3, is receiving broad attention because MCR is associated with increased risk of developing cognitive impairment and dementia2,4,5, as well as disability, falls, and mortality5,6,7,8. For example, MCR is associated with a two to threefold higher risk of incident dementia2,4. Several factors increase risk of MCR, including lower education, older age, cardiovascular risk factors such as obesity and stroke, Parkinson’s disease, physical inactivity and depressive symptoms3,9,10. Furthermore, polygenic risk for obesity is related to MCR11. However, to our knowledge, no research has yet tested whether fundamental psychological dispositions, such as personality traits, are related to MCR.

Personality traits are the enduring pattern of thoughts, feelings and behaviors that characterize each person. The major personality traits described by the Five Factor Model (FFM)12 are neuroticism (the propensity to experience negative emotions and distress), extraversion (the tendency to be outgoing and gregarious), openness (the propensity to be curious and imaginative), agreeableness (the tendency to be trusting and caring), and conscientiousness (the tendency to be organized and self-disciplined). There is replicable evidence for the predictive role of personality traits for health across the lifespan13, including a range of factors implicated in MCR3,9, such as depressive symptoms14, physical inactivity15, and obesity16. Personality is also related to both mobility-related and cognitive outcomes. Higher neuroticism, low conscientiousness and extraversion, for example, are related to increases in frailty over time17,18, and higher neuroticism and lower conscientiousness predict risk of incident falls19. Similarly, higher neuroticism, lower conscientiousness and openness are related to steeper cognitive decline20,21, and higher risk of cognitive impairment and incident dementia22,23,24. Of particular relevance to this study, personality traits are related to the individual components of the MCR syndrome. Indeed, higher neuroticism predicts cognitive complaints21,25 and steeper declines in gait speed over time26. In contrast, higher conscientiousness, extraversion, and openness are associated with fewer cognitive complaints21,25 and slower decline in walking speed over time26. Agreeableness has been related to cognitive complaints21 but not to gait speed26. However, no study has yet examined whether these traits relate to the simultaneous presence of cognitive complaints and slow gait, as defined by MCR.

Using two large samples of older adults, the present study examined the associations between personality traits and the MCR syndrome. It was hypothesized that high neuroticism would be related to higher risk of MCR, whereas high conscientiousness, extraversion and openness would be associated with lower risk of MCR. No link was expected with agreeableness. Additional analysis tested whether physical inactivity, depressive symptoms and BMI accounted for the association between personality and MCR in both samples.

METHOD

Study Sample

Participants were drawn from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS) and the National Health and Aging Trends Study (NHATS). The HRS is approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Michigan, and the NHATS is approved by the Institutional Review Board at the Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants in each study. Both the HRS and NHATS public use files used in this study qualify as anonymized, de-identified, freely available datasets and secondary data analysis using these files qualify for exempt IRB status. Descriptive statistics are in Table 1.

Table 1.

Characteristics of the Samples

HRS NHATS
Variables M/% SD M/% SD
Age 74.01 6.68 78.48 7.06
Sex (% women) 57% - 58% -
Race (% White) 89% - 76% -
Education 12.77 2.82 5.47 2.22
BMIa 28.69 5.54 27.52 5.47
Physical inactivitya 2.63 1.06 58% -
Depressive symptoms 1.18 1.73 1.39 0.60
Cognition 15.20 3.72 19.35 4.42
Neuroticism 1.98 0.68 2.19 0.83
Extraversion 3.22 0.53 3.17 0.73
Openness 2.92 0.54 2.88 0.81
Agreeableness 3.54 0.46 3.61 0.49
Conscientiousness 3.36 0.46 3.28 0.68
Cognitive complaint (%) 41% - 32% -
Gait speed (m/s) 0.83 0.29 0.80 0.26
Motoric Cognitive Risk (%) 6% - 5% -

Note. HRS: N= 6300; NHATS: N= 2083

a

Due to missing data, Ns differ for BMI, physical activity, depressive symptoms, and cognition

