Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2020 Apr 16;9:e53680. doi: 10.7554/eLife.53680

Primate homologs of mouse cortico-striatal circuits

Joshua Henk Balsters 1,2,†,, Valerio Zerbi 2,†,, Jerome Sallet 3, Nicole Wenderoth 2, Rogier B Mars 4,5
Editors: Kate M Wassum6, Thorsten Kahnt7
PMCID: PMC7162658  PMID: 32298231

Abstract

With the increasing necessity of animal models in biomedical research, there is a vital need to harmonise findings across species by establishing similarities and differences in rodent and primate neuroanatomy. Using connectivity fingerprint matching, we compared cortico-striatal circuits across humans, non-human primates, and mice using resting-state fMRI data in all species. Our results suggest that the connectivity patterns for the nucleus accumbens and cortico-striatal motor circuits (posterior/lateral putamen) were conserved across species, making them reliable targets for cross-species comparisons. However, a large number of human and macaque striatal voxels were not matched to any mouse cortico-striatal circuit (mouse->human: 85% unassigned; mouse->macaque 69% unassigned; macaque->human; 31% unassigned). These unassigned voxels were localised to the caudate nucleus and anterior putamen, overlapping with executive function and social/language regions of the striatum and connected to prefrontal-projecting cerebellar lobules and anterior prefrontal cortex, forming circuits that seem to be unique for non-human primates and humans.

Research organism: Human, Mouse, Rhesus macaque

Introduction

Animal models are currently providing crucial insights into neural structure, function, and disorders. According to Dietrich et al. (2014) mice are the most used mammalian species in scientific research. However, with this comes a growing necessity for translational, comparative neuroscience to harmonise these results with our understanding of structure, function, and disease in the human brain. To date, formal comparisons of brain organisation across species have largely focussed on humans and non-human primates, in spite of the steady increase in rodent models in neuroscience (Carlén, 2017; Ellenbroek and Youn, 2016). This is partly driven by the lack of research in the different species using the same methods, but also by a distinct lack of consensus in terminology between research in rodents and research in primates, which is prohibitive of clear translation of results (Laubach et al., 2018). Here, we address the growing need to formally identify common brain circuits between mice, macaques, and humans using the same technique to determine the scope and limits of mouse translational models.

One key issue that has hampered the comparison of rodent and primate brain organisation has been establishing suitable comparative measures (Preuss, 1995). For instance, the discussion of the putative existence of a rat homolog of human prefrontal cortex, different authors have proposed and dismissed a single connection to mediodorsal thalamus (Preuss, 1995; Rose and Woolsey, 1948), the presence of a granular layer IV (Preuss, 1995; Uylings et al., 2003), or equivalence of function (Dalley et al., 2004; Laubach et al., 2018; Narayanan et al., 2013) as diagnostics. These issues highlight the difficulties in understanding homologies across such distantly related species. Importantly, even if homology of areas is established, the homologous region will be embedded into a different large-scale network in the two species, which has consequences for the interpretation of translational results. Moreover, establishing similarity between anatomy and function is only useful if these two levels of description can be related. Ideally, we would want to understand what exactly is similar and different in the anatomical organisation of the different species’ brains and how this relates to their behavioural abilities.

One approach to comparative neuroscience that has been successful at establishing similarities and differences on a continuous scale is that of matching areas across species based on their so-called ‘connectivity fingerprint’. The term connectivity fingerprint was introduced by Passingham et al. (2002) to suggest that brain regions could be partitioned into functionally distinct brain areas based upon their unique set of connections, which in turn constrain the function of an area. For example, Passingham et al. (2002) showed that macaque premotor areas F3 and F5 possess unique connectivity fingerprints, which correspond to their unique functional responses based on electrophysiology (neural firing during memory guided vs visually guided tasks respectively). Whilst connectivity fingerprints were previously used as a way to distinguish between functionally distinct regions within one brain, Mars et al. (2016) proposed that connectivity fingerprint matching could be used as a tool to identify similar brain areas across species. As case in point, Neubert et al. (2014) systematically compared connectivity fingerprints from multiple regions in the human ventrolateral frontal cortex (vlFC) with connectivity fingerprints of regions defined in macaques. The results identified 11 vlFC brain regions with similar fingerprints in both species, and 1 brain region (lateral Frontal Pole; FPl) that was uniquely human. This approach has also been used to compare brain regions in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) (Sallet et al., 2013), medial PFC and orbitofrontal cortex (Neubert et al., 2015), parietal lobe (Mars et al., 2011), and temporoparietal cortex (Mars et al., 2013) in humans and non-human primates. However, to our knowledge, it has never been used to compare neuroanatomical connectivity patterns across humans, non-human primates, and rodents.

This approach is now feasible due to the availability of the same type of neuroimaging data from humans, macaques, and mice. Although tracer-based connectivity mapping is often considered to be the ‘gold standard’ for comparative neuroscience, these methods are too expensive and labour intensive to use in most species and for the entire brain (Mars et al., 2014). The substantial investment of both time and money often means that it is only possible to investigate a small number of subjects which reduces the power of statistical comparisons and the ability to make global inferences. Also, these invasive approaches are far less common in humans, making it difficult to harmonise findings across species using a common methodology. Resting state fMRI (rsfMRI) is increasingly employed as a non-invasive tool to measure connectivity in humans and non-human primates (Neubert et al., 2015; Schaeffer et al., 2018; Vincent et al., 2007) and the availability of mouse resting state data (Grandjean et al., 2020; Zerbi et al., 2015) invites an extension of this work to larger-scale between-species comparisons. The increasing application of rsfMRI across species is likely because of the high degree of consistency between connectivity profiles derived from tracers and connectivity derived using rsfMRI (Grandjean et al., 2017). In addition, rsfMRI repositories are making large numbers of datasets freely available making it possible to conduct analyses using a common methodology across species with samples sizes that are orders of magnitude larger than traditional tracer-based approaches (Milham et al., 2018).

Here, we will use connectivity fingerprint matching to compare cortico-striatal circuits in humans, macaques, and mice. The general architecture of cortical-striatal circuits, with partially separated loops connecting distinct parts of the striatum with distinct parts of the neocortex, seems well preserved across mammals (Haber, 2016; Heilbronner et al., 2016). However, the specific implementation can be expected to differ when the cortex has expanded in particular lineages, including the human (Murray et al., 2016). These circuits are particularly affected by a number of psychiatric conditions (Bradshaw and Enticott, 2001), some of which have been the target of recent mouse models (Zerbi et al., 2018). As such, cortico-striatal circuits are an ideal target to assess the feasibility of mouse-macaque-human translational studies.

Results

Establishing cortico-striatal connectivity fingerprints in mice

The connectivity fingerprinting approach requires extracting a rsfMRI timeseries from a seed region and comparing that with rsfMRI timeseries from a collection of target regions. The strength of the correlation between seed and target timeseries will be an indication of the strength of connectivity between those two regions, and the variability in connectivity across targets will produce the connectivity fingerprint. Figure 1a provides a schematic illustrating how mouse striatal seed regions were created using an independent tracer-based connectivity dataset from the Allen Institute (Oh et al., 2014). The strength of tracer connectivity from 68 cortical injection sites was established for each voxel in the mouse striatum and a hierarchical clustering algorithm was used to cluster voxels with similar and distinct connectivity patterns – an approach commonly referred to as connectivity-based parcellation (Balsters et al., 2018; Balsters et al., 2016; Eickhoff et al., 2015). Figure 1b shows the three clusters in the mouse striatum with unique connectivity patterns based on tracer data; Nucleus Accumbens (NAcc), medial caudoputamen (CP.m), and lateral caudoputamen (CP.l) and their functional connectivity maps.

Figure 1. Images illustrating the creation of mouse striatal seeds, their connectivity, and connectivity fingerprint matching.

(A) Schematic illustrating how mouse striatal seeds were created using tracer connectivity data from the Allen Institute. A connectivity matrix was extracted describing the volume of terminal label in each striatal voxel from 68 cortical injection sites. Correlating the values in this matrix established the similarity of connectivity fingerprints across striatal voxels. Hierarchical clustering was used to sort the correlation matrix into voxels with similar connectivity patterns (lower right dendrogram) and silhouette value was used to establish the number of clusters where within-cluster similarity and between-cluster differences were greatest (lower left plot). The arrow shows that the best solution according to silhouette value was a three cluster solution. (B) RsfMRI voxelwise connectivity maps for the three striatal seeds. (C) A schematic illustrating the process of extracted connectivity fingerprints for the three mouse striatal seeds. Connectivity fingerprints show the strength of connectivity (correlation of rsfMRI timeseries) between each striatal seed region and target regions outside the striatum D) In humans (or macaques), connectivity fingerprints were extracted from each voxel of the striatum - comparing the connectivity strength of striatal voxels with human homologs of the five target regions identified in mice. The similarity of each human voxel fingerprint can then be compared against each of the three mouse striatal fingerprints.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Figure showing the overlap between tracer-based and rsfMRI parcellations of the mouse striatum.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) The 33 cluster segmentation of Chon et al. (2019) from two representative slices. (B) Clusters from Chon et al. (2019) showed highly similar connectivity patterns and clustered into four segments (shown in solid colours). The outline of the tracer-based parcellation is overlayed using the same colour scheme as Figure 1. (C) Similarity matrix showing the spatial correlation between tracer-based and rsfMRI-based striatal clusters and dendrogram showing dice similarity clustering. These highlight the consistency between tracer-based and rsfMRI-based clustering methods.

As a complementary approach to establishing mouse striatal seeds, we transformed the tracer-based striatal parcellation of Chon et al. (2019) into our MRI reference space and created voxelwise functional connectivity maps for each of the 33 seeds. Using the same approach as above, we applied hierarchical clustering to assess the independence of the 33 functional connectivity maps. The 33 striatal seeds showed a high degree of overlap (i.e. similar functional connectivity patterns) and were best characterized as four groups of seeds with distinct connectivity patterns (see Figure 1—figure supplement 1). Dice similarity showed that three of the four clusters were a match to the NAcc, CP.m, and CP.l previously defined using the voxelwise tracer data. One additional cluster was present in this approach within the CP tail; however, further analysis revealed that this cluster fell entirely outside of the striatal mask used in the previous analysis and included voxels in the bordering lateral amygdala nucleus. Given spatial smoothing involved in fMRI analysis, it is commonplace to remove voxels that border adjacent structures to avoid signal contamination which is known to have a significant effect on network detection (Smith et al., 2011). We suggest that this CP.tail segment is likely to be distinct because of signal contamination which is additionally problematic as the basolateral amygdala is one of our targets. Given the high consistency between both approaches, we will continue the analyses using the independent tracer-based seeds.

After creating mouse striatal seeds, we selected 12 target regions across cortical and subcortical regions of the mouse brain. These regions were chosen as they are believed to be homologous across species based on existing literature (see methods and Supplementary file 1). We next compared connectivity fingerprints from the three striatal seed regions in an independent mouse rsfMRI dataset, extracting rsfMRI timeseries from the three striatal seeds and correlating these with timeseries extracted from the twelve target regions (Figure 2). Analysis of the connectivity fingerprints (permutation testing of Manhattan distance – see Materials and methods) demonstrated that these three fingerprints were all significantly different from one another (CP.m vs NAcc: Distance = 2.57, p<0.001; CP.m vs CP.l: Distance = 6.67, p<0.001; NAcc vs CP.l: Distance = 6.15, p<0.001). This confirms that we can extract unique connectivity patterns in the mouse striatum and thus have isolated suitable seeds and targets for testing similarities and differences in connectivity fingerprints across species. Voxel-wise correlation maps for these three seeds can also be seen in Supplementary file 1.

Figure 2. Brain images show unthresholded striatal t-maps of mouse-human similarity for CP.m, NAcc, and CP.l.

Red-yellow voxels indicate increasingly positive correlations of connectivity fingerprints across species, blue-cyan voxels indicate increasing negative correlations of connectivity fingerprints across species. Black outlines indicate voxels that showed statistically significant similarity (TFCE p<0.05). Human and mouse connectivity fingerprints are shown underneath each brain image to highlight similarity in the connectivity pattern across species. Shaded error bars show the standard error of the mean. The data ranges from Z-values −0.1–0.4 and the thick grey circle shows 0.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Images show axial slices through the human striatum including the unthresholded t-maps highlighting the similarity in mouse-human connectivity fingerprints for CP.m.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

The red-yellow/blue-cyan colourmap shows the degree of positive/negative similarity between mouse seed regions and voxels in the human striatum. Black outlines highlight the voxels that show significant similarity (TFCE p<0.05). These images can be explored in Neurovault at https://neurovault.org/collections/NFGTNVFX/.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Images show axial slices through the human striatum including the unthresholded t-maps highlighting the similarity in mouse-human connectivity fingerprints for NAcc.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

The red-yellow/blue-cyan colourmap shows the degree of positive/negative similarity between mouse seed regions and voxels in the human striatum. Black outlines highlight the voxels that show significant similarity (TFCE p<0.05). These images can be explored in Neurovault at https://neurovault.org/collections/NFGTNVFX/.
Figure 2—figure supplement 3. Images show axial slices through the human striatum including the unthresholded t-maps highlighting the similarity in mouse-human connectivity fingerprints for CP.l.

Figure 2—figure supplement 3.

The red-yellow/blue-cyan colourmap shows the degree of positive/negative similarity between mouse seed regions and voxels in the human striatum. Black outlines highlight the voxels that show significant similarity (TFCE p<0.05). These images can be explored in Neurovault at https://neurovault.org/collections/NFGTNVFX/.
Figure 2—figure supplement 4. Brain images show unthresholded striatal t-maps of mouse-human similarity for CP.m, NAcc, and CP.l.

Figure 2—figure supplement 4.

Black outlines indicate voxels that showed statistically significant similarity (TFCE p<0.05). The unassigned voxels are a binary mask indicating voxels that did not met the criteria for statistically significance for mCP, NAcc, and lCP. Plots underneath each brain image show the anatomical and functional localisation of assigned and unassigned voxels. To the right of the colour table, the mean probability for each area at the location of a functional activation is divided by the overall mean probability of the respective area. This quotient indicates how much more likely this area was observed in the functionally defined volume as could be expected if the probabilities at that location would follow their overall distribution. Values greater than 1 indicate that a brain indicates a rather central location of the activation with respect to this area whereas values less than 1 indicate a more peripheral one.

