Abstract
Background and Aims
Reinforcing value, an index of motivation for a drug, is commonly measured using behavioral economic purchase tasks. State-oriented purchase tasks are sensitive to phasic manipulations, but with heterogeneous methods and findings. The aim of this meta-analysis was to characterize the literature examining manipulations of reinforcing value, as measured by purchase tasks and multiple-choice procedures, to inform etiological models and treatment approaches
Methods
A random-effects meta-analysis of published findings in peer-reviewed articles. Following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) protocol, studies were gathered through searches in PsycINFO and PubMed/MEDLINE (published 22 May 2018). Searches returned 34 unique studies (aggregate sample n = 2402; average sample size = 68.94) yielding 126 effect sizes. Measurements included change (i.e. Cohen’s d) in six behavioral economic indices (intensity, breakpoint, Omax, Pmax, elasticity, cross-over point) in relation to six experimental manipulations (cue exposure, stress/negative affect, reinforcer magnitude, pharmacotherapy, behavioral interventions, opportunity cost).
Results
Cue exposure (d range = 0.25–0.44, all Ps < 0.05) and reinforcer magnitude [d = 0.60; 95% confidence interval (CI) = 0.18, 1.01; P < 0.005] manipulations resulted in significant increases in behavioral economic demand across studies. Stress/negative affect manipulations also resulted in a small, significant increase in Omax (d = 0.18; 95% CI = 0.01, 0.34; P = 0.03); all other effect sizes for negative affect/stress were non-significant, albeit similar in size (d range = 0.14–0.18). In contrast, pharmacotherapy (d range = −0.37 to −0.49; Ps < 0.04), behavioral intervention (d = −0.36 to −1.13) and external contingency (d = −1.42; CI = −2.30, −0.54; P = 0.002) manipulations resulted in a significant decrease in intensity. Moderators (substance type) explained some of the heterogeneity in findings across meta-analyses.
Conclusions
In behavioral economic studies, purchase tasks and multiple-choice procedures appear to provide indices that are sensitive to manipulations found to influence motivation to consume addictive substances in field experiments.
Keywords: Alcohol purchase task, behavioral economics, behavioral intervention, craving, cue exposure, demand, pharmacotherapy, reinforcer pathology, reinforcing value, stress
INTRODUCTION
Understanding the behavior-strengthening properties of a commodity, known as reinforcing value, is important for understanding addictive behaviors, as it quantifies the degree of motivation to consume the addictive substance. Reinforcing value has been operationalized using a variation of fixed-ratio tasks known as behavioral economic (BE) demand curves. Demand curves can be derived from hypothetical behavioral tasks that ask participants to purchase and consume a substance across a series of escalating prices [1]. During an alcohol hypothetical purchase task (HPT), for example, participants are presented with a brief hypothetical drinking scenario, after which they report the number of drinks they would consume across a series of escalating prices. HPTs provide an efficient, reliable and valid alternative to estimating reinforcing value using actual drug self-administration paradigms [2,3]. Although purchase task methodology follows a basic pattern, there is wide variability in methodological characteristics and data analysis, as outlined in recent reviews [4,5].
Consumption decisions are plotted on curves that illustrate consumption and expenditure values as a function of drink price (Fig. 1). These curves are used to estimate demand indices, including intensity (consumption when cost is zero), breakpoint (price at which consumption reaches zero), elasticity (rate of decline in consumption as prices increase), Omax (maximum expenditure) and Pmax (price at which demand becomes elastic). Each index represents a theoretically distinct aspect of demand and loads onto one of two factors [7,8]: amplitude, reflecting absolute level of hypothetical consumption (e.g. intensity, Omax) and persistence, reflecting sensitivity to price (e.g. Pmax, elasticity, breakpoint, Omax). Elasticity is thought to represent the ‘essential value’ of the reinforcer [9] and is typically derived from an exponential or exponentiated equation [9,10]. A related measure, known as the multiple-choice procedure, asks participants to choose between a unit of a substance and increasing amounts of money. The price at which choices switch from the substance to money is known as the cross-over point, and is another index of reinforcing value. These indices are correlated with actual behavior and thus represent an efficient way to measure the reinforcing effects of a substance [11–16].
BE demand aggregates a series of cost–benefit decisions between a commodity and ascending monetary values and has typically functioned as a tonic (trait) level index of motivation to use a commodity [17]. Thus, consistent with theory, greater demand amplitude or persistence demarcates greater severity of engagement in a range of substances and behaviors, including alcohol [18–20], cigarettes [21], cannabis [22], opioids [23], heroin [24], cocaine [25], gambling [26], tanning [27,28] and internet use [29,30]. Demand indices, and intensity and Omax in particular, are also associated with a variety of clinical outcomes [31,32]. Although individual differences in demand are generally stable over time [17,33], BE theory recognizes that phasic (state-like) factors may acutely influence demand. For example, several studies have found that the presence of a next-day responsibility, which introduces an opportunity cost for the drinking behavior, decreases alcohol demand [34,35]. Other research suggests that manipulations such as cue exposure [36], stress [37], behavioral therapy [32] pharmacotherapy [38] and reinforcer magnitude [39] also dynamically influence demand.
Effect sizes for studies examining manipulations of demand have also demonstrated wide heterogeneity across indices and commodities [37,40]. Understanding conditions that acutely influence delay discounting, a behavioral economic measure of impulsivity, has long been of interest [41] and has recently been clarified through a meta-analysis [42]. Thus, it is also necessary to systematically quantify the impact of manipulations intended to increase or decrease substance demand. This would aggregate effects of various phasic factors (e.g. cue exposure, stress/negative affect, reinforcer magnitude, pharmacotherapy, behavioral interventions and opportunity costs) which could, in turn, increase understanding of conditions that influence reinforcer value and contribute to the identification of state-level demand indices as surrogate end-points for screening novel interventions. Indeed, change in demand has been shown to predict changes in subsequent alcohol consumption [31,32]. Thus, the aims of this study were to: (1) meta-analyze the published peer-reviewed findings on manipulations of demand (including moderation by substance type and demand parameter) and (2) examine the presence of publication bias (i.e. bias of strength of effect size due to lack of unpublished studies) in this literature.
METHOD
Study selection
Studies were identified using PubMed and PsycINFO (to 22 May 2018) meeting the following inclusion criteria: (i) published, peer-reviewed studies on humans written in English; (ii) within- or between-subjects experimental designs utilizing at least two groups; (iii) reported BE demand, operationalized as indices extracted from purchase tasks (actual or hypothetical) or multiple choice procedure questionnaires, measured before and after (for within-subjects designs) or after manipulation (for between-subjects designs); and (iv) examined substances or drugs. No inclusion restrictions were made on timing or setting.
One author (S.A.) performed all searches. After removing duplicates, two reviewers (S.A. and A.D.) independently screened titles and abstracts of all articles returned in the database searches for clearly eligible or ineligible studies and compared results. Studies that were not marked clearly eligible or ineligible by both reviewers were discussed. Next, a reviewer (S.A.) reviewed the full manuscript of any study in the eligible group and provided brief justification for removal of any studies deemed ineligible at this stage. A second reviewer (A.D.) reviewed each decision, and the two reviewers came to an agreement before studies were removed. Finally, the authors checked reference lists of any recent systematic reviews or meta-analyses examining BE demand (S.A.). Figure 2 depicts the study selection procedure, which followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) standards [43], This review was pre-registered in PROSPERO (CRD42017062 501). The analyses for the current study were not pre-registered.