See method section for differences in measures used in the two samples

The HRS is a nationally representative longitudinal study of adults aged 50 years and older. In 2006, an enhanced face-to-face interview was implemented for a random half of the sample that included a psychosocial questionnaire and physical functioning tests. The other half of the sample was interviewed in 2008. Timed gait was obtained only among individuals aged 65 years and older. Data from the 2006 and 2008 waves were combined. Only participants with complete data on personality traits, demographic factors, gait speed and self-rated memory were included, resulting in a sample of 6,525 individuals (with outliers removed on gait speed). Consistent with existing criteria3,4, individuals with dementia were excluded from the sample based upon validated methods in the HRS, using the modified Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (TICSm)27. A 27-point composite score was computed from a test of immediate and delayed recall, a serial 7 subtraction test, and a backward counting test. In line with existing criteria27, a score of 6 or less defined dementia, leading to the exclusion of 225 participants. The final sample was composed of 6,300 participants aged from 65 to 107 years (57% women, Mean= 74.01, SD= 6.68). Attrition analysis revealed that participants in the final sample were younger (d= .66), more educated (d=1.09), more likely to be white, more extraverted (d= .14), open (d= .48), agreeable (d= .17), and conscientious (d=.50) than those who were excluded.

The NHATS is a nationally representative prospective cohort study of Medicare enrollees aged 65 years and older. Personality was obtained from two-thirds of the sample. Personality was first assessed in 2013 for one-third of the sample, and in 2014 for the second third. Data from both waves were combined. With outliers removed on gait speed, a total of 2,237 participants provided complete data on personality, gait speed, memory perception, and demographic factors. The criteria developed by the NHATS investigators were used to identify dementia status28. Participants were classified as having dementia if a doctor had diagnosed the participant with dementia or Alzheimer’s disease, or if they reported a score of 2 or higher on the AD8 Dementia Screening Interview, or if they scored ≤1.5 SD below the mean on a cognitive tasks in at least two out of three domains: memory (immediate and delayed word recall), orientation (date, month, year, day of the week, President and Vice President) and executive function (clock drawing). Using these criteria, 154 individuals identified as having dementia were excluded, resulting in a final sample of 2,083 participants aged from 67 to 103 years (58% women, Mean= 78.48, SD= 7.06). Participants in the final sample were younger (d= .80), more educated (d= .41), more agreeable (d= .22), open (d= .25) and conscientious (d= .30) than those who were excluded.

Personality

The Midlife Development Inventory (MIDI)29 was used to assess personality traits in both the HRS and the NHATS. A 26-item version was used in the HRS and a 10-item version was used in the NHATS. In both samples, participants were asked to indicate the extent to which adjectives that assessed neuroticism (e.g. nervous), extraversion (e.g. talkative), openness (e.g. creative), agreeableness (e.g. warm), and conscientiousness (e.g. organized) described themselves on a scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4 (a lot).

Motoric Cognitive Risk Syndrome

The MCR syndrome is defined as the presence of both cognitive complaints and slow gait among individuals without dementia and immobility3,4. Past research has specified criteria for the diagnosis of MCR in both the HRS and the NHATS7. In the HRS, two questions were used to elicit cognitive complaints: «How would you rate your memory at the present time? Would you say it is excellent, very good, good, fair, or poor?» and «Compared with the previous interview, would you say your memory is better now, about the same, or worse than it was then?». In line with existing criteria7, a response of fair or poor for the first item or of worse for the second item was coded as indicative of cognitive complaints. These two questions were also used in the NHATS in addition to a third one: « In the last month, how often did your memory problems interfere with your daily activities? Would you say everyday, most days, some days, rarely or never?». Following past research7, a response of everyday, most days, and some days indicated cognitive complaints. Gait speed was measured using a 2.5 meters course in the HRS and a 3-m test in the NHATS. The best of two trials was used in the present study. In each sample, the distance (in meters) was divided by the time (in seconds). The following cut-off values defined by recent research7 were used to categorize slow gait: men < 75 years= 0.61 (HRS) and 0.69 (NHATS) m/s, men ≥75 years= 0.48(HRS) and 0.52 (NHATS) m/s, women < 75 years= 0.54 (HRS) and 0.59 (NHATS) m/s, and women ≥ 75 years= 0.42 (HRS) and 0.40 (NHATS) m/s. Seven individuals in the HRS and one individual in the NHATS were removed because they had values 3 standard deviations above or below the mean.