Mouse to human comparison

These three mouse striatal seeds are commonly considered to reflect associative (CP.m), limbic (NAcc), and motor circuits (CP.l) (Freeze et al., 2013; Gunaydin et al., 2014; Kiyokawa et al., 2012). As such, we predicted that corresponding regions in the human striatum will exist within the human caudate nucleus, NAcc, and posterior putamen respectively. To test this, we extracted connectivity fingerprints for each voxel in the human striatum (defined as Harvard-Oxford subcortical atlas >33% threshold Caudate Nucleus, NAcc, and Putamen) and statistically compared each voxel’s connectivity fingerprint with the three mouse connectivity fingerprints (Figure 1). This approach produced three voxelwise maps (Fishers r-to-z transformed maps) for each participant (one for each mouse fingerprint) illustrating voxelwise similarity with each mouse cortico-striatal circuit. These maps were input into a GLM (one sample t-test) for permutation testing and correction for multiple comparisons (TFCE p<0.05). All the unthresholded statistical maps in the section can be viewed at https://neurovault.org/collections/NFGTNVFX/. Voxels within the human NAcc possessed a statistically similar connectivity fingerprint with both the mouse NAcc and CP.m, whereas connectivity fingerprints within human posterior putamen voxels were statistically similar to the mouse CP.l (see Figure 2). Figure 2 also highlights that even though there was an overlap in statistically similar voxels for mouse NAcc and CP.m in the human NAcc, there were clear subthreshold differences in the human caudate and putamen (positive correlations with CP.m but not NAcc) that distinguish these two striatal seeds. Using the task-based parcellation of Pauli et al. (2016), we were able to assess the functional roles of the assigned human voxels. This showed that the human striatal voxels assigned to CP.m and NAcc were best localised to regions of the striatum processing stimulus value, whereas the human striatal voxels assigned to CP.l were localised to striatal regions contributing to motor control (see Figure 2—figure supplement 4).

Whilst this approach identified common striatal regions in humans and mice, there were a high number of voxels in the human striatum (85%) that did not show a significant correspondence to any of the three striatal connectivity fingerprints from the mouse – we refer to these as unassigned voxels. Unassigned voxels accounted for 85% of the caudate nucleus volume, 77% of the putamen volume, and 5% of the NAcc volume. Functionally, the unassigned voxels were localised to striatal regions associated with executive function, social/language, and action value (Pauli et al., 2016). These unassigned voxels could reflect the expansion of the prefrontal cortex in primates.

To further establish the cortical-striatal connectivity of the human regions identified above, we next used the resulting similarity t-maps to extract weighted timeseries and correlated them with the connectivity pattern of each voxel in the rest of the brain (Figure 3). Human CP.m voxels showed significant connectivity with frontal pole regions (Area 47 m), medial (RCZa, Area 23) and lateral prefrontal cortex (Area 46), anterior insula, supramarginal gyrus (hIP2), occipital pole (V1), cerebellar lobules HVI/Crus I and VIIIb, and hippocampus. The human homologue of NAcc also showed significant connectivity with anterior and middle cingulate cortex (Area 32 dl and area 32 p), occipital pole (V1), and cerebellar lobules Crus I and IX. The human homologue of CP.l showed significant connectivity with the middle frontal gyrus (Area 9/46), precentral gyrus (areas 6 and 4 p), SMA, Rolandic operculum (OP2), supramarginal gyrus (PFm), superior parietal lobule (area 7), precuneus (area 5), fusiform gyrus (FG2), and cerebellar lobule HVI. Full results tables are included in Supplementary file 1.

Figure 3. Unthresholded whole-brain connectivity maps showing regions interconnected with human homologs of CP.m, NAcc, CP.l, and unassigned voxels.

Figure 3.

The bottom row shows cerebellar activations on a flattened representation of the cerebellum and dotted black lines show the lobular boundaries (Diedrichsen and Zotow, 2015). The far-right column shows a thresholded conjunction analysis of voxels that possess significantly greater connectivity with unassigned voxels compared against all three mouse seeds. Outlines from the Yeo cortical parcellation highlight that the significantly different voxels in humans are principally in regions identified as the frontal -parietal network and the cerebellum.

In order to highlight significant differences between humans and mice (rather than relying the absence of a significant effect), we performed a conjunction analysis between unassigned whole-brain connectivity maps and whole-brain connectivity maps from the three mouse seeds (see Figure 3). This analysis identified voxels in the human brain that showed significantly greater connectivity with unassigned voxels compared to all three human-mouse seeds independently (i.e. unassigned >CP .m and unassigned >NAcc and unassigned >CP .l). This analysis revealed striatal connectivity with the frontal pole (FPl and Area 46) that was unique to humans. It also revealed structures known for their connections with the prefrontal cortex: mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus, and cerebellar lobules Crus I and Crus II. Finally, this analysis also refined the localisation of unassigned voxels within the human striatum. Whilst 85% of the human striatal voxels were not statistically similar to any of the three mouse cortico-striatal fingerprints, only 25.67% of those voxels were significantly different. These were localised to two distinct striatal subregions; the first in the rostral caudate nucleus, the second in the anterior portion of the putamen. Both of these striatal regions are associated with executive functions and language processes. Figure 3 shows that these unique cortico-striatal connections in humans also fall principally within the boundaries of the fronto-parietal network (FPN) as defined by Yeo et al. (2011).

Mouse to macaque comparison

We next applied the same procedure to compare cortico-striatal connectivity fingerprints in mice with cortico-striatal connectivity fingerprints in the macaque. Both species underwent similar rs-fMRI protocols including light sedation using anaesthesia, and as such this comparison allows us to account for the potential effects of anaesthesia on rs-fMRI connectivity. We hypothesised that a similar number of voxels will be unassigned in this comparison given that, from an evolutionary perspective rodents and primates diverged around 89 M years ago (Cao et al., 2000).

For each voxel in the macaque striatum (defined as caudate nucleus, NAcc, and putamen maps from the INIA19 macaque atlas; Rohlfing et al., 2012), we extracted a connectivity fingerprint using the same twelve targets. Figure 4 shows that once again, we found regions of significant similarity, specifically the CP.m and NAcc showed significant similarity with voxels in the macaque NAcc, caudate head and caudate tail. The CP.l showed significant overlap with posterior regions of the putamen. This left 69% of voxels in the macaque striatum unassigned, that is, that did not show significant similarity with any mouse connectivity fingerprints. Unassigned voxels accounted for 79% of the caudate nucleus volume, 62% of the putamen volume, and 9% of the NAcc volume.

Figure 4. Brain images show unthresholded striatal t-maps of mouse-macaque similarity for CP.m, NAcc, and CP.l.

Red-yellow voxels indicate increasingly positive correlations of connectivity fingerprints across species, blue-cyan voxels indicate increasing negative correlations of connectivity fingerprints across species. Black outlines indicate voxels that showed statistically significant similarity (TFCE p<0.05). Macaque and mouse connectivity fingerprints are shown underneath each brain image to highlight the similarity in the connectivity pattern across species. Shaded error bars show the standard error of the mean. The data ranges from Z-values −0.1 to 0.4 and the thick grey circle shows 0.

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Images show axial slices through the macaque striatum including the unthresholded t-maps highlighting the similarity in mouse-macaque connectivity fingerprints for CP.m.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

The red-yellow/blue-cyan colourmap shows the degree of positive/negative similarity between mouse seed regions and voxels in the macaque striatum. Black outlines highlight the voxels that show significant similarity (TFCE p<0.05).
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Images show axial slices through the macaque striatum including the unthresholded t-maps highlighting the similarity in mouse-macaque connectivity fingerprints for NAcc.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

The red-yellow/blue-cyan colourmap shows the degree of positive/negative similarity between mouse seed regions and voxels in the macaque striatum. Black outlines highlight the voxels that show significant similarity (TFCE p<0.05).
Figure 4—figure supplement 3. Images show axial slices through the macaque striatum including the unthresholded t-maps highlighting the similarity in mouse-macaque connectivity fingerprints for CP.l.

Figure 4—figure supplement 3.

The red-yellow/blue-cyan colourmap shows the degree of positive/negative similarity between mouse seed regions and voxels in the macaque striatum. Black outlines highlight the voxels that show significant similarity (TFCE p<0.05).

Figure 5 displays regions showing significant connectivity with CP.m, NAcc, and CP.l voxels. CP.m voxels showed significant connectivity with the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (Areas 9 m), premotor cortex (F2, F5), posterior lateral prefrontal cortex (Area 45b), anterior cingulate cortex (Area 24), posterior cingulate cortex (Area 23b), intraparietal sulcus (LIPd), cortex of the superior temporal sulcus and visual areas (V1 and V2). Significant connectivity with subcortical structures included the amygdala and caudate nucleus. Regions showing significant connectivity with NAcc voxels included anterior cingulate cortex (Area 24), premotor cortex (F4 and F5), posterior lateral prefrontal cortex (area 44), cortex of the superior temporal sulcus, and visual cortex (V2). Subcortical connectivity included the macaque NAcc, amygdala, and hippocampus. Regions showing significant connectivity with CP.l voxels included the premotor cortex (F2, F5), anterior cingulate cortex (area 24) and posterior cingulate cortex (Areas 23 and 31), somatosensory cortex on the posterior bank of central sulcus, and Intraparietal sulcus (LIP). Subcortical regions included the putamen (largest activation) and the Caudate nucleus.

Figure 5. Unthresholded whole-brain connectivity maps showing regions interconnected with CP.m, NAcc, CP.l, and unassigned voxels.

Figure 5.

The bottom row shows cerebellar activations. The far-right column shows a thresholded conjunction analysis of voxels that possess significantly greater connectivity with unassigned voxels compared against all three mouse seeds.

The conjunction analysis (i.e. unassigned >CP .m and unassigned >NAcc and unassigned >CP .l) highlighted regions of significantly different in macaques compared to mice. These included dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Area 46d), premotor cortex (F2), anterior insula, cortex of the superior temporal sulcus, inferior parietal lobule (Area 7A), and visual cortex (V1). Subcortical differences were seen in both the rostral caudate nucleus and putamen as in humans. The application of the conjunction analysis showed that only 20% of striatal unassigned voxels were significantly different from all three mouse seeds. These significantly different voxels accounted for 34% of the caudate nucleus, 10% of the putamen, and 1% of NAcc volume (four voxels), suggesting that differences between macaques and mice were largely driven by differences in the caudate nucleus - as in the previous mouse-human analyses. Full results tables are included in Supplementary file 1.

Macaque to human comparisons

In order to determine whether some striatal connectivity features of the unassigned voxels could be reflecting uniquely human specialisations, we applied the same protocols to compare striatal connectivity fingerprints in humans and macaques. Similar to what was done for the mouse, macaque caudate body, NAcc, and putamen were defined as seeds using connectivity-based parcellation (see Materials and methods). We used two target models; 1) the original targets used in the mouse model and 2) an extended model that included additional regions in the lateral and medial PFC that have been shown previously to have homologous connectivity fingerprints across macaques and humans (Neubert et al., 2014; Sallet et al., 2013). This included area 9/46d, area 9/46 v, Area 44, area FPm, and SMA. Connectivity fingerprints for all three seeds were independent in both the mouse model (Caudate vs NAcc: Distance = 3.51, p<0.001; Caudate vs Putamen: Distance = 3.62, p<0.001; NAcc vs Putamen: Distance = 4.96, p<0.001) and the extended model (Caudate vs NAcc: Distance = 4.77, p<0.001; Caudate vs Putamen: Distance = 5.32, p<0.001; NAcc vs Putamen: Distance = 6.12, p<0.001).

For each voxel of the human Caudate Nucleus, NAcc, and Putamen (based on Harvard Oxford subcortical atlas >33% threshold), we extracted the human connectivity fingerprint and compared it to connectivity fingerprints for the macaque caudate body, NAcc, and putamen. Unsurprisingly, the human-macaque comparisons were more closely aligned then the mouse-human comparisons. Specifically, the human voxels in the caudate nucleus were significantly similar to the macaque caudate body, human voxels in the NAcc were significantly similar to the macaque NAcc, and human voxels in the putamen were significantly similar to the macaque putamen (see Figure 6). Using the task-based parcellation of Pauli et al. (2016), the human voxels assigned to the macaque caudate body overlapped with striatal regions dedicated to executive function, social/language processes, and action value. The human voxels assigned to the macaque NAcc overlapped with striatal regions dedicated to stimulus value processing. The human voxels assigned to the macaque putamen best overlapped with striatal motor control regions (see Figure 6—figure supplement 4).

Figure 6. Brain images show unthresholded striatal t-maps of macaque-human similarity for caudate body, NAcc, and putamen.

Red-yellow voxels indicate increasingly positive correlations of connectivity fingerprints across species, blue-cyan voxels indicate increasing negative correlations of connectivity fingerprints across species. Black outlines indicate voxels that showed statistically significant similarity (TFCE p<0.05). Human and macaque connectivity fingerprints are shown underneath each brain image to highlight the similarity in connectivity pattern across species. Shaded error bars show the standard error of the mean. The data ranges from Z-values −0.25–0.4 and the thick grey circle shows 0.

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Images show axial slices through the human striatum including the unthresholded t-maps highlighting the similarity in macaque-human connectivity fingerprints for caudate body.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

The red-yellow/blue-cyan colourmap shows the degree of positive/negative similarity between mouse seed regions and voxels in the human striatum. Black outlines highlight the voxels that show significant similarity (TFCE p<0.05). These images can be explored in Neurovault at https://neurovault.org/collections/NFGTNVFX/.
Figure 6—figure supplement 2. Images show axial slices through the human striatum including the unthresholded t-maps highlighting the similarity in macaque-human connectivity fingerprints for NAcc.

Figure 6—figure supplement 2.

The red-yellow/blue-cyan colourmap shows the degree of positive/negative similarity between mouse seed regions and voxels in the human striatum. Black outlines highlight the voxels that show significant similarity (TFCE p<0.05). These images can be explored in Neurovault at https://neurovault.org/collections/NFGTNVFX/.
Figure 6—figure supplement 3. Images show axial slices through the human striatum including the unthresholded t-maps highlighting the similarity in macaque-human connectivity fingerprints for putamen.

Figure 6—figure supplement 3.