Meta-analytical sample characteristics
Primary meta-analyses were conducted for manipulations with at least three studies. Characteristics of the studies included can be found in Table 1. There were 34 unique studies that met the inclusion criteria, yielding a total of 126 effect sizes. A subgroup of studies were eligible, but data were unavailable [13,60,68–81]. Further, several studies did not fit into a manipulation category with enough effect sizes and were thus not included [47,48,82–95], Each study reported an average sample size of 68.94 (range = 7–328; aggregate sample = 2402). Studies reported demand effect sizes for alcohol (k = 78), cigarettes/nicotine (k = 27), cannabis/tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) (k = 7), cocaine (k = 7), methylphenidate (k = 3), hydromorphone (k = 1), triazolam (k = 1), nabilone (k = 1) and gamma hydroxybutyrate (GHB) (k = 1) using the following indices: intensity (k = 35), cross-over point (k = 22), Omax (k = 20), elasticity (k = 20), breakpoint (k =18) and Pmax (k = 11).
Table 1.
Study | Within versus between | Group | n | Commodity | Manipulation | Number of indices | Number of prices | Price range |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Acker et al. 2013 [44] | Within | Adult smokers | 47 | Cigarettes | Cue exposure | 5 | 18 | $0–5 |
MacKillop et al. 2010 [36] | Within | Heavy drinking college students | 92 | Alcohol | Cue exposure | 5 | 19 | $0–1120 |
Metrik et al. 2016 [45] | Within | Emerging adult cannabis users | 88 | Cannabis | Cue exposure | 4 | 22 | $0–10 |
Owens et al. 2015 [46] | Within | Adult heavy drinkers | 84 | Alcohol | Cue exposure | 3 | 10 | $0–40 |
Rose et al. 2018 [47] | Within | Social drinkers | 32 | Alcohol | Cue exposure | 5 | 25 | £0–15 |
Spelman & Simons, 2018 [48] | Between | Heavy drinking college students | 58 | Alcohol | Cue exposure | 5 | 24 | $0–15 |
Amlung & MacKillop, 2014 [37]* | Within | Emerging adult heavy drinkers | 84 | Alcohol | Negative mood induction | 5 | 18 | $0–15 |
Dahne et al. 2016 [40] | Within | Emerging adult smokers | 73 | Cigarettes | Negative mood induction | 5 | 48 | $0–9 |
Owens et al. 2015 [49] | Within | Emerging adult heavy drinkers | 62 | Alcohol | Negative mood induction | 4 | 16 | $0–1120 |
Rousseau et al. 2011 [50]* | Between | Heavy drinking college students | 44 | Alcohol | Negative mood induction | 1 | 42 | $0–20 |
Bujarski et al. 2012 [38] | Within | Heavy drinking Asian Americans | 32 | Alcohol | Pharmacotherapy | 4 | 16 | $0–120 |
Madden & Kalman, 2010 [51] | Between | Adult smokers | 60 | Cigarettes | Pharmacotherapy | 2 | 26 | $0–1120 |
Murphy et al. 2017 [52] | Within | Adult smokers with a SUD | 110 | Cigarettes | Pharmacotherapy | 3 | 41 | $0–$64 |
Roache et al. 2005* [53] | Within | Adult cocaine users | 11 | Cocaine | Pharmacotherapy | 2 | 70 | $0.25–25 |
Schlienz et al. 2014 [54] | Between | Adult treatment-seeking smokers | 60 | Cigarettes | Pharmacotherapy | 3 | 19 | $0–1120 |
Gentile et al. 2012 [55] | Between | College students | 230 | Alcohol | Opportunity cost | 6 | 19 | $0–15 |
Gilbert et al. 2014 [34] | Within | Heavy drinking college students | 78 | Alcohol | Opportunity cost | 8 | 1 | $0 |
Skidmore & Murphy, 2011 [35] | Within | Heavy drinking college students | 207 | Alcohol | Opportunity cost | 10 | 17 | $0–20 |
Teeters & Murphy, 2015 [20] | Within | Drinking college students | 328 | Alcohol | Opportunity cost | 3 | 17 | $0–20 |
Bulley & Gullo, 2017 [56] | Within | College students | 42 | Alcohol | Behavioral intervention | 5 | 19 | $0–1120 |
Dennhardt et al. 2015 [31] | Within | Heavy drinking college students | 97 | Alcohol | Behavioral intervention | 6 | 19 | $0–20 |
Murphy et al. 2015 [32] | Between | Heavy drinking college students | 133 | Alcohol | Behavioral intervention | 4 | 17 | $0–20 |
Snider et al. 2016 [57] | Within | Adult smokers | 39 | Alcohol | Behavioral Intervention | 2 | 5 | $0–1 |
Stein et al. 2018 [58] | Between | Adult smokers | 117 | Cigarettes | Behavioral intervention | 2 | 13 | $0–64 |
Weidberg et al. 2018 [59] | Within | Treatment-seeking smokers | 92 | Cigarettes | Behavioral intervention | 5 | 19 | €0–1000 |
Benson et al. 2009 [39]* | Within | Drinking college males | 27 | Alcohol | Magnitude | 1 | 17 | $0–20 |
Johnsonet al. 2013 [60]* | Within | Adult drinkers | 14 | GHB, ethanol | Magnitude | 2 | 70 | $0.25–25 |
Johnson et al. 2010 [61] * | Within | Adult cocaine users | 14 | Nicotine, cocaine | Magnitude | 2 | 65 | −$40 to 40 |
Lile et al. 2010 [62]* | Within | Adult cannabis users | 7 | THC, nabilone, methylphenidate | Magnitude | 3 | 9 | $0.10–632 |
Lile et al. 2010 [63]* | Within | Adult cannabis users | 8 | THC, triazolam, hydromorphone, methylphenidate | Magnitude | 4 | 9 | $0.25–64 |
Lile et al. 2013 [64]* | Within | Adult cannabis users | 7 | THC | Magnitude | 1 | 9 | $0.25–64 |
Roache et al. 2005 [53]* | Within | Adult cocaine users | 11 | Cocaine | Magnitude | 1 | 70 | $0.25–25 |
Rush et al. 1999 [65]* | Within | Adult cocaine users | 9 | Cocaine | Magnitude | 1 | 48 | $0.05–5.45 |
Stoops et al. 2003 [66]* | Within | Adult stimulant users | 8 | Methylphenidate | Magnitude | 1 | 9 | $0.25–64 |
Walsh et al. 2010 [67]* | Within | Adult cocaine users | 8 | Cocaine | Magnitude | 2 | 48 | $0.05–5.45 |
Sample size reflects combined sample for between-subjects studies; for all studies, the largest sample size analyzed was reported. Individual effect sizes for all indices reported in each study can be found in the Supporting information. For studies reporting the same demand index multiple times at various magnitudes (e.g. cigarette demand for cigarettes with low, medium and high amounts of nicotine), we only included effect sizes for the greatest magnitude difference to minimize violations of the assumptions of independence. Where two separate versions of a multiple-choice procedure or purchase task was used, the largest price range and number of prices were reported in the far-right columns.
These studies included a multiple-choice procedure and reported an effect size for cross-over point.
GHB = gamma hydroxybutyrate; THC = tetrahydrocannabinol.