Covariates

Age, sex, education and race were controlled for in the two samples. Education was reported in years in the HRS and measured on a scale ranging from 1 (No schooling completed) to 9 (Master’s professional or doctoral degree) in the NHATS. A 8-item version of the Centers for Epidemiologic Research Depression (CES-D) scale was used in the HRS30. The sum of participants’ report of eight specific symptoms for much of the past week was used to create a total depressive symptom score. In the NHATS, the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ)-2 was used31. Participants were asked to report how often they had little interest or pleasure in doing things, and how often they felt down and depressed or hopeless during the last month, using a scale from 1 (not at all) to 4 (nearly every day). BMI was calculated as kg/m2, from measured height and weight in the HRS and from reported height and weight in the NHATS. Two items were used in the HRS to assess physical inactivity that asked how frequently participants participated in vigorous and moderate activities using a scale ranging from 1 (more than once a week) to 4 (hardly ever or never). In the NHATS, individuals were asked to report whether they ever spend time on vigorous activities in the last month (yes/no). Cognitive performance was the total TICSm score in the HRS and a global cognitive score in the NHATS that was the sum of performance on memory, orientation and executive function tests.

Statistical analysis

In both the HRS and the NHATS, logistic regression analysis was conducted to test whether personality traits were related to the likelihood of MCR syndrome. Age, sex, education and race were controlled for in each sample. Follow-up analysis included physical inactivity, depressive symptoms and BMI as additional covariates. A second follow-up analysis controlled for cognitive performance. Personality traits were standardized and examined separately. A random-effect meta-analysis using the Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software combined the estimates from both samples.

In sensitivity analyses, individuals with clinical levels on only one of the two components of the MCR (slow gait or cognitive complaints) were excluded from the analysis. In addition, the same analysis was conducted excluding individuals with cognitive impairment not dementia.

RESULTS

As hypothesized, higher neuroticism was related to higher likelihood of MCR whereas higher extraversion and conscientiousness were associated with a lower probability of MCR syndrome in both the HRS and the NHATS (see Table 2, Model 1). Also consistent with the hypothesis, higher openness was related to a lower likelihood of MCR in the HRS. Unexpectedly, an association was found between a higher agreeableness and lower likelihood of the syndrome in the HRS. Specifically, the results suggested that a one SD higher neuroticism was related to 21–36% higher risk of MCR respectively in the NHATS and the HRS. In contrast, for every SD increase in extraversion and conscientiousness, the likelihood of MCR decreased by 20–30% and 25–30% respectively in the NHATS and the HRS. A one SD higher score on openness and agreeableness was related to a 25% and 20% lower likelihood of MCR, respectively, in the HRS (see Table 2). The meta-analysis confirmed the overall pattern, except for agreeableness, which was not significantly related to MCR (see Table 2).

Table 2.