The red-yellow/blue-cyan colourmap shows the degree of positive/negative similarity between mouse seed regions and voxels in the human striatum. Black outlines highlight the voxels that show significant similarity (TFCE p<0.05). These images can be explored in Neurovault at https://neurovault.org/collections/NFGTNVFX/.
Figure 6—figure supplement 4. Brain images with unthresholded striatal t-maps showing macaque-human similarity for Caudate Nucleus, NAcc, and Putamen.

Figure 6—figure supplement 4.

Black outlines indicate voxels that showed statistically significant similarity (TFCE p<0.05). The unassigned voxels are a binary mask indicating voxels that did not met the criteria for statistically significance for any of the trhee macaque seeds. Plots underneath each brain image show the anatomical and functional localisation of assigned and unassigned voxels. To the right of the colour table, the mean probability for each area at the location of a functional activation is divided by the overall mean probability of the respective area. This quotient indicates how much more likely this area was observed in the functionally defined volume as could be expected if the probabilities at that location would follow their overall distribution. Values greater than 1 indicate that a brain indicates a rather central location of the activation with respect to this area, whereas values less than 1 indicate a more peripheral one.

The human-macaque comparison produced fewer unassigned voxels than the human-mouse comparison, with only 31% of striatal voxels unassigned using the same model used in mice, and 20% unassigned using the extended model. These voxels were localised anatomically to the NAcc (30% of NAcc voxels unassigned using the reduced mouse model, 24% using the extended model) and Caudate (35% of caudate voxels unassigned using the reduced mouse model, 23% using the extended model), and showed functional association with striatal regions associated with executive functions. Although 30/23% of unassigned voxels were localised in the NAcc, the conjunction analysis isolating significant differences between species showed that only voxels in the caudate nucleus were significantly different between species.

As previously, we extracted weighted timeseries from each similarity t-map to investigate voxelwise connectivity (Figure 7). The human-macaque caudate nucleus maps showed significant connectivity with mid cingulate cortex (RCZa, CCZ) and middle (Area 46) and inferior frontal gyri (pars triangularis), inferior parietal lobule (hIP3), precuneus (Area 7 p), occipital pole (V1 and V3), cerebellar lobules HVI and Crus II. Human-macaque NAcc maps showed significant connectivity with the anterior cingulate cortex (area 32pl) and cerebellar lobule Crus IX. Human-macaque putamen maps showed significant connectivity with regions in the mid cingulate cortex (RCZa) and lateral prefrontal cortex (area 46) and premotor cortex (area 6), supramarginal gyrus (area PF), middle temporal gyrus, occipital lobe (V3), and cerebellar lobule HVI.

Figure 7. Unthresholded whole-brain connectivity maps showing regions interconnected with caudate body, NAcc, putamen, and unassigned voxels.

Figure 7.

The bottom row shows cerebellar activations on a flattened representation of the cerebellum and dotted black lines show the lobular boundaries (Diedrichsen and Zotow, 2015). The far-right column shows a thresholded conjunction analysis of voxels that possess significantly greater connectivity with unassigned voxels compared against all three mouse seeds. Outlines from the Yeo cortical parcellation highlight that the significantly different voxels in humans are principally in regions identified as the frontal -parietal network and the cerebellum.

When comparing connectivity differences between assigned vs unassigned voxels using the conjunction analysis, the unassigned voxels showed significant connectivity with frontal pole (FPl), precuneus, occipital pole (V1), and subcortical structures known for projecting to the prefrontal cortex (the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus and the cerebellar lobule Crus I). Coupling with the FPl could reflect the expansion of the lateral frontal pole since the last common ancestor to humans and macaques (Neubert et al., 2014). The number of unassigned voxels was similar to the number of voxels that were significantly different (31% unassigned voxels but 18.1% of them were significantly different to our three fingerprints). Although unassigned voxels were localised to both the NAcc and caudate nucleus, only voxels in the caudate nucleus were significantly different in humans compared to macaques. These have been localised to regions of caudate connected with the FPN that process executive functions and action value (Choi et al., 2012; Pauli et al., 2016). As with mice, this unique cluster of voxels was co-localised with FPN as defined by Yeo et al. (2011). All the statistical maps reported in the section can be viewed at https://neurovault.org/collections/NFGTNVFX/.

Discussion

As the use of mouse models for studying function and disease rapidly increases in neuroscience, it is crucial to develop methods that can harmonise results across species. Here, for the first time, we have used rsfMRI as a common methodology to establish similarities and differences in striatal-cortical organisation in humans, macaques, and mice. Using our connectivity fingerprint matching approach, we could identify NAcc consistently across species making it a reliable target for translational neuroscience. Although portions of the caudate nucleus and putamen in both humans and macaques showed similar connectivity fingerprints with the mouse, there were also large regions of the human and macaque striatum that appeared to be unique and unassigned. Regions of significant difference across species appear to be mostly localised to the anterior putamen and caudate body. In both human-mouse and human-macaque comparisons, unassigned voxels showed significantly greater connectivity with the lateral frontal pole (Area 46 in mice and FPl in both mice and macaques) and prefrontal-projecting subcortical structures including the dorsomedial nucleus of the thalamus and cerebellar lobules Crus I and Crus II.

Striatum

Consistent with previous parcellations of the mouse striatum, our connectivity based parcellation using Allen tracer data identified three regions with unique connectivity fingerprints; the NAcc, medial and lateral caudoputamen (CP.m and CP.l). Previous studies have suggested that these regions map on to the distinct functional domains of limbic, association, and sensorimotor, respectively (Balleine et al., 2009; Hunnicutt et al., 2016; Thorn et al., 2010; Yin and Knowlton, 2006). Our results suggest that the connectivity fingerprint of the NAcc is highly conserved across mice and primates. Human and macaque striatal voxels assigned to the mouse NAcc, and the human voxels assigned to the macaque NAcc, were all discretely localised within the boundaries of the human NAcc as defined by the Harvard-Oxford subcortical atlas and INIA atlas in primates (see Figure 8). This is consistent with tracer studies by Mailly et al. (2013) and Heilbronner et al. (2016) who also demonstrated an overlap in NAcc connectivity fingerprints across rats and non-human primates. This conserved brain network could underpin their similar functional role in motivation and reinforcement learning. For example, lesions to the rat NAcc have been shown to alter performance in value-based decision-making paradigms such as delayed discounting tasks (Cardinal et al., 2001). Similarly, non-invasive imaging studies in humans have linked individual variability in NAcc activity to individual variability in delayed discounting preferences, mirroring what has been shown in rats (Hariri et al., 2006). Aberrant performance on value-based decision-making paradigms, as well as altered NAcc activity, has been proposed as a hallmark for a number of psychiatric conditions including Obsessive Compulsive Disorder, addiction, schizophrenia and others (Gunaydin and Kreitzer, 2016). Given the high degree of conservation across species in NAcc connectivity, we would suggest that the NAcc is a reliable translational target for mouse models of psychiatric conditions.

Figure 8. Brain images showing the spatial overlap of the NAcc across species.

Figure 8.

Voxels highlight regions of statistically significant similarity across species (TFCE p<0.05). The light green outline in each image shows the anatomical boundaries of the human and macaque NAcc.

The CP.m and CP.l are believed to contribute to associative and sensorimotor motor processes respectively and would thus be expected to correspond to regions of the caudate and putamen with similar functional properties (Balleine et al., 2009; Yin and Knowlton, 2006). Our results showed that voxels in the posterior segment of the putamen in both humans and macaques shared a significantly similar connectivity fingerprint with the mouse CP.l (see Figures 2 and 4), suggesting that sensorimotor cortico-striatal circuits could also be comparable across species. This is crucial for translational models of sensorimotor deficits such as Parkinson’s disease. However, 85% of voxels in the human striatum failed to show significant similarity with any of the three mouse striatal seeds. These unassigned voxels were localised to the caudate nucleus and putamen. The conjunction analysis highlighted voxels that were significantly different in humans compared to mice (as opposed to voxels that failed to reach the statistical significance threshold). This approach confirmed that 25% of voxels in human striatum – specifically the anterior portion of the putamen and caudate body - possess significantly different connectivity fingerprints in humans compared to mice. These regions of the striatum have been shown to receive projections from the lateral prefrontal cortex in both humans and non-human primates (Alexander et al., 1986; Choi et al., 2017; Verstynen et al., 2012), and our comparison with task-based parcellations of the striatum suggest that these regions process action value, executive functions, and social and language processes in a meta-analysis of over 10,000 human fMRI studies (Pauli et al., 2016). Although these results could reflect the effects of anaesthesia on rs-fMRI in mice, we believe this is unlikely given that 1) significant similarities were found between humans and mice for limbic (NAcc) and sensorimotor (CP.l) cortico-striatal networks, and 2) rs-fMRI data collected from macaques used a similar anaesthesia protocol to that of mice and yet the macaque caudate nucleus showed significant similarity with most voxels in the human caudate nucleus (Figure 6). Rather, these results are likely to reflect evolutionary pressures acting upon brain circuits underpinning cognitive behaviours (Passingham and Wise, 2012). It has been argued that evolution has led to increased cognitive development particularly in the primate and human lineages, whereas human motor and appetitive behaviours are more conserved across species (Murray et al., 2016). As such, one would predict that limbic (NAcc) and sensorimotor (CP.l) cortico-striatal circuits would be largely conserved, whereas associative cortico-striatal circuits would have changed most across species. We suggest that these differences between human and mouse striatal fingerprints are more likely to reflect differences in connectivity with regions of the lateral prefrontal cortex that likely underpin human behaviours that have developed most with evolution, that is executive functions, and social/language abilities. As such we would caution researchers using mouse models for disorders predominantly affecting these processes as there appears to be less clear translation between mice and humans.

Prefrontal cortex

Most theories on the functions of the prefrontal cortex have been derived from studies in humans and nonhuman primates, awaiting translation in rodents. Multiple approaches have been taken to establish homologs across species, including similarities in cytoarchitecture, similarities in connectivity, and equivalence of function. While each of these approaches has been used to debate the existence of the granular PFC in rodents, there is at least increasing consensus that mouse prefrontal regions IL, PL, and Cg could be equivalent to human and macaque areas 25, 32, and 24 respectively (Bicks et al., 2015). Heilbronner et al. (2016) showed similar striatal projection patterns in rats and macaques using tracers from IL/25, PL/32, and Cg/24 in both these species. However, there still is quite some debate on whether other parts of human prefrontal cortex are present in the rodent (Laubach et al., 2018). At the very least, human prefrontal cortex as extended substantially in absolute terms, although its relative extension compared to other primates is also a matter of fierce debate (Barton and Venditti, 2013; Passingham and Smaers, 2014). For the definition of our connectivity fingerprints, we have only used regions whose homology across species has been well established in the literature (see Materials and methods and Supplementary file 1 tables 1-3). However, our results speak to the debate about translational results in prefrontal cortex in two ways.

First, and most obvious, we find voxels in the human striatum that possess a connectivity fingerprint that is not found in either the mouse or macaque. These voxels all tended to have a strong functional connectivity with, among others, parts of the human lateral prefrontal cortex (area 46 and lateral frontal pole (FPl)). Although area 46 has a clear homolog in the macaque, it is part of granular dorsolateral prefrontal cortex which is believed to be an anthropoid primate specialisation (Passingham and Wise, 2012; Preuss and Goldman-Rakic, 1991) and therefore not present in the mouse. The lateral part of the frontal pole has been identified in humans based on both cytoarchitecture and connectivity (Bludau et al., 2014), but a macaque homolog is not detectable using rs-fMRI raising the possibility that this is a human or at least ape specialisation (Neubert et al., 2014). The fact that the unassigned striatal voxels predominantly show connectivity with these areas suggests the presence of a unique cortical-striatal loop. Based on our findings, this loop would be mostly involved in higher order human behaviours, as are associated with FPl (Hartogsveld et al., 2018; Vendetti and Bunge, 2014), suggesting that these might be difficult to study using the translational paradigm.

Our findings also warrant a second caution with regard to the translation of prefrontal results. Even though human striatal regions possessed similar connectivity fingerprints to those of mice, it is possible that similar circuits could include novel projections in humans (Mars et al., 2018). For instance, voxels in the human striatum assigned to CP.m showed the expected connectivity with homologous target regions such as the medial frontal cortex, but also with regions in the dorsal prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) and inferior parietal lobule (Figure 3). Since it’s disputed whether homologs of dlPFC and inferior parietal lobule exist within mice, it seems likely the CP.m network is embedded within a larger network in humans that includes novel regions. We therefore urge caution interpreting translational results: even if regions of interest are homologous, they might be embedded in larger networks that include novel areas.

Cerebellum

Humans showed unique striatal connectivity with parts of association cortex, including the anterior prefrontal cortex, but also with large regions of cerebellar cortex. Specifically, the cerebellar lobules showing unique striatal connectivity in humans were Crus I and Crus II, which have been shown to be interconnected with the prefrontal cortex (Kelly and Strick, 2003; O'Reilly et al., 2010), and contribute to cognitive processes including rule-guided behaviour (Balsters et al., 2013; Balsters and Ramnani, 2011; Balsters and Ramnani, 2008) and language (Lesage et al., 2017; Lesage et al., 2012; Mariën et al., 2014). Tracer studies in rats and non-human primates have shown that prefrontal projections to the cerebellar cortex are conserved across species (Kelly and Strick, 2003; Schmahmann and Pandya, 1997; Wiesendanger and Wiesendanger, 1982); however, research also suggests that prefrontal-cerebellar circuits have selectively expanded in humans compared to other species. Specifically, prefrontal projections to the pontine nucleus (Ramnani et al., 2006), volume of prefrontal-projecting cerebellar lobules Crus I and Crus II (Balsters et al., 2010; Luo et al., 2017), and the volume and connections of the dorsal dentate nucleus (Baizer, 2014; Matano, 2001; Steele et al., 2016) have all expanded in humans relative to motor cerebellar circuitry across species. Studies of cerebellar evolution have generally suggested that Crus I and Crus II are homologous across species (Larsell, 1952) even though they have expanded in humans. The findings of this study could suggest that along with selective expansion, there may be additionally novel cortico-cerebellar connections in humans that have contributed to the expansion of the cortico-cerebellar system in humans. Given that the Area FPl homolog in non-human primates and mice is unclear or absent, it is plausible that this region has generated novel projections to both the striatum and cerebellum. The finer grained 17-Network parcellation of the human cerebral cortex by Yeo et al. (2011), includes a region similar to Area FPl identified in this study (Network 13). Buckner et al. (2011) analysis of cerebellar connectivity using the Yeo cortical parcellation shows that this region projects to Crus I and Crus II, adding further evidence to suggest that the some of the expansion of Crus I and Crus II could be due to novel inputs from the frontal pole in humans.