Meta-analytical approach
The primary effect size was Cohen’s d. Results were calculated using a random-effects model based on major differences in study designs. Effects for within-subjects studies were quantified with a modified Cohen’s d effect size that accounts for the shared variance between measurement points [96]. Elasticity effect sizes were multiplied by −1 before use in analyses due to inverse relations with other demand indices. For studies reporting more than one effect size, individual meta-analyses were perfonned to acquire a pooled effect size for use in primary meta-analyses.
Meta-analyses for each demand index with three or more independent effect sizes were perfonned for six theoretically distinct manipulation conditions: (1) controlled exposure to substance cues (e.g. holding a lit cigarette; inhaling the smell of a beer), intended to induce craving (i.e. cue exposure [97]); (2) exposure to stress induction (e.g. guided imagery or Trier Social Stress Test paradigms [98,99]); (3) increases in the magnitude of the dose of the active ingredient (reinforcer magnitude); (4) phannacotherapies (i.e. targeting pharmacological mechanisms to influence substance use); (5) behavioral interventions (i.e. episodic future thinking interventions, brief motivational interventions and cognitive behavioral therapy for smoking); and (6) introduction of a potential loss from an alternative when choosing to engage with the substance (e.g. drinking and driving or a next-day class or test; opportunity cost). Moderator analyses examined unique effects between commodities (alcohol, cigarettes, etc.) when two or more separate studies contributed effect sizes.1 Moderators were tested using the Q statistic, which measures the between-group difference in a mixed-effects analysis [100]. To examine the influence of each individual effect size on the aggregate Cohen’s d for each meta-analysis, we conducted sensitivity analyses that re-estimate the overall effect with each effect size systematically excluded (i.e. jack-knife analysis [101]). Publication bias was assessed based on four indices (Table 3): (1) the classic fail-safe n, (2) Orwin’s fail-safe n at 50% of the reported effect size [102]; the two-tailed Begg–Mazumdar test [103]; and (4) the one-tailed Egger’s test [104]. Results for Hedge’s g and publication bias are reported in the Supporting information, in addition to all forest plots and sensitivity analyses.
Table 3.
k | Fail-safe n | Orwin’s fail-safe n | Kendall’s τ (P-value) | Egger’s test (P-value) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cue exposure | |||||
Intensity | 6 | > 56 | 7 | 0.07 (0.85) | 2.40 (0.05) |
Breakpoint | 6 | > 53 | 7 | 0.33 (0.35) | 2.29 (0.15) |
Omax | 6 | > 100 | 6 | 0.20 (0.57) | 3.56 (0.08) |
Pmax | 5 | > 13 | 5 | 0.40 (0.33) | 1.31 (0.33) |
Elasticity | 4 | > 27 | 5 | 1.00 (0.04) | 5.42 (< 0.001) |
Stress/negative affect | |||||
Intensity | 3 | > 1 | 3 | 1.00 (0.12) | 6.54 (0.02) |
Breakpoint | 3 | > 1 | 4 | 0.33 (0.60) | 6.57 (0.20) |
Omax | 3 | > 4 | 4 | 0.33 (0.60) | 4.64 (0.33) |
Elasticity | 3 | > 2 | 3 | 1.00 (0.12) | 5.34 (0.09) |
Reinforcer magnitude | |||||
Cross-over point | 10 | > 65 | 10 | 0.24 (0.33) | 1.78 (0.07) |
Pharmacotherapies | |||||
Intensity | 4 | > 26 | 5 | 1.00 (0.04) | 6.97 (0.08) |
Breakpoint | 3 | > 7 | 4 | 1.00 (0.12) | 0.67 (0.15) |
Behavioral interventions | |||||
Intensity | 6 | > 170 | 7 | −0.33 (0.35) | −7.05 (0.04) |
Omax | 4 | > 27 | 4 | −0.67 (0.17) | −10.61 (0.25) |
Elasticity | 5 | > 21 | 6 | 0.20 (0.62) | −0.33 (0.48) |
Opportunity costs | |||||
Intensity | 4 | > 344 | 5 | −0.33 (0.50) | −9.46 (0.15) |
k = number of effect sizes; d = Cohen’s d effect size; SE = standard error.
RESULTS
Cue exposure
Cue exposure was associated with significant, small-to-medium magnitude increases in intensity, breakpoint, Omax, Pmax and elasticity (Table 2). There was significant heterogeneity across cue exposure effect sizes for all demand indices except breakpoint; moderator analysis for studies examining alcohol demand demonstrated larger, significant effect sizes for intensity (d = 0.34), breakpoint (d = 0.39), Omax (d = 0.51), Pmax (d = 0.31) and elasticity (d = 0.77); there were not enough effect sizes to examine moderator analyses for other commodities. There was mixed evidence of publication bias across demand indices (Table 3). The fail-safe n estimates indicated that a large number of studies would need to be unpublished for the aggregate two-tailed P-value to exceed 0.05 for intensity, breakpoint, Omax and elasticity. The fail-safe n was smaller for Pmax, indicating a greater likelihood of publication bias for this index (> 13). The Orwin’s fail-safe n, however, indicated that a small number of studies would need to be unpublished to reduce effect sizes in half, which was consistent across demand indices (range = 5–7). The Begg–Muzumdar and Egger’s tests were non-significant for all demand indices except for Pmax, suggesting that results for Pmax may be biased.
Table 2.
k | d | SE | variance | Lower limit | Upper limit | Z-value | P-value | Heterogeneity statistics |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Q | pQ | I2 | |||||||||
Cue exposure | |||||||||||
Intensity | 6 | 0.29 | 0.09 | 0.01 | 0.12 | 0.45 | 3.35 | 0.001 | 18.12 | 0.003 | 72.41 |
Breakpoint | 6 | 0.33 | 0.08 | 0.01 | 0.18 | 0.48 | 4.34 | < 0.001 | 9.13 | 0.10 | 45.26 |
Omax | 6 | 0.44 | 0.09 | 0.01 | 0.27 | 0.62 | 4.98 | < 0.001 | 12.89 | 0.02 | 61.22 |
Pmax | 5 | 0.25 | 0.12 | 0.01 | 0.02 | 0.48 | 2.09 | 0.04 | 11.94 | 0.02 | 66.49 |
Elasticity | 4 | 0.43 | 0.16 | 0.03 | 0.11 | 0.75 | 2.65 | 0.008 | 15.11 | 0.002 | 80.14 |
Stress/negative affect | |||||||||||
Intensity | 3 | 0.17 | 0.16 | 0.03 | −0.14 | 0.49 | 1.08 | 0.28 | 16.25 | < 0.001 | 87.69 |
Breakpoint | 3 | 0.14 | 0.11 | 0.01 | −0.08 | 0.36 | 1.26 | 0.21 | 6.54 | 0.04 | 69.40 |
Omax | 3 | 0.18 | 0.08 | 0.007 | 0.01 | 0.34 | 2.13 | 0.03 | 3.41 | 0.18 | 41.39 |
Elasticity | 3 | 0.16 | 0.12 | 0.01 | −0.07 | 0.39 | 1.35 | 0.18 | 8.01 | 0.03 | 75.00 |
Reinforcer magnitude | |||||||||||
Cross-over Point | 10 | 0.60 | 0.21 | 0.04 | 0.18 | 1.01 | 2.82 | 0.005 | 46.86 | < 0.001 | 80.80 |
Pharmacotherapies | |||||||||||
Intensity | 4 | −0.49 | 0.24 | 0.06 | −0.96 | −0.02 | −2.04 | 0.04 | 15.90 | 0.001 | 81.13 |
Breakpoint | 3 | −0.37 | 0.17 | 0.03 | −0.70 | −0.04 | −2.21 | 0.03 | 4.56 | 0.10 | 56.10 |
Behavioral interventions | |||||||||||
Intensity | 6 | −10.13 | 0.32 | 0.10 | −1.75 | −0.51 | −3.56 | < 0.001 | 84.42 | < 0.001 | 94.08 |
Omax | 4 | −0.48 | 0.20 | 0.04 | −0.87 | −0.09 | −2.42 | 0.02 | 15.63 | 0.001 | 80.80 |
Elasticity | 5 | −0.36 | 0.25 | 0.07 | −0.86 | 0.14 | −1.43 | 0.15 | 37.44 | < 0.001 | 89.32 |
Opportunity costs | |||||||||||
Intensity | 4 | −1.42 | 0.45 | 0.20 | −2.30 | −0.54 | −3.15 | 0.002 | 196.05 | < 0.001 | 98.47 |
k = number of effect sizes; d = Cohen’s d effect size; SE = standard error.