Summary of Logistic Regression Predicting Motoric Cognitive Risk Syndrome from Personality Traits

HRS NHATS Meta-Analysis
Model 1a Model 2b Model 1c Model 2d Model 1 Model 2
Predictors Odds ratio
(95%CI)
Odds ratio
(95%CI)
Odds ratio
(95%CI)
Odds ratio
(95%CI)
Odds ratio
(95%CI)
Odds ratio
(95%CI)
Neuroticism 1.36 (1.22–1.51)*** 1.15(1.01–1.29)* 1.21(1.00–1.47)* 1.12(0.91–1.38) 1.32 (1.21–1.45)*** 1.14(1.03–1.27)*
Extraversion .69 (.62-.76)*** .77(.69-.86)*** .79 (.66-.95)* .80(.66-.97)* .71 (.65-.79)*** .78(.71-.86)***
Openness .74(.66-.82)*** .79(.70-.88)*** .85(.70–1.03) .88(.72–1.08) .77(.70-.85)*** .81(.73-.90)***
Agreeableness .83(.74-.92)*** .86(.77-.97)* 1.04 (.86–1.27) 1.07(.87–1.31) .92(.74-.1.14) .94(.76–1.17)
Conscientiousness .67(.60-.74)*** .77(.69-.86)*** .76(.63-.91)** .84(.69–1.01) .70(.62-.78)*** .79(.72-.87)***

Note.

*

p<.05,

**

p<.01,

***

p< .001.

Model 1 is the association between each trait and motoric cognitive risk syndrome controlling for age, sex, education, and race. Model 2 is Model 1 plus the inclusion of BMI, physical activity, and depressive symptoms as additional covariates.

a

N= 6,300,

b

N= 5,955,

c

N= 2,083,

d

N= 2,039

The link between neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness was attenuated in the HRS when depressive symptoms, physical inactivity and BMI were included in the model. In the NHATS, the association between extraversion and MCR was reduced but persisted, whereas the associations with neuroticism and conscientiousness were reduced to non-significance (Table 2, Model 2). When both samples were combined, the meta-analysis revealed that the associations between neuroticism, extraversion, openness and conscientiousness persisted while including depressive symptoms, physical activity and BMI (Table 2).

Sensitivity analysis revealed that the overall pattern of associations remained unchanged when individuals with clinical levels on only one of the two components of the MCR were excluded. The only exception was the emergence of a significant contribution of openness on the risk of MCR in the NHATS (Odds Ratio : 0.73, 95%CI : 0.59–0.90, p< .01). The pattern of associations was also similar when excluding participants with cognitive impairment not dementia, with the exception of a nonsignificant association between neuroticism and MCR in the NHATS (Odds Ratio : 1.19, 95%CI : 0.97–1.46, p=.10). Additional analyses also adjusted for performance on cognitive tests. The association between personality traits and MCR was reduced but remained significant in the HRS. In the NHATS, the link between neuroticism and MCR became not significant (Odds Ratio: 1.21, 95%CI : 1.00–1.47, p=.05).

DISCUSSION

Based upon two large samples of older adults, the present study revealed that personality is related to the MCR syndrome. Consistent with the hypothesis, results from both samples and a meta-analysis revealed that higher neuroticism was related to higher likelihood of MCR, whereas high extraversion and conscientiousness were associated with lower risk, controlling for demographic factors. Higher openness was related to lower risk of MCR in the HRS and in the meta-analysis. An unexpected association was found between higher agreeableness and a lower probability of MCR in the HRS, but this link was not significant when estimates from the two samples were combined in the meta-analysis. This study adds to existing research on the factors related to the risk of MCR11 by showing for the first time an association with personality traits.

Neuroticism, extraversion, and conscientiousness were the most consistent personality correlates of MCR, as indicated by their replicable association with the syndrome across the two samples. These traits are related to several behavioral, psychological and health related factors implicated in the likelihood of MCR. In particular, high neuroticism, low extraversion, and low conscientiousness are associated with higher depressive symptoms14, lower physical activity15, and obesity16 that increase the risk of MCR3,9,10. In line with these studies, depressive symptoms, physical activity, and BMI partially accounted for the link between these traits and MCR, suggesting that these factors may act as mediators of this association.