Caveats and cautions

Here, we used rsfMRI as a common methodology to compare connectivity fingerprints across humans, nonhuman primates, and mice. However, it is important to acknowledge the limitations of rsfMRI for making inferences about connectivity compared to tracer-based methods or diffusion weighted imaging (DWI)-based tractography. Tracer-based methods have often been considered to be the ‘gold standard’ for connectivity research, and comparative neuroanatomy. The level of detail available using tracer methods goes far beyond what is currently possible using fMRI. For example, tracer-based methods illustrate direction-specific monosynaptic connections, something that is not currently available using rsfMRI. When interpreting the results presented here, it is important to consider that differences across species could reflect network level differences as opposed to specific changes in monosynaptic connections (Mars et al., 2018; Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2019). For example, in both mouse-human and macaque-human analyses, we found increased functional connectivity with unassigned voxels in the anterior putamen/caudate and the occipital lobe. It is unclear whether this reflects a polysynaptic network effect or whether this reflects a change in monosynaptic connections between the occipital lobe and basal ganglia previously shown in mice (Hunnicutt et al., 2016; Khibnik et al., 2014) and primates (Saint-Cyr et al., 1990; Schmahmann and Pandya, 2006). Tracer-based methods can also provide details regarding cell-specific connectivity that distinguishes between different classes of interneurons. Ährlund-Richter et al. (2019) recently produced a whole brain atlas of cell-specific connections to medial PFC in mice, providing greater insight into the unique structure-function relationships that support information processing within the medial PFC. Although such methods provide exquisite levels of neuroanatomical detail, the lack of comparable methods in humans makes it difficult to translate these findings across species. While rsfMRI is unlikely to ever reach the level of specificity achieved by tracer methods, we believe the ability to use this technique in all species makes it a powerful tool for comparative neuroanatomy.

DWI is another technique that can be used across species as an alternative to rsfMRI. Previous studies have used DWI with great effect to compare human and nonhuman primate brain connectivity (Folloni et al., 2019; Mars et al., 2018); however, DWI has proven less successful in rodents. Calabrese et al. (2015) and Sinke et al. (2018) both found low similarity between DWI and tracer-based tracts in mice and rats, respectively. In comparison, Grandjean et al. (2017) found 87–88% overlap between rsfMRI connectivity and tracer connectivity in cortico-cortico circuits and cortico-striatal circuits. The strong agreement between the modalities is perhaps surprising, since rsfMRI connectivity and studies on neuronal tracers are fundamentally different. Anterograde viral tracers, such as those used for the Allen Mouse Brain Connectome, are unidirectional, follow individual axons and do not cross synapses. In contrast, rsfMRI is bidirectional and is not necessarily constrained by synapses. For example, the connections present in rsfMRI but missing from the anterograde tracer data are believed to be false positives, but they could also be real retrograde connections or real polysynaptic connections. This once again highlights one of the difficulties in comparing rsfMRI data with tracer studies. It’s often unclear whether differences across species are due to evolutionary changes in neuroanatomy or differences in methodologies. As such, it’s crucially important to compare like-with-like using the same methods across species. As nonhuman data sharing repositories grow, it will also be possible to address further interesting questions regarding gender differences (currently all the mice and macaque data were male).

One caveat to the approach used here is the definition of striatal seeds in the mouse. Data from both tracers and rsfMRI suggested that a three cluster solution best explained cortico-striatal connectivity in mice. While many studies in mice refer to a three domain striatal model (i.e. associative, limbic, motor), this concept is largely based upon research in non-human primates and rats (Balleine et al., 2009; Thorn et al., 2010; Yin and Knowlton, 2006). Although some anatomical studies in mice are providing greater support for the three domain model (Hunnicutt et al., 2016), there is a need for more operant studies in mice and more large scale anatomical studies in rats. It is also possible that whilst the three cluster striatal solution is the most consistent across subjects (mice), this may be an oversimplification of the cortico-striatal system. Studies in humans using rsfMRI connectivity have found that the caudate nucleus can be segmented into 1–9 regions, whilst the putamen can be segmented into 1–6 clusters (Choi et al., 2012; Janssen et al., 2015; Jaspers et al., 2017; Jung et al., 2014; Tian et al., 2020). The recent work of Tian et al. (2020), used a novel gradient-based approach with 1000 rsfMRI datasets at 3T and 7T. Their results highlight the various scales of resolution, ranging from scale I (1 putamen, 1 caudate, and 1 NAcc) to scale III (4 putamen, 4 caudate, 2 NAcc subdivisons). However, MRI based parcellations of the striatum appear to be much coarser than those seen in mice using tracer methods. For example, Hintiryan et al. (2016) utilised a novel neuroanatomical approach to map the cortico-striatal projectome, identifying 29 striatal regions with distinct connectivity fingerprints, and Chon et al. (2019) further extended this to present a 33 striatal cluster solution. Even using state of the art fMRI acquisition and analysis methods, the limitations of fMRI and its spatial resolution make a detailed analysis such as that of Hintiryan et al. (2016) and Chon et al. (2019) unfeasible. Advances in MRI methods may make it possible to investigate higher resolution parcellations of the striatum and other cortical and subcortical structures in the future, which may yield greater correspondence between mice and human brain circuits. We note, however, that the current data are of the best resolution and quality currently in general use (Grandjean et al., 2020; Glasser et al., 2016; Milham et al., 2018) and it is likely most translational studies will not be working with data of superior quality.

As well as seed definition, connectivity fingerprint matching relies on the accurate definitions of target areas across species. Indeed, connectivity fingerprint matching is heavily reliant on the fact that 1) connectivity strength will differ across targets, and 2) targets are anatomical and functional homologs across species. Here, we chose to focus on cortico-striatal circuits because their connectivity has been well-defined in humans, nonhuman primates, and mice (Alexander et al., 1986; Haber, 2016; Hintiryan et al., 2016; Verstynen et al., 2012). In addition, we selected targets that have already been used in previous studies comparing humans with macaques (Mars et al., 2013; Mars et al., 2011; Neubert et al., 2015; Neubert et al., 2014; Sallet et al., 2013), and have been shown project to different striatal regions. This produced our extended primate target model (Supplementary file 1 tables 2,3) of 17 targets which are broadly categorised as cingulate, orbitofrontal, subcortical, motor, temporal, ventrolateral PFC, dorsolateral PFC, SMA, and frontal pole. Given the debate surrounding mouse homologs of the lateral PFC, frontal pole, and SMA, these targets were not included in our reduced mouse model (see Supplementary file 1 table 1). However, it is important to note that in our human-macaque analysis, there was very little difference in the results generated using the reduced vs extended target models, suggesting that the results based upon the reduced mouse model are stable and not impacted by the addition of extra targets. As this line of research continues, it will be possible to refine our target models based on the results of previous studies.

Conclusions

Here, we demonstrate the potential of connectivity fingerprint matching to bridge the gap between mouse and primate neuroanatomy. Our results highlight the core properties of a mouse to primate striatum, including similarities in connectivity fingerprints for NAcc across species that could be a useful model for translational neuroscience. However, we would caution against researchers comparing medial and lateral regions of the mouse caudoputamen with the primate caudate nucleus and putamen. Although homologs were identified, there were also clear differences in connectivity patterns that require further investigation. We propose that these differences reflect the expansion of frontal cortex in primates, along with the relative expansion of area FPl in humans. These results will hopefully add to the on-going debates surrounding similarities in cortical brain regions across species, that is the existence of the PFC in mice. Further studies using connectivity fingerprint matching could help to refine where similarities and differences exist across species in other brain structures including medial prefrontal cortex and orbitofrontal regions.

Materials and methods

Targets and seeds

Reduced/mouse model

The reduced model includes twelve target regions common to all species: 1) Infralimbic (Area 25), 2) Prelimbic (Area 32), 3) Cingulate areas (Area 24), 4) Retrosplenial Cortex (Area 30), 5) Lateral Orbitofrontal Cortex (Area 13), 6) Basolateral Amgydala, 7) Dorsal (Anterior) Hippocampus, 8) Ventral (Posterior) Hippocampus, 9) Primary Motor Cortex (M1), 10) Primary Somatosensory Cortex (S1), 11) Supplemental Somatosensory Cortex (S2), 12) Temporal association area (TPJp). Targets were 3 × 3×3 voxels in all species. Further details can be found in Supplementary file 1 tables 1-3.

Extended/primate model

The extended model includes the twelve reduced model targets and five additional targets that are common to macaques and humans, but not mice: 13) Area 9/46d, 14) Area 9/46 v, 15) Area 44d, 16) FPm, 17) SMA. Targets were 3 × 3×3 voxels in all species. Further details can be found in Supplementary file 1 tables 2 and 3.

Mouse striatal seeds

Allen Institute database of tracer experiments were obtained and processed as described in Grandjean et al. (2017). Briefly, viral-tracer maps were downloaded using the query form from the Allen Institute database. We limited the scope of our analysis to the injections carried in the Isocortex of wild-type C57BL/6 animals, with injection volume ranging from 0.12 to 0.4 μl. This resulted in 68 tracer experiments, which were resampled at 200 μm3 and coregistered into the Allen Reference Atlas (v3) using Advanced Normalisation Tools (ANTS) (version 2.1, http://picsl.upenn.edu/software/ants/). The connectivity between striatal voxels and cortical seeds was determined after normalising the fluorescence with the volume of injection (for further details, see Oh et al., 2014).

We established connectivity strength (Z-transformed terminal tracer volume) between 68 injection sites in isocortex and each voxel in the caudoputamen, NAcc, and fundus. We then used connectivity-based parcellation to partition the mouse basal ganglia into regions with unique connectivity fingerprints (Balsters et al., 2016; Balsters et al., 2018). The optimal solution based on silhouette value was three regions with unique connectivity fingerprints based on anterograde tracers. These are labelled medial caudoputamen (CP.m), Nucleus accumbens (NAcc), and lateral caudoputamen (CP.l). The percent variance explained by the first eigenvariate for each seed was 57.72% ± 6.42, 49.18% ± 6.12, and 60.14% ± 6.89 for CP.m, NAcc, and CP.l, respectively.

Macaque seeds

Connectivity-based parcellation was applied to the macaque resting state data in order to create seeds with unique connectivity fingerprints. The optimal solution based on silhouette value was a 5 cluster solution keeping the NAcc and putamen whole, and segmenting the caudate nucleus into three segments (the body and two segments in the tail of the caudate). We focussed our analysis on the caudate body, NAcc, and putamen given the small number of voxels contributing to the caudate tail seeds. The percent variance explained by the first eigenvariate for each seed was equivalent to that of mice - 56.14% ± 6.39, 51.83% ± 5.87, and 56.28% ± 7.56 for Caudate body, NAcc, and putamen, respectively.

MRI data acquisition

Mouse

Mouse fMRI and anatomical scans were collected from 20 wildtype C57BL/6J animals (males, median age = 82 days; median weight 26 grams). Animals were caged in standard housing (maximum 5 animals/cage), with food and water ad libitum, and a 12 h day/night cycle. All MRI scans were conducted in the light phase. Protocols for animal care, magnetic resonance imaging, and anaesthesia were carried out under the authority of personal and project licenses in accordance with the Swiss federal guidelines for the use of animals in research, and under licensing from the Zürich Cantonal veterinary office.

Anaesthesia was induced with 4% isoflurane and the animals were endotracheally intubated and the tail vein cannulated. Mice were positioned on a MRI-compatible cradle, and artificially ventilated at 80 breaths per minute, 1:4 O2 to air ratio, and 1.8 ml/h flow (CWE, Ardmore). A bolus injection of medetomidine 0.05 mg/kg and pancuronium bromide 0.2 mg/kg was administered, and isoflurane was reduced to 1%. After 5 min, an infusion of medetomidine 0.1 mg/kg/hr and pancuronium bromide 0.4 mg/kg/hr was administered, and isoflurane was further reduced to 0.5%. The animal temperature was monitored using a rectal thermometer probe, and maintained at 36.5°C ± 0.5 during the measurements. The preparation of the animals did not exceed 20 min.

Data acquisition was performed on a Biospec 70/16 small animal MR system (Bruker BioSpin MRI, Ettlingen, Germany) with a cryogenic quadrature surface coil (Bruker BioSpin AG, Fällanden, Switzerland). After standard adjustments, shim gradients were optimized using mapshim protocol, with an ellipsoid reference volume covering the whole brain. For functional connectivity acquisition, a standard gradient-echo EPI sequence (GE-EPI, repetition time TR = 1000 ms, echo time TE = 15 ms, in-plane resolution RES = 0.22 × 0.2 mm2, number of slice NS = 20, slice thickness ST = 0.4 mm, slice gap SG = 0.1 mm) was applied to acquire 2000 vol in 38 min. In addition, we acquired anatomical T2*-weighted images (FLASH sequence, in-plane resolution of 0.05 × 0.02 mm, TE = 3.51, TR = 522 ms). The levels of anaesthesia and mouse physiological parameters were monitored following an established protocol to obtain a reliable measurement of functional connectivity (Grandjean et al., 2014; Zerbi et al., 2015).

Macaque

Macaque fMRI and anatomical scans were collected from 10 healthy macaque monkeys (Macaca mulatta, 10 males, median age = 4.98 years; median weight 9.25 kg). Protocols for animal care, magnetic resonance imaging, and anaesthesia were carried out under the authority of personal and project licenses in accordance with the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 (ASPA).