Negative affect/stress
Negative affect/stress was associated with significant, small changes in demand Omax. The effect of negative affect/stress was not significant for any other demand index (Table 2). There was significant heterogeneity in effect sizes for intensity, breakpoint and elasticity, but not for Omax. Moderator analysis for studies examining alcohol demand demonstrated larger and significant effect sizes for intensity (d = 0.31, P = 0.001), breakpoint (d = 0.25, P = 0.003), Omax (d = 0.26, P = 0.002) and elasticity (d = 0.26, P = 0.04); there were not enough effect sizes to examine moderator analyses for other commodities. There was evidence of potential publication bias for stress/negative affect studies. The fail-safe n and Orwin’s fail-safe n was low across demand indices. The Begg–Muzumdar test was non-significant for all demand indices. The Egger’s test was also non-significant for all demand indices except for intensity.
Reinforcer magnitude
Reinforcer magnitude was associated with a significant, medium magnitude increase in the cross-over point index of reinforcing value (Table 2). There was significant heterogeneity across reinforcer magnitude effect sizes. Moderator analysis demonstrated a large effect of reinforcer magnitude on the cross-over point for alcohol (d = 1.42, P < 0.001), a medium-to-large effect on the cross-over point for cocaine (d = 0.64, P < 0.001) and a small, non-significant effect on the cross-over point for methylphenidate (d = 0.11, P = 0.17). Publication bias indices suggested a lack of publication bias for reinforcer magnitude studies. The fail-safe n was robust, and Orwin’s fail-safe n indicated that 10 studies would need to be unpublished for the cross-over point effect size to be reduced by half. Further, both Begg–Muzumdar and Egger’s tests were non-significant.
Pharmacotherapy
Pharmacotherapy interventions examined effects of bupropion (dopamine and norepinephrine agonist; used for smoking cessation and depression), isradipine (anti-hypertensive; high blood pressure treatment), naltrexone (opioid antagonist; alcohol or opioid dependence maintenance) and varenicline (partial nicotinic agonist; smoking cessation). Pharmacotherapy interventions were associated with significant, small-to-medium magnitude reductions in demand intensity and breakpoint (Table 2). There was significant heterogeneity among pharmacotherapy effect sizes for intensity, but not for breakpoint. Moderator analysis for studies examining cigarette demand demonstrated the same effect size for intensity (d = −0.49, P = 0.16) and a slightly smaller effects size for breakpoint (d = −0.33, P = 0.31), although both were non-significant due to limited power. The fail-safe n values were large for intensity and small for breakpoint. Orwin’s fail-safe n values were small for both intensity and breakpoint. The Begg–Muzumdar’s test was significant for intensity; both the Begg–Muzumdar and Egger’s tests were non-significant for breakpoint.
Behavioral interventions
Behavioral interventions were associated with significant, medium magnitude reductions in Omax and large effect size reductions in intensity (Table 2). The effect of behavioral interventions on demand elasticity was non-significant. There was significant heterogeneity across effect sizes for intensity, Omax and elasticity. Moderator analysis for studies examining alcohol demand demonstrated smaller effect size for intensity (d = −0.70, P = 0.003), Omax (d = −0.28, P = 0.005) and elasticity (d = −0.14, P = 0.27). Moderator analysis for studies examining cigarette demand demonstrated larger, albeit non-significant, effect sizes for both intensity (d = −2.02, P = 0.22) and elasticity (d = −0.80, P = 0.22). Fail-safe n estimates were large for intensity and moderate for Omax and elasticity. Across demand indices, Orwin’s fail-safe n indicated that a small number of effect sizes would be necessary to reduce the effect size by half. The Begg–Muzumdar and Egger’s tests were non-significant for all demand indices, with the exception of the Egger’s test for intensity.
Opportunity cost
Opportunity costs were associated with significant, medium-to-large magnitude reductions in demand intensity (Table 2). There was significant heterogeneity across opportunity cost effect sizes. All studies examined changes in alcohol demand; thus, moderator analyses were not explored. Fail-safe n estimates were large for intensity. Orwin’s fail-safe n indicated that a small number of effect sizes would be necessary to reduce the effect size by half. Both the Begg–Muzumdar and Egger’s tests were non-significant.
DISCUSSION
Our results suggest that behavioral economic demand is sensitive to a variety of experimental manipulations that directly result in significant fluctuations in demand and, theoretically, strength of motivation to use substances. Thus, although when measured as a general trait-like property, demand is a stable individual difference measure of substance use severity [5], these results suggest that there are a variety of contextual factors that can contribute to dynamic within-person fluctuations in demand. Our findings suggest that cue exposure increases phasic motivation (across demand indices) for drugs, possibly resulting in triggered episodes of use/relapse even in the context of a more general desire to abstain. Negative affect/stress effect sizes for alcohol demand were significant, suggesting that negative affect increases motivation to consume alcohol. These results are consistent with research elucidating a small effect of negative affect on drug craving [105], and with novel perspectives highlighting the positively reinforcing aspects of alcohol as a pathway connecting internalizing psychopathology and alcohol misuse [106–108]. Negative affect/stress effect sizes for tobacco demand were small and non-significant, although not enough studies were available to examine aggregate effect sizes. Reinforcer magnitude had a medium effect on demand for alcohol and cocaine, suggesting that more potent forms of these substances may result in greater consumption for these substances.
The opportunity cost meta-analysis indicated that increasing the response costs of drinking (e.g. by stipulating a next-day academic responsibility) may effectively compete with the immediate reinforcement associated with alcohol. It is unclear if this effect is specific to alcohol or if opportunity costs may also have an effect on motivation for other substances, as the current analyses only examined alcohol. Our results also supported the efficacy of interventions to decrease demand for both alcohol and cigarettes. Interestingly, many of the interventions we examined were brief (i.e. typically no longer than two 1-hour sessions) and resulted in subsequent change in actual alcohol consumption following the intervention [31,32]. It is possible that more intensive treatments would result in even greater decreases in demand. Indeed, one reviewed study found a large reduction in demand among patients in a smoking cessation treatment program [59].
The studies that examined the effects of pharmacotherapy on demand for alcohol or cigarettes generally observed reductions. Naltrexone (target substance alcohol) and bupropion (target substance nicotine) were each associated with reductions in a single study; thus, these promising results require replication. Varenicline was effective in one study but not in another, and also requires additional research. Although further research is required, these results imply that purchase tasks are consistently sensitive to manipulations that influence motivation for substances and may be viable surrogate end-points for drugs seeking Food and Drug Administration or Health Canada approval as a means of decreasing substance misuse, and more generally to help guide regulatory decision-making [109].
The meta-analysis revealed differences in effect size by demand index, possibly suggesting different mechanistic properties across manipulations. For example, our results suggest that demand intensity is sensitive to change in manipulations attempting to reduce demand, but less likely to increase among during manipulations trying to increase demand (indicating a possible ceiling effect). Further, persistence indices (breakpoint, elasticity and Omax) are generally more susceptible to increases in demand from cue exposure compared to intensity. Most interventions attempting to reduce demand target peak level consumption, the aspect of consumption most related to harm. In contrast, cue exposure may reflect a strong desire to consume at least some of the substance, rather than a high total amount per se, and thus manifest in change in persistence. More research should be performed throughout demand indices in each manipulation condition in order to extend our understanding of how different manipulations influence theoretically distinct aspects of motivation to consume a substance.
Taken together, our results extend knowledge of behavioral economic models of substance misuse by characterizing phasic factors that reliably change reinforcing value for different commodities, suggesting that a tonic- phasic reinforcer pathology approach may best fit models of substance use. More specifically, an individual’s demand may have a fixed set point, or base rate, but may also vary substantially in response to various contextual factors, including those examined in the current meta-analysis. Thus, these variables may precipitate clinically meaningful changes in motivation to use drugs that may inform models of recovery and relapse [110–112]. Although our study describes the aggregate effects of these unique conditions, there is probably considerable within-subject variability in the malleability of demand, which may represent an important risk factor for greater severity of substance misuse. A few studies have begun to investigate these individual differences; for example, those with a family history of alcohol use are less sensitive to next-day responsibilities among college students [113]. Further, those high in drinking to cope demonstrate a greater increase in reinforcing value following a stress induction [50]. An extension of these findings might use an aggregate of purchase task data across manipulations. Individual differences in the change in demand from one context to another could be conceptualized as a ‘meta-elasticity’ that accounts for change in demand not only as a function of price, but also as a function of other environmentally relevant factors.
Limitations and future directions
First, several of our moderator meta-analyses only included a small number of studies and were not adequately powered to detect differences in effect sizes with null hypothesis significance testing. Secondly, many of the studies explored manipulations of demand among heavy drinking college students or adult community smokers, which precludes examination of moderating effects of population type and dependence level. Thirdly, studies examined a limited number of commodities including alcohol, cigarettes and cannabis, and some manipulations (i.e. opportunity cost) only examined one of these commodities. Studies should test the effect of these manipulations on demand for other drugs, including opioids. Fourthly, the variability in measurement time-frame and intervention type for behavioral and pharmacotherapy manipulations should be considered. Fifthly, recent work has considered the effect of task differences on purchase task results and should be considered in future analyses [4,93,114]. Sixthly, evidence was mixed for most publication bias indices, with results of the negative affect/stress manipulation suggesting possible publication bias. It should be noted that the studies did included exhibit significant heterogeneity, which could contribute to higher levels of detected publication bias using these indices. Seventhly, although the meta-analytical method was pre-registered, the analyses were not, and the results should be considered exploratory.
Although the results explored the effect of six manipulations on demand, the search also returned studies of other phasic factors that could not be aggregated due to the small number of studies, including pain induction [82], happy-hour beer purchasing (more broadly, value framing; [91], excise tax increases on cigarette purchasing [92], cannabis quality [90], devaluation (i.e. substance that turns beer bitter [47]), time constraints on alcohol consumption [93], alcohol administration [83], self-control depletion [94], sexual arousal [48], availability of concurrent commodities [84,85,95,115], deprivation from the addictive commodity [86,94] and income constraint [87,88]. Many of these studies report a statistically significant effect of the manipulation on BE demand and may ultimately be included in future meta-analyses. Other potential manipulations also exist, such as the number of peers present during drinking [116] or the economy type (open versus closed), which could extend basic research into applied domains. Examining the reinforcing value of candidate addictive behaviors (e.g. tanning, internet use) under such conditions would be a further test of their addictive properties [117]. Given the presumed difference in pharmacological potency between substance and behavioral addictions [118], graded effect sizes may exist between manipulations and demand for different indices, which future moderator analyses across commodities could illuminate empirically. Further, changes in some demand indices for the manipulations reported in the current analyses have not been reported often enough for inclusion, and thus future studies should fully report all demand indices to more adequately determine the extent of heterogeneity across different aspects of demand.
Clinical implications
Our results suggest that behavioral and pharmacological interventions, along with opportunity costs, decrease motivation to consume a range of substances. Further, the results provide an empirical basis for identifying situations that increase substance motivation which could be targeted with intervention. More generally, this study supports the utility of ongoing assessment of demand as a dynamic indicator of motivation that could identify high- risk moments (elevated demand) when individuals may be especially receptive to intervention. Brief demand measures [83,119] may be useful in daily diary or ecological momentary assessment studies to more efficiently track the clinical utility of these shifts in motivation. If successful, these could eventually be useful within the context of a personalized treatment protocol delivered remotely that can react spontaneously to changes in demand reported by the client in real time. The results also highlight opportunities for public health-level interventions that change the context through increasing the response costs, decreasing the reinforcing magnitude and minimizing cues associated with use (i.e. advertising). For example, significant changes in behavioral economic demand due to differences in reinforcer magnitude demonstrate support for policy decisions attempting to control alcohol and other regulated drugs by reducing the levels of the addictive compound available in each unit of the substance (US FDA 83 FR 11818). Results also appeared similar to cannabis demand which, if replicated with further studies, could inform policy decisions regarding regulation of cannabis.
CONCLUSION
The results provide meta-analytical evidence for significant effects of six manipulations that either increase or decrease motivation to consume a substance, albeit of varying magnitude. A number of other manipulation conditions were retrieved through the search with only one or two studies available, suggesting that the literature is inchoate but growing. Nonetheless, the results provide support for a tonic-phasic reinforcer pathology approach and, more generally, for the utility of HPTs for revealing acute effects of diverse experimental manipulations on the reinforcing value of different addictive commodities.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by National Institute of Health Grants R01 AA024930-01 (MPIs: J.M. and J.G.M.) and F31 AA027140 (Principal Investigator: S.F.A.). M.A. is partially supported by the Peter Boris Centre for Addictions Research. J.Mc.K. holds the Peter Boris Chair in Addictions Research, which partially supported his role. These funding sources had no role other than financial support.
Footnotes
Declaration of interests
The authors declare no conflicts of interest, except for J.M. who is a principal in BEAM Diagnostics, Inc.
Further moderator analyses examining differences in effect sizes by study design (within versus between) and measure type (purchase task versus multiple-choice procedure) are reported in Supporting information, Table S5. Most studies (k = 28) used within-subjects designs, and most studies used purchase tasks outside reinforcer magnitude analyses. Thus, moderator analyses for these two variables were performed in analyses pooling all effect sizes from single study into a single effect size and recoding all effects in the same direction.
References
- 1.Murphy JG, MacKillop J Relative reinforcing efficacy of alcohol among college student drinkers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2006; 14: 219–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Petry NM, Bickel WK Polydrug abuse in heroin addicts: a behavioral economic analysis. Addiction 1998; 93: 321–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Petry NM, Bickel WK Can simulations studies substitute for reality? Addiction 1998; 93: 605. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kaplan BA, Foster RNS, Reed DD, Amlung M, Murphy JG, Mackillop J Understanding alcohol motivation using the alcohol purchase task: a methodological systematic review. Drug Alcohol Depend 2018; 191: 117–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Aston ER, Cassidy RN Behavioral economic demand assessments in the addictions. Curr Opin Psychol 2019; 30: 42–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.MacKillop J, Murphy JG, Tidey JW, Kahler CW, Ray LA, Bickel WK Latent structure of facets of alcohol reinforcement from a behavioral economic demand curve. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2009; 203: 33–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Skidmore JR, Murphy JG, Martens MP Behavioral economic measures of alcohol reward value as problem severity indicators in college students. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2014; 22: 198–210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bidwell LC, MacKillop J, Murphy JG, Tidey JW, Colby SM Latent factor structure of a behavioral economic cigarette demand curve in adolescent smokers. Addict Behav 2012; 37: 1257–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Hursh SR, Silberberg A Economic demand and essential value. Psychol Rev 2008; 115: 186–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Koffarnus MN, Franck CT, Stein J, Bickel WK A modified exponential behavioral economic demand model to better describe consumption data. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2015; 23: 504–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Amlung MT, Acker J, Stojek MK, Murphy JG, MacKillop J Is talk ‘cheap’? An initial investigation of the equivalence of alcohol purchase task performance for hypothetical and actual rewards. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2012; 36: 716–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Amlung MT, MacKillop J Further evidence of close correspondence for alcohol demand decision making for hypothetical and incentivized rewards. Behav Processes 2015; 113: 187–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Griffiths RR, Rush CR, Puhala KA Validation of the multiple-choice procedure for investigating drug reinforcement in humans. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 1996; 30: 42–7. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Zvorsky I, Nighbor TD, Kurti AN, DeSarno M, Naudé G, Reed DD et al. Sensitivity of hypothetical purchase task indices when studying substance use: a systematic literature review. Prev Med (Balt) 2019; https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0091743519302658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Strickland JC, Campbell EM, Lile JA, Stoops WW Utilizing the commodity purchase task to evaluate behavioral economic demand for illicit substances: a review and meta-analysis. Addiction 2019; 10.1111/add.14792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.González-Roz A, Jackson J, Murphy C, Rohsenow DJ, MacKillop J Behavioral economic tobacco demand in relation to cigarette consumption and nicotine dependence: a meta-analysis of cross-sectional relationships. Addiction 2019; 114: 1926–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Acuff SF, Murphy JG Further examination of the temporal stability of alcohol demand. Behav Processes 2017; 141: 33–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Morris V, Amlung MT, Kaplan BA, Reed DD, Petker T, MacKillop J Using crowdsourcing to examine behavioral economic measures of alcohol value and proportionate alcohol reinforcement. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2017; 25: 314–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.MacKillop J, Murphy JG A behavioral economic measure of demand for alcohol predicts brief intervention outcomes. Drug Alcohol Depend 2007; 89: 227–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Teeters JB, Murphy JG The behavioral economics of driving after drinking among college drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2015; 39: 896–904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Johnson MW, Bickel WK Replacing relative reinforcing efficacy with behavioral economic demand curves. J Exp Anal Behav 2006; 85: 73–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Aston ER, Metrik J, Amlung MT, Kahler CW, MacKillop J Interrelationships between marijuana demand and discounting of delayed rewards: convergence in behavioral economic methods. Drug Alcohol Depend 2016; 169: 141–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Pickover AM, Messina BG, Correia CJ, Garza KB, Murphy JG A behavioral economic analysis of the nonmedical use of prescription drugs among young adults. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2016; 24: 38–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Jacobs EA, Bickel WK Modeling drug consumption in the clinic using simulation procedures: demand for heroin and cigarettes in opioid-dependent outpatients. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 1999; 7: 412–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Bruner NR, Johnson M W Demand curves for hypothetical cocaine in cocaine-dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2014; 231: 889–97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Weinstock J, Mulhauser K, Oremus EG, D’Agostino AR Demand for gambling: development and assessment of a gambling purchase task. Int Gambl Stud 2016; 16: 316–27. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Becirevic A, Reed DD, Amlung MT, Murphy JG, Stapleton JL, Hillhouse JJ An initial study of behavioral addiction symptom severity and demand for indoor tanning. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2017; 25: 346–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Reed DD, Kaplan BA, Becirevic A, Roma PG, Hursh SR Toward quantifying the abuse liability of ultraviolet tanning: a behavioral economic approach to tanning addiction. J Exp Anal Behav 2016; 106: 93–106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Broadbent J, Dakki MA How much is too much to pay for internet access? A behavioral economic analysis of internet use. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 2015; 18: 457–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Acuff SF, MacKillop J, Murphy JG Applying behavioral economic theory to problematic internet use: an initial investigation. Psychol Addict Behav 2018; 32: 846–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Dennhardt AA, Yurasek AM, Murphy JG Change in delay discounting and substance reward value following a brief alcohol and drug use intervention. J Exp Anal Behav 2015; 103: 125–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Murphy JG, Dennhardt AA, Martens MP, Yurasek AM, Skidmore JR, MacKillop J et al. Behavioral economic predictors of brief alcohol intervention outcomes. J Counsulting Clin Psychol 2015; 83: 1033–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Few LR, Acker J, Murphy CM, MacKillop J Temporal stability of a cigarette purchase task. Nicotine Tob Res 2012; 14: 761–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Gilbert LJ, Murphy JG, Dennhardt AA A behavioral economic analysis of the effect of next-day responsibilities on drinking. Psychol Addict Behav 2014; 28: 1253–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Skidmore JR, Murphy JG The effect of drink price and next-day responsibilities on college student drinking: a behavioral economic analysis. Psychol Addict Behav 2011; 25: 57–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.MacKillop J, O’Hagen S, Lisman SA, Murphy JG, Ray LA, Tidey JW et al. Behavioral economic analysis of cue-elicited craving for alcohol. Addiction 2010; 105: 1599–607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Amlung MT, MacKillop J Understanding the effects of stress and alcohol cues on motivation for alcohol via behavioral economics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2014; 38: 1780–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Bujarski S, MacKillop J, Ray LA Understanding naltrexone mechanism of action and pharmacogenetics in Asian Americans via behavioral economics: a preliminary study. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2012; 20: 181–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Benson TA, Little CS, Henslee AM, Correia CJ Effects of reinforcer magnitude and alternative reinforcer delay on preference for alcohol during a multiple-choice procedure. Drug Alcohol Depend 2009; 100: 161–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Dahne J, Murphy JG, MacPherson L Depressive symptoms and cigarette demand as a function of induced stress. Nicotine Tob Res 2017; 19: 49–58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Koffarnus MN, Jarmolowicz DP, Mueller ET, Bickel WK Changing delay discounting in the light of the competing neurobehavioral decision systems theory: a review. J Exp Anal Behav 2013; 99: 32–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Rung JM, Madden GJ Experimental reductions of delay discounting and impulsive choice: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Exp Psychol Gen 2018; 147: 1349–831. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Stewart LA, Clarke M, Rovers M, Riley RD, Simmonds M, Stewart G et al. Preferred reporting items for a systematic review and meta-analysis of individual participant data: the PRISMA-IPD statement. JAMA 2015; 313: 1657–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Acker J, MacKillop J Behavioral economic analysis of cue-elicited craving for tobacco: a virtual reality study. Nicotine Tob Res 2013; 15: 1409–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Metrik J, Aston ER, Kahler CW, Rohsenow DJ, McGeary JE, Knopik VS et al. Cue-elicited increases in incentive salience for marijuana: craving, demand, and attentional bias. Drug Alcohol Depend 2016; 167: 82–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Owens MM, Murphy CM, MacKillop J Initial development of a brief behavioral economic assessment of alcohol demand. Psychol Conscious 2015; 2: 144–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Rose AK, Brown K, MacKillop J, Field M, Hogarth L Alcohol devaluation has dissociable effects on distinct components of alcohol behaviour. Psychopharmacology 2018; 235: 1233–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Spelman PJ, Simons JS Effects of sexual arousal and alcohol cues on acute motivation for alcohol. Arch Sex Behav 2018; 47: 1577–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Owens MM, Ray LA, MacKillop J Behavioral economic analysis of stress effects on acute motivation for alcohol. J Exp Anal Behav 2015; 103: 77–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Rousseau GS, Irons JG, Correia CJ The reinforcing value of alcohol in a drinking to cope paradigm. Drug Alcohol Depend 2011; 118: 1–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Madden GJ, Kalman D Effects of bupropion on simulated demand for cigarettes and the subjective effects of smoking. Nicotine Tob Res 2010; 12: 416–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Murphy CM, MacKillop J, Martin RA, Tidey JW, Colby SM, Rohsenow DJ Effects of varenicline versus transdermal nicotine replacement therapy on cigarette demand on quit day in individuals with substance use disorders. Psychopharmacology 2017; 234: 2443–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Roache JD, Johnson BA, Ait-Daoud N, Mauldin JB, Thornton JE, Wells LT et al. Effects of repeated-dose isradipine on the abuse liability of cocaine. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2005; 13: 319–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Schlienz NJ, Hawk LW, Tiffany ST, O’Connor RJ, Mahoney MC The impact of pre-cessation varenicline on behavioral economic indices of smoking reinforcement. Addict Behav 2014; 39: 1484–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Gentile ND, Librizzi EH, Martinetti M P Academic constraints on alcohol consumption in college students: a behavioral economic analysis. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2012; 20: 390–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Bulley A, Gullo MJ The influence of episodic foresight on delay discounting and demand for alcohol. Addict Behav 2017; 66: 1–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Snider SE, LaConte SM, Bickel WK Episodic future thinking: expansion of the temporal window in individuals with alcohol dependence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2016; 40: 1558–66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Stein JS, Tegge AN, Turner JK, Bickel WK Episodic future thinking reduces delay discounting and cigarette demand: an investigation of the good-subject effect. J Behav Med 2018; 41: 269–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Weidberg S, Vallejo-Seco G, González-Roz A, García-Pérez A, Secades-Villa R In-treatment cigarette demand among treatment-seeking smokers with depressive symptoms. Addict Behav 2018; 82: 35–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Johnson BA, Roache JD, Ait-Daoud N, Gunderson EW, Haughey HM, Wang XQ et al. Topiramate’s effects on cocaine-induced subjective mood, craving and preference for money over drug taking. Addict Biol 2013; 18: 405–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Johnson MW, Strain EC, Griffiths RR Effects of oral caffeine pretreatment on response to intravenous nicotine and cocaine. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2010; 10.1037/a0020509 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Lile JA, Kelly TH, Hays LR Substitution profile of the cannabinoid agonist nabilone in human subjects discriminating A9-tetrahydrocannabinol. Clin Neuropharmacol 2010; 10.1097/WNE0b013e3181e77428 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Lile JA, Kelly TH, HaysL. R The reinforcing, self-reported performance and physiological effects of A9-tetrahydrocan- nabinol, triazolam, hydromorphone, and methylphenidate in cannabis users. Behav Pharmacol 2010; 10.1097/EBP0b013e32833470d7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Lile JA, Kelly TH, Charnigo RJ, Stinchcomb AL, Hays LR Pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic profile of supratherapeutic oral doses of D9-THC in cannabis users. J Clin Pharmacol 2013; 53: 680–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Rush CR, Baker RW, Wright K Acute physiological and behavioral effects of oral cocaine in humans: A dose- response analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend 1999; 10.1016/S0376-8716(98)00164-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Stoops WW, Glaser PEA, Rush CR Reinforcing, subjectrated, and physiological effects of intranasal methylphenidate in humans: A dose-response analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend 2003; 10.1016/S0376-8716(03)00131-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Walsh SL, Donny EC, Nuzzo PA, Bigelow GE Cocaine abuse versus cocaine dependence: Cocaine self-administration and pharmacodynamic response in the human laboratory. Drug Alcohol Depend 2010; 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.07.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Jones HE, Garrett BE, Griffiths RR Subjective and physiological effects of intravenous nicotine and cocaine in cigarette smoking cocaine abusers. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1999; 288: 188–97. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Garrett BE, Griffiths RR Physical dependence increases the relative reinforcing effects of caffeine versus placebo. Psychopharmacology 1998; 139: 195–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Helmus TC, Tancer M, Johanson CE Reinforcing effects of diazepam under anxiogenic conditions in individuals with social anxiety. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2005; 13: 348–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Tancer M, Johanson CE Reinforcing, subjective, and physiological effects of MDMA in humans: a comparison with d-amphetamine and mCPP. Drug Alcohol Depend 2003; 72: 33–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.McClure EA, Vandrey RG, Johnson MW, Stitzer ML Effects of varenicline on abstinence and smoking reward following a programmed lapse. Nicotine Tob Res 2013; 15: 139–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Johnson BA, Roache JD, Ait-Daoud N, Wallace C, Wells L, Dawes M et al. Effects of isradipine, a dihydropyridine-class calcium-channel antagonist, on d-methamphetamine’s subjective and reinforcing effects. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2005; 8: 203–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Field M, Santarcangelo M, Sumnall H, Goudie A, Cole J Delay discounting and the behavioural economics of cigarette purchases in smokers: the effects of nicotine deprivation. Psychopharmacology 2006; 186: 255–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Madden GJ, Bickel WK Abstinence and price effects on demand for cigarettes: a behavioral-economic analysis. Addiction 1999; 94: 577–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Hitsman B, MacKillop J, Lingford-Hughes A, Williams TM, Ahmad E, Adams S et al. Effects of acute tyrosine/phenylalanine depletion on the selective processing of smoking-related cues and the relative value of cigarettes in smokers. Psychopharmacology 2008; 196: 611–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Plebani JG, Lynch KG, Yu Q, Pettinati HM, O’Brien CP, Kampman KM Results of an initial clinical trial of varenicline for the treatment of cocaine dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend 2012; 121: 163–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Selby P, Andriash K, Zawertailo L, Persad D, Zack M, Busto UE Escalating doses of transdermal nicotine in heavy smokers: effects on smoking behavior and craving. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2013; 33: 667–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Higgins ST, Heil SH, Sigmon SC, Tidey JW, Gaalema DE, Hughes JR et al. Addiction potential of cigarettes with reduced nicotine content in populations with psychiatric disorders and other vulnerabilities to tobacco addiction. JAMA Psychiat 2017; 74: 1056–64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Mumford GK, Rush CR, Griffiths RR Abecarnil and alprazolam in humans: behavioral, subjective and reinforcing effects. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1995; 272: 570–80. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Griffiths RR, Troisi JR, Silverman K, Mumford GK Multiple-choice procedure: an efficient approach for investigating drug reinforcement in humans. Behav Pharmacol 1993; 4: 3–13. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Moskal D, Maisto SA, De Vita M, Ditre J W Effects of experimental pain induction on alcohol urge, intention to consume alcohol, and alcohol demand. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2018; 26: 65–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Amlung MT, McCarty KN, Morris DH, Tsai CL, McCarthy DM Increased behavioral economic demand and craving for alcohol following a laboratory alcohol challenge. Addiction 2015; 110: 1421–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Lau-Barraco C, Schmitz JM Drug preference in cocaine and alcohol dual-dependent patients. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 2008; 34: 211–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Hindocha C, Lawn W, Freeman TP, Curran HV Individual and combined effects of cannabis and tobacco on drug reward processing in non-dependent users. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2017; 234: 3153–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.MacKillop J, Brown CL, Stojek MK, Murphy CM, Sweet LH, Niaura RS Behavioral economic analysis of withdrawal- and cue-elicited craving for tobacco: an initial investigation. Nicotine Tob Res 2012; 14: 1426–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Sze YY, Stein JS, Bickel WK, Paluch RA, Epstein LH Bleak present, bright future: online episodic future thinking, scarcity, delay discounting, and food demand. Clin Psychol Sci 2017; 5: 683–97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Koffarnus MN, Wilson AG, Bickel WK Effects of experimental income on demand for potentially real cigarettes. Nicotine Tob Res 2015; 17: 292–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Smith TT, Cassidy RN, Tidey JW, Luo X, Le CT, Hatsukami DK et al. Impact of smoking reduced nicotine content cigarettes on sensitivity to cigarette price: further results from a multi-site clinical trial. Addiction 2016; 112: 349–59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Vincent PC, Collins RL, Liu L, Yu J, De Leo JA, Earleywine M The effects of perceived quality on behavioral economic demand for marijuana: a web-based experiment. Drug Alcohol Depend 2017; 170: 174–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Kaplan BA, Reed DD Happy hour drink specials in the alcohol purchase task. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2018; 26: 156–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Grace RC, Kivell BM, Laugesen M Assessing the temporal stability of a cigarette purchase task after an excise tax increase for factory-made and roll-your-own smokers. Nicotine Tob Res 2015; 17: 1393–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Kaplan BA, Reed DD, Murphy JG, Henley AJ, Reed FDD, Roma PG et al. Time constraints in the alcohol purchase task. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2017; 25: 186–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Heckman BW, MacQueen DA, Marquinez NS, MacKillop J, Bickel WK, Brandon TH Self-control depletion and nicotine deprivation as precipitants of smoking cessation failure: a human laboratory model. J Consult Clin Psychol 2017; 85: 381–96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Snider SE, Cummings KM, Bickel WK Behavioral economic substitution between conventional cigarettes and e-cigarettes differs as a function of the frequency of e- cigarette use. Drug Alcohol Depend 2017; 177: 14–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JPT, Rothstein HR Effect sizes based on means In: Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JPT, Rothstein H, editors. Introduction to Meta-analysis. Chichester: Wiley; 2009, pp. 21–32. [Google Scholar]
- 97.Reynolds EK, Monti P M The cue reactivity paradigm in addiction research In: MacKillop J and de Wit H (Eds.), The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Addiction Psychopharmacology. Chichester: Wiley; 2013, pp. 381–410. [Google Scholar]
- 98.Richards JM, Patel N, Daniele-Zegarelli T, MacPherson L, Lejuez CW, Ernst M Social anxiety, acute social stress, and reward parameters interact to predict risky decision-making among adolescents. J Anxiety Disord 2015; 29: 25–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Thomas S, Bacon A Stress and affective inductions in addiction research In: MacKillop J and de Wit H (Eds.), The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Addiction Psychopharmacology. Chichester: Wiley; 2013, pp. 411–34. [Google Scholar]
- 100.Cochran WG The combination of estimates from different experiments. Biometrics 1954; 10: 101. [Google Scholar]
- 101.Tukey JW Bias and confidence in not-quite large samples. Ann Math Statist 1958; 29: 614. [Google Scholar]
- 102.Orwin RG A fail-safe N for effect size in meta-analysis. J Educ Stat 1983; 8: 15 7. [Google Scholar]
- 103.Begg CB, Mazumdar M Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics 1994; 50: 1088. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C, Mulrow C, Smith GD et al. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ (Clin Res Edn) 1997; 315: 629–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Bresin K, Mekawi Y, Verona E The effect of laboratory manipulations of negative affect on alcohol craving and use: a meta-analysis. Psychol Addict Behav 2018; 32: 617–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Acuff SF, Luciano MT, Soltis KE, Joyner KJ, Mcdevitt-Murphy ME, Murphy JG Access to environmental reward mediates the relation between posttraumatic stress symptoms and alcohol problems and craving. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2018; 25: 412–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Vujanovic AA, Wardle MC, Smith LJ, Berenz EC Reward functioning in posttraumatic stress and substance use disorders. Curr Opin Psychol 2017; 14: 49–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Koob GF The dark side of addiction: the Horsley Gantt to Joseph Brady connection. J Nerv Ment Dis 2017; 205: 270–2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Schwartz LP, Roma PG, Henningfield JE, Hursh SR, Cone EJ, Buchhalter AR et al. Behavioral economic demand metrics for abuse deterrent and abuse potential quantification. Drug Alcohol Depend 2019; 198: 13–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.McKay JR Making the hard work of recovery more attractive for those with substance use disorders. Addiction 2017; 112: 751–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Witkiewitz K, Marlatt GA Relapse prevention for alcohol and drug problems: that was zen, this is tao. Am Psychol 2004; 59: 224–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Hendershot CS, Witkiewitz K, George WH, Marlatt GA Relapse prevention for addictive behaviors. Subst Abuse Treat Prev Policy 2011; 6: 17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Murphy JG, Yurasek AM, Meshesha LZ, Dennhardt AA, MacKillop J, Skidmore JR et al. Family history of problem drinking is associated with less sensitivity of alcohol demand to a next-day responsibility. J Stud Alcohol Drugs 2014; 75: 653–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Roma PG, Hursh SR, Hudja S Hypothetical purchase task questionnaires for behavioral economic assessments of value and motivation. Manag Decis Econ 2016; 37: 306–23. [Google Scholar]
- 115.Martinetti MP, Caughron RL, Berman HL, André J, Sokolowski MBC, Wiley S et al. The behavioral economics of alcohol demand in French and American University students. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2019; 43: 531–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Murphy JG, Barnett NP, Colby SM Alcohol-related and alcohol-free activity participation and enjoyment among college students: a behavioral theories of choice analysis. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2006; 14: 339–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Block JJ Issues for DSM-V: internet addiction. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165: 306–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Wise RA Addiction In: Pashler H, Gallistel R, editors. Steven’s Handbook of Experimental Psychology, 3rd edn, Learning, Motivation, And Emotion. Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons; 2002; 3, pp. 801–40. [Google Scholar]
- 119.Murphy CM, Cassidy RN, Martin RA, Tidey JW, Mackillop J, Rohsenow DJ et al. Brief assessment of cigarette demand (BACD): initial development and correlational results in adults and adolescents. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2019; 27: 496–501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.