Furthermore, high neuroticism may increase the likelihood of MCR syndrome because of its association with higher stress reactivity32. In contrast, high extraversion, openness, agreeableness and conscientiousness are related to lower stressor-related affect32, which may lower the risk of MCR. Personality may also relate to MCR syndrome through biological mechanisms. Indeed, higher extraversion, openness, conscientiousness and low neuroticism are related to lower risk of physiological dysfunction33 and better cardiovascular fitness34, that may benefit both gait speed and cognition35,36, resulting in lower MCR.

The present study is the first to identify an association between personality and the risk of MCR among older adults. Therefore, it adds to existing knowledge on the biological, behavioral and health-related factors related to this syndrome5. Furthermore, these findings extend existing research on the link between personality and dementia. Indeed, higher neuroticism, and lower conscientiousness, openness and agreeableness are related to higher risk of Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias22,23. Given that MCR is a significant pre-dementia syndrome4, it could be an intermediate manifestation of the risk of dementia associated with these traits. Furthermore, MCR is related to higher risk of falls6, and as such, it could mediate the association between higher neuroticism and lower conscientiousness and the risk of incident falls19.

The strengths of the present study include the examination of the association between personality traits and MCR, using two large samples of older adults, comprehensive assessments of personality, and established criteria for MCR. However, there are also limitations, such as the cross-sectional design. The present study focused on personality as a predictor of MCR, but reciprocal associations may exist, such that MCR may be related to personality changes over time. As for clinical dementia37, it is plausible that MCR would lead to maladaptive personality changes. Further research using longitudinal designs are needed to test for the reciprocal associations between personality and incident MCR. In addition, brief personality measures are used in the two samples. Future research is needed to explore whether specific personality facets are related to MCR. Furthermore, depressive symptoms were assessed using different instruments assessing different features of depression. In the HRS, depressive symptoms were computed on the basis of a report of symptoms over one week whereas the PHQ-2 asked only about anhedonia and quality of mood over the last month. Furthermore, participants included in the study had relatively more favorable personality profiles than those who were excluded because of a dementia diagnosis. It is likely that these participants were less apathetic, with intact initiative or motivation to complete the study. Although participants with dementia were excluded from the analysis, some individuals were characterized by cognitive impairment, which may be associated with their self-report of personality traits38, 39. Our sensitivity analyses indicated that the associations were similar when participants with mild cognitive impairments were excluded from the analyses. This pattern suggests that the results are not only due to the presence of cognitive impairments. Furthermore, a 36-year longitudinal study based on self-report ratings found no personality changes in the preclinical phase of dementia40, suggesting that personality changes become evident during the prodromal and clinical phases of dementia37,38,41. However, research based on observer rating of neuropsychiatric symptoms have found that these symptoms (also known as behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia and the related mild behavioral impairment) may precede cognitive symptoms in some individuals42, 43. To better understand the relation between personality traits and behavioral symptoms in the early phases leading to dementia, future longitudinal research should include both self-reports and observers’ ratings of personality traits and behavioral symptoms as there may be differences across what individuals self-report versus what informants observe.

Despite these limitations, the present study found that personality is related to MCR syndrome. High neuroticism was related to higher probability of MCR, whereas high extraversion, openness, agreeableness and conscientiousness were associated with a reduced likelihood of MCR among individuals without dementia. Therefore, this study contributes to a better understanding of risk for MCR. Indeed, personality assessment may help to identify individuals that may be targeted by interventions focused on reducing the risk of MCR, and ultimately of dementia.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS:

The HRS is funded by the National Institute on Aging (NIAU01AG009740) and conducted by the University of Michigan. HRS data are available at: https://hrs.isr.umich.edu/data-products/access-to-public-data. The NHATS is sponsored by the National Institute on Aging (NIA U01AG032947) and conducted by the Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health. NHATS data are available at: https://www.nhatsdata.org/.

Financial Disclosure: This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging of the National Institutes of Health under Awards Number R21AG057917 and R01AG053297. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Sponsor’s Role: The funding sources had no role in the design, methods, data analysis, manuscript preparation and reporting of this study.

Footnotes

Conflicts of Interest: The authors have no conflicts of interest.

References

  • 1.Chhetri JK, Chan P, Vellas B, Cesari M. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome: Predictor of dementia and age-related negative outcomes. Front Med (Lausanne) 2017; 4:166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Verghese J, Annweiler C, Ayers E, et al. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome: Multicountry prevalence and dementia risk. Neurology 2014;83(8):718–726. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Verghese J, Ayers E, Barzilai N, et al. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome: Multicenter incidence study. Neurology 2014; 83(24):2278–2284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Verghese J, Wang C, Lipton RB, Holtzer R. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome and the risk of dementia. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2013; 68(4):412–418. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Doi T, Shimada H, Makizako H, Tsutsumimoto K, Verghese J, Suzuki T. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome: Association with incident dementia and disability. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017; 59(1):77–84 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Callisaya ML, Ayers E, Barzilai N, et al. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome and falls risk: A multi-center study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016; 53(3):1043–1052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ayers E, Verghese J. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome and risk of mortality in older adults. Alzheimers Dement. 2016;12(5):556–564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Beauchet O, Sekhon H, Launay CP, Chabot J, Rolland Y, Schott AM, Allali G. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome and mortality: Results from the EPIDOS cohort. Eur J Neurol. 2019;26(5):794–e56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Doi T, Verghese J, Shimada H, Makizako H, Tsutsumimoto K, Hotta R, Nakakubo S, Suzuki T. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome: Prevalence and risk factors in japanese seniors. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2015;16(12): 1103.e21–1103.e25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Maguire FJ, Killane I, Creagh AP, Donoghue O, Kenny RA, Reilly RB. Baseline association of motoric cognitive risk syndrome with sustained attention, memory, and global cognition. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2018;19(1):53–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Sathyan S, Wang T, Ayers E, Verghese J. Genetic basis of motoric cognitive risk syndrome in the Health and Retirement Study. Neurology. 2019; 26;92(13):e1427–e1434. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.McCrae RR, John OP. An introduction to the five‐factor model and its applications. J Pers 1992; 60(2): 175–215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Friedman HS, Kern ML. Personality, well-being, and health. Annu Rev Psychol. 2014;65:719–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Koorevaar AML, Hegeman JM, Lamers F, Dhondt ADF, van der Mast RC, Stek ML, Comijs HC. Big Five personality characteristics are associated with depression subtypes and symptom dimensions of depression in older adults. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2017; 32(12): e132–e140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Sutin AR, Stephan Y, Luchetti M, Artese A, Oshio A, Terracciano A. The Five-Factor Model of personality and physical inactivity: A meta-analysis of 16 samples. J Res Pers. 2016; 63:22–28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Sutin AR, Terracciano A. Personality traits and body mass index: Modifiers and mechanisms. Psychol Health. 2015;31:259–275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Gale CR, Mõttus R, Deary IJ, Cooper C, Sayer AA. Personality and risk of frailty: the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Ann Behav Med. 2016;51(1):128–136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Stephan Y, Sutin AR, Canada B, Terracciano A. Personality and frailty: Evidence from four samples. J Res Pers. 2016; 66:46–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Canada B, Stephan Y, Sutin A, Terracciano A. Personality and falls among older adults: Evidence from a longitudinal cohort. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Caselli RJ, Dueck AC, Locke DE, Henslin BR, Johnson TA, Woodruff BK, Hoffman-Snyder C, Geda YE. Impact of personality on cognitive aging: A prospective cohort study. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc 2016; 22, 765–776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Luchetti M, Terracciano A, Stephan Y, Sutin AR. Personality and cognitive decline in older adults: Data from a longitudinal sample and meta-analysis. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2015;71(4):591–601. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Terracciano A, Sutin AR, An Y, et al. Personality and risk of Alzheimer’s disease: New data and meta-analysis. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10(2):179–186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Terracciano A, Stephan Y, Luchetti M, Albanese E, Sutin AR. Personality traits and risk of cognitive impairment and dementia. J Psychiatr Res. 2017; 89:22–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kaup AR, Harmell AL, Yaffe K. Conscientiousness is associated with lower risk of dementia among black and white older adults. Neuroepidemiology 2019; 52:86–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Slavin MJ, Brodaty H, Kochan NA, Crawford JD, Trollor JN, Draper B, Sachdev PS. Prevalence and predictors of “subjective cognitive complaints” in the Sydney Memory and Ageing Study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;18(8):701–710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Stephan Y, Sutin AR, Bovier-Lapierre G, Terracciano A. Personality and walking speed across adulthood: Prospective evidence from five samples. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2018; 9(7):773–780. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Crimmins EM, Kim JK, Langa KM, Weir DR. Assessment of cognition using surveys and neuropsychological assessment: the Health and Retirement Study and the Aging, Demographics, and Memory Study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2011;66 Suppl 1(Suppl 1):i162–i171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Kasper JD, Freedman VA, Spillman BC. Classification of persons by dementia status in the National Health and Aging Trends Study. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University School of Public Health; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Zimprich D, Allemand M, Lachman ME. Factorial structure and age-related psychometrics of the MIDUS personality adjective items across the lifespan. Psychol Assess 2012; 24: 173–86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Wallace R, Herzog AR, Ofstedal MB, Steffick D, Fonda S, Langa K. Documentation of Affective Functioning Measures in the Health and Retirement Study. Survey Research Center, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, 2000 [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB. The Patient Health Questionnaire-2: Validity of a two-item depression screener. Medical Care 2003; 41:1284–1292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Leger KA, Charles ST, Turiano NA, Almeida DM. Personality and stressor-related affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2016;111(6):917–928. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Stephan Y, Sutin AR, Luchetti M, Terracciano A. Allostatic load and personality: A 4-year longitudinal study. Psychosom Med. 2016;78(3):302–310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Terracciano A, Schrack JA, Sutin AR, Chan W, Simonsick EM, Ferrucci L. Personality, metabolic rate and aerobic capacity. PLoS One. 2013;8(1):e54746. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Booth T, Royle NA, Corley J, et al. Association of allostatic load with brain structure and cognitive ability in later life. Neurobiol Aging. 2015;36(3):1390–1399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Rosso AL, Sanders JL, Arnold AM, et al. Multisystem physiologic impairments and changes in gait speed of older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;70(3):319–324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Islam M, Mazumder M, Schwabe-Warf D, Stephan Y, Sutin AR, Terracciano A. Personality changes with dementia from the informant perspective: New data and meta-analysis. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2019; 20(2):131–137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Caselli RJ, Langlais BT, Dueck AC, et al. Personality changes during the transition from cognitive health to mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2018; 66(4):671–678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Terracciano A, Stephan Y, Luchetti M, Sutin AR. Cognitive impairment, dementia, and personality stability among older adults. Assessment. 2018; 25(3):336–347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Terracciano A, An Y, Sutin AR, Thambisetty M, Resnick SM. Personality change in the preclinical phase of Alzheimer disease. JAMA Psychiatry 2017; 74(12): 1259–1265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Waggel SE, Lipnicki DM, Delbaere K, Kochan NA, Draper B, Andrews G, Sachdev PS, Brodaty H. Neuroticism scores increase with late-life cognitive decline. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2015; 30(9):985–993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Wise EA, Rosenberg PB, Lyketsos CG, Leoutsakos JM. Time course of neuropsychiatric symptoms and cognitive diagnosis in National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Centers volunteers. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2019; 11:333–339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Creese B, Brooker H, Ismail Z, Wesnes KA, Hampshire A, Khan Z, Megalogeni M, Corbett A, Aarsland D, Ballard C. Mild behavioral impairment as a marker of cognitive decline in cognitively normal older adults. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2019; 27(8):823–834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES