Abstract
The aim of the current study was to examine microstructural differences in white matter relevant to procedural and declarative memory between adolescents/young adults with and without Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) using diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). The findings showed atypical age-related changes in white matter structures in the corticostriatal system, in the corticocerebellar system, and in the medial temporal region in individuals with DLD. Results highlight the importance of considering the age factor in research on DLD. Future studies are needed to examine the developmental relationship between long-term memory and individual differences in language development and learning.
Keywords: Procedural memory brain system, declarative memory brain system, diffusion tensor imaging, developmental language disorder
1. Introduction
There is a long history of research attempting to characterize the relationship between language development and different forms of memory, with a presumption that language development is not dependent solely on processes specifically tied to language, but it also relies on domain-general cognitive processes, such as memory acquisition, processing, and retrieval (Adams & Gathercole, 2009; Baddeley, 2003; Kidd, 2012; Ullman & Pierpont, 2005; but see Chomsky, 2011; van der Lely, 2005). Developmental language disorder (DLD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder in language development and learning, which is not associated with any known condition, such as autistic spectrum disorders (ASD), intellectual disabilities, or congenital hearing impairment (Bishop, Snowling, Thompson, Greenhalgh, & CATALISE-2 consortium, 2017). Thus, it provides a valuable testing ground for hypotheses regarding the role of memory in language development and learning. Previous studies have consistently shown a role of procedural and declarative memory in DLD at the behavioral level (for a review, see Lum & Conti-Ramsden, 2013; Lum, Conti-Ramsden, Morgan, & Ullman, 2014; Ullman, Earle, Walenski, & Janacsek, 2020). In the current study, we aimed to look at whether there were brain structural differences in these two memory brain systems in individuals with and without DLD, and whether these differences varied across age.
1.1. Development of Long-term Memory Brain Systems
Procedural and declarative memory differ with regard to their supporting brain structures as well as the cognitive processes involved in memory and learning (Eichenbaum & Cohen, 2004; Gabrieli, 1998; Squire, 1992). We used the terms “procedural memory brain system” and “declarative memory brain system” to indicate that the focus of the current study was on the neural systems involved in learning, representation, and use of the procedural and declarative information respectively.
1.1.1. Procedural Memory Brain System.
The procedural memory system supports incremental and implicit learning of rule- or pattern-based relations via repetitive exposures (Cohen & Bacdayan, 1994; Packard & Knowlton, 2002). Learning that relies upon this system requires relatively few cognitive resources, and can even occur without having the intention to do so. Although the learning process is gradual and slow, rules/patterns can be applied quickly and automatically once they are acquired (Squire, 1992).
Results pertaining to development of procedural memory are mixed (see Zwart et al., 2019, for a review). Reber (1993) hypothesized that procedural learning should demonstrate early ontogenetic maturation because it mainly recruits evolutionarily primitive brain regions (e.g., the basal ganglia and the cerebellum). However, this hypothesis has been well challenged. For example, Thomas et al. (2004) showed age-related improvement in procedural learning on the serial reaction time (SRT) task, a commonly used paradigm to investigate procedural learning. Janacsek et al. (2012) further argued that the period between birth and adolescence is critical for development of procedural learning, and at around the age of 12, the cognitively controlled processes are more mature and thus are useful for targeted explicit learning at the cost of becoming less sensitive to probabilities of sequences/events during procedural learning. This suggests a developmental shift in the interaction between simple probability-based procedural learning and explicit higher-order learning in early adolescence (Nemeth, Janacsek, & Fiser, 2013).
From a neuroanatomical perspective, the procedural memory system is composed of a network of interconnected brain structures, including the corticostriatal and the corticocerebellar systems. The corticostriatal system is a collective term describing the reciprocal connections between the cortex and the basal ganglia, and some of the connections are via the thalamus (Alexander, DeLong, & Strick, 1986; Lehericy et al., 2004). The prominent white matter structures in the corticostriatal system include (but are not limited to) the internal capsule, which contains both ascending and descending axons between the cortex and the basal ganglia (Schmahmann & Pandya, 2006). The corticocerebellar system is comprised of several major white matter structures, including the superior and middle cerebellar peduncles (Habas & Cabanis, 2007; Leitner, Travis, Ben-Shachar, Yeom, & Feldman, 2015; Naidich et al., 2009). The superior cerebellar peduncle contains primarily efferent fibers that emerge from the deep cerebellar nuclei, traveling to the thalamus, and then to the cerebral cortex. The middle cerebellar peduncle is a major afferent projection that carries input fibers from the contralateral cerebral cortex via pontine nuclei across the midline of the cerebellum to the cerebellar cortex.
Recent DTI studies showed that the corticostriatal tracts and the corticocerebellar tracts reached maturation during adolescence/early adulthood, as revealed by increased fractional anisotropy (FA) with age (Asato et al., 2010; Lebel et al., 2012; Simmonds et al., 2014; Tamnes et al., 2010). In addition, growth continues into adulthood in regional termination zones (i.e., white matter regions adjacent to gray matter) in cortical and basal ganglia regions, suggesting a critical role of the cortical-subcortical network in cognitive development beyond adolescence (Simmonds et al., 2014).
1.1.2. Declarative Memory Brain System.
The declarative memory system supports acquisition and flexible use of context-free knowledge about the world (i.e., semantic memory) as well as context-specific information unique to an individual (i.e., episodic memory) (Tulving, 1972). In the classic definition, learning that relies on the declarative memory system typically requires conscious effort to encode and retrieve information, and therefore, in contrast with procedural memory, declarative memory can be directly measured by recognition or recall of previously learned stimuli (Eichenbaum & Cohen, 2004; Gabrielli, 1998; Squire, 1992; but see Dew & Cabeza, 2011; Henke, 2010).
From a neuroanatomical perspective, the declarative memory system is primarily supported by the medial temporal region (Eichenbaum & Lipton, 2008; Squire, Stark, & Clark, 2004). The hippocampus, located in the medial temporal lobes, plays a critical role in binding together different inputs to form representations of the relations among the constituent elements of scenes or events. Two white matter structures have been consistently shown to be associated with declarative memory functioning, including: 1) the cingulum bundle, which forms a ring from the orbitofrontal cortex, along the dorsal surface of the corpus callosum, then down the temporal lobe towards the pole while connecting the thalamic nuclei with the cingulate gyrus, and 2) the fornix, one of the major output structures of the hippocampus (Bubb, Metzler-Baddeley, & Aggleton, 2018; Sepulcre et al., 2008; Schmahmann & Pandya, 2006). DTI studies have shown a prolonged development of the cingulum bundle and the fornix (Lebel et al, 2012; Simmonds et al., 2014), which are consistent with behavioral findings showing continuous development of declarative memory until early adulthood (Ronnlund et al., 2005). Simmonds et al. (2014) also found a distinct phase of growth trajectory in the cingulate part of the cingulum bundle, with rapid growth in childhood, followed by a slowing of growth in early-middle adolescence, and acceleration of growth again in late adolescence/early adulthood.
1.2. Procedural and Declarative Memory Systems in DLD
While little is known about the fundamental processes underlying DLD, many attempts have been made to look at the role of memory in DLD (e.g., Archibald & Gathercole, 2006; Lum & Conti-Ramsden, 2013). One of the predominant hypotheses concerning the memory-language connection in DLD is the Procedural Deficit Hypothesis (PDH; Ullman & Pierpont, 2005). According to the PDH, individuals with DLD have an altered procedural memory system, resulting in impaired acquisition of rule- or pattern-based aspects of language (e.g., grammar or phonotactics) as well as impaired non-linguistic function mediated by the same brain system. In contrast, individuals with DLD are predicted to have an intact (or even strengthened) declarative memory system, which not only supports lexical-semantic aspects of language development and learning, but also compensates for poor grammatical learning by using explicit rules or chunking (Ullman & Pullman, 2015).
A growing body of behavioral research has been conducted to test and further specify the PDH (see Ullman et al., 2020, for a review). Evidence concerning the procedural memory system in DLD is relatively consistent, showing that individuals with DLD have poor procedural learning, and this deficit is associated with their grammatical ability (Hedenius et al., 2011; Lee & Tomblin, 2015; Tomblin, Mainela-Arnold, & Zhang, 2007; but see Gabriel et al., 2011). However, results are mixed with regard to the relationship between the declarative memory system and DLD. Individuals with DLD were reported to have poor declarative memory and learning in the verbal domain (Bishop & Hsu, 2015; Lukacs et al., 2017; Lum, Gelgic, & Conti-Ramsden, 2010). Lum et al. (2012, 2015) argued that this poor performance is indeed associated with comorbid poor working memory capacity. Disagreement also exists in terms of the nonverbal domain of the declarative memory system in DLD. Some researchers showed typical or even enhanced declarative memory for non-verbal information in DLD (Lukacs et al., 2017; Lum et al., 2010; Riccio, Cash, & Cohen, 2007), whereas others found that individuals with DLD have difficulty memorizing paired associates in nonverbal declarative learning tasks (Bishop & Hsu, 2015; Collisson et al., 2014; Lee, 2018; Poll, Miller, & van Hell, 2015). The discrepancy in previous studies may be attributed to different task-dependent mnemonic processes underlying different tests/tasks. Therefore, an alternative to using behavioral tasks to test these memory processes is to examine the brain structures that are known to subserve them.
To sum up, behavioral findings support the notion that both procedural and declarative memory systems may play a role in language learning difficulties of individuals with DLD. However, no studies have directly examined the procedural and declarative memory brain systems in DLD with a hypothesis driven approach (i.e., brain regions of interest were selected based on the PDH), although a few studies included substructures of the basal ganglia as ancillary analyses. For example, previous studies showed structural alterations in the caudate nucleus in individuals with DLD (Badcock et al., 2012; Soriano-Mas et al., 2009; but see Jernigan et al., 1991), but reports in Badcock et al. (2012) and Soriano-Mas et al. (2009) differed with respect to the direction of volumetric difference (i.e., smaller vs. larger volumes in the caudate nucleus in DLD). This difference may be, at least partially, attributed to the discrepancy in subject age, as well as imaging methods and analyses across studies (see Liegeois et al., 2014; Mayes et al., 2015, for a review). Lee, Nopoulos & Tomblin (2013) showed that young adults with DLD had larger relative volumes of the putamen, the nucleus accumbens, and the hippocampus than those without DLD; however, they did not find significant difference in the structures of the caudate nucleus after the intracranial volumes were controlled.
White matter structures pertaining to procedural and declarative memory were not yet examined in individuals with and without DLD. In addition, DLD is a neurodevelopmental disorder that persists across the lifespan, and thus the age factor needs to be taken into account in data interpretation. As far as we know, there is only one exploratory study in the literature examining age-related brain structural changes in children with DLD across a wide age range (Soriano-Mas et al., 2009).
1.3. The Current Study
The aim of the current study was to examine the differences in white matter structures relevant to procedural and declarative memory between adolescents/young adults with and without DLD. This wide age range provided an opportunity to examine whether group differences varied across age. We hypothesized that individuals with DLD would show a significant difference in white matter structures within the corticostriatal system as well as in the medial temporal region, which is reflective of poor functioning of procedural and declarative memory in DLD respectively. However, we did not expect to find a group difference in the corticocerebellar system given that previous behavioral studies showed intact cerebellar-dependent learning (i.e., eyeblink conditioning) in children and adolescents with DLD (Hardiman, Hsu, & Bishop, 2013; Steinmetz & Rice, 2010).
2. Material and Methods
2.1. Participants
Participants were recruited at two different time points, with the gap of two years. The recruitment methods were different with respect to subject sources (i.e., from longitudinal database vs. from school-wide screening at local public schools) and test batteries. However, the diagnostic criteria for each group (i.e., DLD vs. Comparison) was comparable between the first and the second recruitment (see Section 2.1.3 for statistical analyses). Table 1 summarizes the demographic information and test scores of the participants.
Table 1.
Demographics and test scores for the DLD and the Comparison group.
DLD (n=14) |
Comparison (n=12) |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | P | |
Cohort 1 | |||||
Age (year) | 22.42 | 2.02 | 22.12 | .53 | .62 |
Sex (Male:Female) | 5:9 | 4:8 | .90 | ||
Handedness (Right:Left) | 13:1 | 12:0 | .35 | ||
Language Composite Scores | 69.51 | 13.01 | 100.13 | 11.80 | <.001 |
Nonverbal IQ | 89.36 | 13.40 | 112.50 | 10.03 | <.001 |
DLD (n=17) |
Comparison (n=51) |
||||
M | SD | M | SD | P | |
Cohort 2 | |||||
Age (year) | 16.98 | 1.54 | 16.77 | 1.45 | .61 |
Sex (Male:Female) | 7:10 | 20:31 | .89 | ||
Handedness (Right:Left) | 11:6 | 47:4 | .01 | ||
Language Composite Scores | 78.67 | 5.27 | 102.99 | 9.96 | <.001 |
Nonverbal IQ | 85.82 | 8.67 | 101.88 | 15.26 | <.001 |
2.1.1. Recruitment for Cohort 1.
Participants were recruited from a longitudinal study (see Tomblin & Nippold, 2014, for details of the longitudinal study). Their language ability at the time of their participation was based on three language tests: 1) Word Derivations, a subtest from The Test of Adolescent and Adult Language, Fourth Edition (TOAL-4; Hammill, Brown, Larsen, & Wiederholt, 2007) to assess knowledge of derivational morphology, 2) Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Fourth Edition (PPVT-4; Dunn & Dunn, 2007) to assess receptive vocabulary, and 3) a modified version of the Token Test (de Renzi & Faglioni, 1978; Morice & McNicol, 1985) to assess sentence comprehension. The diagnosis of language disorders was reconfirmed if participants with a history of DLD had language composite scores below one standard deviation below the mean. All participants were required to have nonverbal IQ above 70, as measured on two nonverbal subtests (i.e., Block Design and Matrix Reasoning) of Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI, Wechsler, 1999).
2.1.2. Recruitment for Cohort 2.
Participants were recruited from high schools and colleges in the Midwest with the age range of 14 to 20 years. None of them had a diagnosis of neurodevelopmental disorders (e.g., intellectual disabilities, ASD) or hearing loss. This recruitment method was different from the one used in first recruitment. The sample of adolescents was obtained via a school-wide screening of language ability. Adolescents were then drawn from the screened pool to provide a sample that had a roughly rectangular distribution, with oversampling of adolescents in the high and low ends of the normal distribution with regard to language. Those selected were then given a battery of language tests, including: 1) PPVT-4 (Dunn & Dunn, 2007), 2) Expressive Vocabulary Test, Second Edition (EVT-2; Williams, 1997) to assess expressive vocabulary, 3) Recalling Sentences in Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, Fourth Edition (CELF-4; Semel et al., 2003) to assess the ability to recall and reproduce sentences of syntactic complexity, 4) Understanding Spoken Paragraphs in CELF-4 to assess passage comprehension, and 5) Word Derivations in TOAL-4 (Hammill et al., 2007). All participants had nonverbal IQ above 70 on Block Design and Matrix Reasoning from WASI (Wechsler, 1999). The language tests used in the second recruitment were carefully selected based on Tomblin, Freese, and Records (1992) and Fidler, Plante, and Vance (2011), with the aim to assess multiple domains of language in adolescents and young adults.
2.1.3. Establishing Comparability of Language Measures Used in Two Cohorts.
Measurement of language ability at different points in development often requires use of different measures. Nevertheless, it has been shown that oral language in childhood and adolescence consists largely of a uni-dimensional latent trait (Anthony, Davis, Williams, & Anthony, 2014; Bornstein, Hahn, Putnick, & Suwalsky, 2013; Language and Reading Research Consortium, 2015; Tomblin & Zhang, 2006). In this study, there were two tests in common across the cohorts (i.e., PPVT and Word Derivations in TOAL-4) along with the tests that were unique in each cohort. Results showed that the common measures were strongly correlated with the unique measures employed for Cohort 1, r=.60, p=.001, and for Cohort 2, r=.81, p<.001. The Fisher’s r-to-z transformation showed that these two correlations were not significantly different, z=−1.9, p=.06, suggesting that language measures used in different cohorts indeed represent a similar latent language ability trait, and thus the diagnostic label for each group (i.e., DLD vs. Comparison) is comparable between the first and the second recruitment. The standard scores based on the test norms were averaged for each subject and used to assign individuals to groups. Individuals with DLD were required to have average language scores below one standard deviation below the mean. Those with scores equal to or greater than one standard deviation below the mean were assigned to the Comparison group.
2.2. MRI Acquisition
All image scans were obtained at the University of Iowa Hospital and Clinics using the Siemens 3T Trio scanner.
2.2.1. Anatomical Image.
The T1 weighted images were acquired in the coronal plane using a 3D MP-RAGE sequence with the following parameters: echo time (TE)=2.86 ms, repetition time (TR)=2300 ms, inversion time=900 ms, flip angle=10°, number of excitations (NEX)=1, field of view (FOV)=256×256×256 mm, and slice thickness=1 mm. The PD/T2 weighted images were acquired with the following parameters: TE=430 ms, TR=4800 ms, NEX=1, slice thickness=1.4 mm, FOV=256×256×256 mm, and turbo factor=137.
2.2.2. Diffusion Tensor Image.
The diffusion weighted images were collected using a 2D twice refocused echo-planar spin-echo sequence with the following parameters: TE=81 ms, TR=9000 ms, FOV=256×256 mm, Matrix=128×128, slice thickness=2.0 mm, the number of diffusion directions=64, and b-values=1000 s/mm2.
2.3. Image Preprocessing and Processing
2.3.1. Anatomical Image Processing.
The anatomical data were preprocessed using AutoWorkup (Magnotta et al., 2002). The primary steps of the standard pipeline include: 1) anterior commissure (AC)-posterior commissure (PC) alignment of T1 volume, 2) co-registration of T2 weighted images to AC-PC aligned T1, defining of Talairach parameters to warp the Talairach grid onto the raw space of each subject, 3) tissue classification of white matter, gray matter, and cerebrospinal fluid, and 4) skull stripping using an artificial neural network (Magnotta et al., 1999). After completion of AutoWorkup, all scans were individually inspected for quality control.
2.3.2. Diffusion Tensor Image Processing.
The DTIPrep (Liu et al., 2010) was used to perform several quality assurance steps as well as removing volumes within a scan that did not meet its minimal quality criteria before tensor image estimation. The automatic pipeline in the software includes: 1) protocol verification, 2) slice-wise checking, and 3) detection and removal of artifacts caused by eddy-currents, head motion, bed vibration and pulsation, venetian blind artifacts, as well as intensity inconsistencies (Oguz et al., 2014). The final dataset contained an averaged baseline image and diffusion-weighted images that passed all the quality assurance tests. Next, we used the Guided Tensor Restore Anatomical Connectivity Tractography (GTRACT; Cheng et al., 2006) software to process the output files from the DTIPrep, including estimation of tensor images and subsequent computation of fractional anisotropy (FA) images. Johns Hopkins University (JHU) white matter tractography atlas (Hua et al., 2008; Wakana et al., 2007) was then used to label white matter structures of interest, as shown below.
Major white matter structures in the corticostriatal system (see Figure 1): bilateral anterior limbs of internal capsule (ALIC), and bilateral posterior limbs of internal capsule (PLIC)
Major white matter structures in the corticocerebellar system (see Figure 2): bilateral superior cerebellar peduncle (SCP), and the middle cerebellar peduncle (MCP)
Major white matter structures in the medial temporal region (see Figure 3): bilateral fornix, bilateral cingulate gyrus part of the cingulum bundle (CGC), and bilateral hippocampal part of the cingulum bundle (CGH)
Figure 1.
JHU white matter tractography atlas of regions of interest in the corticostriatal system. ALIC: Anterior limb of internal capsule; PLIC: Posterior limb of internal capsule; RLIC.
Figure 2.
JHU white matter tractography atlas of regions of interest in the corticocerebellar system. CP: Cerebellar peduncle.
Figure 3.
JHU white matter tractography atlas of regions of interest in the medial temporal region. CGC: Cingulum_cingulate gyrus; CGH: Cingulum_hippocampus.
2.4. Statistical Analysis
FA was used as the primary diffusion measure in the analyses; it is one of the primary indices of white matter microstructural integrity (Alexander et al., 2007). Recent studies have consistently demonstrated developmental increases in FA from childhood into early adulthood (e.g., Barnea-Goraly et al., 2005; Schmithorst et al., 2002).
An analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) within the general linear model was used to compute the effects of Age, Group, and Age-by-Group interactions on FAs in the white matter structures of interest, with sex, handedness, and nonverbal IQ entered as covariates. All statistical analyses were carried out using SPSS version 25. In addition, we used false discovery rate (FDR) to correct for multiple comparisons in each hypothesis testing (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995). The p-values in each table (i.e., Table 3, Table 4, and Table 5 respectively) defined the domain of the correction.
Table 3.
Fractional anisotropy (FA) of the white matter structures in the corticostriatal system. Multivariate analyses were carried out, with sex, handedness, and nonverbal IQ as covariates. An asterisk indicates that the p-value remains significant after false discovery rate (FDR) adjustment. ηp2: partial eta-squared; L: Left; R: Right.
F | P | ηp2 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Anterior Limb of Internal Capsule | R | Age | 5.31 | .02* | .06 |
Group | 10.11 | .002* | .10 | ||
Age × Group | 3.49 | .07 | .04 | ||
L | Age | 5.05 | .027* | .06 | |
Group | 8.52 | .004* | .09 | ||
Age × Group | 8.00 | .006* | .08 | ||
Posterior Limb of Internal Capsule | R | Age | 7.02 | .01* | .08 |
Group | 5.26 | .02* | .06 | ||
Age × Group | 2.95 | .09 | .03 | ||
L | Age | 7.33 | .008* | .08 | |
Group | 2.59 | .11 | .03 | ||
Age × Group | 6.46 | .01* | .07 |
Table 4.
Fractional anisotropy (FA) of the white matter structures in the corticocerebellar system. Multivariate analyses were carried out, with sex, handedness, and nonverbal IQ as covariates. An asterisk indicates that the p-value remains significant after false discovery rate (FDR) adjustment. ηp2: partial eta-squared; L: Left; R: Right.
F | P | ηp2 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Middle Cerebellar Peduncle | Age | .68 | .41 | .01 | |
Group | 7.18 | .009* | .08 | ||
Age × Group | 6.91 | .01* | .07 | ||
Superior Cerebellar Peduncle | R | Age | .20 | .66 | < .01 |
Group | 10.91 | .001* | .11 | ||
Age × Group | 12.52 | .001* | .13 | ||
L | Age | .96 | .33 | .01 | |
Group | 7.49 | .008* | .08 | ||
Age × Group | 6.01 | .016* | .07 |
Table 5.
Fractional anisotropy (FA) of the fiber tracts in the medial temporal region. Multivariate analyses were carried out, with sex, handedness, and nonverbal IQ as covariates. An asterisk indicates that the p-value remains significant after false discovery rate (FDR) adjustment. ηp2: partial eta-squared; L: Left; R: Right.
F | P | ηp2 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Fornix | R | Age | 2.88 | .09 | .03 |
Group | 13.89 | <.001 | .14 | ||
Age × Group | 5.76 | .02* | .06 | ||
L | Age | 4.08 | .05 | .05 | |
Group | 11.00 | .001* | .11 | ||
Age × Group | 6.28 | .01* | .07 | ||
Cingulate gyrus part of the cingulum bundle | R | Age | 13.49 | <.001* | .13 |
Group | 4.77 | .03 | .05 | ||
Age × Group | 3.35 | .07 | .04 | ||
L | Age | 2.57 | .11 | .03 | |
Group | 2.93 | .09 | .03 | ||
Age × Group | 2.03 | .16 | .02 | ||
Hippocampal part of the cingulum bundle | R | Age | 8.76 | .004* | .09 |
Group | 6.62 | .01* | .07 | ||
Age × Group | 3.40 | .07 | .04 | ||
L | Age | 8.06 | .006* | .09 | |
Group | 4.22 | .04 | .05 | ||
Age × Group | 1.08 | .30 | .01 |
3. Results
The mean and standard deviations of the FA values in the brain structures of interest as well as in the control structures for each group were summarized in Table 2. Levene’s test showed that variances in the dependent variables were not significantly different (.78 > ps > .054), except for the MCP, F(1, 92)=5.59, p=.02.
Table 2.
Summary of fractional anisotropy (FA) values in the fiber tracts of interest as well as in the control tracts. DLD: Developmental language disorder; L: Left; R: Right; SD: Standard deviation.
Fiber Tracts | Group | Mean | SD | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Corticostriatal System | ||||
Anterior limb of internal capsule | R | Comparison | .39 | .03 |
DLD | .37 | .03 | ||
L | Comparison | .39 | .03 | |
DLD | .37 | .03 | ||
Posterior limb of internal capsule | R | Comparison | .53 | .03 |
DLD | .52 | .03 | ||
L | Comparison | .54 | .03 | |
DLD | .53 | .03 | ||
Corticocerebellar System | ||||
Superior cerebellar peduncle | R | Comparison | .34 | .03 |
DLD | .32 | .03 | ||
L | Comparison | .35 | .03 | |
DLD | .32 | .03 | ||
Middle cerebellar peduncle | Comparison | .36 | .02 | |
DLD | .35 | .03 | ||
Medial Temporal Region | ||||
Fornix | R | Comparison | .34 | .02 |
DLD | .32 | .02 | ||
L | Comparison | .36 | .02 | |
DLD | .34 | .02 | ||
Cingulate gyrus part of the cingulum bundle | R | Comparison | .30 | .05 |
DLD | .28 | .05 | ||
L | Comparison | .32 | .05 | |
DLD | .30 | .04 | ||
Hippocampal part of the cingulum bundle | R | Comparison | .22 | .03 |
DLD | .20 | .03 | ||
L | Comparison | .22 | .03 | |
DLD | .21 | .03 |
3.1. Corticostriatal System in DLD across Age
The results of ANOVAs concerned with the white matter structures in the corticostriatal system were shown in Table 3. Significant main effects involving Age and Group without significant interactions were found in two structures of interest, including the right ALIC and the right PLIC. In these cases, the DLD group had significantly lower FA values than the Comparison group, and the Age effect reflected an increase in FA with age.
In addition, a significant interaction between Age and Group was found in both brain structures of interest (i.e., ALIC and PLIC), particularly in the left hemisphere. These findings suggest atypical age-related microstructural changes in the corticostriatal system in individuals with DLD.
Follow-up tests on these significant interactions showed that across the brain structures of interest, the same pattern was evident: comparison participants showed gradually increased FA across age in the left ALIC, r=.45, p<.001, and in the left PLIC, r=.45, p<.001, whereas individuals with DLD did not show a significantly increased FA across age in the left ALIC, r=−.13, p=.51, or in the left PLIC, r=−.05, p=.78. The left ALIC was used as an example to illustrate the interaction pattern (see Figure 4).
Figure 4.
Age-by-group interaction effect in the left anterior limb of the internal capsule (ALIC). FA: Fractional anisotropy.
3.2. Corticocerebellar System in DLD across Age
Table 4 summarizes the ANOVA results performed on the measures of the white matter structures in the corticocerebellar system. The Age-by-Group interaction effect was significant in the MCP and in the bilateral SCP. We used the right SCP as an example to show different developmental trajectories of FA changes between the DLD and the Comparison group (see Figure 5). In Figure 5, comparison participants showed gradually increased FA in the right SCP across age, r=.36, p=.004, whereas individuals with DLD showed a significant decrease in FA across age, r=−.43, p=.02. These findings suggest atypical age-related microstructural changes in the corticocerebellar system in individuals with DLD.
Figure 5.
Age-by-group interaction effect in the left superior cerebellar peduncle (SCP). FA: Fractional anisotropy.
3.3. White Matter Structures in the Medial Temporal Region in DLD across Age
Table 5 provides the results of the ANOVA for the brain structures in the medial temporal region. The Age-by-Group interaction effect was only significant in the right and left fornix. Age-related change in the left fornix in both groups was plotted in Figure 6, showing that FA increased with age for the Comparison group, r=.44, p<.001, whereas this was not evident in the DLD group, r=−.10, p=.59. In terms of the cingulum bundle, while the Age-by-Group interaction was not significant, the Group main effect was found in the right CGH. The DLD group showed significantly lower FA in the right CGH than the Comparison group, suggesting poor white matter microstructure in the right CGH independent of age.
Figure 6.
Age-by-group interaction effect in the left fornix. FA: Fractional anisotropy.
4. Discussion
The aim of the current study was to examine differences in white matter microstructures relevant to procedural and declarative memory in individuals with and without DLD. The findings showed atypical age-related changes in white matter structures in the corticostriatal system, in the corticocerebellar system, and in the medial temporal region in individuals with DLD. Specifically, FA increased across age in the comparison participants with normal language abilities, but did not increase in individuals with DLD.
4.1. Corticostriatal System in DLD
The corticostriatal system is one of the major neural networks supporting procedural learning. Within this neural network, the basal ganglia modulate cognition and behavior by gating or selecting a subset of representations or movements activated by the cortex (Seger, 2006). It has been well established that the corticostriatal system mediates a wide variety of learning behaviors, including habit learning, motor skill learning, sequential learning, categorization learning, and reinforcement learning (see Koziol & Budding, 2009, for a review). In addition, growing studies have shown a critical role of the corticostriatal system in different domains of language learning and processing, syntax and phonology in particular (Chan, Ryan, & Bever, 2013; Crosson et al., 2003; Dominey, Inui, & Hoen, 2009; Tettamanti et al., 2005; see Bohsali & Crosson, 2016, for a review).
Previous studies showed that most corticostriatal white matter tracts reach 90% of the maximum FA values during adolescence/early adulthood (Barnea-Goraly et al., 2005; Lebel et al., 2012). In the current study, we found atypical age-related microstructural changes in the white matter structures in the corticostriatal system in individuals with DLD. That is, individuals with DLD did not have a significant increase in FA in the corticostriatal white matter structures across time, whereas comparison participants did. Animal and human studies have shown that maturation of corticostriatal connectivity supports developmental change in goal-directed as well as value-guided behavior (Somerville & Casey, 2010). Lee (2017) showed that adolescents/young adults with DLD had difficulty coding the motivational value of prospective incentives, and thus were not able to choose the most optimal actions in order to maximize future outcomes. Thus, future studies are suggested to conduct seed-based tractography analysis to examine white matter integrity of the executive and motivational corticostriatal loops in DLD (Seger, 2006; Lawrence et al., 1998; see Alexander et al., 1986, for five corticostriatal loops), which will provide information on the specificity of preserved and/or altered behavioral functioning in individuals with DLD and thus provide new ideas for intervention.
4.2. Corticocerebellar System in DLD
The corticocerebellar system is the other major neural network supporting procedural learning. Unlike the basal ganglia, which primarily involve selective gating of cortical representations, the cerebellum engages in online amplification and refinement of behaviors, serving as an error detection mechanism to detect deviations of timing or sequential events so that behavior can be adjusted to cope with environmental changes (Ide & Li, 2011). It is also proposed that the cerebellum involves updating of predictions about action consequences via error-driven learning (Koziol et al., 2014). Recent studies showed that this domain-general mechanism mediated a variety of language processing and learning (Timmann & Daum, 2007; Mariën et al., 2014). For example, Moberget et al. (2014) found an association between increased cerebellar activations and processing of predictive sentences (i.e., the initial part of a sentence gives clues to what will be at the end of the sentence), implicating a generalized role of the cerebellum in processing of prediction error signals. In addition, the corticocerebellar system is also shown to engage in grammatical, semantic, and phonological processing (Adamaszek & Kirkby, 2015; Stoodley & Schmahmann, 2009). Despite mounting evidence, the exact role of the corticocerebellar system in language remains to be elucidated.
Cerebellar white matter reaches maturation during adolescence/young adulthood (Lebel et al., 2012; Simmonds et al., 2014). In the current study, we showed that individuals with DLD had structural alteration in white matter structures in the corticocerebellar system, and this alteration varied across age. If structural alterations at the brain level are likely to be reflected on the behavioral level, the current data seem contrary to prior behavioral findings showing typical learning in eyeblink conditioning in children and adolescents with DLD (Hardiman et al., 2013; Steinmetz & Rice, 2010; see Ullman et al., 2020, for a review). This discrepancy may be attributed to the age factor. The age range in our current sample is between 14 to 28 years, which is older than that in Hardiman et al. (2013; age: 7–11 years) and Steinmetz and Rice (2010; age: 9–20 years). In other words, it is possible that behavioral differences in eyeblink conditioning between individuals with and with DLD cannot be sensitively detected until later years.
The other possible explanation to the discrepancy between our brain imaging finding and prior behavioral finding may have to do with the degree of task demands placed on the cerebellum as well as its connection with the cortex. Eyeblink conditioning is a primitive learning process, which places relatively less demand on the cerebral cortex. Thus, it is likely that information processing within the cerebellar macrostructure is spared in individuals with DLD, which is then able to support eyeblink conditioning behavior. However, if we use a learning paradigm that strongly involves the reciprocal connection between the cerebellum and the cerebral cortex, we might expect to see impaired learning in DLD.
4.3. Medial Temporal Region in DLD
The medial temporal region comprises a neural system of anatomically related structures that is necessary for declarative memory (Squire et al., 2004). Recently, researchers have attempted to make a connection between declarative memory and language. For example, Ullman (2001, 2004) proposed the Declarative/Procedural model, wherein the division between grammar and vocabulary in language is closely tied to the division between procedural and declarative memory. Several studies have shown that the mental lexicon uses the brain systems that also subserve the acquisition of episodic information in healthy individuals (e.g., Bartha-Doering et al., 2018; Breitenstein et al., 2005; Davis & Gaskell, 2009).
Duff and her colleagues extend the contribution of declarative memory to language even further, proposing that the same processes underlying the declarative memory system, such as relational binding of items into integrated representations as well as flexible use and online maintenance of those representations, are indeed the same processes essential for various aspects of language processing and use, in addition to word learning (Duff & Brown-Schmidt, 2012). By studying patients with hippocampal amnesia, they showed that the hippocampal declarative memory system involves language processing and use beyond the lexical domain, including reported speech (Duff et al., 2007), verbal play in social interactions (Duff et al., 2009), and the use of definite references during online processing (Duff et al., 2011). Duff’s work mainly focused on the role of the hippocampus in language in adults with brain lesions, thus it remains unclear with respect to the associations between other structures of the medial temporal region and language development and learning.
In the current study, individuals with DLD showed atypical age-related changes in the white matter microstructure of the fornix. The fornix is the major output structure of the hippocampus, arcing around the thalamus and connecting the medial temporal lobes to the hypothalamus (Thomas, Koumellis, & Dineen, 2011). Integrity of this white matter structure plays an important role in the formation and consolidation of declarative memory (Mabbott et al., 2009; Sepulcre et al., 2008). Therefore, our findings suggest that individuals with DLD may also have declarative memory deficits, and this poor functioning may be more easily detected during adolescence/young adulthood. In the literature, results are mixed regarding declarative memory functioning in DLD. Some researchers showed impaired declarative memory for verbal and nonverbal information in DLD, whereas others found no evidence of such mnemonic difficulty (Bishop & Hsu, 2015; Kuppuraj, Rao, & Bishop, 2016; Lum et al., 2012; Poll et al., 2015; Records et al., 1995). This discrepancy may lie in different task-dependent processes that rely on distinct neural circuits in the medial temporal regions. Moreover, the age factor may also, at least in part, account for different results in different studies.
At this point, we are not able to falsify the declarative memory compensation hypothesis, an extension of the PDH stating that declarative memory is relatively spared and therefore is able to compensate for procedural memory and other dysfunctions in DLD (Ullman & Pierpont, 2005). A better understanding of declarative memory is important in yielding new ways to treat people with neurodevelopmental disorders (Ullman & Pullman, 2015). Future work is needed to examine how declarative memory-related brain regions play a role in functional compensation when the procedural memory brain system is impaired along development in DLD.
4.4. Domain-General Cognitive Processes and Individual Differences in Language
DLD is a label given to individuals who have poor language ability with unknown causes, and this label is created based on an arbitrary cutoff language score. However, if we treat language as a continuous variable, DLD simply represents the lower end of a normal distribution with regard to language ability, instead of a qualitatively distinct clinical group (Dollaghan, 2011; Leonard, 2014). And thus, findings of DLD studies not only provide clinical implications for language intervention, but they also inform us the neurobiological mechanisms underlying individual differences in language development and learning (Tomblin & Christensen, 2009).
The corticostriatal system, the corticocerebellar system, and the medial temporal region are not dedicated to language behaviors only, as they are also involved in memory function. Our current results suggest that language development and learning is associated with domain-general cognitive processes; however, it remains unclear about the causality. An intriguing question raised is that the DLD and the Comparison group do not appear to differ in the diffusivity measure at younger age. In other words, the discrepancy in language ability between individuals with and without DLD continues to exist across age, whereas the difference in the microstructure of the brain region of interest gradually emerges. One explanation is that structural alterations in the memory brain systems are driven by poor language ability along development. It is also possible that language and cognitive deficits in DLD are comorbid, developing in parallel. Another explanation is that the relationship between language and memory follows a nonlinear developmental trajectory. Thus, our current findings simply reflect the brain-behavior relationship in the tail of the developmental process. These hypotheses await testing, results of which will shed light on the role of domain-general cognitive ability in language development and learning.
5. Conclusion
In sum, individuals with DLD showed atypical age-related changes in the microstructure of white matter in the corticostriatal system, the corticocerebellar system, and the medial temporal region. These findings support and further specify the PDH, providing preliminary biological evidence of an impaired procedural memory system in DLD. At the same time, questions remain concerning the effect of altered white matter microstructure of the fornix on declarative memory functioning in DLD.
Despite significant findings, the study has several limitations. First, we used JHU brain atlas to label the main regions of interest. Thus, we were not able to specifically delineate the connections between different parts of the cortex and different parts of the basal ganglia. Seed-based tractography will be a necessary next step to parcellate different corticostriatal loops. Second, this is a cross-sectional study, and thus caution needs to be taken when interpreting the results pertaining to the trajectory of brain development. Longitudinal designs will be more appropriate to directly address this issue. Third, it is unclear whether these atypical FA findings could be accounted for by system-wide poor white matter integrity in the brain. Exploratory whole-brain analyses are needed to identify brain structures that are relatively intact and thus can serve as control regions for comparison. Last, no behavioral measures on procedural and declarative memory were included in the current study, and thus we were not able to further explore the brain-behavior association in DLD. Future studies are needed to examine the developmental relationship between memory performance and individual differences in language.
Highlights.
Atypical age-related change in WM structures in corticostriatal system in DLD
Atypical age-related change in WM structures in corticocerebellar system in DLD
Atypical age-related change in WM structures in medial temporal region in DLD
Importance of considering the age factor in research on DLD
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (NIDCD) [Grant R21DC013733]. We would like to thank the staff and students in the Child Language Research Center and the MACLab at the University of Iowa for their help with subject recruitment and data collection, as well as Eric Axelson in the Nopoulos Lab for his assistance in image preprocessing and processing. We also want to express our gratitude to our participants and their parents for agreeing to take part in this study.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Contributor Information
Joanna C. Lee, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242, United States
Peggy C. Nopoulos, Department of Psychiatry, The University of Iowa, The Roy J and Lucille A Carver College of Medicine, Iowa City, IA 52242, United States
J. Bruce Tomblin, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242, United States.
References
- Adamaszek M, & Kirkby KC (2015). Cerebellum and grammar processing In Marien P & Manto M (Eds.), the linguistic cerebellum (pp.81–105). Cambridge, Massachusetts: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Adams A, & Gathercole SE (2009). Limitations in working memory: Implications for language development. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 35(1), 95–116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alexander AL, Lee JE, Lazar M, & Field AS (2007). Diffusion tensor imaging of the brain. Neurotherapeutics, 4, 316–329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alexander GE, DeLong MR, & Strick PL (1986). Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 9, 357–381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anthony JL, Davis C, Williams JM, & Anthony TI (2014). Preschoolers’ oral language ability: A multilevel examination of dimensionality. Learning and Individual Differences, 35, 56–61. [Google Scholar]
- Archibald LMD, & Gathercole SE (2006). Short-term and working memory in specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 41(6), 675–693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asato M, Terwilliger R, Woo J, & Luna B (2010). White matter development in adolescence: A DTI study. Cerebral Cortex, 20(9), 2122–2131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Badcock NA, Bishop DVM, Hardiman MJ, Barry JG, & Watkins KE (2012). Co-localisation of abnormal brain structure and function in specific language impairment. Brain and Language, 120(3), 310–320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley A (2003). Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36(3), 189–208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnea-Goraly N, Menon V, Eckert M, Tamm L, Bammer R, Karchemskiy A, …Reiss AL (2005). White matter development during childhood and adolescence: A cross-sectional diffusion tensor imaging study. Cerebral Cortex, 15(12), 1848–1854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartha-Doering L, Novak A, Kollndorfer K, Kasprian G, Schuler A, Berl MM, …Seidl R (2018). When two are better than one: Bilateral mesial temporal lobe contributions associated with better vocabulary skills in children and adolescents. Brain and Language, 184, 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benjamini Y, & Hochberg Y (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B, 57(1), 289–300. [Google Scholar]
- Bishop DVM, & Hsu HJ (2015). The declarative system in children with specific language impairment: A comparison of meaningful and meaningless auditory-visual paired associative learning. BMC Psychology, 3(3), 1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop DVM, Snowling MJ, Thompson PA, Greenhalgh T, & CATALISE-2 consortium. (2017). Phase 2 of CATALISE: a multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12721 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bohsali A, & Crosson B (2016). The basal ganglia and language: A tale of two loops In Soghomonian JJ (Ed.), The Basal ganglia (pp. 217–242). Basel, Switzerland: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Bubb EJ, Metzler-Baddeley C, & Aggleton JP (2018). The cingulum bundle: Anatomy, function, and dysfunction. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 92, 104–127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bornstein MH, Hahn CS, Putnick DL, & Suwalsky JTD (2014). Stability of core language skill from early childhood to adolescence: A latent variable approach. Child Development, 85(4), 1346–1356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breitenstein C, Jansen A, Deppe M, Foerster AF, Sommer J, Wolbers T, & Knecht S (2005). Hippocampus activity differentiates good from poor learners of a novel lexicon. NeuroImage, 25(3), 958–968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan SH, Ryan L, & Bever TG (2013). Role of the striatum in language: Syntactic and conceptual sequencing. Brain and language, 125(3), 283–294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng P, Magnotta VA, Wu D, Nopoulos P, Moser DJ, Paulsen J, et al. (2006). Evaluation of the GTRACT diffusion tensor tractography algorithm: a validation and reliability study. NeuroImage, 31(3), 1075–1085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky N (2011). Language and other cognitive systems. What is special about language? Language Learning and Development, 7(4), 263–278. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen MD, & Bacdayan P (1994). Organizational routines are stored as procedural memory: Evidence from a laboratory study. Organization Science, 5(4), 554–568. [Google Scholar]
- Collisson BA, Grela B, Spaulding T, Rueckl JG, & Magnuson JS (2014). Individual differences in the shape bias in preschool children with specific language impairment and typical language development: Theoretical and clinical implications. Developmental Science, 18(3), 378–388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crosson B, Benefield H, Cato MA, Sadek JR, Moore AB, C E, …Briggs RW (2003). Left and right basal ganglia and frontal activity during language generation: contributions to lexical, semantic, and phonological processes. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9(7), 1061–1077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis MH, & Gaskell MG (2009). A complementary systems account of word learning: Neural and behavioral evidence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 364, 3773–3800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Renzi E, & Faglioni P (1978). Normative data and screening power of a shortened version of the Token Test. Cortex, 14, 41–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dollaghan CA (2011). Taxometric analyses of specific language impairment in 6year-old children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 54, 1361–1371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dominey PF, Inui T, & Hoen M (2009). Neural network processing of natural language: II. Towards a unified model of corticostriatal function in learning sentence comprehension and non-linguistic sequencing. Brain and Language, 109(2–3), 80–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duff MC, & Brown-Schmidt S (2012). The hippocampus and the flexible use and processing of language. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(69), 1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duff MC, Gupta R, Hengst J, Tranel D, & Cohen NJ (2011). The use of definite references signals declarative memory: Evidence from hippocampal amnesia. Psychological Science, 22, 666–673. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duff MC, Hengst JA, Tranel D, & Cohen NJ (2009). Hippocampal amnesia disrupts verbal play and the creative use of language in social interaction. Aphasiology, 23(7–8), 926–939. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duff MC, Hengst J, Tranel D, & Cohen NJ (2007). Talking across time: using reported speech as a communicative resource in amnesia. Aphasiology, 21, 1–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunn LM, & Dunn DM (2007). Peabody picture vocabulary test-fourth edition (PPVT-4). MN: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
- Eichenbaum H, & Cohen NJ (2004). From conditioning to conscious recollection: Memory systems of the brain. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Eichenbaum H, & Lipton PA (2008). Towards a functional organization of the medial temporal lobe memory system: Role of the parahippocampal and medial entorhinal cortical areas. Hippocampus, 18(12), 1314–1324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fidler LJ, Plante E, & Vance R (2011). Identification of adults with developmental language impairments. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 20, 2–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gabriel A, Maillart C, Guillaume M, Stefaniak N, & Meulemans T (2011). Exploration of serial structure procedural learning in children with language impairment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(2), 336–343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gabrieli JDE (1998). Cognitive neuroscience of human memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 87–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Habas C, & Cabanis EA (2007). Anatoimcal parcellation of the brainstem and cerebellar white matter: A preliminary probabilistic tractography study at 3T. Neuroradiology, 49(10), 849–863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammill DD, Brown VL, Larsen SC, Wiederholt JL (2007). Test of adolescent and adult language. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. [Google Scholar]
- Hardiman MJ, Hsu HJ, & Bishop D (2013). Children with specific language impairment are not impaired in the acquisition and retention of Pavlovian delay and trace conditioning of the eyeblink response. Brain and Language, 127(3), 428–439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hedenius M, Persson J, Tremblay A, Adi-Japha E, Veríssimo J, & Dye CD (2011). Grammar predicts procedural learning and consolidation deficits in children with Specific Language Impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(6), 2362–2375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henke K (2010). A model for memory systems based on processing modes rather than consciousness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 523–532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hua K, Zhang J, Wakana S, Jiang H, Li X, Reich DS, et al. (2008). Tract probability maps in stereotaxic spaces: analyses of white matter anatomy and tractspecific quantification. NeuroImage, 39(1), 336–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Idle JS, & Li CR (2011). A cerebellar thalamic cortical circuit for error-related cognitive control. NeuroImage, 54(1), 455–464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Janacsek K, Fiser J, Nemeth D (2012). The best time to acquire new skills: age-related differences in implicit sequence learning across the human lifespan. Developmental Science, 15, 496–505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jernigan T, Hesselink J, Sowell E, & Tallal PA (1991). Cerebral structure on magnetic resonance imaging in language- and learning-impaired children. Archives of Neurology, 48, 539–545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kidd E (2012). Implicit statistical learning is directly associated with the acquisition of syntax. Developmental Psychology, 48(1), 171–184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koziol LF, & Budding DE (2009). Subcortical structures and cognition. New York, NY: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Koziol LF, Budding D, Andreasen N, D’Arrigo S, Bulgheroni S, Imamizu H, et al. (2014). Consensus paper: the cerebellum’s role in movement and cognition. Cerebellum, 13(1), 151–188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuppuraj S, Rao P, & Bishop DVM (2016). Declarative capacity does not trade-off with procedural capacity in children with specific language impairment. Autism & Developmental Language Impairment, 1, 1–17. [Google Scholar]
- Language and Reading Research Consortium. (2015). The dimensionality of language asbility in young children. Child Development, 86(6), 1948–1965. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence AD, Sahakian BJ, & Robbins TW (1998). Cognitive functions and corticostriatal circuits: insights from Huntington’s disease. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 379–388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lebel C, Walker L, Leemans A, Phillips L, & Beaulieu C (2008). Microstructural maturation of the human brain from childhood to adulthood. NeuroImage, 40(3), 1044–1455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lebel C, Gee M, Camicioli R, Wieler M, Martin W, & Beaulieu C (2012). Diffusion tensor imaging of white matter tract evolution over the lifespan. NeuroImage, 60(1), 340–352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee JC (2018). Episodic memory retrieval in adolescents with and without developmental language disorders (DLD). International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 53(2), 271–281 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee JC, & Tomblin JB (2015). Procedural learning and individual differences in language. Language Learning and Development, 11(3), 215–236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee JC, Nopoulos PC, & Tomblin JB (2013). Abnormal subcortical components of the corticostriatal system in young adults with DLI: a combined structural MRI and DTI study. Neuropsychologia, 51(11), 2154–2161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lehericy S, Ducros M, van de Mooretele P, Francois C, Thivard L, Poupon C, … & Kim D (2004). Diffusion tensor fiber tracking shows distinct corticostriatal circuits in humans. Annals of Neurology, 55(4), 522–529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leitner Y, Travis KE, Ben-Shachar M, Yeom KW, & Feldman HM (2015). Tract profiles of the cerebellar white matter pathways in children and adolescents. The Cerebellum, 14(6), 613–623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leonard LB (2014). Children with specific language impairment and their contribution to the study of language development. Journal of Child Language, 41(S1), 38–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liegeois F, Mayes A, & Morgan A (2014). Neural correlates of developmental speech and language disorders: Evidence from Neuroimaging. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, 1, 215–227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu Z, Wang Y, Gerig G, Gouttard S, Tao R, Fletcher T, et al. (2010). Quality control of diffusion weighted images. Proceedings of SPIE, 11, 7628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukacs A, Kemeny F, Lum JAG, & Ullman MT (2017). Learning and overnight retention in declarative memory in specific language impairment. PLoS ONE 12(1), e0169474. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lum JAG, & Conti-Ramsden G (2013). Long-term memory: A review and meta-analysis of studies of declarative and procedural memory in specific language impairment. Topics in Language Disorders, 33(4), 282–297. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lum JAG, Conti-Ramsden G, Page D, & Ullman MT (2012). Working, declarative and procedural memory in specific language impairment. Cortex, 48, 1138–1154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lum JAG, Conti-Ramsden G, Morgan AT, & Ullman MT (2014). Procedural learning deficits in specific language impairment (SLI): A meta-analysis of serial reacdtion time task. Cortex, 51, 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lum JAG, Gelgec C, & Conti-Ramsden G (2010). Procedural and declarative memory in children with and without specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 45, 96–107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lum JA,G, Kidd E, Davis S, & Conti-Ramsden G (2010). Longitudinal study of declarative and procedural memory in primary school-aged children. Australian Journal of Psychology, 62(3), 139–148. [Google Scholar]
- Lum JAG, Ullman MT, & Conti-Ramsden G (2015). Verbal declarative memory impairments in specific language impairment are related to working memory deficits. Brain and Language, 142, 76–85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mabbott DJ, Rovet J, Noseworthy MD, Smith ML, & Rockel C (2009). The relations between white matter and declarative memory in older children and adolescents. Brain Research, 1294, 80–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magnotta VA, Harris G, Andreasen NC, O’Leary DS, Yuh WT, & Heckel D (2002). Structural MR image processing using the BRAINS2 toolbox. Computerized Medical Imaging and Graphics, 26(4), 251–264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magnotta VA, Heckel D, Andreasen NC, Cizadlo T, Corson PW, Ehrhardt JC, et al. (1999). Measurement of brain structures with artificial neural networks: two- and three-dimensional applications. Radiology, 211(3), 781–790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mariën P, Ackermann H, Adamaszek M, Barwood CHS, Beaton A, Desmond J, et al. (2014). Consensus paper: Language and the cerebellum: an ongoing enigma. Cerebellum, 13(3), 386–410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayes AK, Reilly S, & Morgan AT (2015). Neural correlates of childhood language disorder: A systematic review. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 57(8), 706–717. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moberget T, Gullesen EH, Andersson S, Ivry RB, & Endestad T (2014). Generalized role for the cerebellum in encoding internal models: Evidence from semantic processing. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(8), 2871–2878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morice R, & McNicol D (1985). The comprehension and production of complex syntax in schizophrenia. Cortex, 21, 567–580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naidich Th. P., Duvernoy HM, Delman BN, Sorensen AG, Kollias SS, & HGaacke EM (2009). Duvernoy’s atlas of the human brain stem and cerebellum. New York, NY: Springer Sciences + Business Media LLC. [Google Scholar]
- Nemeth D, Janacsek K, & Fiser J (2013). Age-dependent and coordinated shift in performance between implicit and explicit skill learning. Frontiers in computational neuroscience, 7, 147. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00147 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oguz I, Zhang H, Rumple A, & Sonka M (2014). RATS: Rapid automatic tissue segmentation in rodent brain MRI. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 221, 175–182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Packard MG, & Knowlton BJ (2002). Learning and memory functions of the basal ganglia. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 25, 563–593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poll GH, Miller CA, & van Hell JG (2015). Evidence of compensatory processing in adults with developmental language impairment: Testing the predictions of the procedural deficit hypothesis. Journal of Communication Disorders, 53, 84–102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reber AS (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: An essay on the cognitive unconscious. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Records NL, Tomblin JB, & Buckwalter PR (1995). Auditory verbal learning and memory in young adults with specific language impairment. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 9, 187–193. [Google Scholar]
- Riccio CA, Cash DL, & Cohen MJ (2007). Learning and memory performance of children with specific language impairment (SLI). Applied Neuropsychology, 14, 255–261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ronnlund M, Nyberg L, Backman L, & Nilsson L (2005). Stability, growth, and decline in adult life span development of declarative memory: Cross-sectional and longitudinal data from a population-based study. Psychology and Aging, 20, 3–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmahmann JD, & Pandya DN (2006). Fiber pathways of the brain. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Schmithorst VJ, Wilke M, Dardzinski BJ, & Holland SK (2002). Correlation of white matter diffusivity and anisotropy with age during childhood and adolescence: A cross-sectional diffusion-tensor MR imaging study. Radiology, 222, 212–218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seger CA (2006). The basal ganglia in human learning. Neuroscientist, 12(4), 285–290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Semel EM, Wiig EH, & Secord W (2003). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals, fourth edition (CELF-4). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Sepulcre J, Masdeu JC, Sastre-Garriga J, Goni J, Velez-de-Mendizabal N, Duque B, et al. (2008). Mapping the brain pathways of declarative verbal memory: Evidence from white matter lesions in the living human brain. NeuroImage, 42(3), 1237–1243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simmonds D, Hallquist MN, Asato M, & Luna B (2014). Developmental stages and sex differences of white matter and behavioral development through adolescence: A longitudinal diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) study. NeuroImage, 15(92), 356–368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Somerville LH & Casey BJ (2010). Developmental neurobiology of cognitive control and motivational systems. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 20, 236–241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soriano-Mas C, Pujol J, Ortiz H, Deus J, Lopez-Sala A, & Sans A (2008). Age-related brain structural alterations in children with specifi language impairment. Human Brain Mapping, 30(5), 1626–1636. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squire LR (1992). Declarative and nondeclarative memory: Multiple brain systems supporting learning and memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 232–243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squire LR, Stark CEL, & Clark RE (2004). The medial temporal lobe. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 279–306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinmetz AB, & Rice ML (2010). Cerebellar-dependent delay eyeblink conditioning in adolescents with Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorder, 2(4), 243–251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoodley CJ, & Schmahmann JD (2009). Functional topography in the human cerebellum: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. NeuroImage, 44(2), 489–501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamnes C, Østby Y, Fjell A, Westlye L, Due-Tønnessen P, & Walhovd K (2010). Brain maturation in adolescence and young adulthood: Regional age-related changes in cortical thickness and white matter volume and microstructure. Cerebral Cortex, 20(3), 534–548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tettamanti M, Moro A, Messa C, Moresco RM, Rizzo G, Carpinelli A, et al. (2005). Basal ganglia and language: Phonology modulates dopaminergic release. Neuroreport, 16(4), 397–401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas AG, Koumellis P, & Dineen RA (2011). The fornix in health and disease: An imaging review. RadioGraphics, 31, 1107–1121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas KM, Hunt RH, Vizueta N, Sommer T, Durston S, Yang Y, & Worden MS (2004). Evidence of developmental differences in implicit sequence learning: An fMRI study of children and adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(8), 1339–1351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Timmann D, & Daum I (2007). Cerebellar contributions to cognitive functions: A progress report after two decades of research. Cerebellum, 6(3), 159–162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomblin JB, & Christensen MH (2009). Explaining developmental communication disorders, in Paul R & Flipsen P Jr.(Eds.), Speech sound disorders in children: In honor of Lawrence D. Shriberg (pp.35–49). San Diego, CA: Plural Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Tomblin JB, Freese PR, & Records NL (1992). Diagnosing specific language impairment in adults for the purpose of pedigree analysis. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 832–844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomblin JB, Mainela-Arnold E, & Zhang X (2007). Procedural learning in adolescents with and without specific language impairment. Language Learning and Development, 3(4), 269–293. [Google Scholar]
- Tomblin JB, & Zhang X (2006). The dimensionality of language ability in school-age children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(6), 1193–1208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tulving E (1972). Episodic and semantic memory In Tulving E & Donaldson W (Eds.), Organization of Memory (pp. 381–403). New York: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Ullman MT (2001). A neurocognitive perspective on language: The declarative/procedural model. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 717–726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ullman MT (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92(1), 231–0270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ullman MT, Earle FS, Walenski M, & Janacsek K (2020). The neurocognition of developmental disorders of language. Annual Review of Psychology, 71, 389–417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ullman MT, & Pierpont EI (2005). Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex, 41, 399–433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ullman MT, & Pullman MY (2015). A compensatory role for declarative memory in neurodevelopmental disorders. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 51, 205–222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van der Lely HKJ (2005). Domain-specific cognitive systems: Insight from grammatical-SLI. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 53–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wakana S, Caprihan A, Panzenboeck MM, Fallon JH, Perry M, Gollub RL, et al. (2007). Reproducibility of quantitative tractography methods applied to cerebral white matter. NeuroImage, 36(3), 630–644. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Williams K (1997). Expressive vocabulary test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. [Google Scholar]
- Zwart FS, Vissers C. Th. W. M., Kessels RPC, & Maes JHR (2019). Procedural learning across the lifespan: A systematic review with implications for atypical development. Journal of Neuropsychology, 13, 149–182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]