Anaesthesia was induced using intramuscular injection of ketamine (10 mg/kg) either combined with xylazine (0.125–0.25 mg/kg) or with midazolam (0.1 mg/kg) and buprenorphine (0.01 mg/kg). Macaques also received injections of atropine (0.05 mg/kg intramuscularly), meloxicam (0.2 mg/kg intravenously) and ranitidine (0.05 mg/kg intravenously). Anaesthesia was maintained with isoflurane. The anaesthetized animals were either placed in an MRI compatible stereotactic frame (Crist Instrument Co., Hagerstown, MA) or resting on a custom-made mouth mold (Rogue Research, Mtl, QC, CA). All animals were then brought in a horizontal 3T MRI scanner with a full-size bore. Resting-state fMRI data collection commenced approximately 4 hr after anaesthesia induction, when the peak effect of ketamine was unlikely to be still present. In accordance with veterinary instruction, anaesthesia was maintained using the lowest possible concentration of isoflurane gas. The depth of anaesthesia was assessed using physiological parameters (continuous monitoring of heart rate and blood pressure as well as clinical checks for muscle relaxation prior to scanning). During the acquisition of the MRI data, the median expired isoflurane concentration was 1.083% (ranging between 0.6% and 1.317%). Isoflurane was selected for the scans as resting-state networks have previously been demonstrated to closely match known anatomical circuits using this agent (Neubert et al., 2014; Vincent et al., 2007). Slight individual differences in physiology cause slight differences in the amount of anaesthetic gas concentrations needed to impose a similar level of anaesthesia on different monkeys.

All but one animal were maintained with intermittent positive pressure ventilation to ensure a constant respiration rate during the functional scan; one macaque was breathing without assistance. Respiration rate, inspired and expired CO2, and inspired and expired isoflurane concentration were monitored and recorded using VitalMonitor software (Vetronic Services Ltd., Devon). In addition to these parameters, core temperature was monitored using a Opsens temperature sensor (Opsens, Quebec, Canada) and pulse rate, SpO2 (>95%) were monitored using a Nonin sensor (Nonin Mediacal Inc, Minnesota) throughout the scan.

A four-channel phased-array radio-frequency coil in conjunction with a local transmission coil was used for data acquisition (Dr. H. Kolster, Windmiller Kolster Scientific, Fresno, CA). Whole-brain blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) fMRI data were collected for 1600 volumes from each animal (except for one with 950 volumes), using the following parameters: 36 axial slices, in-plane resolution 1.5 × 1.5 mm, slice thickness 1.5 mm, no slice gap, TR = 2280 ms, TE = 30 ms. Structural scans with a 0.5 mm isotropic resolution were acquired for each macaque in the same session, using a T1-weighted MP-RAGE sequence.

Human

Twenty volumetric (as opposed to grayordinate) resting state fMRI datasets (Age 22–35 years; 13 male) were downloaded from the Human Connectome Project (HCP) (UgurbilWU-Minn K, for the Van Essen et al., 2013). Whole-brain BOLD EPI images were collected for approximately 15mins (1200 volumes) using a standardised protocol (2 mm isotropic resolution, 72 slices, TR = 720 ms, TE = 33.1 ms, multiband factor = 8). Only the first session with phase encoding left-to-right (LR) was used. Rs-fMRI data were already pre-processed using the FIX pipeline (automatic ICA rejection and regression of 24 head motion parameters) (Griffanti et al., 2014; Salimi-Khorshidi et al., 2014) and normalised into MNI space.

Resting state pre-processing and analysis

In all species, we used an ICA nuisance regression pre-processing strategy (FIX for human and mouse, manual component rejection for macaque). For all species, resting state analyses were conducted using CONN (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon, 2012). Data were bandpass filtered according to recent recommendations (Mouse: 0.01–0.25 Hz; Macaque: 0.01–0.087 Hz; Human: 0.01–0.15 Hz), linear detrended, and despiked. Lowpass filter limits were set to be 5 volumes of data in all species; however, this was further reduced for the human data in order to avoid physiological artefacts which are believed to occur at frequencies > 0.2 Hz (Baria et al., 2011).

Resting state analyses began with the creation of three template connectivity fingerprints in mice (CP.m, NAcc, CP.l) and macaques (caudate, NAcc, and putamen). For each dataset, we extracted the principle eigenvariate from three striatal seeds and correlated these with timeseries extracted from target regions. To confirm that of each striatal seed has a unique connectivity fingerprint we used the MrCat toolbox (https://github.com/neuroecology/MrCat) to establish the Manhattan distance between striatal connectivity fingerprints within species (i.e. comparing CP.m and CP.l fingerprints in mice) and used permutation testing (10,000 permutations) to test for significance. This same procedure was used to confirm that macaque striatal connectivity fingerprints were significantly different from one another.

Individual connectivity fingerprints within species were averaged (robust mean) to create template connectivity fingerprints for each striatal seed (see Figures 2, 4 and 6). In the comparison species, we extracted a connectivity fingerprint for each striatal voxel (correlation between the voxel and target timeseries). This voxel-based fingerprint was correlated with each of the template fingerprints from a different species and the resulting correlation value assigned to the voxel. This produced three correlation maps for each participant (one for each striatal seed) describing the correlation between each voxel fingerprint and the template fingerprint. Maps were Fisher’s r-to-Z transformed and run through permutation testing in FSL’s randomise (10,000 permutations, TFCE corrected p<0.05) to establish which voxels showed a significantly similar connectivity fingerprint.

T-Maps generated in the previous step were used as ROIs in CONN to generate whole-brain connectivity maps. A weighted timeseries from only positive (i.e. similar) voxels was extracted and used to identify connected regions across all grey matter voxels within the right hemisphere. Significant connectivity was established using permutation testing (10,000 permutations) and correction for multiple comparisons (p<0.001 voxel threshold, cluster-extent p<0.05 FDR). A conjunction analysis was employed to compare connectivity maps for assigned and unassigned voxels (Friston et al., 2005; Price and Friston, 1997). This approach is a more stringent comparison as it requires connectivity in the unassigned voxel map to be significantly greater than the connectivity for all the assigned voxel maps (i.e. unassigned >CP .m and unassigned >NAcc and unassigned >CP .l).

Anatomical and functional localisation

Mouse-to-human and macaque-to-human striatal homologs were localised using the Harvard Oxford subcortical atlas and a task-based parcellation of the striatum (Pauli et al., 2016). Rather than using the traditional approach of localisation based on local maxima, we used the distribution-based cluster assignment method outlined in Eickhoff et al. (2007) to highlight the central tendency of activations and avoid localising to peripheral structures. This approach compares probability distributions for the underlying anatomical regions with probability distributions for the functional activation cluster, allowing one to make judgments about whether brain regions are over or under-represented. Specifically, the mean probability for area X at the location of the functional activation is divided by the overall mean probability for area X in all voxels where it was observed. This provides a quotient which indicates how much more (or less) likely an area was observed in the functionally defined volume than could be expected if the probabilities at that location would follow their overall distribution. A quotient >1 indicates a rather central location of the activation with respect to this area, whereas a quotient <1 a more peripheral one. Cortical activations were localised using a combination of cytoarchitectonic probability maps from the Anatomy Toolbox (Eickhoff et al., 2007; Eickhoff et al., 2006; Eickhoff et al., 2005) and connectivity-based parcellation maps available in FSLEYES (Mars et al., 2013; Mars et al., 2011; Neubert et al., 2015; Neubert et al., 2014; Sallet et al., 2013; Tziortzi et al., 2014). Cerebellar activations were localised using the probabilistic cerebellar atlas (Diedrichsen et al., 2009).

Acknowledgements

VZ is supported by an SNSF AMBIZIONE grant PZ00P3_173984/1. JS is supported by a Wellcome Trust grant 105651/Z/14/Z. NW is supported by ETH Research Grant ETH-38 16–2. RBM is supported by the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BBSRC) UK [BB/N019814/1] and the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research NWO [452-13-015]. The Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging is supported by core funding from the Wellcome Trust [203139/Z/16/Z].

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Joshua Henk Balsters, Email: Joshua.Balsters@rhul.ac.uk.

Valerio Zerbi, Email: valerio.zerbi@hest.ethz.ch.

Kate M Wassum, University of California, Los Angeles, United States.

Thorsten Kahnt, Northwestern University, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Schweizerischer Nationalfonds zur Förderung der Wissenschaftlichen Forschung PZ00P3_173984/1 to Valerio Zerbi.

  • Wellcome 105651/Z/14/Z to Jerome Sallet.

  • Eidgenössische Technische Hochschule Zürich ETH-38 16-2 to Nicole Wenderoth.

  • Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council BB/N019814/1 to Rogier B Mars.

  • Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek 452-13-015 to Rogier B Mars.

  • Wellcome 203139/Z/16/Z to Jerome Sallet, Rogier B Mars.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Project administration.

Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Project administration.

Resources, Data curation, Supervision, Methodology.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Methodology.

Conceptualization, Software, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration.

Ethics

Human subjects: Human subjects: Fully described in the core HCP literature referenced here; the paper is only using publicly available datasets on human imaging data.

Animal experimentation: Mouse: Protocols for animal care, magnetic resonance imaging, and anesthesia were carried out under the authority of personal and project licenses in accordance with the Swiss federal guidelines for the use of animals in research, and under licensing from the Zürich Cantonal veterinary office (license number ZH198/16). Macaque: All non-human primate procedures were carried out in accordance with Home Office (UK) Regulations and European Union guidelines (EU directive 86/609/EEC; EU Directive 2010/63/EU).

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. Supplementary tables.
elife-53680-supp1.docx (158.1KB, docx)
Transparent reporting form

Data availability

The human resting state fMRI data was obtained from the Human Connectome Project (www.humanconnectome.org).

The following datasets were generated:

References

  1. Ährlund-Richter S, Xuan Y, van Lunteren JA, Kim H, Ortiz C, Pollak Dorocic I, Meletis K, Carlén M. A whole-brain atlas of monosynaptic input targeting four different cell types in the medial prefrontal cortex of the mouse. Nature Neuroscience. 2019;22:657–668. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0354-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alexander GE, DeLong MR, Strick PL. Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1986;9:357–381. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.09.030186.002041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Baizer JS. Unique features of the human brainstem and cerebellum. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2014;8:202. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Balleine BW, Liljeholm M, Ostlund SB. The integrative function of the basal ganglia in instrumental conditioning. Behavioural Brain Research. 2009;199:43–52. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.10.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Balsters JH, Cussans E, Diedrichsen J, Phillips KA, Preuss TM, Rilling JK, Ramnani N. Evolution of the cerebellar cortex: the selective expansion of prefrontal-projecting cerebellar lobules. NeuroImage. 2010;49:2045–2052. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Balsters JH, Whelan CD, Robertson IH, Ramnani N. Cerebellum and cognition: evidence for the encoding of higher order rules. Cerebral Cortex. 2013;23:1433–1443. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Balsters JH, Mantini D, Apps MAJ, Eickhoff SB, Wenderoth N. Connectivity-based parcellation increases network detection sensitivity in resting state fMRI: an investigation into the cingulate cortex in autism. NeuroImage: Clinical. 2016;11:494–507. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.03.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Balsters JH, Mantini D, Wenderoth N. Connectivity-based parcellation reveals distinct cortico-striatal connectivity fingerprints in autism spectrum disorder. NeuroImage. 2018;170:412–423. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.02.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Balsters JH, Ramnani N. Symbolic representations of action in the human cerebellum. NeuroImage. 2008;43:388–398. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Balsters JH, Ramnani N. Cerebellar plasticity and the automation of first-order rules. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:2305–2312. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4358-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Baria AT, Baliki MN, Parrish T, Apkarian AV. Anatomical and functional assemblies of brain BOLD oscillations. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:7910–7919. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1296-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Barton RA, Venditti C. Human frontal lobes are not relatively large. PNAS. 2013;110:9001–9006. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1215723110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bicks LK, Koike H, Akbarian S, Morishita H. Prefrontal cortex and social cognition in mouse and man. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6:1805. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bludau S, Eickhoff SB, Mohlberg H, Caspers S, Laird AR, Fox PT, Schleicher A, Zilles K, Amunts K. Cytoarchitecture, probability maps and functions of the human frontal pole. NeuroImage. 2014;93:260–275. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bradshaw J, Enticott P. Developmental Disorders of the Frontostriatal System. Psychology Press; 2001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Buckner RL, Krienen FM, Castellanos A, Diaz JC, Yeo BT. The organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011;106:2322–2345. doi: 10.1152/jn.00339.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Calabrese E, Badea A, Cofer G, Qi Y, Johnson GA. A diffusion MRI tractography connectome of the mouse brain and comparison with neuronal tracer data. Cerebral Cortex. 2015;25:4628–4637. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cao Y, Fujiwara M, Nikaido M, Okada N, Hasegawa M. Interordinal relationships and timescale of eutherian evolution as inferred from mitochondrial genome data. Gene. 2000;259:149–158. doi: 10.1016/S0378-1119(00)00427-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cardinal RN, Pennicott DR, Sugathapala CL, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Impulsive choice induced in rats by lesions of the nucleus accumbens core. Science. 2001;292:2499–2501. doi: 10.1126/science.1060818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Carlén M. What constitutes the prefrontal cortex? Science. 2017;358:478–482. doi: 10.1126/science.aan8868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Choi EY, Yeo BT, Buckner RL. The organization of the human striatum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2012;108:2242–2263. doi: 10.1152/jn.00270.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Choi EY, Ding S-L, Haber SN. Combinatorial inputs to the ventral striatum from the temporal cortex, frontal cortex, and amygdala: implications for segmenting the striatum. Eneuro. 2017;4:ENEURO.0392-17.2017. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0392-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Chon U, Vanselow DJ, Cheng KC, Kim Y. Enhanced and unified anatomical labeling for a common mouse brain atlas. Nature Communications. 2019;10:5067. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-13057-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dalley JW, Cardinal RN, Robbins TW. Prefrontal executive and cognitive functions in rodents: neural and neurochemical substrates. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2004;28:771–784. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Diedrichsen J, Balsters JH, Flavell J, Cussans E, Ramnani N. A probabilistic MR atlas of the human cerebellum. NeuroImage. 2009;46:39–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Diedrichsen J, Zotow E. Surface-Based display of Volume-Averaged cerebellar imaging data. PLOS ONE. 2015;10:e0133402. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0133402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Dietrich MR, Ankeny RA, Chen PM. Publication trends in model organism research. Genetics. 2014;198:787–794. doi: 10.1534/genetics.114.169714. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Eickhoff SB, Stephan KE, Mohlberg H, Grefkes C, Fink GR, Amunts K, Zilles K. A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. NeuroImage. 2005;25:1325–1335. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Eickhoff SB, Heim S, Zilles K, Amunts K. Testing anatomically specified hypotheses in functional imaging using cytoarchitectonic maps. NeuroImage. 2006;32:570–582. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.04.204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Eickhoff SB, Paus T, Caspers S, Grosbras MH, Evans AC, Zilles K, Amunts K. Assignment of functional activations to probabilistic cytoarchitectonic Areas revisited. NeuroImage. 2007;36:511–521. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Eickhoff SB, Thirion B, Varoquaux G, Bzdok D. Connectivity-based parcellation: critique and implications. Human Brain Mapping. 2015;36:4771–4792. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22933. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ellenbroek B, Youn J. Rodent models in neuroscience research: is it a rat race? Disease Models & Mechanisms. 2016;9:1079–1087. doi: 10.1242/dmm.026120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Folloni D, Sallet J, Khrapitchev AA, Sibson N, Verhagen L, Mars RB. Dichotomous organization of amygdala/temporal-prefrontal bundles in both humans and monkeys. eLife. 2019;8:e47175. doi: 10.7554/eLife.47175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Freeze BS, Kravitz AV, Hammack N, Berke JD, Kreitzer AC. Control of basal ganglia output by direct and indirect pathway projection neurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:18531–18539. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1278-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Friston KJ, Penny WD, Glaser DE. Conjunction revisited. NeuroImage. 2005;25:661–667. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Glasser MF, Smith SM, Marcus DS, Andersson JL, Auerbach EJ, Behrens TE, Coalson TS, Harms MP, Jenkinson M, Moeller S, Robinson EC, Sotiropoulos SN, Xu J, Yacoub E, Ugurbil K, Van Essen DC. The human connectome project's neuroimaging approach. Nature Neuroscience. 2016;19:1175–1187. doi: 10.1038/nn.4361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Grandjean J, Schroeter A, Batata I, Rudin M. Optimization of anesthesia protocol for resting-state fMRI in mice based on differential effects of anesthetics on functional connectivity patterns. NeuroImage. 2014;102:838–847. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.08.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Grandjean J, Zerbi V, Balsters JH, Wenderoth N, Rudin M. Structural basis of Large-Scale functional connectivity in the mouse. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2017;37:8092–8101. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0438-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Grandjean J, Canella C, Anckaerts C, Ayrancı G, Bougacha S, Bienert T, Buehlmann D, Coletta L, Gallino D, Gass N, Garin CM, Nadkarni NA, Hübner NS, Karatas M, Komaki Y, Kreitz S, Mandino F, Mechling AE, Sato C, Sauer K, Shah D, Strobelt S, Takata N, Wank I, Wu T, Yahata N, Yeow LY, Yee Y, Aoki I, Chakravarty MM, Chang WT, Dhenain M, von Elverfeldt D, Harsan LA, Hess A, Jiang T, Keliris GA, Lerch JP, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Okano H, Rudin M, Sartorius A, Van der Linden A, Verhoye M, Weber-Fahr W, Wenderoth N, Zerbi V, Gozzi A. Common functional networks in the mouse brain revealed by multi-centre resting-state fMRI analysis. NeuroImage. 2020;205:116278. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Griffanti L, Salimi-Khorshidi G, Beckmann CF, Auerbach EJ, Douaud G, Sexton CE, Zsoldos E, Ebmeier KP, Filippini N, Mackay CE, Moeller S, Xu J, Yacoub E, Baselli G, Ugurbil K, Miller KL, Smith SM. ICA-based artefact removal and accelerated fMRI acquisition for improved resting state network imaging. NeuroImage. 2014;95:232–247. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Gunaydin LA, Grosenick L, Finkelstein JC, Kauvar IV, Fenno LE, Adhikari A, Lammel S, Mirzabekov JJ, Airan RD, Zalocusky KA, Tye KM, Anikeeva P, Malenka RC, Deisseroth K. Natural neural projection dynamics underlying social behavior. Cell. 2014;157:1535–1551. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.05.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gunaydin LA, Kreitzer AC. Cortico–Basal Ganglia Circuit Function in Psychiatric Disease. Annual Review of Physiology. 2016;78:1–24. doi: 10.1146/annurev-physiol-021115-105355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Haber SN. Corticostriatal circuitry. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience. 2016;18:7–21. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-6434-1_135-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hariri AR, Brown SM, Williamson DE, Flory JD, de Wit H, Manuck SB. Preference for immediate over delayed rewards is associated with magnitude of ventral striatal activity. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:13213–13217. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3446-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hartogsveld B, Bramson B, Vijayakumar S, van Campen AD, Marques JP, Roelofs K, Toni I, Bekkering H, Mars RB. Lateral frontal pole and relational processing: activation patterns and connectivity profile. Behavioural Brain Research. 2018;355:2–11. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Heilbronner SR, Rodriguez-Romaguera J, Quirk GJ, Groenewegen HJ, Haber SN. Circuit-Based corticostriatal homologies between rat and primate. Biological Psychiatry. 2016;80:509–521. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.05.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Hintiryan H, Foster NN, Bowman I, Bay M, Song MY, Gou L, Yamashita S, Bienkowski MS, Zingg B, Zhu M, Yang XW, Shih JC, Toga AW, Dong HW. The mouse cortico-striatal projectome. Nature Neuroscience. 2016;19:1100–1114. doi: 10.1038/nn.4332. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hunnicutt BJ, Jongbloets BC, Birdsong WT, Gertz KJ, Zhong H, Mao T. A comprehensive excitatory input map of the striatum reveals novel functional organization. eLife. 2016;5:e19103. doi: 10.7554/eLife.19103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Janssen RJ, Jylänki P, Kessels RP, van Gerven MA. Probabilistic model-based functional parcellation reveals a robust, fine-grained subdivision of the striatum. NeuroImage. 2015;119:398–405. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Jaspers E, Balsters JH, Kassraian Fard P, Mantini D, Wenderoth N. Corticostriatal connectivity fingerprints: probability maps based on resting-state functional connectivity. Human Brain Mapping. 2017;38:1478–1491. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Jung WH, Jang JH, Park JW, Kim E, Goo EH, Im OS, Kwon JS. Unravelling the intrinsic functional organization of the human striatum: a parcellation and connectivity study based on resting-state FMRI. PLOS ONE. 2014;9:e106768. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0106768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Kelly RM, Strick PL. Cerebellar loops with motor cortex and prefrontal cortex of a nonhuman primate. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:8432–8444. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-23-08432.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Khibnik LA, Tritsch NX, Sabatini BL. A direct projection from mouse primary visual cortex to dorsomedial striatum. PLOS ONE. 2014;9:e104501. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0104501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kiyokawa Y, Wakabayashi Y, Takeuchi Y, Mori Y. The neural pathway underlying social buffering of conditioned fear responses in male rats. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;36:3429–3437. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08257.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Larsell O. The morphogenesis and adult pattern of the lobules and fissures of the cerebellum of the white rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1952;97:281–356. doi: 10.1002/cne.900970204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Laubach M, Amarante LM, Swanson K, White SR. What, if anything, is rodent prefrontal cortex? Eneuro. 2018;5:ENEURO.0315-18.2018. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0315-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Lesage E, Morgan BE, Olson AC, Meyer AS, Miall RC. Cerebellar rTMS disrupts predictive language processing. Current Biology. 2012;22:R794–R795. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Lesage E, Hansen PC, Miall RC. Right lateral cerebellum represents linguistic predictability. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2017;37:6231–6241. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3203-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Luo Y, Fujita H, Nedelescu H, Biswas MS, Sato C, Ying S, Takahashi M, Akita K, Higashi T, Aoki I, Sugihara I. Lobular homology in cerebellar hemispheres of humans, non-human primates and rodents: a structural, axonal tracing and molecular expression analysis. Brain Structure and Function. 2017;222:2449–2472. doi: 10.1007/s00429-017-1436-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Mailly P, Aliane V, Groenewegen HJ, Haber SN, Deniau JM. The rat prefrontostriatal system analyzed in 3D: evidence for multiple interacting functional units. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:5718–5727. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5248-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Mariën P, Ackermann H, Adamaszek M, Barwood CH, Beaton A, Desmond J, De Witte E, Fawcett AJ, Hertrich I, Küper M, Leggio M, Marvel C, Molinari M, Murdoch BE, Nicolson RI, Schmahmann JD, Stoodley CJ, Thürling M, Timmann D, Wouters E, Ziegler W. Consensus paper: language and the cerebellum: an ongoing enigma. Cerebellum. 2014;13:386–410. doi: 10.1007/s12311-013-0540-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Mars RB, Jbabdi S, Sallet J, O'Reilly JX, Croxson PL, Olivier E, Noonan MP, Bergmann C, Mitchell AS, Baxter MG, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, Tomassini V, Miller KL, Rushworth MF. Diffusion-weighted imaging tractography-based parcellation of the human parietal cortex and comparison with human and macaque resting-state functional connectivity. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:4087–4100. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5102-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Mars RB, Sallet J, Neubert F-X, Rushworth MFS. Connectivity profiles reveal the relationship between brain Areas for social cognition in human and monkey temporoparietal cortex. PNAS. 2013;110:10806–10811. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1302956110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Mars RB, Neubert F-X, Verhagen L, Sallet JÃ, Miller KL, Dunbar RIM, Barton RA. Primate comparative neuroscience using magnetic resonance imaging: promises and challenges. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2014;8:298. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Mars RB, Verhagen L, Gladwin TE, Neubert FX, Sallet J, Rushworth MF. Comparing brains by matching connectivity profiles. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2016;60:90–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Mars RB, Eichert N, Jbabdi S, Verhagen L, Rushworth MFS. Connectivity and the search for specializations in the language-capable brain. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2018;21:19–26. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Matano S. Brief communication: proportions of the ventral half of the cerebellar dentate nucleus in humans and great apes. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2001;114:163–165. doi: 10.1002/1096-8644(200102)114:2&#x0003c;163::AID-AJPA1016&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-F. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Milham MP, Ai L, Koo B, Xu T, Amiez C, Balezeau F, Baxter MG, Blezer ELA, Brochier T, Chen A, Croxson PL, Damatac CG, Dehaene S, Everling S, Fair DA, Fleysher L, Freiwald W, Froudist-Walsh S, Griffiths TD, Guedj C, Hadj-Bouziane F, Ben Hamed S, Harel N, Hiba B, Jarraya B, Jung B, Kastner S, Klink PC, Kwok SC, Laland KN, Leopold DA, Lindenfors P, Mars RB, Menon RS, Messinger A, Meunier M, Mok K, Morrison JH, Nacef J, Nagy J, Rios MO, Petkov CI, Pinsk M, Poirier C, Procyk E, Rajimehr R, Reader SM, Roelfsema PR, Rudko DA, Rushworth MFS, Russ BE, Sallet J, Schmid MC, Schwiedrzik CM, Seidlitz J, Sein J, Shmuel A, Sullivan EL, Ungerleider L, Thiele A, Todorov OS, Tsao D, Wang Z, Wilson CRE, Yacoub E, Ye FQ, Zarco W, Zhou YD, Margulies DS, Schroeder CE. An open resource for Non-human primate imaging. Neuron. 2018;100:61–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.08.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Murray EA, Wise SP, Graham KS. The Evolution of Memory Systems. Oxford University Press ; 2016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  70. Narayanan NS, Cavanagh JF, Frank MJ, Laubach M. Common medial frontal mechanisms of adaptive control in humans and rodents. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:1888–1895. doi: 10.1038/nn.3549. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Neubert FX, Mars RB, Thomas AG, Sallet J, Rushworth MF. Comparison of human ventral frontal cortex Areas for cognitive control and language with Areas in monkey frontal cortex. Neuron. 2014;81:700–713. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.11.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Neubert F-X, Mars RB, Sallet J, Rushworth MFS. Connectivity reveals relationship of brain Areas for reward-guided learning and decision making in human and monkey frontal cortex. PNAS. 2015;112:E2695–E2704. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1410767112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. O'Reilly JX, Beckmann CF, Tomassini V, Ramnani N, Johansen-Berg H. Distinct and overlapping functional zones in the cerebellum defined by resting state functional connectivity. Cerebral Cortex. 2010;20:953–965. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Oh SW, Harris JA, Ng L, Winslow B, Cain N, Mihalas S, Wang Q, Lau C, Kuan L, Henry AM, Mortrud MT, Ouellette B, Nguyen TN, Sorensen SA, Slaughterbeck CR, Wakeman W, Li Y, Feng D, Ho A, Nicholas E, Hirokawa KE, Bohn P, Joines KM, Peng H, Hawrylycz MJ, Phillips JW, Hohmann JG, Wohnoutka P, Gerfen CR, Koch C, Bernard A, Dang C, Jones AR, Zeng H. A mesoscale connectome of the mouse brain. Nature. 2014;508:207–214. doi: 10.1038/nature13186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Passingham RE, Stephan KE, Kötter R. The anatomical basis of functional localization in the cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2002;3:606–616. doi: 10.1038/nrn893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Passingham RE, Smaers JB. Is the prefrontal cortex especially enlarged in the human brain allometric relations and remapping factors. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 2014;84:156–166. doi: 10.1159/000365183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Passingham RE, Wise SP. The Neurobiology of the Prefrontal Cortex. Oxford University Press; 2012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  78. Pauli WM, O’Reilly RC, Yarkoni T, Wager TD. Regional specialization within the human striatum for diverse psychological functions. PNAS. 2016;113:1907–1912. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1507610113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Preuss TM. Do rats have prefrontal cortex? the Rose-Woolsey-Akert program reconsidered. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1995;7:1–24. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1995.7.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Preuss TM, Goldman-Rakic PS. Myelo- and cytoarchitecture of the granular frontal cortex and surrounding regions in the strepsirhine primate Galago and the anthropoid primate Macaca. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1991;310:429–474. doi: 10.1002/cne.903100402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Price CJ, Friston KJ. Cognitive conjunction: a new approach to brain activation experiments. NeuroImage. 1997;5:261–270. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Ramnani N, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, Richter MC, Pinsk MA, Andersson JL, Rudebeck P, Ciccarelli O, Richter W, Thompson AJ, Gross CG, Robson MD, Kastner S, Matthews PM. The evolution of prefrontal inputs to the cortico-pontine system: diffusion imaging evidence from macaque monkeys and humans. Cerebral Cortex. 2006;16:811–818. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Rohlfing T, Kroenke CD, Sullivan EV, Dubach MF, Bowden DM, Grant KA, Pfefferbaum A. The INIA19 template and NeuroMaps atlas for primate brain image parcellation and spatial normalization. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics. 2012;6:27. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2012.00027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Rose J, Woolsey CN. The orbitofrontal cortex and its connections with the mediodorsal nucleus in rabbit, sheep and cat. Research Publications - Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease. 1948;27:210–232. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Saint-Cyr JA, Ungerleider LG, Desimone R. Organization of visual cortical inputs to the striatum and subsequent outputs to the pallido-nigral complex in the monkey. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1990;298:129–156. doi: 10.1002/cne.902980202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Salimi-Khorshidi G, Douaud G, Beckmann CF, Glasser MF, Griffanti L, Smith SM. Automatic denoising of functional MRI data: combining independent component analysis and hierarchical fusion of classifiers. NeuroImage. 2014;90:449–468. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Sallet J, Mars RB, Noonan MP, Neubert FX, Jbabdi S, O'Reilly JX, Filippini N, Thomas AG, Rushworth MF. The organization of dorsal frontal cortex in humans and macaques. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:12255–12274. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5108-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Schaeffer DJ, Gilbert KM, Gati JS, Menon RS, Everling S. Intrinsic functional boundaries of lateral frontal cortex in the common marmoset monkey. The Neurosci. 2018;39:2595. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2595-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Schmahmann JD, Pandya DN. Anatomic organization of the basilar pontine projections from prefrontal cortices in rhesus monkey. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;17:438–458. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-01-00438.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Schmahmann JD, Pandya DN. Fiber Pathways of the Brain. Oxford University Press; 2006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  91. Sinke MRT, Otte WM, Christiaens D, Schmitt O, Leemans A, van der Toorn A, Sarabdjitsingh RA, Joëls M, Dijkhuizen RM. Diffusion MRI-based cortical connectome reconstruction: dependency on tractography procedures and neuroanatomical characteristics. Brain Structure and Function. 2018;223:2269–2285. doi: 10.1007/s00429-018-1628-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Smith SM, Miller KL, Salimi-Khorshidi G, Webster M, Beckmann CF, Nichols TE, Ramsey JD, Woolrich MW. Network modelling methods for FMRI. NeuroImage. 2011;54:875–891. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Steele CJ, Anwander A, Bazin P-L, Trampel R, Schaefer A, Turner R, Ramnani N, Villringer A. Human cerebellar Sub-millimeter diffusion imaging reveals the motor and Non-motor topography of the dentate nucleus. Cerebral Cortex. 2016;27:4537–4548. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Thiebaut de Schotten M, Croxson PL, Mars RB. Large-scale comparative neuroimaging: where are we and what do we need? Cortex. 2019;118:188–202. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.11.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Thorn CA, Atallah H, Howe M, Graybiel AM. Differential dynamics of activity changes in dorsolateral and dorsomedial striatal loops during learning. Neuron. 2010;66:781–795. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Tian Y, Margulies DS, Breakspear M, Zalesky A. Hierarchical organization of the human subcortex unveiled with functional connectivity gradients. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.01.13.903542. [DOI] [PubMed]
  97. Tziortzi AC, Haber SN, Searle GE, Tsoumpas C, Long CJ, Shotbolt P, Douaud G, Jbabdi S, Behrens TE, Rabiner EA, Jenkinson M, Gunn RN. Connectivity-based functional analysis of dopamine release in the striatum using diffusion-weighted MRI and positron emission tomography. Cerebral Cortex. 2014;24:1165–1177. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Uylings HB, Groenewegen HJ, Kolb B. Do rats have a prefrontal cortex? Behavioural Brain Research. 2003;146:3–17. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2003.09.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Van Essen DC, Smith SM, Barch DM, Behrens TE, Yacoub E, Ugurbil K, WU-Minn HCP Consortium The WU-Minn human connectome project: an overview. NeuroImage. 2013;80:62–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Vendetti MS, Bunge SA. Evolutionary and developmental changes in the lateral frontoparietal network: a little Goes a long way for higher-level cognition. Neuron. 2014;84:906–917. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Verstynen TD, Badre D, Jarbo K, Schneider W. Microstructural organizational patterns in the human corticostriatal system. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2012;107:2984–2995. doi: 10.1152/jn.00995.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Vincent JL, Patel GH, Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Baker JT, Van Essen DC, Zempel JM, Snyder LH, Corbetta M, Raichle ME. Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain. Nature. 2007;447:83–86. doi: 10.1038/nature05758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Nieto-Castanon A. Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connectivity. 2012;2:125–141. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Wiesendanger R, Wiesendanger M. The corticopontine system in the rat. II. the projection pattern. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1982;208:227–238. doi: 10.1002/cne.902080303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Yeo BT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, Roffman JL, Smoller JW, Zöllei L, Polimeni JR, Fischl B, Liu H, Buckner RL. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011;106:1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Yin HH, Knowlton BJ. The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2006;7:464–476. doi: 10.1038/nrn1919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Zerbi V, Grandjean J, Rudin M, Wenderoth N. Mapping the mouse brain with rs-fMRI: an optimized pipeline for functional network identification. NeuroImage. 2015;123:11–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Zerbi V, Ielacqua GD, Markicevic M, Haberl MG, Ellisman MH, A-Bhaskaran A, Frick A, Rudin M, Wenderoth N. Dysfunctional autism risk genes cause Circuit-Specific connectivity deficits with distinct developmental trajectories. Cerebral Cortex. 2018;28:2495–2506. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Thorsten Kahnt1
Reviewed by: Mark Laubach2, Samantha White3

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

This study uses a connectivity fingerprint matching approach to compare cortico-striatal connectivity profiles across species (mice, nonhuman primates, and humans). The findings show that connectivity patterns of the nucleus accumbens and cortico-striatal motor circuits are similar across all species, whereas those of the caudate and anterior putamen are unique for non-human primates and humans. Much clinical research is directly based on preclinical findings in mice, implicitly assuming correspondence across species. This is one of the rare studies that attempts to empirically test this assumption.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Primate homologs of mouse cortico-striatal circuits" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, and the evaluation has been overseen by Kate Wassum as the Senior Editor. The following individuals involved in review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Mark Laubach co-reviewed with Samantha White (Reviewer #2).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

Using connectivity fingerprint matching, this study compares cortico-striatal connectivity profiles across species (mice, nonhuman primates, and humans). The results demonstrate that the connectivity patterns of the nucleus accumbens and motor control regions are similar across species, and thus conserved. In contrast, the cortico-caudate and anterior putamen connectively profiles are not similar.

All reviewers agreed that this is an interesting and important study. However, reviewers also raised a number of concerns regarding the interpretation and discussion of the findings which need to be addressed. The most important issue that was discussed among reviewers was whether a 3-fold parcellation is sufficient to capture the complexity of the striatum and whether more fine-grained parcellations would provide more valuable information. The authors are strongly encouraged to consider more fine-grained parellations.

Essential revisions:

1) How was the Allen Institute database of tracer experiments transformed to Figure 1—figure supplement 1? Based on the literature and the tracer injections in the Allen Institute database, there is more precision to cortico-striatal projections. The authors likely combined all the tracer injections located in each of the three striatal segments. However, the rational for parcellating the striatum into these three rather large regions is unclear. Based on anatomic maps across species, three regions are not likely to be sufficient, given the complexity of cortico-striatal projections (even in the rodent brain). If the authors want to establish an imaging fingerprint for the mouse, it might better to use the data from the Allan Institute to first develop a segmentation of the mouse striatum that contained more than three large areas. For example, the shell might be separated from the core. The ICP is quite large and should be further segmented into components based on cortical inputs. It would be important to segment the mouse striatum in more than 3 general areas.

2) Discussion was lacking on limitations of using resting state data to infer connectivity, especially in comparison to DTI (which your group has also used) as well as traditional tract-tracing methods (Heilbronner) and viruses (Ährlund-Richter). The manuscript would benefit from some additional text on these issues.

a) Why did you use resting state data and not DTI? Please add a comment in the manuscript.

b) Are there are any specific connections that you found that are not reported in traditional tracer studies? If so, how can you account for such data? Network effects? By contrast, are there known connections that you did not find any evidence for? Please add a comment about these issues in the manuscript.

c) Neuroimaging does not allow for examining cell specific connectivity, as in viral tracer studies, and it would be good if you acknowledged the recent study by Ährlund-Richter and colleagues (PMID: 30886408) and described the limits on your interpretations that are due to using imaging, and not cell specific tracing. For example, some effects in your functional connectivity analysis might be mediated by connections between interneurons and projection cells or by inputs to interneurons, and this would not be detectable with either your method or traditional tracers.

d) Related to this, several of the connections identified in the paper are somewhat surprising, given the known anatomy, i.e. connectivity of V1 to the nucleus accumbens. While imaging results don't necessarily reflect all direct connections, for the most part they should be somewhat related. A more thorough review of how the results interface with the known anatomic connections is important, especially in the case in which connections seem to outliers.

3) Throughout the paper, the term "rodent" is used to discuss anatomical data from studies in mice. You should revise your text to replace rodent with mice. In addition, your manuscript would benefit from some text discussing why you are using data from mice, and not from rats. Obviously, this has to do with the Allen atlas. But what are the limitations of basing understanding the rodent brain on this one organism? After all, they are separated by ~27M of divergent evolution (see Cao et al. (2000), PMID: 11163972).

4) In the text referring to Figure 1 (subsection “Mouse to human comparison”, first paragraph), you cite the study by Hunnicutt. It reported on anatomical parcellation, not functional data. It would be good to cite primary functional studies at this point in the manuscript. However, here you must be careful to only cite studies in mice, unless you are also willing to explain why you would use studies in rats to explain function and studies in mice to explain structure throughout the manuscript). Maybe best would be to cite what you can for evidence for effects of lesions etc on learning, flexibility, decision making etc in mice, and also follow up with citations from the larger behavioral literature on rats. And then comment on the need for more large scale anatomy in rats and more carefully done operant studies in mice.

5) The conclusions that the connectivity patterns for the nucleus accumbens and motor areas across species, but not the caudate nucleus and anterior putamen is to be expected, and not surprising. The conservation of connectivity profiles across species for the n. accumbens and motor results are consistent with the well-known connections of these two evolutionarily conserved areas that mediate basic functions- appetitive and motor behaviors. The evolutionary expansion of cortex is primarily in cognitive areas, which are directly linked to the caudate and anterior putamen. This should be discussed.

6) Too little is said about whether or not the target brain areas that constitute the fingerprint can be considered homologous across the three species. The current analysis is just as much a test of the similarities in the striatum as it is a test of whether or not the target areas are similar across species. The degree to which the latter is true will affect the results and conclusions about striatal circuits. I think this deserves a bit more attention in the Discussion.

eLife. 2020 Apr 16;9:e53680. doi: 10.7554/eLife.53680.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) How was the Allen Institute database of tracer experiments transformed to Figure 1—figure supplement 1? Based on the literature and the tracer injections in the Allen Institute database, there is more precision to cortico-striatal projections. The authors likely combined all the tracer injections located in each of the three striatal segments. However, the rational for parcellating the striatum into these three rather large regions is unclear. Based on anatomic maps across species, three regions are not likely to be sufficient, given the complexity of cortico-striatal projections (even in the rodent brain). If the authors want to establish an imaging fingerprint for the mouse, it might better to use the data from the Allan Institute to first develop a segmentation of the mouse striatum that contained more than three large areas. For example, the shell might be separated from the core. The ICP is quite large and should be further segmented into components based on cortical inputs. It would be important to segment the mouse striatum in more than 3 general areas.

We’d like to thank the reviewers for opportunity to address these points. Regarding the first point, “How was the Allen Institute database of tracer experiments transformed to Figure 1—figure supplement 1?”. We have added the following information to the Materials and methods section ‘Mouse Striatal Seeds’.

“Allen Institute database of tracer experiments were obtained and processed as described in Grandjean et al., 2017. [...] The connectivity between striatal voxels and cortical seeds was determined after normalizing the fluorescence with the volume of injection (for further details, see Oh et al., 2014).”

Regarding the second point “the rational for parcellating the striatum into these three rather large regions is unclear”. Figure 1 now provides a more detailed schematic explaining how we used tracer label to create mouse striatal seeds. Each striatal voxel had 68 values (one per tracer injection site) indicating the density of tracer label from each injection site terminating at that voxel. As such, each voxel possessed a connectivity fingerprint dictating the pattern of cortico-striatal tracer-based connectivity for each injection site. The three segments were derived through a data-driven approach using hierarchical clustering to sort a correlation matrix of values indicating how similar the tracer-based connectivity fingerprint of each voxel was to one another, i.e. does a neighbouring voxel have the same connectivity pattern or a different one? Finding the optimal solution using such a method is not a simple subject (see Eickhoff et al., 2015). Previously, we have used silhouette value as a way to find the solution that maximises within-segment similarity and between-segment differences. Using this approach, the optimal solution was a three segment solution. Figure 1 includes the silhouette values for each solution (2-20 segments) highlighting that the 3 segment solution was the optimal solution.

