Abstract
Background and aims
Research on adolescent predictors of later alcohol misuse is typically conducted on samples of singletons, and associations may be confounded by between-family differences. To address potential confounding, we applied a co-twin comparison design to evaluate whether differences between co-twins in a wide array of adolescent risk factors predicted differences in young adult alcohol misuse.
Design
Longitudinal study in which associations between characteristics of the sample as adolescents were used to predict young adult alcohol misuse in individual-level analyses and co-twin comparisons.
Setting
Finland.
Participants
A total of 3402 individuals (1435 complete twin pairs; 36% monozygotic; 57% female) from the FinnTwin12 study.
Measurements
The young adult alcohol misuse outcome was a composite score of alcohol use and intoxication frequency. Adolescent predictors included factor scores representing academic performance, substance use, externalizing problems, internalizing problems, peer environment, physical health and relationship with parents; and single measures tapping alcohol expectancies, life events and pubertal development.
Findings
In individual-level analyses, individuals with higher adolescent substance use, externalizing problems, time with friends, peer deviance, sports involvement, sleeping difficulties, parental discipline, positive alcohol expectancies and difficulty of life events reported higher alcohol misuse in young adulthood (Ps < 0.019, R2 = 0.0003–0.0310%). Conversely, those with higher adolescent internalizing problems, parent–child relationship quality and time with parents reported lower alcohol misuse (Ps < 0021, R2 = 0.0018–0.0093%). The associations with adolescent substance use and alcohol expectancies remained significant in co-twin comparisons (Ps < 0.049, R2 = 0.0019–0.0314%). Further, academic performance emerged as a significant predictor, such that individuals with higher grades compared with their co-twin reported higher young adult alcohol misuse (Ps < 0.029, R2 = 0.0449–0.0533%).
Conclusions
Adolescent substance use, positive alcohol expectancies and higher academic performance appear to be robust predictors of later alcohol misuse.
Keywords: Alcohol, adolescence, co-twin comparisons, FinnTwin12, fixed effects, young adulthood
INTRODUCTION
Alcohol use typically increases throughout adolescence and peaks during young adulthood [1–3]. Characterizing early adolescent predictors of later alcohol misuse can identify relevant targets for preventive intervention efforts and mitigate adverse alcohol-related outcomes, which range from lower educational attainment to heightened risk for cardiovascular disease and cancer [4–7]. However, the majority of studies examining prospective predictors of later alcohol misuse are conducted on samples of singletons [8–11], and associations may be confounded by between-family differences, such as socio-economic status and parenting practices. The co-twin comparison design addresses potential confounding by evaluating whether differences in adolescent risk factors between twins predict differences in their young adult alcohol misuse. Because twin siblings share a rearing environment and half or all their genetic variation, a co-twin comparison design assesses whether observed individual-level associations remain significant after controlling for factors that vary between families.
Adolescent predictors of later alcohol misuse range from characteristics of the individual to aspects of their social context [8,10]. In the current study, we focus on adolescent domains shown to be predictive of young adult alcohol use outcomes in prior studies, including academic achievement, peer environment, parent–child relationship characteristics, early adolescent substance use, physical health, externalizing behaviors and internalizing problems. Individuals with higher grades report lower alcohol use throughout adolescence [12] and young adulthood [13]. Further, social and familial factors, such as affiliations with deviant peers [12,14], lower levels of parental autonomy granting, monitoring, warmth and involvement [12,15,16]and higher levels of relational tension and discipline [15,17] are associated with increased risk for adolescent and young adult alcohol use and misuse.
Individual-level characteristics in adolescence, such as substance use, physical health, externalizing behaviors and internalizing problems, are also relevant for subsequent alcohol misuse. There is significant continuity in substance use from adolescence to young adulthood [18,19], such that adolescents with higher cigarette use [8,20] and alcohol use [21] are at increased risk for later alcohol misuse. In addition, persistently inactive adolescents report more frequent intoxication than those who are physically active [22], and poor self-rated health [23] and sleeping difficulties [24,25] are positively associated with heavy drinking. Externalizing behaviors, including conduct problems, inattention and impulsivity, are also associated with alcohol use problems [9,11,26–28], although research examining the influence of internalizing problems is less clear [29]. Some studies suggest that low self-esteem [30] and more severe depressive symptoms [20,31] in adolescence increase risk for heavy drinking in young adulthood, while others indicate that social maladjustment [9] depressive affect [8] and increased social anxiety [32] predict fewer alcohol use problems.
As reviewed above, there is an extensive literature on the adolescent predictors of young adult alcohol misuse among samples of unrelated individuals. However, as with all observational research, these associations are prone to confounding by between-family differences, which may have significant implications for our understanding of pathways to young adult alcohol misuse. For instance, parental divorce is associated with both adolescent internalizing problems [33] and excessive alcohol use in young adulthood [20]. Thus, a family-level factor, such as parental divorce, could explain the observed association between internalizing problems and later alcohol misuse within samples of unrelated individuals. Co-twin comparisons address familial confounding and strengthen inferences in observational research by evaluating whether differences between twins in purported adolescent predictors map onto differences in alcohol-related outcomes [34–36], effectively controlling for the genetic and environmental influences that twin siblings share. Continuing the above example, if differences between twins in their internalizing problems predict differences in their young adult alcohol misuse, this suggests that the association between internalizing problems and alcohol misuse is not purely attributable to parental divorce (or any other factor shared by co-twins). Internalizing problems may therefore be an important target for alcohol misuse preventive intervention efforts. Conversely, if differences between twins in adolescence do not prospectively predict differences in alcohol misuse, this is consistent with confounding by familial factors (i.e. genetic and environmental influences, such as parental divorce, that twins share) or a causal pathway in which the adolescent predictor mediates genetic or shared environmental influences on subsequent alcohol misuse. Thus, co-twin comparisons can differentiate valuable targets for preventive intervention efforts from markers of non-causal familial liability. In prior studies, Irons et al. [37] and Savage et al. [38] used the co-twin design to examine adolescent alcohol exposure, parental monitoring and peer deviance as adolescent predictors of young adult alcohol misuse. When examined within families, associations with adolescent alcohol exposure and parental monitoring remained robust, although peer deviance was no longer a significant risk factor for subsequent alcohol misuse [37,38]. Such findings underscore the importance of using complementary methods, such as co-twin comparisons, to understand the nature of individual-level associations.
In the current study, we employed a co-twin comparison design in a sample of Finnish twins followed longitudinally from adolescence to young adulthood. We examined a series of adolescent risk and protective factors for alcohol misuse, including academic performance, early adolescent substance use, externalizing problems, internalizing problems, parent–child relationship quality, peer environment and physical health. The alcohol misuse outcome included frequency of alcohol use and frequency of intoxication. Although these measures converge among individuals with high alcohol intake, heavy drinking occasions are a particularly important predictor of alcohol use disorder (AUD) among individuals with moderate alcohol consumption [39]. Our pre-registered hypotheses were informed by prior studies characterizing the genetic and environmental architecture of purported risk factors and alcohol-related outcomes [22,32,37,38,40–43]. We expected that academic performance, externalizing problems, physical health and relationship with parents would not be robust predictors of young adult alcohol misuse in co-twin comparison analyses. Conversely, we predicted that associations for alcohol expectancies and life events would be positive and robust. We did not have specific hypotheses for adolescent substance use, internalizing problems and peer environment due to contrasting hypotheses at the level of individual predictors. Our aims were as follows:
To evaluate adolescent predictors of young adult alcohol misuse in individual-level analyses, which are comparable to prior studies conducted on samples of unrelated individuals.
To use the co-twin comparison design to evaluate whether observed individual-level associations remain significant after controlling for genetic and environmental influences shared by twin siblings.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Sample
Participants were from FinnTwin12, a population-based, longitudinal study of Finnish twins born 1983–87 [44,45]. Participants were identified through Finland’s Central Population Registry. A family questionnaire was mailed to each twin family in the year before the twins reached age 12 years. This questionnaire was returned by 2724 families, 87% of those identified. For those who returned the family questionnaire, individual questionnaires were mailed to both parents and the two co-twins. Ratings were also completed by parents and teachers. Zygosity was determined based on co-twins’ [46] and parents’ [47] responses to items developed for zygosity classification, and sex was ascertained from Finland’s Central Population Registry. Follow-up assessments occurred at ages 14, 17.5 and as young adults (average age = 22 years, range = 20–26 years). For the current study, adolescent predictors were derived from assessments at ages 12 and 14. Response rate for the age 14 assessment was 92%. We limited analyses to 3402 individuals (1435 complete twin pairs; 36% monozygotic; 57% female) who completed the young adult follow-up assessment; 66% of the original sample was retained throughout young adulthood. Sex significantly predicted young adult participation [odds ratio (OR) = 6.00, 95% confidence interval (CI) = 4.33, 8.29], such that females were six times more likely to participate in follow-up than males. Frequency of alcohol use (OR = 0.88, 95% CI = 0.77, 1.01) and frequency of intoxication at age 14 (OR = 0.88, 95% CI = 0.77, 1.00) did not significantly predict study retention.
Measures
Young adult alcohol use and intoxication frequency
Frequency of alcohol use in young adulthood was assessed with one item: ‘How often do you drink alcohol?’. Frequency of intoxication was also assessed with one item: ‘How often do you drink so that you get at least slightly intoxicated?’. Participants selected from nine response options. Responses were recoded as pseudo-continuous days of drinking per month and days intoxicated per month, respectively (daily = 30 days, a couple of times per week = 8 days, once per week = 4 days, a couple of times per month = 2 days, once per month = 1 day, bimonthly = 0.5 days, 2–4 times per year = 0.25 days, once per year or less = 0.083 days, never = 0 days; [48]).
Adolescent risk and protective factors
Table 1 provides information regarding measures of adolescent risk and protective factors. At ages 12 and 14, participants reported on their leisure time activities; participation in organized activities; sports participation; sleeping difficulties; parental autonomy granting, discipline, monitoring, tension and warmth; time spent with parents; and pubertal development. At age 14, participants also reported on their cigarette smoking; daily smoking; frequency of alcohol use; frequency of alcohol intoxication; self-esteem; peer deviance; peer drinking; peer drug use; peer smoking; perceived health; physical activity; alcohol expectancies; life events; and perceived difficulty of life events. In addition, parents and teachers provided ratings for grade point average, aggression, impulsivity, depression, social anxiety and social adjustment at age 12, and teachers reported on grade point average at age 14.
Table 1.
Adolescent predictors for young adult alcohol misuse.
ACA | Grades (PR) | ‘Which twin had the higher grade point average last spring?’; age 12 |
Grades (TR) | Grade point average using the Finnish GPA system (1 = below 6 to 5 = above 9); ages 12 and 14 | |
SUB | Cigarette smoking | Two items: ‘Have you ever smoked?’, ‘How many cigarettes have you smoked?’. Recoded, such that 0 = never smoked to 4 = smoked more than 50 cigarettes [70]; age 14 |
Daily smoking | Present smoking habits (0 = smokes, but not daily to 1 = smokes at least once per day) [62]; age 14 | |
Frequency of alcohol use | ‘How often do you drink alcohol?’ Recoded as days of drinking per month; age 14 | |
Frequency of intoxication | ‘How often do you drink alcohol so that you get at least slightly intoxicated?’ Recoded as days intoxicated per month; age 14 | |
EXT | Aggression (PR/TR) | Aggression subscale of MPNI [71]; age 12 |
Impulsivity (PR/TR) | Hyperactivity–impulsivity subscale of MPNI [71]; age 12 | |
INT | Depression (PR/TR) | Depression subscale of MPNI [71]; age 12 |
Self-esteem | 10-item Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale [72]; age 14 | |
Social anxiety (PR/TR) | Social anxiety subscale of MPNI [71]; age 12 | |
PEER ENV | Adjustment (PR/TR) | Adjustment subscale of MPNI [71]; age 12 |
Leisure time activities | Three items: frequency of spending ‘time with friends in your home’, ‘time with friends in their home’, ‘time with friends in places where youth meet up’ (1 = daily to 5 = never). Recoded as number of activities with friends per month; ages 12 and 14 | |
Organized activities | Frequency of participation in ‘clubs, boy/girl scouts, or other organized activities’ (1 = daily to 5 = never). Recoded as number of organized activities per month; ages 12 and 14 | |
Peer deviance | Number of friends who drink, smoke, use drugs, or get into trouble at school [73]; age 14 | |
Peer drinking | Number of friends who drink alcohol (1 = none to 4 = more than 5); age 14 | |
Peer drug use | Number of acquaintances who have tried drugs (1 = none to 4 = more than 5); age 14 | |
Peer smoking | Number of friends who smoke cigarettes (1 = none to 4 = more than 5); age 14 | |
Sports participation | Frequency of participation in team sports (1 = daily to 5 = never). Recoded as number of sports-related activities per month; ages 12 and 14. | |
HEA | Perceived health | ‘How do you rate your health?’ (1 = very poor to 5 = very good); age 14 |
Physical activity | ‘How often do you exercise or do sports during your free time?’ (1 = never to7 = just about every day). Recoded as number of times engaged in physical activity per month; age 14. | |
Sleeping difficulties | ‘How often have you experienced difficulties falling asleep since last summer?’ (0 = rarely or never to 4 = about once a month). Recoded as number of nights affected by sleeping problems per month; ages 12 and 14 | |
PARENTS | Autonomy granting | Four items: ‘my parents listen to my opinions’, ‘my parents give me credit’, ‘my parents encourage me to be independent’, ‘my parents try to clear things by talking when I’ve behaved badly’ (1 = rarely to 4 = never) [15]; ages 12 and 14 |
Discipline | Two items: ‘my parents punish me if I do something I’m not supposed to’ (1 = rarely to 4 = never); ‘strict’ home atmosphere (1 = does not hold true to 5 = holds completely true) [15]; ages 12 and 14 | |
Monitoring | Three items: ‘my parents know my plan for the day’, ‘my parents know my interests, activities, and whereabouts’, ‘my parents know where I am and who I’m with when I’m not at home’ (1 = rarely to 4 = never)15; ages 12 and 14 | |
Tension | Three items: home atmosphere is ‘unfair’, ‘quarrelsome’, ‘indifferent’ (1 = does not hold true to 5 = holds completely true) [15]; ages 12 and 14 | |
Time with parents | Six items: frequency of engaging in ‘discussions’, ‘movies’, ‘sports’, ‘hobbies’, ‘camping/traveling/visiting’, and ‘outdoor recreation’ with parents (1 = every day to 5 = never). Recoded as number of activities with parents per month; ages 12 and 14 | |
Warmth | Four items: home atmosphere is ‘warm/caring’, ‘encouraging/supportive’, ‘trusting/understanding’, ‘open’ (1 = does not hold true to 5 = holds completely true) [15]; ages 12 and 14 | |
UNCAT | Alcohol expectancies | Degree to which alcohol makes people ‘sleepy’, ‘talkative’, ‘sad’, ‘angry’, ‘ill’, ‘friendly’, ‘confused’, ‘mean’, ‘content’, ‘fun’, ‘depressed’ (1 = never to 3 = often); age 14 |
Difficulty of life events | ‘How difficult were these changes for you overall?’ (1 = changes have been positive to 5 = changes have been difficult); age 14 | |
Life events | Checklist of 15 stressful life events experienced in the past two years; age 14 | |
Pubertal development | Pubertal Development Scale [74]. Recoded as within-sex z-scores; ages 12 and 14 |
ACA = academic performance; SUB = early adolescent substance use; EXT = externalizing problems; INT = internalizing problems; PEER ENV = peer environment; HEA = physical health; PARENTS = relationship with parents; UNCAT = uncategorized predictors; PR = parent-reported; TR = teacher-reported; MPNI = Multidimensional Peer Nomination Inventory.
Statistical methods
The analytical plan for this project was pre-registered and can be viewed through the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/3vrn5/register/565fb3678c5e4a66b5582f67). We grouped adolescent predictors into the following domains: academic performance, early adolescent substance use, externalizing problems, internalizing problems, peer environment and relationship with parents. Following basic descriptive statistics and log-transformation of skewed variables, we performed a series of analyses aimed at item reduction using a split-half exploratory factor analysis (EFA) and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) approach. To do this, we split the sample into two, randomly selecting one twin from each pair for inclusion in each split-half. The first split-half sample included 1440 unrelated individuals, and the second split-half sample included the remaining 1431 individuals. Within each categorized domain, we determined the number of retained factors based on several complementary methods: parallel analysis [49], the Kaiser rule [50], number of factors needed to account for 60% of the total variance [51] and scree plots [52]. We conducted EFA in the first split-half using the ‘umxEFA’ function in the R {umx} package [53]. We used a factor loading cut-off of 0.40. We then conducted CFA in the first split-half using the ‘cfa’ function in the R {lavaan} package [54]. We used maximum likelihood estimation, with a comparative fit index (CFI) > 0.90 and a standardized root mean squared residual (SRMR) < 0.08 as criteria for acceptable model fit [55]. We conducted CFA in the second split-half to re-evaluate the model derived from the initial half. Finally, we used the ‘lavPredict’ function in {lavaan} [54] to derive factor scores for the full sample within each categorized domain. Several variables (alcohol expectancies, life events, perceived difficulty of life events and pubertal development) did not clearly fit into the domains identified above. For this reason, these predictors were examined separately and were not included in item reduction. For our alcohol misuse outcome, we created an overall sum score for frequency of alcohol use and frequency of intoxication (r = 0.64).
First, we examined associations of each factor score with alcohol misuse in individual-level analyses, using a linear mixed model to adjust for non-independence of individuals within the same family. We then conducted co-twin comparisons using a twin fixed-effects model, which evaluates whether observed individual-level associations remain significant after controlling for genetic and environmental influences shared by co-twins. Each factor score was examined in a separate model to avoid potential problems with collinearity or suppression effects. Additional information on the twin fixed-effects model can be found in the Supporting information. Finally, we conducted a series of co-twin comparisons for monozygotic (MZ) twins (251 pairs), who share 100% of their genetic variation, to enable more stringent control for shared genetic variation. All analyses were run using the R {plm} package version 1.6–6 [56] and included sex as a covariate. We adopted a P-value threshold of 0.05, given that our directional hypotheses and analytical plan were pre-registered, which is consistent with Nosek et al. [57] and Rubin [58].
Non-significant co-twin associations may reflect influences of familial factors; alternatively, null associations may be driven by insufficient variability between twins [59]. We explored the latter possibility in two ways. First, we calculated twin correlations for the overall sample and sample of MZ twins only. Next, we used the ‘phtest’ function in the R {plm} package [56] to conduct the Durbin–Wu–Hausman test [60], allowing us to more formally examine whether non-significant co-twin associations were explained by insufficient within-family variability.
RESULTS
The Supporting information contains detailed information regarding EFA/CFA for item reduction, inter-item correlations within each domain (Supporting information, Tables S1–S6), criteria for factor retention (Supporting information, Table S7, Fig. S1), factor loadings for EFA in the first split-half sample (Supporting information, Table S8), loadings used to construct factor scores (Supporting information, Table S9) and basic descriptive statistics (Supporting information, Table S10). On average, participants reported 3.79 days of drinking per month [standard deviation (SD) = 4.36] and 1.23 days intoxicated per month (SD = 1.23). Measures included within each factor score are summarized in Table 2. Results for individual-level and co-twin analyses are shown in Table 3 and Fig. 1, which presents beta hat estimates by analysis type (individual-level, co-twin comparisons and comparisons of MZ twins only). We review results from the individual-level and co-twin analyses below.
Table 2.
Measures used to construct factor scores within each domain.
Factor | Measures | |
---|---|---|
ACA | Academic performance | Grades (TR; age 12) |
Grades (TR; age 14) | ||
SUB | Substance use | Cigarette smoking |
Frequency of alcohol use | ||
Frequency of intoxication | ||
EXT | Externalizing problems | Aggression (TR) |
Impulsivity (PR) | ||
Impulsivity (TR) | ||
INT | Parent-reported internalizing | Depression (PR) |
Social anxiety (PR) | ||
Teacher-reported internalizing | Depression (TR) | |
Social anxiety (TR) | ||
PEER ENV | Time with friends | Leisure time activities (age 12) |
Leisure time activities (age 14) | ||
Peer deviance | Peer deviance | |
Peer drinking | ||
Peer drug use | ||
Peer smoking | ||
Sports involvement | Sports participation (age 12) | |
Sports participation (age 14) | ||
PARENTS | Age 12 relationship quality | Autonomy granting (age 12) |
Monitoring (age 12) | ||
Tension (age 12) | ||
Warmth (age 12) | ||
Age 14 relationship quality | Autonomy granting (age 14) | |
Monitoring (age 14) | ||
Tension (age 14) | ||
Warmth (age 14) | ||
Parental discipline | Discipline (age 12) | |
Time with parents | Time with parents (age 12) |
All results in text refer to factor scores. ACA = academic performance; SUB = early adolescent substance use; EXT = externalizing problems; INT = internalizing problems; PEER ENV = peer environment; PARENTS = relationship with parents; PR = parent-reported; TR = teacher-reported.
Table 3.
Results for individual-level and co-twin analyses.
Analysis type | (95% CI) | P | ΔR2 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
ACA | Academic performance | Individual | 0.026 (−0.034, 0.086) | 0.39 | 0.0252 |
Co-twin | 0.135 (0.014, 0.256) | 0.029 | 0.0533 | ||
Co-twin MZ | 0.361 (0.107, 0.615) | 0.006 | 0.0449 | ||
SUB | Substance use | Individual | 0.257 (0.188, 0.326) | < 0.001 | 0.0178 |
Co-twin | 0.139 (0.001,0.277) | 0.049 | 0.0019 | ||
Co-twin MZ | 0.083 (−0.159, 0.325) | 0.50 | 0.0010 | ||
EXT | Externalizing problems | Individual | 0.217 (0.148, 0.287) | < 0.001 | 0.0113 |
Co-twin | 0.099 (−0.027, 0.226) | 0.13 | 0.0004 | ||
Co-Twin MZ | −0.164 (−0.440, 0.113) | 0.25 | 0.0028 | ||
INT | Parent-reported internalizing | Individual | −0.087 (−0.160, −0.013) | 0.021 | 0.0018 |
Co-twin | −0.033 (−0.151, 0.085) | 0.59 | −0.0011 | ||
Co-twin MZ | −0.063 (−0.272, 0.146) | 0.55 | 0.0007 | ||
Teacher-reported internalizing | Individual | −0.162 (−0.240, −0.084) | < 0.001 | 0.0051 | |
Co-twin | −0.067 (−0.208, 0.075) | 0.36 | −0.0007 | ||
Co-Twin MZ | −0.030 (−0.316, 0.257) | 0.84 | 0.0001 | ||
PEER ENV | Peer deviance | Individual | 0.167 (0.116, 0.219) | < 0.001 | 0.0120 |
Co-twin | 0.051 (−0.040, 0.142) | 0.27 | 0.0010 | ||
Co-twin MZ | 0.022 (−0.115, 0.159) | 0.75 | 0.0002 | ||
Sports involvement | Individual | 0.187 (0.105, 0.269) | < 0.001 | 0.0060 | |
Co-twin | 0.100 (−0.047, 0.248) | 0.18 | 0.0014 | ||
Co-twin MZ | 0.121 (−0.142, 0.385) | 0.37 | 0.0017 | ||
Time with friends | Individual | 0.264 (0.193, 0.334) | < 0.001 | 0.0157 | |
Co-twin | 0.132 (0.000, 0.264) | 0.051 | 0.0029 | ||
Co-twin MZ | 0.039 (−0.180, 0.259) | 0.73 | 0.0003 | ||
HEA | Age 12 sleeping difficulties | Individual | 0.079 (0.013, 0.146) | 0.019 | −0.0003 |
Co-twin | 0.066 (−0.028, 0.160) | 0.17 | −0.0045 | ||
Co-twin MZ | −0.030 (−0.165, 0.105) | 0.66 | 0.0005 | ||
Age 14 sleeping difficulties | Individual | 0.102 (0.044, 0.160) | 0.001 | 0.0034 | |
Co-twin | 0.070 (−0.009, 0.149) | 0.084 | −0.0016 | ||
Co-twin MZ | −0.021 (−0.130, 0.089) | 0.71 | 0.0003 | ||
Perceived health | Individual | −0.025 (−0.085, 0.036) | 0.42 | 0.0019 | |
Co-twin | −0.021 (−0.110, 0.068) | 0.65 | −0.0008 | ||
Co-twin MZ | −0.025 (−0.162, 0.113) | 0.73 | 0.0003 | ||
Physical activity | Individual | 0.003 (−0.059, 0.065) | 0.92 | 0.0012 | |
Co-twin | 0.013 (−0.088, 0.113) | 0.80 | −0.0008 | ||
Co-twin MZ | −0.070 (−0.236, 0.095) | 0.40 | 0.0015 | ||
PARENTS | Age 12 relationship quality | Individual | −0.117 (−0.188, −0.045) | 0.001 | 0.0030 |
Co-twin | −0.036 (−0.178, 0.107) | 0.62 | 0.0002 | ||
Co-twin MZ | 0.079 (−0.129, 0.286) | 0.46 | 0.0011 | ||
Age 14 relationship quality | Individual | −0.198 (−0.267, −0.129) | < 0.001 | 0.0093 | |
Co-twin | −0.067 (−0.196, 0.063) | 0.31 | 0.0007 | ||
Co-twin MZ | 0.026 (−0.168, 0.219) | 0.80 | 0.0001 | ||
PARENTS | Parental discipline | Individual | 0.110 (0.040, 0.181) | 0.002 | 0.0028 |
Co-twin | 0.000 (−0.122, 0.123) | 0.99 | 0.0000 | ||
Co-twin MZ | −0.034 (−0.201, 0.134) | 0.69 | 0.0003 | ||
Time with parents | Individual | −0.086 (−0.149, −0.023) | 0.008 | 0.0021 | |
Co-twin | −0.014 (−0.124, 0.096) | 0.81 | 0.0000 | ||
Co-twin MZ | −0.164 (−0.328, 0.000) | 0.051 | 0.0077 | ||
UNCAT | Age 12 pubertal development | Individual | 0.032 (−0.032, 0.095) | 0.33 | 0.0008 |
Co-twin | −0.010 (−0.126, 0.107) | 0.87 | −0.0040 | ||
Co-twin MZ | 0.099 (−0.106, 0.304) | 0.35 | 0.0020 | ||
Age 14 pubertal development | Individual | −0.018 (−0.081, 0.045) | 0.57 | 0.0004 | |
Co-twin | −0.078 (−0.179, 0.022) | 0.13 | −0.0021 | ||
Co-twin MZ | −0.096 (−0.279, 0.087) | 0.30 | 0.0024 | ||
Alcohol expectancies | Individual | 0.292 (0.178, 0.405) | < 0.001 | 0.0310 | |
Co-twin | 0.308 (0.021, 0.594) | 0.037 | 0.0314 | ||
Co-twin MZ | 0.286 (−0.223, 0.795) | 0.28 | 0.0181 | ||
Difficulty of life events | Individual | 0.090 (0.021, 0.159) | 0.011 | −0.0063 | |
Co-Twin | −0.013 (−0.134, 0.109) | 0.84 | −0.0193 | ||
Co-twin MZ | 0.010 (−0.181, 0.202) | 0.92 | 0.0000 | ||
Life events | Individual | 0.054 (−0.011, 0.118) | 0.10 | 0.0021 | |
Co-twin | −0.056 (−0.178, 0.067) | 0.38 | −0.0005 | ||
Co-twin MZ | −0.118 (−0.311, 0.075) | 0.23 | 0.0031 |
ΔR2 refers to the change in variance explained from a sex-only baseline model. ACA = academic performance; SUB = early adolescent substance use; EXT = externalizing problems; INT = internalizing problems; PEER ENV = peer environment; HEA = physical health; PARENTS = relationship with parents; UNCAT = uncategorized predictors; CI = confidence interval’ MZ = monozygotic.
Figure 1.
Examining adolescent predictors of young adult alcohol misuse in individual-level and co-twin comparisons. Error bars denote 95% confidence intervals of estimates [Colour figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
Individual-level analyses
In individual-level analyses, adolescents with higher substance use, externalizing problems, time spent with friends, peer deviance, sports involvement, sleeping difficulties at ages 12 and 14, parental discipline, perceived difficulty of life events and positive alcohol expectancies reported higher alcohol misuse in young adulthood. By contrast, individuals with higher parent- and teacher-reported internalizing problems, time spent with parents and parent–child relationship quality at ages 12 and 14 exhibited lower risk for alcohol misuse. Adolescent academic performance, perceived health, physical activity, pubertal development and adolescent stressful life events did not significantly predict young adult alcohol misuse.
Co-twin comparisons
When tested using co-twin comparisons, early adolescent substance use and positive alcohol expectancies positively predicted young adult alcohol misuse. Further, adolescent academic performance emerged as a significant predictor, such that individuals with higher grades in adolescence compared to their co-twin reported higher young adult alcohol misuse. Associations with externalizing problems, parent- and teacher-reported internalizing problems, time spent with friends, peer deviance, sports involvement, sleeping difficulties at ages 12 and 14, parental discipline, time spent with parents, parent–child relationship quality at ages 12 and 14 and perceived difficulty of life events were no longer significant.
Co-twin comparisons (MZ twins only)
Adolescent academic performance positively predicted young adult alcohol misuse in comparisons of MZ twins. Associations with early adolescent substance use and positive alcohol expectancies were no longer significant, although the point estimate for alcohol expectancies was not markedly reduced (Fig. 1).
Sensitivity analyses
As shown in Table S11, twin correlation coefficients ranged from 0.10 to 0.65 in the overall sample and from 0.15 to 0.87 in the MZ-only sample. The Durbin–Wu–Hausman test was significant for associations with externalizing problems, time with friends, peer deviance, age 14 sleeping difficulties, parental discipline, perceived difficulty of life events, age 14 parent–child relationship quality and time with parents (Ps < 0.045), suggesting that a within-family estimator is more efficient than a between-family estimator. Because there was sufficient within-family variability to examine associations between each of these adolescent factors and young adult alcohol misuse, the non-significant co-twin associations for these factors are most consistent with confounding by familial influences. By contrast, the Durbin–Wu–Hausman test was not significant for sports involvement, age 12 sleeping difficulties, internalizing problems and age 12 parent–child relationship quality (Ps > 0.063), which is consistent with the interpretation that the non-significant co-twin comparisons for these factors may reflect insufficient variability between twins to detect an effect.
DISCUSSION
We employed a co-twin comparison design to evaluate the degree to which adolescent predictors of young adult alcohol misuse were robust versus attributable to familial influences. In individual-level analyses, we found that individuals with higher adolescent substance use, externalizing problems, time spent with friends, peer deviance, sports involvement, sleeping difficulties, parental discipline, positive alcohol expectancies and perceived difficulty of life events reported higher alcohol misuse in young adulthood. Conversely, those with higher internalizing problems, parent–child relationship quality and time with parents reported lower alcohol misuse. These findings are consistent with an extensive literature examining adolescent predictors of young adult alcohol misuse among samples of unrelated individuals [8,9,11–20,24–28,61]. Internalizing problems have been identified as both a risk [20,30,31] and protective [8,9,38] factor for alcohol use problems in prior studies; in the present study, adolescent internalizing problems predicted lower young adult alcohol misuse. Our findings, using non-clinical depression and social anxiety measures, are consistent with prior research in Finland demonstrating that social anxiety protects against subsequent alcohol use [32], but should be considered tentative in the context of the broader, mixed literature on the relationship between internalizing problems and alcohol misuse.
Co-twin comparisons complement individual-level analyses by examining whether differences between twins in adolescence predict differences in young adult alcohol misuse after controlling for genetic and environmental influences that twin siblings share. When tested using co-twin comparisons, associations with adolescent substance use and positive alcohol expectancies remained significant. Further, academic performance did not predict alcohol misuse in individual-level analyses but emerged as a significant predictor after controlling for genetic and environmental influences that twins share. A positive and robust association between adolescent academic performance and young adult alcohol misuse was unexpected, as prior studies have identified early adolescent academic achievement as a protective factor for alcohol misuse [12,13]. However, previous work in the FinnTwin12 sample has similarly demonstrated that educational attainment at age 17 positively predicts frequency of alcohol use in young adulthood [62]. One potential explanation is that the higher achieving twin may have been more likely to attend university, and heavy drinking is prevalent in early years of study at university [63]. Adolescent academic performance predicts higher educational attainment at the young adult assessment [62], strengthening the plausibility of this explanation.
These findings lend valuable insight into relevant targets for preventive intervention efforts. We found that adolescent academic performance, substance use and positive alcohol expectancies were robust predictors of young adult alcohol misuse when evaluating differences between twins, supporting these factors as valuable targets for preventive intervention efforts or for identifying individuals who are at particular risk. In contrast, many previously documented adolescent risk and protective factors for young adult alcohol misuse, such as externalizing behaviors and peer deviance, were not robust in co-twin comparison analyses. The absence of significant co-twin associations does not conclusively eliminate these factors as valuable targets for preventive intervention efforts. For example, it remains plausible that these adolescent predictors mediate genetic and shared environmental influences on alcohol misuse. Adolescent factors within these causal familial pathways would exhibit reduced co-twin associations but remain effective targets for preventive intervention. Our findings should therefore be verified across multiple methodologies with varied assumptions [64] before recommendations for preventive intervention programs are warranted.
Our results should be considered in light of several limitations. First, although the co-twin design permits control for genetic and environmental influences that twins share, confounding by unmeasured individual-level characteristics remains plausible and precludes a strong causal interpretation of results. For example, if one co-twin experienced a traumatic event, differential trauma exposure between twins could explain the observed association between adolescent substance use [65] and young adult alcohol misuse [66]. Secondly, among statistically significant adolescent predictors, several 95% confidence intervals approached zero (Table 3), including individual-level analyses for parent-reported internalizing and age 12 sleeping difficulties, as well as co-twin analyses for academic performance and adolescent substance use. These findings should be interpreted carefully prior to replication by future work. Thirdly, co-twin analyses involve increased risk of Type 2 error when compared to individual-level analyses, as co-twin associations compound measurement error [67] and involve an effective reduction in sample size [59]. Results from comparisons of MZ twins only should be interpreted with caution, given reduced power to detect effects in this subset (251 pairs). Finally, limitations of the available measures and sample characteristics should be noted. Several adolescent predictors (e.g. aggression, impulsivity, social anxiety) were parent-reported and may therefore reflect the parent’s perception rather than the adolescent’s actual behavior. In addition, it unknown whether the effects observed in our Finnish population-based sample will generalize broadly to other populations, although it is encouraging that much of the literature on alcohol use and misuse in Finland replicates across Europe and the United States (and vice versa; e.g. [68,69]).
Co-twin comparison designs enable stronger inferences not possible in samples of unrelated individuals by controlling for genetic and environmental influences that twins share. The current study yields novel insights by using a co-twin comparison design to examine a range of adolescent risk and protective factors for young adult alcohol misuse. Although many well-known adolescent correlates of young adult alcohol use problems, such as externalizing and internalizing problems, were not robust predictors of young adult alcohol misuse, our results support early adolescent substance use and positive alcohol expectancies as valuable targets for preventive intervention efforts aiming to reduce alcohol misuse in young adulthood.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism of the National Institutes of Health under award numbers R01AA012502, R01AA015416, K02AA018755 and K01AA024152; and the Academy of Finland (grants 100 499, 205 585, 118 555, 141 054, 265 240, 263 278 and 264 146). J.K. has been supported by the Academy of Finland (grants 265 240, 263 278, 308 248, 312 073). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the funders.
Footnotes
Declaration of interests
None.
Supporting Information
Additional supporting information may be found online in the Supporting Information section at the end of the article.
Figure S1. Supporting Information.
Data S1. Supporting Information.
References
- 1.Lee MR, Sher KJ ‘Maturing out’ of binge and problem drinking. Alcohol Res Curr Rev 2018; 39: 31–42. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Chen P, Jacobson KC Developmental trajectories of substance use from early adolescence to young adulthood: gender and racial/ethnic differences. J Adolesc Health 2012; 50: 154–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Swendsen J, Burstein M, Case B, Conway KP, Dierker L, He J, et al. Use and abuse of alcohol and illicit drugs in US adolescents. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2012; 69: 390–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Rehm J, Mathers C, Popova S, Thavorncharoensap M, Teerawattananon Y, Patra J Global burden of disease and injury and economic cost attributable to alcohol use and alcohol-use disorders. Lancet 2009; 373: 2223–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Rehm J, Baliunas D, Borges GLG, Graham K, Irving H, Kehoe T, et al. The relation between different dimensions of alcohol consumption and burden of disease: an overview. Addiction 2010; 105: 817–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Rehm J, Gmel GE, Gmel G, Hasan OSM, Imtiaz S, Popova S, et al. The relationship between different dimensions of alcohol use and the burden of disease-an update. Addiction 2017; 112: 968–1001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Sloan F, Grossman D, Platt A Heavy episodic drinking in early adulthood and outcomes in midlife. J Stud Alcohol Drugs 2011; 72: 459–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Merline A, Jager J, Schulenberg JE Adolescent risk factors for adult alcohol use and abuse: stability and change of predictive value across early and middle adulthood. Addiction 2008; 103: 84–99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Maggs JL, Patrick ME, Feinstein L Childhood and adolescent predictors of alcohol use and problems in adolescence and adulthood in the National Child Development Study. Addiction 2008; 103: 7–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Edwards AC, Gardner CO, Hickman M, Kendler KS A prospective longitudinal model predicting early adult alcohol problems: evidence for a robust externalizing pathway. Psychol Med 2016; 46: 957–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Foster KT, Hicks BM, Zucker RA Positive and negative effects of internalizing on alcohol use problems from childhood to young adulthood: the mediating and suppressing role of externalizing. J Abnorm Psychol 2018; 127: 394–403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Wichers M, Gillespie NA, Kendler KS Genetic and environmental predictors of latent trajectories of alcohol use from adolescence to adulthood: a male twin study. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2013; 37: 498–506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Haller M, Handley E, Chassin L, Bountress K Developmental cascades: linking adolescent substance use, affiliation with substance use promoting peers, and academic achievement to adult substance use disorders. Dev Psychopathol 2010; 22: 899–916. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Englund MM, Egeland B, Oliva EM, Collins WA Childhood and adolescent predictors of heavy drinking and alcohol use disorders in early adulthood: a longitudinal developmental analysis. Addiction 2008; 103: 23–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Latendresse SJ, Rose RJ, Viken RJ, Pulkkinen L, Kaprio J, Dick DM Examining the etiology of associations between perceived parenting and adolescents’ alcohol use: common genetic and/or environmental liabilities? J Stud Alcohol Drugs 2010; 71: 313–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Nash SG, McQueen A, Bray JH Pathways to adolescent alcohol use: family environment, peer influence, and parental expectations. J Adolesc Health 2005; 37: 19–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Alati R, Najman JM, Kinner SA, Mamun AA, Williams GM, O’Callaghan M, et al. Early predictors of adult drinking: a birth cohort study. Am J Epidemiol 2005; 162: 1098–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Van Ryzin MJ, Fosco GM, Dishion TJ Family and peer predictors of substance use from early adolescence to early adulthood: an 11-year prospective analysis. Addict Behav 2012; 37: 1314–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Hussong AM, Chassin L Stress and coping among children of alcoholic parents through the young adult transition. Dev Psychopathol 2004; 16: 985–1006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Huurre T, Lintonen T, Kaprio J, Pelkonen M, Marttunen M, Aro H Adolescent risk factors for excessive alcohol use at age 32 years. A 16-year prospective follow-up study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2010; 45: 125–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Toumbourou JW, Evans-Whipp TJ, Smith R, Hemphill SA, Herrenkohl TI, Catalano RF Adolescent predictors and environmental correlates of young adult alcohol use problems. Addiction 2014; 109: 417–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Korhonen T, Kujala UM, Rose RJ, Kaprio J Physical activity in adolescence as a predictor of alcohol and illicit drug use in early adulthood: a longitudinal population based twin study. Twin Res Hum Genet 2009; 12: 261–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ng Fat L, Shelton N Associations between self-reported illness and non-drinking in young adults. Addiction 2012; 107: 1612–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Wong MM, Roberson G, Dyson R Prospective relationship between poor sleep and substance-related problems in a national sample of adolescents. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2015; 39: 355–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Wong MM, Brower KJ, Nigg JT, Zucker RA Childhood sleep problems, response inhibition, and alcohol and drug outcomes in adolescence and young adulthood. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2010; 34: 1033–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Ridder EM Conduct and attentional problems in childhood and adolescence and later substance use, abuse and dependence: results of a 25-year longitudinal study. Drug Alcohol Depend 2007; 88: S14–S26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Chassin L, Flora DB, King KM Trajectories of alcohol and drug use and dependence from adolescence to adulthood: the effects of familial alcoholism and personality. J Abnorm Psychol 2004; 113: 483–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Farmer RF, Gau JM, Seeley JR, Kosty DB, Sher KJ, Lewinsohn PM Internalizing and externalizing disorders as predictors of alcohol use disorder onset during three developmental periods. Drug Alcohol Depend 2016; 164: 38–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hussong AM, Ennett ST, Cox MJ, Haroon M A systematic review of the unique prospective association of negative affect symptoms and adolescent substance use controlling for externalizing symptoms. Psychol Addict Behav 2017; 31: 137–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Boden JM, Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ Does adolescent self-esteem predict later life outcomes? A test of the causal role of self-esteem. Dev Psychopathol 2008; 20: 319–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Pesola F, Shelton K, Heron J, Munafo M, Hickman M, van den Bree M The developmental relationship between depressive symptoms in adolescence and harmful drinking in emerging adulthood: the role of peers and parents. J Youth Adolesc 2015; 44: 1752–66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Savage JE, Kaprio J, Korhonen T, Pulkkinen L, Rose RJ,Verhulst B, et al. The effects of social anxiety on alcohol and cigarette use across adolescence: results from a longitudinal twin study in Finland. Psychol Addict Behav 2016; 30: 462–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Weaver JM, Schofield TJ Mediation and moderation of divorce effects on children’s behavior problems. J Fam Psychol 2015; 29: 39–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Lahey BB, D’Onofrio BM All in the family: comparing siblings to test causal hypotheses regarding environmental influences on behavior. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2010; 19: 319–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.D’Onofrio BM, Lahey BB, Turkheimer E, Lichtenstein P Critical need for family-based, quasi-experimental designs in integrating genetic and social science research. Am J Public Health 2013; 103: S46–S55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Dick DM, Johnson JK, Viken RJ, Rose RJ Testing between-family associations in within-family comparisons. Psychol Sci 2000; 11: 409–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Irons DE, Iacono WG, McGue M Tests of the effects of adolescent early alcohol exposures on adult outcomes. Addiction 2015; 110: 269–78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Savage JE, Rose RJ, Pulkkinen L, Silventoinen K, Korhonen T, Kaprio J, et al. Early maturation and substance use across adolescence and young adulthood: a longitudinal study of Finnish twins. Dev Psychopathol 2018; 30: 79–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Greenfield TK, Ye Y, Bond J, Kerr WC, Nayak MB, Kaskutas LA, et al. Risks of alcohol use disorders related to drinking patterns in the U.S. general population. J Stud Alcohol Drugs 2014; 75: 319–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Benner AD, Kretsch N, Harden KP, Crosnoe R Academic achievement as a moderator of genetic influences on alcohol use in adolescence. Dev Psychol 2014; 50: 1170–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kendler KS, Schmitt E, Aggen SH, Prescott CA Genetic and environmental influences on alcohol, caffeine, cannabis, and nicotine use from early adolescence to middle adulthood. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008; 65: 674–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Samek DR, Keyes MA, Iacono WG, Mcgue M Peer deviance, alcohol expectancies, and adolescent alcohol use: explaining shared and nonshared environmental effects using an adoptive sibling pair design. Behav Genet 2013; 43: 286–96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Edwards AC, Maes HH, Prescott CA, Kendler KS Multiple mechanisms influencing the relationship between alcohol consumption and peer alcohol use. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2015; 39: 324–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Kaprio J The Finnish twin cohort study: an update. Twin Res Hum Genet 2013; 16: 157–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Kaprio J Twin studies in Finland 2006. Twin Res Hum Genet 2006; 9: 772–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Sarna S, Kaprio J, Sistonen P, Koskenvuo M Diagnosis of twin zygosity by mailed questionnaire. Hum Hered 1978; 28: 241–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Goldsmith H A zygosity questionnaire for young twins: a research note. Behav Genet 1991; 21: 257–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Barr PB, Salvatore JE, Maes H, Aliev F, Latvala A, Viken R, et al. Education and alcohol use: a study of gene-environment interaction in young adulthood. Soc Sci Med 2016; 162: 158–67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Horn JL A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika 1965; 30: 179–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Kaiser HF The application of electronic computers to factor analysis. Educ Psychol Meas 1960; 20: 141–51. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Hair JF, Black WC, Babin BJ, Andersen RE Exploratory factor analysis In: Multivariate Data Analysis, 7th edn. New York, NY: Pearson Education Limited; 2014, pp. 89–150. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Cattell RB The scree test for the number of factors. Multivar Behav Res 1966; 1: 245–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Bates TC, Maes H, Neale MC Umx: twin and path-based structural equation modeling in R. Twin Res Hum Genet 2019; 22: 27–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Rosseel Y Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw 2012; 48: 1–36. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Hu L, Bentler PM Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J 1999; 6: 1–55. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Croissant Y, Millo G Panel data econometrics in R: the plm package. J Stat Softw 2008; 27: 1–43. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Nosek BA, Ebersole CR, DeHaven AC, Mellor DT The preregistration revolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2018; 115: 2600–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Rubin M An evaluation of four solutions to the forking paths problem: adjusted alpha, preregistration, sensitivity analyses, and abandoning the Neyman–Pearson approach. Rev Gen Psychol 2017; 21: 321–9. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Boardman JD, Fletcher JM To cause or not to cause? That is the question, but identical twins might not have all of the answers. Soc Sci Med 2015; 127: 198–200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Hausman JA Specification tests in econometrics. Econometrica 1978; 46: 1251–71. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Patrick ME, Wray-Lake L, Finlay AK, Maggs JL The long arm of expectancies: adolescent alcohol expectancies predict adult alcohol use. Alcohol Alcohol 2010; 45: 17–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Latvala A, Rose RJ, Pulkkinen L, Dick DM, Korhonen T, Kaprio J Drinking, smoking, and educational achievement: cross-lagged associations from adolescence to adulthood. Drug Alcohol Depend 2014; 137: 106–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Wicki M, Kuntsche E, Gmel G Drinking at European universities? A review of students’ alcohol use. Addict Behav 2010; 35: 913–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Munafò MR, Smith GD Robust research needs many lines of evidence. Nature 2018; 553: 399–401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Dixon LJ, Leen-Feldner EW, Ham LS, Feldner MT, Lewis SF Alcohol use motives among traumatic event-exposed, treatment-seeking adolescents: associations with posttraumatic stress. Addict Behav 2009; 34: 1065–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Overstreet C, Berenz EC, Kendler KS, Dick DM, Amstadter AB Predictors and mental health outcomes of potentially traumatic event exposure. Psychiatry Res 2017; 247: 296–304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.McGue M, Osler M, Christensen K Causal inference and observational research: the utility of twins. Perspect Psychol Sci 2010; 5: 546–56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Kendler KS, Gardner C, Dick DM Predicting alcohol consumption in adolescence from alcohol-specific and general externalizing genetic risk factors, key environmental exposures and their interaction. Psychol Med 2011; 41: 1507–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Button TMM, Lau JYF, Maughan B, Eley TC Parental punitive discipline, negative life events and gene–environment interplay in the development of externalizing behavior. Psychol Med 2008; 38: 29–39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Dick DM, Pagan JL, Viken R, Purcell S, Kaprio J,Pulkkinen L, et al. Changing environmental influences on substance use across development. Twin Res Hum Genet 2007; 10: 315–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Pulkkinen L, Kaprio J, Rose RJ Peers, teachers and parents as assessors of the behavioural and emotional problems of twins and their adjustment: the multidimensional peer nomination inventory. Twin Res Hum Genet 1999; 2: 274–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Rosenberg M Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1965. [Google Scholar]
- 73.Salvatore JE, Aliev F, Edwards AC, Evans DM, Macleod J, Hickman M, et al. Polygenic scores predict alcohol problems in an independent sample and show moderation by the environment. Genes 2014; 5: 330–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Petersen AC, Crockett L, Richards M, Boxer A A self-report measure of pubertal status: reliability, validity, and initial norms. J Youth Adolesc 1988; 17: 117–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.