To further validate this three segment solution, we investigated cortico-striatal connectivity in our rsfMRI data using an independent 33 segment parcellation of the striatum recently published by Chon et al., 2019. However, the parcellation from Chon et al., 2019, was derived from the cortico-striatal projectome at a much finer spatial resolution than our rsfMRI data. When using the same data-driven approach as applied to the tracer-based data, our hierarchical clustering analysis revealed that the optimal solution was 4 clusters of segments; NAcc, CP.l, CP.m, and a section of the CP.tail. This indicates a high degree of overlap between the 33 cortico-striatal connectivity fingerprints, most likely because the nuanced connectivity differences that distinguished each of the 33 striatal segments in the original study were not visible at the coarser resolution of our rsfMRI dataset. We are not suggesting here that the Chon parcellation is wrong, rather that these subtle differences between segments were not visible at the resolution of our data. Note that the clustering results of the Chon parcellation showed a remarkable consistency with the results of the voxelwise tracer-based analysis (quantified using dice similarity – see Figure 1—figure supplement 1). Here, the CP.tail fails to have a corresponding match (all spatial correlations are 0). This is because the CP.tail segment falls entirely outside of the striatal mask used in the previous analysis and included voxels in the bordering lateral amygdala nucleus. Given spatial smoothing involved in fMRI analysis it is commonplace to remove voxels that border adjacent structures to avoid signal contamination which is known to have a significant effect on network detection (Smith et al., 2011). We suggest that this CP.tail segment is likely to be distinct because of this signal contamination which is additionally problematic as the basolateral amygdala is one of our targets. As such we have kept the same translational analysis across species based on three cortico-striatal segments defined using an independent tracer dataset and included this additional analysis in Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

Given the consistency between both tracer-based and rsfMRI-based segmentations of the mouse striatum, we believe that at this resolution a coarse three segment description is optimal. However, we would agree with the reviewers that whilst such a solution is optimal given the resolution of the data, it is unlikely to fully capture the complexity of cortico-striatal circuits. As such, we have also extended our previous discussion of this caveat to include additional discussion of the recent atlas by Chon et al., 2019, and examples from studies of the human cortico-striatal system such as the recent work by Tian et al., 2020. A copy of this new section is included below,

“One caveat to the approach used here is the definition of striatal seeds in the mouse. […] We note, however, that the current data are of the best resolution and quality currently in general use (Grandjean et al., 2020; Marcus et al., 2016; Milham et al., 2018Click or tap here to enter text. and it is likely most translational studies will not be working with data of superior quality.”

2) Discussion was lacking on limitations of using resting state data to infer connectivity, especially in comparison to DTI (which your group has also used) as well as traditional tract-tracing methods (Heilbronner) and viruses (Ährlund-Richter). The manuscript would benefit from some additional text on these issues.

We agree that the following issues require further discussion. We have now amended our ‘Conclusions and cautions’ section of the Discussion to create a larger methodological caveats section that addresses all of the issues raised in these points. We have copied the entire section below for the authors to read, and dealt with each section (a-d) individually within this document.

“Caveats and cautions

Here, we used rsfMRI as a common methodology to compare connectivity fingerprints across humans, nonhuman primates, and mice. […] As this line of research continues, it will be possible to refine our target models based on the results of previous studies.”

a) Why did you use resting state data and not DTI? Please add a comment in the manuscript.

In our previous work, we used DTI to infer anatomical similarities between macaques and humans (e.g., Mars et al., 2016; 2018; reviewed in Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2018), but also resting state fMRI (Mars et al., 2011; Neubert et al., 2014; 2015). For this work we focused on mouse-human and mouse-macaque similarities and we decided resting-state fMRI would be a better connectivity metric. Our prior work demonstrated the strong agreement between inter-modal functional connectivity obtained through rsfMRI (i.e. functional connectome) and the anatomical ground truth provided by viral tracer injections in the mouse brain (i.e. structural connectome) (Grandjean et al., 2017). Specifically, for cortico-striatal connectivity, we found that approximately 88% of the voxels in the striatum have similar fingerprints between the two modalities. This provided the critical foundation for using rsfMRI connectivity as a translational tool to study inter-species brain connectivity similarities.

Whilst it is possible to obtain connectivity data in the mouse using diffusion-MRI data, previous studies have found a much poorer overlap between diffusion metrics and tracer data compared to rsfMRI. Calbrese et al., 2015, used ex-vivo samples and an extremely high resolution (43um isotropic), which required a scan time of 235 hours per sample. Despite the high quality of the diffusion tractography data, the authors observed relatively poor correspondence with neuronal tracer data, particularly with regard to 3D colocalization analysis. Strong correlation results were only observed in the coarsest, parent structure-level connectivity analysis, but were low for cortico-striatal connections. Similarly, another study (Sinke et al., 2018) confirmed that the degree of similarity between tractography-based connectivity and neuronal tracer data in rats is low and led to a substantial number of false positive and false negative connections. This issue was apparent even when using the most sophisticated tractography algorithms, including multi-shell multi-tissue constrained spherical deconvolution and global tractography. For these reasons, we decided not to include structural connectivity comparisons between species in the manuscript. We have included the following in the Discussion.

“DWI is another technique that can be used across species as an alternative to rsfMRI. […] In comparison, Grandjean et al., 2017, found 87-88% overlap between rsfMRI connectivity and tracer connectivity in cortico-cortico circuits and cortico-striatal circuits.”

b) Are there are any specific connections that you found that are not reported in traditional tracer studies? If so, how can you account for such data? Network effects? By contrast, are there known connections that you did not find any evidence for? Please add a comment about these issues in the manuscript.

Here, the reviewers have raised an important issue regarding the similarities and differences between methods of detecting brain connectivity. As the reviewers rightly point out, connectivity derived from rsfMRI is not limited to mono-synaptic connections and reflects whole network dynamics. That said, in our previous work (Grandjean et al., 2017), we showed that in mice there was 87-88% overlap between tracer-based connectivity and rsfMRI connectivity for the isocortex and striatum respectively. However, we also found that cortico-thalamic circuits failed to show a good overlap between methods (8.8%). We proposed that this could reflect the regional effects of anaesthesia in rsfMRI or the relationship between connectivity strength and the amount of terminal label used for each injection site in the Allen Institute tracer data. These issues were discussed in Grandjean et al., 2017, as this was the focus of that paper, whereas here our aim was to bypass differences in methods used across species and compare like-with-like (i.e. rsfMRI in all species). We have included these issues in our new Discussion section ‘Caveats and cautions’, but we believe an exhaustive comparison of tracer-based vs. resting state connectivity goes beyond the scope of this paper.

“The strong agreement between the rsfMRI and tracer modalities is perhaps surprising, since rsfMRI connectivity and studies on neuronal tracers are fundamentally different. […] As such, it is crucially important to compare like-with-like using the same methods across species.”

c) Neuroimaging does not allow for examining cell specific connectivity, as in viral tracer studies, and it would be good if you acknowledged the recent study by Ährlund-Richter and colleagues (PMID: 30886408) and described the limits on your interpretations that are due to using imaging, and not cell specific tracing. For example, some effects in your functional connectivity analysis might be mediated by connections between interneurons and projection cells or by inputs to interneurons, and this would not be detectable with either your method or traditional tracers.

We agree with the reviewers that MRI is clearly limited in cellular specificity, as highlighted by the exquisite work of Ährlund-Richter et al., 2019. However, we argue in the Introduction that for all the specificity of tracer studies, such methods are rarely applied to humans making them less useful for comparative neuroscience. Here, we have aimed to harmonise connectivity research across humans, nonhuman primates, and mice by using the same (all-be-it less specific) methodology. We have added these points into the ‘Caveats and cautions’ section:

“Tracer based methods can also provide details regarding cell specific connectivity that distinguishes between different classes of interneurons. […] While rsfMRI is unlikely to ever reach the level of specificity achieved by tracer methods, we believe the ability to use this technique in all species makes it a powerful tool for comparative neuroanatomy.”

d) Related to this, several of the connections identified in the paper are somewhat surprising, given the known anatomy, i.e. connectivity of V1 to the nucleus accumbens. While imaging results don't necessarily reflect all direct connections, for the most part they should be somewhat related. A more thorough review of how the results interface with the known anatomic connections is important, especially in the case in which connections seem to outliers.

As previously stated, we believe a thorough comparison of tracer-based connectivity and resting state connectivity is beyond the scope of this paper. In the majority of cases we would suggest that our activations are in keeping with both tracer studies and previous rsfMRI studies (Choi et al., 2012; Haber, 2016). For example, it is clear that connectivity with our unassigned voxels tracks the boundaries of the frontoparietal network as defined by Yeo et al., 2011. Even some of the more surprising findings do in fact coincide with known anatomy. For example, Hunnicutt et al., 2016, and Khibnik et al., 2014, show visual projections to the dorsomedial striatum in mice. In primates, this projection is likely to be conserved through the inferior longitudinal fasciculus which is known to enter the Muratoff bundle and terminate in the body of the caudate nucleus (Saint-Cyr et al., 1990; Schmahmann and Pandya, 2007). As such there is evidence in both mice and primates for anatomical connections between occipital lobe and the caudate nucleus to facilitate this rsfMRI activation. Unfortunately, it is not possible to say whether these results are due to monosynaptic or polysynaptic effects. This caution has been raised in the ‘Caveats and cautions’ section of the Discussion.

“The level of detail available using tracer methods goes far beyond what is currently possible using fMRI. […] It is unclear whether this reflects a polysynaptic network effect or whether this reflects a change in monosynaptic connections between the occipital lobe and basal ganglia previously shown in mice (Hunnicutt et al., 2016; Khibnik et al., 2014) and primates (Saine-Cyr et al., 1990; Schmahmann and Pandya, 2006).”

3) Throughout the paper, the term "rodent" is used to discuss anatomical data from studies in mice. You should revise your text to replace rodent with mice. In addition, your manuscript would benefit from some text discussing why you are using data from mice, and not from rats. Obviously, this has to do with the Allen atlas. But what are the limitations of basing understanding the rodent brain on this one organism? After all, they are separated by ~27M of divergent evolution (see Cao et al. (2000), PMID: 11163972).

We thank the reviewer for addressing this important point. Recent studies have shown reproducible and qualitatively similar resting-sate networks in both mouse and rat (Zhiwei et al., 2018; Grandjean et al., 2020). However, no one has quantitatively and systematically assessed the similarities and differences between resting-state fMRI connectivity in rats and mice. Thus, we agree with the reviewer that the term “rodent” was improperly used in a number of instances. To correct this, we have changed rodent to either ‘rat’ or ‘mouse’ when referring to a specific paper or dataset which uses a specific species. However, we have kept the term rodent in a small number of instances, for example, “there is a vital need to harmonise findings across species by establishing similarities and differences in rodent and primate neuroanatomy”. In this instance we believe the comment applies to both rats and mice and therefore warrants the use of the term rodent.

This increased specificity also led us to address the reviewers second point, why use mice instead of rats? According to Dietrich et al., 2014, and UK Home Office statistics from 2018 (https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/835935/annual-statistics-scientific-procedures-living-animals-2018.pdf) mice are the most used mammalian species in scientific research. In addition, there are a wealth of freely available neuroscientific resources in mice that are not available in rats including tracer-based connectivity data from the Allen Institute, as well as the new mouse rsfMRI database (Grandjean et al., 2020). We have included these points in the Introduction.

“Animal models are currently providing crucial insights into neural structure, function, and disorders. […] However, with this comes a growing necessity for translational, comparative neuroscience to harmonise these results with our understanding of structure, function, and disease in the human brain.”

4) In the text referring to Figure 1 (subsection “Mouse to human comparison”, first paragraph), you cite the study by Hunnicutt. It reported on anatomical parcellation, not functional data. It would be good to cite primary functional studies at this point in the manuscript. However, here you must be careful to only cite studies in mice, unless you are also willing to explain why you would use studies in rats to explain function and studies in mice to explain structure throughout the manuscript). Maybe best would be to cite what you can for evidence for effects of lesions etc on learning, flexibility, decision making etc in mice, and also follow up with citations from the larger behavioral literature on rats. And then comment on the need for more large scale anatomy in rats and more carefully done operant studies in mice.

We agree with the reviewer and have removed the reference to Hunnicutt here. As the reviewer points out most studies supporting three functional subdivisions of the striatum are based on primate and rat research. We have replaced the Hunnicutt reference with the following optogenetic mice studies investigating motor control (Freeze et al., 2013), reward processing (Gunaydin et al., 2014), and reinforcement learning (Kravitz et al., 2012). We have also added the following warning to the striatum section of the Discussion, highlighting the species differences.

“While many studies in mice refer to a three domain striatal model (i.e. associative, limbic, motor), this concept is largely based upon research in non-human primates and rats (Balleine et al., 2009; Thorn et al., 2010; Yin and Knowlton, 2006). Although some anatomical studies in mice are providing greater support for the three domain model (Hunnicutt et al., 2016) there is a need for more operant studies in mice and more large scale anatomical studies in rats.”

5) The conclusions that the connectivity patterns for the nucleus accumbens and motor areas across species, but not the caudate nucleus and anterior putamen is to be expected, and not surprising. The conservation of connectivity profiles across species for the n. accumbens and motor results are consistent with the well-known connections of these two evolutionarily conserved areas that mediate basic functions- appetitive and motor behaviors. The evolutionary expansion of cortex is primarily in cognitive areas, which are directly linked to the caudate and anterior putamen. This should be discussed.

We are pleased that the reviewers agree with our findings. We had previously focussed on evolution from an anatomical perspective, i.e. the evolution of PFC, and cerebellum. However, we believe it is also worth discussing the behavioural changes co-occurring with evolution. As such we have included the following section in the Discussion (striatum section).

“Rather, these results are likely to reflect evolutionary pressures acting upon brain circuits underpinning cognitive behaviours (Passingham and Wise, 2012. […] As such we would caution researchers using mouse models for disorders predominantly affecting these processes as there appears to be less clear translation between mice and humans.”

6) Too little is said about whether or not the target brain areas that constitute the fingerprint can be considered homologous across the three species. The current analysis is just as much a test of the similarities in the striatum as it is a test of whether or not the target areas are similar across species. The degree to which the latter is true will affect the results and conclusions about striatal circuits. I think this deserves a bit more attention in the Discussion.

We agree that a key feature of the translational analysis is the choice of targets. Indeed, this was an aspect of the study we spent a great deal of time on, and Supplementary tables 1-3 in Supplementary file 1 provide citations justifying each of our targets. In the case of human and macaque targets, we used targets that were justified in multiple previously published studies (Mars et al., 2011; 2013; Neubert et al., 2014; 2015; Sallet et al., 2013). The addition of mice forced us to identify which of the established targets was viable in a mouse model, leading us to create both a mouse (reduced) model with 12 targets and a macaque (extended) model with 17 targets. It’s worth noting that in our final analysis (macaque to human) we ran both the extended and reduced models and found that there was very little difference, supporting the idea that our core 12 target model was robust and unlikely to be influenced by 1-2 targets. We have added the following to our Discussion section on methods.

“As well as seed definition, connectivity fingerprint matching relies on the accurate definitions of target areas across species. […] As this line of research continues, it will be possible to refine our target models based on the results of previous studies.”

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Supplementary file 1. Supplementary tables.
    elife-53680-supp1.docx (158.1KB, docx)
    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    The human resting state fMRI data was obtained from the Human Connectome Project (www.humanconnectome.org).

    The following datasets were generated:


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES