Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2020 Apr 20;15(4):e0225358. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0225358

Harnessing digital health to objectively assess cognitive impairment in people undergoing hemodialysis process: The Impact of cognitive impairment on mobility performance measured by wearables

He Zhou 1, Fadwa Al-Ali 2, Changhong Wang 1, Abdullah Hamad 2, Rania Ibrahim 2, Talal Talal 3, Bijan Najafi 1,*
Editor: Luigi Lavorgna4
PMCID: PMC7170239  PMID: 32310944

Abstract

Cognitive impairment is prevalent but still poorly diagnosed in hemodialysis adults, mainly because of the impracticality of current tools. This study examined whether remotely monitoring mobility performance can help identifying digital measures of cognitive impairment in hemodialysis patients. Sixty-nine diabetes mellitus hemodialysis patients (age = 64.1±8.1years, body mass index = 31.7±7.6kg/m2) were recruited. According to the Mini-Mental State Exam, 44 (64%) were determined as cognitive-intact, and 25 (36%) as cognitive-impaired. Mobility performance, including cumulated posture duration (sitting, lying, standing, and walking), daily walking performance (step and unbroken walking bout), as well as postural-transition (daily number and average duration), were measured using a validated pendant-sensor for a continuous period of 24-hour during a non-dialysis day. Motor capacity was quantified by assessing standing balance and gait performance under single-task and dual-task conditions. No between-group difference was observed for the motor capacity. However, the mobility performance was different between groups. The cognitive-impaired group spent significantly higher percentage of time in sitting and lying (Cohens effect size d = 0.78, p = 0.005) but took significantly less daily steps (d = 0.69, p = 0.015) than the cognitive-intact group. The largest effect of reduction in number of postural-transition was observed in walk-to-sit transition (d = 0.65, p = 0.020). Regression models based on demographics, addition of daily walking performance, and addition of other mobility performance metrics, led to area-under-curves of 0.76, 0.78, and 0.93, respectively, for discriminating cognitive-impaired cases. This study suggests that mobility performance metrics could be served as potential digital biomarkers of cognitive impairment among hemodialysis patients. It also highlights the additional value of measuring cumulated posture duration and postural-transition to improve the detection of cognitive impairment. Future studies need to examine potential benefits of mobility performance metrics for early diagnosis of cognitive impairment/dementia and timely intervention.

Introduction

With aging of population, the burden of cognitive impairment appears to increase among patients with end-stage renal disease (ESRD) undergoing hemodialysis (HD) [1, 2]. As more patients of older age receive HD, cognitive impairment has become highly prevalent in this population [36]. At the same time, HD-associated factors can also increase the risk of cognitive impairment and cognitive decline among HD patients [4, 5]. Cognitive impairment leads to overall diminished quality of life and high medical costs associated with coexisting medical conditions and expensive care [7]. Early detection and routine assessment of cognitive function become crucial for delaying further cognitive decline in HD patients [8].

Ideally, HD patients should undergo routine screenings of cognitive function. However, routine assessments using current tools, such as Mini-Mental State Exam, (MMSE) [9], Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) [10], and Trail Making Test (TMT) [11], need be administered in a clinical setting under the supervision of a well-trained professional. Studies have reported that the accuracy and reliability of these screening tools depend on the experience and skills of the examiner, as well as the individual’s educational level [12, 13]. Usually, in a regular dialysis clinic, the nurse does not equip with the professional experience or skills. Regular referral to a neuropsychological clinic could be also impractical as many HD patients have limited mobility, suffer from post-dialysis fatigue, and rarely accept to go to different locations than their regular dialysis clinics for the purpose of cognitive screening. Thus, it is not surprising that emerging literature has demonstrated that although cognitive impairment commonly occurs in HD population, it is still poorly diagnosed [14, 15].

“Mobility performance” depicts enacted mobility in real-life situations [16]. It is different than “motor capacity”, which refers to an individual’s motor function assessed under supervised condition [16]. Mobility performance requires multifaceted coordination between different parts of neuropsychology [17]. This includes motor capacity, intimate knowledge of environment, and difficulty of navigation through changing environments [18]. Understanding the association between mobility performance and cognitive function could help to design an objective tool for remote and potentially early detection of cognitive decline. Previous studies have demonstrated that in older adults, people with cognitive impairment exhibit lower level of activity [1921]. However, in previous studies, the assessment of mobility performance mainly relied on self-reported questionnaires [1921], Actigraphy [22, 23], or accelerometer-derived step count [24]. Although self-reported questionnaire is easy to access without the need of any equipment or device, its main limitation is lacking of objectivity [25]. Previous studies using Actigraphy or step count only provided limited information about mobility performance (activity level and daily step). They also neglected information about posture and postural-transition, which have been demonstrated to be more reliable than activity level or number of daily steps [26]. Considering the motor capacity in patients undergoing HD is usually deteriorated [27], and these patients are highly sedentary with reduced daily activity level [27], it may not be efficient enough to capture cognitive impairment in HD population by just using activity level or step count alone.

In this study, we used a pendant-like wearable sensor to mine potential digital biomarkers from mobility performance for capturing cognitive impairment and tracking the cognitive decline in HD population. We measured detailed metrics of mobility performance including cumulated posture duration (sitting, lying, standing, and walking), daily walking performance (step count and number of unbroken walking bout), as well as postural-transition (daily number and average duration). We hypothesized that 1) HD patients with cognitive impairment have lower mobility performance than those without cognitive impairment; 2) the mobility performance derived digital biomarkers can determine cognitive impairment in HD patients, yielding better results than using daily walking performance alone.

Materials and methods

Study population

This study is a secondary analysis of a clinical trial focused on examining the benefit of exercise in adult HD patients (ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT03076528). The clinical trial was offered to all eligible HD patients visited the Fahad Bin Jassim Kidney Center (Hamad Medical Corporation, Doha, Qatar) for HD process. To be eligible, the subject should be a senior (age 50 years or older), be diagnosed with diabetes and ESRD that require HD, and have capacity to consent. Subjects were excluded if they had major amputation; were non-ambulatory or had severe gait or balance problem (e.g., unable to walk a distance of 15-meter independently with or without assistive device or unable to stand still without moving feet), which may affect their daily physical activity; had active foot ulcer or active infection; had major foot deformity (e.g. Charcot neuroarthropathy); had changes in psychotropic or sleep medications in the past 6-week; were in any active intervention (e.g. exercise intervention); had any clinically significant medical or psychiatric condition; or were unwilling to participate. All subjects signed a written consent approved by the Institutional Review Board at the Hamad Medical Corporation in Doha, Qatar. For the final data analysis, we only included those who had at least 24-hour valid mobility performance data during a non-dialysis day. Only baseline data without any intervention was used for the purpose of this study.

Demographics, clinical data, and motor capacity

Demographics and relevant clinical information for all subjects were collected using chart-review and self-report, including age, gender, height, weight, fall history, duration of HD, and daily number of prescription medicines. Body mass index (BMI) was calculated based on height and weight information.

All subjects underwent clinical assessments, including MMSE [9], Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression scale (CES-D) [28], Physical Frailty Phenotype [29], neuropathy screening using Vibration Perception Threshold test (VPT) [30], vascular assessment using Ankle Brachial Index test (ABI) [31], and glycated hemoglobin test (HbA1c) [32]. The CES-D short-version scale was used to measure self-reported depression symptoms. A cutoff of CES-D score of 16 or greater was used to identify subjects at risk for clinical depression [28]. The Physical Frailty Phenotype, including unintentional weight loss, weakness (grip strength), slow gait speed (15-foot gait test), self-reported exhaustion, and self-reported low physical activity, was used to assess frailty [29]. Subjects with 1 or 2 positive criteria were considered pre-frail, and those with 3 or more positive criteria were considered frail. Subjects negative for all criteria were considered robust [29]. Plantar numbness was evaluated by the VPT measured on six plantar regions of interest, including the left and right great toes, 5th metatarsals, and heels. In this study, we used the maximum value of VPT measures under regions of interest for both feet to evaluate the Diabetic Peripheral Neuropathy (DPN) status. A subject was designated with DPN if his/her maximum VPT reached 25 volts or greater [30]. The ABI was calculated as the ratio of the systolic blood pressure measured at the ankle to the systolic blood pressure measured at the upper arm. A subject was designated with the Peripheral Artery Disease (PAD) if his/her ABI value was either greater than 1.2 or smaller than 0.8 [31].

Motor capacity was quantified by assessing standing balance and walking performance [33]. Standing balance was measured using wearable sensors (LegSysTM, BioSensics LLC., MA, USA) attached to lower back and dominant front lower shin. Subject stood in the upright position, keeping feet close together but not touching, with arms folded across the chest, for 30-second. Center of mass sway (unit: cm2) was calculated using validated algorithms [34]. We assessed walking performance under both single-task and dual-task conditions to determine the impact of cognitive impairment on motor capacity. Walking performance was measured using the same wearable sensors attached to both front lower shins. Subjects were asked to walk with their habitual gait speed for 15-meter with no cognitive task (single-task condition). Then, they were asked to repeat the test while loudly counting backward from a random number (dual-task condition: motor task + working memory) [33]. Gait speeds under both conditions were calculated using validated algorithms [35].

Determination of cognitive impairment

Cognitive impairment was defined as a MMSE score less than 28 as recommended by Tobias et al. and Damian et al. studies [36, 37]. In these studies, researchers have demonstrated that MMSE cutoff score of 28 yields the highest sensitivity and specificity to identify those with cognitive impairment compared to the commonly used lower cutoff scores.

Sensor-derived monitoring of mobility performance

Mobility performance was characterized by 1) cumulated posture duration, including percentage of sitting, lying, standing, and walking postures of 24-hour; 2) daily walking performance, including step count and number of unbroken walking bout (an unbroken walking bout was defined as at least three consecutive steps within 5 seconds interval [38]); and 3) postural-transition, including total number of postural-transition such as sit-to-stand, stand-to-sit, walk-to-stand, stand-to-walk, walk-to-sit (direct transition from walking to sitting with standing pause less than 1 seconds [39]), and sit-to-walk (direct transition from sitting to walking with standing pause less than 1 seconds [39]), as well as average duration of postural-transition (time needed for rising from a chair or sitting on a chair [40]). Mobility performance was recorded for a continuous period of 24-hour using a validated pendant sensor (PAMSysTM, BioSensics LLC., MA, USA, Fig 1) worn during a non-dialysis day. We selected a non-dialysis day because the data during a day of dialysis could be biased by the long period of sitting/lying during HD process and the post dialysis fatigue. The PAMSysTM sensor contains a 3-axis accelerometer (sampling frequency of 50 Hz) and built-in memory for recording long-term data. The description of methods to extract metrics of interest was described in details in our previous studies [3842].

Fig 1. A patient wearing the sensor as a pendant.

Fig 1

Detailed metrics of mobility performance, including cumulated posture duration (sitting, lying, standing, and walking), daily walking performance (step count and number of unbroken walking bout), as well as postural-transition (daily number and average duration), were measured.

Statistical analysis

All continuous data was presented as mean ± standard deviation. All categorical data was expressed as percentage. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used for between-group comparison of continuous demographics and clinical data, as well as mobility performance metrics. Analysis of Chi-square was used for comparison of categorical demographics and clinical data. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was employed to compare differences between groups for motor capacity metrics and mobility performance metrics, with adjustment for age and BMI. A 2-sided p<0.050 was considered to be statistically significant. The effect size for discriminating between groups was estimated using Cohen’s d effect size and represented as d [43]. The Pearson correlation coefficient was used to evaluate the degree of agreement between mobility performance metrics and motor capacity variable for both groups with and without cognitive impairment. The correlation coefficient was also interpret as effect size [43, 44]. A multivariate linear regression model was used to determine the association between mobility performance metrics and MMSE. In this model, MMSE was the dependent variable, and mobility performance metrics and demographics were the independent variables. R2 and p-value were calculated for the multivariate linear regression model. The Pearson correlation coefficient was used to evaluate the degree of agreement between the regression model and MMSE. Further, binary logistic regression analysis was employed to examine the relationship between each study variable and cognitive impairment. First, univariate logistic regression was employed to investigate the relationship of the test variables using “cognitive-impaired/cognitive-intact” as the dependent variable. Nagelkerke R Square (R2), odds ratio (OR), 95% confidence interval (95% CI), and p-value were calculated for each explanatory variable. Second, stepwise multivariate logistic regression, using variables found with p<0.20 in the univariate analysis, was performed to investigate independent effects of variables in predicting cognitive impairment. Then, these variables with independent effects were used to build models for prospective cognitive impairment prediction. In Model 1 (reference model), we only used demographics as independent variables. Then, to examine additional values of mobility performance metrics, two other models were examined. In Model 2, independent variables included demographics and daily walking performance. In Model 3, we added cumulated posture duration and postural-transition as additional independent variables. The receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve and area-under-curve (AUC) were calculated for prediction models. All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics 25 (IBM, IL, USA).

Results

Eighty-one subjects satisfied the inclusion and exclusion criteria of this study. However, the mobility performance data was available and valid for 69 subjects. Reasons of unavailable and invalid mobility performance data were refusal of wearing the sensor (n = 9) and wearing duration less than 24-hour (n = 3). Table 1 summarizes demographics, clinical data, and motor capacity of the remaining subjects. According to the MMSE, 44 subjects (64%) were classified as cognitive-intact, and 25 (36%) were classified as cognitive-impaired. The average MMSE score of the cognitive-impaired group was 22.6±3.7, which was significantly lower than the cognitive-intact group with 29.2±0.9 (p<0.001). The cognitive-impaired group was significantly older than the cognitive-intact group (p = 0.001). Female percentage was significantly higher in the cognitive-impaired group (p = 0.008). The cognitive-impaired group was shorter than the cognitive-intact group (p = 0.009). But there was no between-group difference regarding the BMI. No between-group difference was observed for subjects’ weight, fall history, duration of HD, number of prescription medications, prevalence of at risk for clinical depression, prevalence of frailty and pre-frailty, VPT, prevalence of DPN, prevalence of PAD, and HbA1c (p>0.050). No between group difference was observed for motor capacity metrics including standing balance and walking performance (p>0.050). For the dual-task walking, the cognitive-impaired group had lower dual-task walking speed than the cognitive-intact group. But the difference did not reach statistical significance.

Table 1. Demographics, clinical data, and motor capacity of the study population.

Cognitive-Intact (n = 44) Cognitive-Impaired (n = 25) p-value
Demographics
    Age, years 61.8 ± 6.7 68.1 ± 8.8 0.001*
    Sex (Female), % 43% 76% 0.008*
    Height, m 1.63 ± 0.09 1.50 ± 0.29 0.009*
    Weight, kg 83.4 ± 21.5 76.3 ± 16.6 0.156
    Body Mass Index, kg/m2 31.8 ± 8.6 31.4 ± 5.4 0.804
Clinical data
    Had fall in last 12-month, % 21% 36% 0.158
    Duration of HD, years 4.6 ± 5.4 3.5 ± 2.3 0.354
    Number of prescription medications, n 8 ± 3 8 ± 3 0.233
    Mini-mental State Exam, units on a scale 29.2 ± 0.9 22.6 ± 3.7 <0.001*
    Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression, units on a scale 13.1 ± 6.3 16.0 ± 12.6 0.209
        At risk for clinical depression, % 27% 44% 0.157
    Robust, % 2% 0 0.448
    Pre-frailty & frailty, % 98% 100% 0.448
    Vibration Perception Threshold, V 32.1 ± 16.5 34.6 ± 16.0 0.544
        Diabetic Peripheral Neuropathy, % 61% 68% 0.534
    Peripheral Arterial Disease, % 56% 68% 0.322
    Glycated Hemoglobin, % 6.7 ± 1.5 6.6 ± 1.3 0.783
Motor Capacity
    Static balance (center of mass sway), cm2 0.39 ± 0.38 0.21 ± 0.39 0.087
    Single-task walking speed, m/s 0.49 ± 0.19 0.44 ± 0.20 0.345
    Dual-task walking speed, m/s 0.46 ± 0.19 0.43 ± 0.19 0.682

At risk for clinical depression was assessed by Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression score with a cutoff of 16 or greater

Diabetic Peripheral Neuropathy was assessed by maximum Vibration Perception Threshold value with a cutoff of 25-volt or greater

*: significant difference between groups

†: Results were adjusted by age and BMI

Table 2 summarizes between-group comparison for mobility performance metrics during 24-hour. The cognitive-impaired group spent significantly higher percentage of time in sitting and lying (d = 0.78, p = 0.005, Fig 2) but spent significantly lower percentage of time in standing (d = 0.70, p = 0.010, Fig 2) and walking (d = 0.77, p = 0.007, Fig 2). They also took significantly less steps (d = 0.69, p = 0.015) and unbroken walking bout (d = 0.56, p = 0.048) than the cognitive-intact group. Longer durations of sit-to-stand transition (d = 0.37, p = 0.143) and stand-to-sit transition (d = 0.50, p = 0.044) were observed in the cognitive-impaired group. Significant reductions of number of postural-transition were also observed in the cognitive-impaired group, including total number of transition to walk (d = 0.60, p = 0.035), number of stand-to-walk transition (d = 0.60, p = 0.036), number of walk-to-sit transition (d = 0.65, p = 0.020), total number of transition to stand (d = 0.62, p = 0.024), and number of walk-to-stand transition (d = 0.58, p = 0.044). When results were adjusted by demographic covariates including age and BMI, several mobility performance metrics remained significant for comparing between the cognitive-impaired and cognitive-intact groups (Table 2).

Table 2. Mobility performance (in 24-hour) comparison for cognitive-intact and cognitive-impaired groups.

Cognitive- Intact Cognitive- Impaired Mean Difference % Cohen’s d p-value Adjusted p-value
Cumulated Posture Duration
    Sitting + lying percentage, % 82.0 ± 11.3 89.1 ± 6.3 9% 0.78 0.005* 0.028*
    Standing percentage, % 15.3 ± 9.2 9.9 ± 5.9 -35% 0.70 0.010* 0.061
    Walking percentage, % 2.6 ± 3.0 0.9 ± 0.9 -65% 0.77 0.007* 0.010*
Daily Walking Performance
    Step count, n 1827 ± 2382 608 ± 688 -67% 0.69 0.015* 0.024*
    Number of unbroken walking bout, n 62 ± 85 27 ± 25 -57% 0.56 0.048* 0.083
Postural-transition
    Average duration of stand-to-sit transition, s 2.9 ± 0.2 3.0 ± 0.2 3% 0.37 0.143 0.128
    Average duration of sit-to-stand transition, s 3.0 ± 0.2 3.1 ± 0.3 4% 0.50 0.044* 0.023*
    Total number of transition to walk, n 63 ± 89 24 ± 23 -63% 0.60 0.035* 0.068
        Number of sit-to-walk transition, n 8 ± 8 4 ± 5 -44% 0.51 0.061 0.183
        Number of stand-to-walk transition, n 54 ± 82 19 ± 19 -66% 0.60 0.036* 0.064
    Total number of transition to sit, n 149 ± 71 119 ± 56 -20% 0.46 0.077 0.300
        Number of walk-to-sit transition, n 13 ± 14 6 ±7 -53% 0.65 0.020* 0.039*
        Number of stand-to-sit transition, n 108 ± 64 88 ± 51 -18% 0.34 0.186 0.561
    Total number of transition to stand, n 175 ± 107 121 ± 61 -31% 0.62 0.024* 0.094
        Number of sit-to-stand transition, n 111 ± 68 87 ± 50 -22% 0.40 0.126 0.456
        Number of walk-to-stand transition, n 50 ± 78 17 ± 17 -65% 0.58 0.044* 0.083

Effect sizes were calculated as Cohen’s d

*: significant difference between groups

†: Results were adjusted by age and BMI

Fig 2. Cumulated posture duration (as percentage of 24-hour) for the cognitive-intact group and cognitive-impaired group.

Fig 2

Error bar represents the standard error. “d” denotes the Cohen’s d effect size. “*” denotes when the between-group comparison achieved a statistically significant level (p<0.050).

Fig 3 illustrates the correlation between motor capacity and mobility performance among HD patients with and without cognitive impairment. A significant correlation with medium effect size was observed between single-task walking speed and number of stand-to-sit transition among HD patients without cognitive impairment (r = 0.39, p = 0.012, Fig 3A). But the correlation among cognitive-impaired subjects was insignificant (r = -0.18, p = 0.417). Similarly, a significant correlation with medium effect size was observed between single-task walking speed and number of sit-to-stand transition among HD patients without cognitive impairment (r = 0.42, p = 0.006, Fig 3B). But the correlation was diminished among cognitive-impaired subjects (r = -0.19, p = 0.378).

Fig 3.

Fig 3

Correlations between single-task walking speed and (A) number of stand-to-sit transition and (B) number of sit-to-stand transition among HD patients with and without cognitive impairment.

Results from the multivariate linear regression model (R2 = 0.400, p = 0.019) revealed that “age” (B = -0.225, p<0.001) and “average duration of sit-to-stand transition” (B = -4.768, p = 0.017) were independent predictors of MMSE. A significant correlation with large effect size of r = 0.64 (p<0.001) was determined between the regression model and MMSE (Fig 4).

Fig 4. A significant correlation was observed between the multivariate linear regression model and MMSE.

Fig 4

In the univariate regression analysis, 5 variables in demographics and all variables in the mobility performance were associated with cognitive impairment (p<0.20) (Table 3). Two demographic variables and 11 mobility performance variables remained in the multivariate model suggesting that they are independent predictors (Table 3). These variables were used to build regression models. ROC curves for the 3 models were displayed in Fig 5. The AUC for Model 1 (demographics alone) was 0.76, with a sensitivity of 44.0%, specificity of 88.6%, and accuracy of 72.5% for predicting cognitive impairment. The AUC for Model 2 (demographics + daily walking performance) was 0.78, with a sensitivity of 44.0%, specificity of 79.5%, and accuracy of 66.7% for predicting cognitive impairment. The highest AUC (0.93) was obtained by Model 3 (demographics + daily walking performance + cumulated posture duration + postural-transition), with a sensitivity of 72.0%, specificity of 93.2%, and accuracy of 85.5% for distinguishing cognitive-impaired cases.

Table 3. Results of univariate and multivariate logistic regression.

R2 OR 95% CI p-value
Demographics
    Age 0.190 1.116 1.036–1.201 0.004^
    Sex 0.136 4.167 1.394–12.451 0.011
    Height 0.206 0.917 0.862–0.975 0.006^
    Weight 0.044 0.980 0.952–1.008 0.161
    BMI 0.001 0.992 0.928–1.059 0.800
    Had fall in last 12-month 0.038 2.187 0.730–6.552 0.162
    Duration of HD 0.017 0.940 0.816–1.084 0.396
    Number of prescription medications 0.031 1.116 0.931–1.336 0.235
Cumulated Posture Duration
    Sitting + lying percentage 0.167 1.094 1.022–1.172 0.010^
    Standing percentage 0.141 0.907 0.838–0.982 0.016^
    Walking percentage 0.174 0.642 0.441–0.935 0.021^
Daily Walking Performance
    Step count 0.158 0.999 0.999–1.000 0.027
    Number of unbroken walking bout 0.110 0.986 0.971–1.001 0.066^
Postural-transition
    Average duration of stand-to-sit transition 0.042 4.515 0.583–34.965 0.149
    Average duration of sit-to-stand transition 0.078 7.427 0.975–56.590 0.053^
    Total number of transitions to walk 0.132 0.984 0.968–1.000 0.050^
        Number of sit-to-walk transition 0.078 0.921 0.841–1.008 0.075
        Number of stand-to-walk transition 0.136 0.981 0.963–1.000 0.051^
    Total number of transitions to sit 0.068 0.992 0.983–1.001 0.083^
        Number of walk-to-sit transition 0.121 0.935 0.880–0.994 0.032^
        Number of stand-to-sit transition 0.038 0.994 0.984–1.003 0.190
    Total number of transitions to stand 0.111 0.993 0.986–0.999 0.031
        Number of sit-to-stand transition 0.051 0.993 0.983–1.002 0.133^
        Number of walk-to-stand transition 0.130 0.979 0.959–1.001 0.056^

^: Variables remained in the multivariate model

Fig 5. ROCs of different models for predicting cognitive impairment: Model 1 used “demographics” (AUC = 0.76), Model 2 used a combination of “demographics” and “daily walking performance” (AUC = 0.78), and Model 3 used a combination of “demographics”, “daily walking performance”, “cumulate posture duration”, and “postural-transition” (AUC = 0.93).

Fig 5

Discussions

To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the association between mobility performance and cognitive condition in patients with diabetes and ESRD undergoing HD process. The results suggest that although HD patients with and without cognitive impairment have similar motor capacity, those with cognitive impairment have lower mobility performance. We were able to confirm our hypothesis that mobility performance metrics during a non-dialysis day could be used as potential digital biomarkers of cognitive impairment among HD patients. Specifically, several mobility performance metrics measurable using a pendant sensor enable significant discrimination between those with and without cognitive impairment with medium effect size (maximum Cohen’s d = 0.78). In addition, a metric constructed by the combination of demographics and mobility performance metrics yields a significant correlation with large effect size with the MMSE (r = 0.64, p<0.001). By adding mobility performance together with demographics into the binary logistic regression model, it enables distinguishing between those with and without cognitive impairment. This combined model yields relatively high sensitivity, specificity, and accuracy, which is superior to using demographics alone. Our results also suggest that despite cognitive-impaired HD patients have poor daily walking performance, just monitoring daily walking performance may not be sufficient to distinguish those with cognitive impairment. Additional mobility performance metrics, including cumulated posture duration and postural-transition, could increase the AUC from 0.78 to 0.93 for detection of cognitive-impaired cases.

Previous studies investigating association between mobility performance and cognitive impairment showed that activity level and daily steps are positively associated with cognitive function in older adults [1924]. Results of this study are in line with the previous studies. They showed that cognitive-impaired HD patients have lower walking percentage and step count than cognitive-intact HD patients. Additionally, we found the cognitive-impaired HD patients have less number of postural-transition than cognitive-intact HD patients during daily living. The limited number of postural-transition has been identified as a factor which may contribute to the muscle weakness and activity limitations, causing physical frailty [39, 45]. Frailty together with cognitive impairment (known as ‘cognitive frailty’) has been shown to be a strong and independent predictor of further cognitive decline over time [46, 47].

Mobility performance in daily life depends not only on motor capacity, but also on intact cognitive function and psychosocial factors [48]. Studies have shown that cognitive impairment is associated with reduced mobility performance [4850]. However, an individual’s scores in supervised tests are poorly related to mobility performance in real life [4850]. Results of this study show that among cognitive-intact HD patients, mobility performance is associated with motor capacity. However among HD patients with cognitive impairment, motor capacity is poorly related to mobility performance. This demonstrates that cognitive function is a moderator between motor capacity and mobility performance among patients undergoing HD process. This is aligned with the study of Feld et al. [51], in which it was demonstrated that gait speed does not adequately predict whether stroke survivors would be active in the community. Similar observation was reported by Toosizadeh et al. study [52], in which no agreement between motor capacity and mobility performance was observed among people with Parkinson’s disease, while a significant agreement was observed among age-matched healthy controls.

In previous studies, to better link motor capacity with cognitive decline, dual-task walking test was proposed [53]. By adding cognitive challenges into motor task, the dual-task walking speed can expose cognitive deficits through the evaluation of locomotion. Previous studies have shown that dual-task walking speed for cognitive-impaired older adults was statistically lower than cognitive-intact ones among non-dialysis population [54]. Surprisingly, we didn’t observe significant between-group difference in our sample. A previous systematic review has pointed out that older adults with mobility limitation are more likely to prioritize motor performance over cognitive performance [55]. We speculate that because of the poor motor capacity among HD population, subjects would prioritize motor task over cognitive task. Thus the effect of cognitive impairment may not be noticeable in this motor-impaired population by dual-task walking speed. If this can be confirmed in the follow up study, it may suggest that dual-task paradigm may not be a sufficient test to determine cognitive deficit among population with poor motor capacity.

In this study, we found the cognitive-impaired group had higher percentage of female. This finding is in line with the previous studies [56, 57]. For example, Beam et al. examined gender differences in incidence rates of any dementia, Alzheimer’s disease (AD) alone, and non-Alzheimer’s dementia alone in 16926 women and men in the Swedish Twin Registry aged 65+. They reported that incidence rates of any dementia and AD were greater in women than men, particularly in older ages (age of 80 years and older) [56]. Similarly, Wang et al. suggested that females compared to males showed significantly worse performance in cognitive function [57]. In this study, we did not adjust the results by gender because previous studies have demonstrated that gender does not affect mobility performance in HD population [5861].

A major limitation of this study is the relatively low sample size, which could be underpowered for the clinical conclusion. On the other hand, this study could be considered as a cohort study as all participants were recruited from the Fahad Bin Jassim Kidney Center of Hamad Medical Corporation, which supports the majority of HD patients in the state of Qatar. All eligible subjects who received HD in this center were offered to participate in this study. Another limitation of this study is that mobility performance metrics were only measured in a single non-dialysis day. We excluded mobility performance monitoring during the dialysis day because we anticipated that data could be biased by the long process of HD (often 4-hour). Patients are holding a sitting or lying posture during the HD process. They also suffer the post-dialysis fatigue on the dialysis day. In addition, the measured single-day mobility performance may not be able to accurately represent the condition of HD patients (including both weekdays and weekends). Several previous literature reported three or more days of accelerometry data may more reliably and accurately model mobility performance in adult population [62, 63]. It would be interesting to investigate whether multiple days of monitoring could model mobility performance more accurately in HD patients in the future study, since HD patients may have fluctuation in mobility performance due to post-dialysis fatigue and change of renal function [64].

Conclusion

This study suggests that mobility performance metrics remotely measurable using a pendant sensor during a non-dialysis day could be served as potential digital biomarkers of cognitive impairment among HD patients. Interestingly, motor capacity metrics, even assessed under the cognitively demanding condition, are not sensitive to cognitive impairment among HD patients. Results suggest that despite cognitive-impaired HD patients have poor daily walking performance, just monitoring daily walking performance may not be sufficient to determine cognitive impairment cases. Additional mobility performance metrics such as cumulated posture duration and postural-transition can improve the discriminating power. Further researches are encouraged to evaluate the ability of sensor-derived mobility performance metrics to determine early cognitive impairment or dementia, as well as to track potential change in cognitive impairment over time in response to HD process. Future studies are also recommended for the potential use of sensor-derived metrics to determine modifiable factors, which may contribute in cognitive decline among HD patients.

Acknowledgments

We thank Mincy Mathew, Priya Helena Peterson, Ana Enriquez, and Mona Amirmazaheri for assisting with data collection.

Data Availability

The minimal data set is available from the Data Archiving and Networking Services (DANS) public repository (DOI: https://doi.org/10.17026/dans-xy5-n8c8).

Funding Statement

Support was provided by the Qatar National Research Foundation (Award numbers: NPRP 7-1595-3-405 and NPRP 10-0208-170400). There was no additional external funding received for this study. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the sponsor.

References

  • 1.Wolfgram DF. Filtering the Evidence: Is There a Cognitive Cost of Hemodialysis? Journal of the American Society of Nephrology: JASN. 2018. April;29(4):1087–9. 10.1681/ASN.2018010077 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 5875964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Ying I, Levitt Z, Jassal SV. Should an elderly patient with stage V CKD and dementia be started on dialysis? Clinical journal of the American Society of Nephrology: CJASN. 2014. May;9(5):971–7. 10.2215/CJN.05870513 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 4011441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Sehgal AR, Grey SF, DeOreo PB, Whitehouse PJ. Prevalence, recognition, and implications of mental impairment among hemodialysis patients. American journal of kidney diseases: the official journal of the National Kidney Foundation. 1997. July;30(1):41–9. 10.1016/s0272-6386(97)90563-1 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Murray AM, Tupper DE, Knopman DS, Gilbertson DT, Pederson SL, Li S, et al. Cognitive impairment in hemodialysis patients is common. Neurology. 2006. July 25;67(2):216–23. 10.1212/01.wnl.0000225182.15532.40 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kurella Tamura M, Larive B, Unruh ML, Stokes JB, Nissenson A, Mehta RL, et al. Prevalence and correlates of cognitive impairment in hemodialysis patients: the Frequent Hemodialysis Network trials. Clinical journal of the American Society of Nephrology: CJASN. 2010. August;5(8):1429–38. 10.2215/CJN.01090210 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 2924414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Fazekas G, Fazekas F, Schmidt R, Kapeller P, Offenbacher H, Krejs GJ. Brain MRI findings and cognitive impairment in patients undergoing chronic hemodialysis treatment. Journal of the neurological sciences. 1995. December;134(1–2):83–8. 10.1016/0022-510x(95)00226-7 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Tyrrell J, Paturel L, Cadec B, Capezzali E, Poussin G. Older patients undergoing dialysis treatment: cognitive functioning, depressive mood and health-related quality of life. Aging & mental health. 2005. July;9(4):374–9. 10.1080/13607860500089518 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Bradford A, Kunik ME, Schulz P, Williams SP, Singh H. Missed and delayed diagnosis of dementia in primary care: prevalence and contributing factors. Alzheimer disease and associated disorders. 2009. Oct-Dec;23(4):306–14. 10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181a6bebc . Pubmed Central PMCID: 2787842. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.O'Bryant SE, Humphreys JD, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, Graff-Radford NR, Petersen RC, et al. Detecting dementia with the mini-mental state examination in highly educated individuals. Archives of neurology. 2008. July;65(7):963–7. 10.1001/archneur.65.7.963 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 2587038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bedirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2005. April;53(4):695–9. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Reitan RM. Validity of the Trail Making Test as an Indicator of Organic Brain Damage. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 1958;8(3):271–6. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Ashendorf L, Jefferson AL, O'Connor MK, Chaisson C, Green RC, Stern RA. Trail Making Test errors in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia. Archives of clinical neuropsychology: the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists. 2008. March;23(2):129–37. 10.1016/j.acn.2007.11.005 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 2693196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.van der Vleuten CP, van Luyk SJ, van Ballegooijen AM, Swanson DB. Training and experience of examiners. Medical education. 1989. May;23(3):290–6. 10.1111/j.1365-2923.1989.tb01547.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kurella Tamura M, Yaffe K. Dementia and cognitive impairment in ESRD: diagnostic and therapeutic strategies. Kidney international. 2011. January;79(1):14–22. 10.1038/ki.2010.336 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 3107192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Gesualdo GD, Duarte JG, Zazzetta MS, Kusumota L, Say KG, Pavarini SCI, et al. Cognitive impairment of patients with chronic renal disease on hemodialysis and its relationship with sociodemographic and clinical characteristics. Dementia & neuropsychologia. 2017. Jul-Sep;11(3):221–6. 10.1590/1980-57642016dn11-030003 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 5674665. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Jansen CP, Toosizadeh N, Mohler MJ, Najafi B, Wendel C, Schwenk M. The association between motor capacity and mobility performance: frailty as a moderator. European review of aging and physical activity: official journal of the European Group for Research into Elderly and Physical Activity. 2019;16:16 10.1186/s11556-019-0223-4 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 6787993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Plehn K, Marcopulos BA, McLain CA. The relationship between neuropsychological test performance, social functioning, and instrumental activities of daily living in a sample of rural older adults. The Clinical neuropsychologist. 2004. February;18(1):101–13. 10.1080/13854040490507190 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Yogev-Seligmann G, Hausdorff JM, Giladi N. The role of executive function and attention in gait. Movement disorders: official journal of the Movement Disorder Society. 2008. February 15;23(3):329–42; quiz 472. 10.1002/mds.21720 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 2535903. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Etgen T, Sander D, Huntgeburth U, Poppert H, Forstl H, Bickel H. Physical activity and incident cognitive impairment in elderly persons: the INVADE study. Archives of internal medicine. 2010. January 25;170(2):186–93. 10.1001/archinternmed.2009.498 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Weuve J, Kang JH, Manson JE, Breteler MM, Ware JH, Grodstein F. Physical activity, including walking, and cognitive function in older women. Jama. 2004. September 22;292(12):1454–61. 10.1001/jama.292.12.1454 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Laurin D, Verreault R, Lindsay J, MacPherson K, Rockwood K. Physical activity and risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in elderly persons. Archives of neurology. 2001. March;58(3):498–504. 10.1001/archneur.58.3.498 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Brown BM, Peiffer JJ, Sohrabi HR, Mondal A, Gupta VB, Rainey-Smith SR, et al. Intense physical activity is associated with cognitive performance in the elderly. Translational psychiatry. 2012. November 20;2:e191 10.1038/tp.2012.118 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 3565765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Buchman AS, Wilson RS, Bennett DA. Total daily activity is associated with cognition in older persons. The American journal of geriatric psychiatry: official journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry. 2008. August;16(8):697–701. 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31817945f6 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Blumenthal JA, Smith PJ, Mabe S, Hinderliter A, Welsh-Bohmer K, Browndyke JN, et al. Lifestyle and Neurocognition in Older Adults With Cardiovascular Risk Factors and Cognitive Impairment. Psychosomatic medicine. 2017. Jul-Aug;79(6):719–27. 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000474 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 5493327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Loney T, Standage M, Thompson D, Sebire SJ, Cumming S. Self-report vs. objectively assessed physical activity: which is right for public health? Journal of physical activity & health. 2011. January;8(1):62–70. 10.1123/jpah.8.1.62 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.de Bruin ED, Najafi B, Murer K, Uebelhart D, Aminian K. Quantification of everyday motor function in a geriatric population. Journal of rehabilitation research and development. 2007;44(3):417–28. 10.1682/jrrd.2006.01.0003 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Zhou H, Al-Ali F, Rahemi H, Kulkarni N, Hamad A, Ibrahim R, et al. Hemodialysis Impact on Motor Function beyond Aging and Diabetes-Objectively Assessing Gait and Balance by Wearable Technology. Sensors. 2018. November 14;18(11). 10.3390/s18113939 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 6263479. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Weissman MM, Sholomskas D, Pottenger M, Prusoff BA, Locke BZ. Assessing depressive symptoms in five psychiatric populations: a validation study. American journal of epidemiology. 1977. September;106(3):203–14. 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a112455 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Fried LP, Tangen CM, Walston J, Newman AB, Hirsch C, Gottdiener J, et al. Frailty in older adults: evidence for a phenotype. The journals of gerontology Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences. 2001. March;56(3):M146–56. 10.1093/gerona/56.3.m146 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Bracewell N, Game F, Jeffcoate W, Scammell BE. Clinical evaluation of a new device in the assessment of peripheral sensory neuropathy in diabetes. Diabetic medicine: a journal of the British Diabetic Association. 2012. December;29(12):1553–5. 10.1111/j.1464-5491.2012.03729.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Wyatt MF, Stickrath C, Shah A, Smart A, Hunt J, Casserly IP. Ankle-brachial index performance among internal medicine residents. Vascular medicine. 2010. April;15(2):99–105. 10.1177/1358863X09356015 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Organization WH. Use of glycated haemoglobin (HbA1c) in the diagnosis of diabetes mellitus. 2011. Geneva (Switzerland): The Organization Google Scholar. 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Bahureksa L, Najafi B, Saleh A, Sabbagh M, Coon D, Mohler MJ, et al. The Impact of Mild Cognitive Impairment on Gait and Balance: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Studies Using Instrumented Assessment. Gerontology. 2017;63(1):67–83. 10.1159/000445831 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 5107359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Najafi B, Horn D, Marclay S, Crews RT, Wu S, Wrobel JS. Assessing postural control and postural control strategy in diabetes patients using innovative and wearable technology. Journal of diabetes science and technology. 2010. July 1;4(4):780–91. 10.1177/193229681000400403 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 2909506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Aminian K, Najafi B, Bula C, Leyvraz PF, Robert P. Spatio-temporal parameters of gait measured by an ambulatory system using miniature gyroscopes. Journal of biomechanics. 2002. May;35(5):689–99. 10.1016/s0021-9290(02)00008-8 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Luck T, Then FS, Luppa M, Schroeter ML, Arelin K, Burkhardt R, et al. Association of the apolipoprotein E genotype with memory performance and executive functioning in cognitively intact elderly. Neuropsychology. 2015. May;29(3):382–7. 10.1037/neu0000147 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Damian AM, Jacobson SA, Hentz JG, Belden CM, Shill HA, Sabbagh MN, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment and the mini-mental state examination as screening instruments for cognitive impairment: item analyses and threshold scores. Dementia and geriatric cognitive disorders. 2011;31(2):126–31. 10.1159/000323867 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Najafi B, Aminian K, Paraschiv-Ionescu A, Loew F, Bula CJ, Robert P. Ambulatory system for human motion analysis using a kinematic sensor: monitoring of daily physical activity in the elderly. IEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering. 2003. June;50(6):711–23. 10.1109/TBME.2003.812189 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Parvaneh S, Mohler J, Toosizadeh N, Grewal GS, Najafi B. Postural Transitions during Activities of Daily Living Could Identify Frailty Status: Application of Wearable Technology to Identify Frailty during Unsupervised Condition. Gerontology. 2017;63(5):479–87. 10.1159/000460292 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 5561495. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Najafi B, Aminian K, Loew F, Blanc Y, Robert PA. Measurement of stand-sit and sit-stand transitions using a miniature gyroscope and its application in fall risk evaluation in the elderly. IEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering. 2002. August;49(8):843–51. 10.1109/TBME.2002.800763 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Lindberg CM, Srinivasan K, Gilligan B, Razjouyan J, Lee H, Najafi B, et al. Effects of office workstation type on physical activity and stress. Occup Environ Med. 2018. October;75(10):689–95. 10.1136/oemed-2018-105077 . Pubmed Central PMCID: PMC6166591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Razjouyan J, Naik AD, Horstman MJ, Kunik ME, Amirmazaheri M, Zhou H, et al. Wearable Sensors and the Assessment of Frailty among Vulnerable Older Adults: An Observational Cohort Study. Sensors. 2018. April 26;18(5). 10.3390/s18051336 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 5982667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd Hillsdale, NJ: erlbaum; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Rosenthal JA. Qualitative descriptors of strength of association and effect size. Journal of social service Research. 1996;21(4):37–59. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Bohannon RW. Daily sit-to-stands performed by adults: a systematic review. Journal of physical therapy science. 2015. March;27(3):939–42. 10.1589/jpts.27.939 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 4395748. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Zhou H, Lee H, Lee J, Schwenk M, Najafi B. Motor Planning Error: Toward Measuring Cognitive Frailty in Older Adults Using Wearables. Sensors. 2018. March 20;18(3). 10.3390/s18030926 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 5876674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Zhou H, Razjouyan J, Halder D, Naik AD, Kunik ME, Najafi B. Instrumented Trail-Making Task: Application of Wearable Sensor to Determine Physical Frailty Phenotypes. Gerontology. 2019;65(2):186–97. 10.1159/000493263 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 6426658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Giannouli E, Bock O, Mellone S, Zijlstra W. Mobility in Old Age: Capacity Is Not Performance. BioMed research international. 2016;2016:3261567 10.1155/2016/3261567 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 4789440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Kaspar R, Oswald F, Wahl HW, Voss E, Wettstein M. Daily mood and out-of-home mobility in older adults: does cognitive impairment matter? Journal of applied gerontology: the official journal of the Southern Gerontological Society. 2015. February;34(1):26–47. 10.1177/0733464812466290 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Verhaeghen P, Martin M, Sedek G. Reconnecting cognition in the lab and cognition in real life: The role of compensatory social and motivational factors in explaining how cognition ages in the wild. Neuropsychology, development, and cognition Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition. 2012;19(1–2):1–12. 10.1080/13825585.2011.645009 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 3775600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Feld JA, Zukowski LA, Howard AG, Giuliani CA, Altmann LJP, Najafi B, et al. Relationship Between Dual-Task Gait Speed and Walking Activity Poststroke. Stroke. 2018. May;49(5):1296–8. 10.1161/STROKEAHA.117.019694 . Pubmed Central PMCID: PMC6034633. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Toosizadeh N, Mohler J, Lei H, Parvaneh S, Sherman S, Najafi B. Motor Performance Assessment in Parkinson's Disease: Association between Objective In-Clinic, Objective In-Home, and Subjective/Semi-Objective Measures. PLoS One. 2015;10(4):e0124763 10.1371/journal.pone.0124763 . Pubmed Central PMCID: PMC4409065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Montero-Odasso M, Muir SW, Speechley M. Dual-task complexity affects gait in people with mild cognitive impairment: the interplay between gait variability, dual tasking, and risk of falls. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation. 2012. February;93(2):293–9. 10.1016/j.apmr.2011.08.026 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Muir SW, Speechley M, Wells J, Borrie M, Gopaul K, Montero-Odasso M. Gait assessment in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease: the effect of dual-task challenges across the cognitive spectrum. Gait & posture. 2012. January;35(1):96–100. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.08.014 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Wollesen B, Wanstrath M, van Schooten KS, Delbaere K. A taxonomy of cognitive tasks to evaluate cognitive-motor interference on spatiotemoporal gait parameters in older people: a systematic review and meta-analysis. European review of aging and physical activity: official journal of the European Group for Research into Elderly and Physical Activity. 2019;16:12 10.1186/s11556-019-0218-1 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 6661106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Beam CR, Kaneshiro C, Jang JY, Reynolds CA, Pedersen NL, Gatz M. Differences Between Women and Men in Incidence Rates of Dementia and Alzheimer's Disease. Journal of Alzheimer's disease: JAD. 2018;64(4):1077–83. 10.3233/JAD-180141 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 6226313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Wang L, Tian T, Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging I. Gender Differences in Elderly With Subjective Cognitive Decline. Frontiers in aging neuroscience. 2018;10:166 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00166 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 5994539. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Avesani CM, Trolonge S, Deleaval P, Baria F, Mafra D, Faxen-Irving G, et al. Physical activity and energy expenditure in haemodialysis patients: an international survey. Nephrology, dialysis, transplantation: official publication of the European Dialysis and Transplant Association—European Renal Association. 2012. June;27(6):2430–4. 10.1093/ndt/gfr692 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Johansen KL, Chertow GM, Ng AV, Mulligan K, Carey S, Schoenfeld PY, et al. Physical activity levels in patients on hemodialysis and healthy sedentary controls. Kidney international. 2000. June;57(6):2564–70. 10.1046/j.1523-1755.2000.00116.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Wong SW, Chan YM, Lim TS. Correlates of physical activity level among hemodialysis patients in Selangor, Malaysia. Malaysian journal of nutrition. 2011. December;17(3):277–86. . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Araújo Filho JCd, Amorim CTd, Brito ACNdL, Oliveira DSd, Lemos A, Marinho PÉdM. Physical activity level of patients on hemodialysis: a cross-sectional study. Fisioterapia e Pesquisa. 2016;23(3):234–40. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Hart TL, Swartz AM, Cashin SE, Strath SJ. How many days of monitoring predict physical activity and sedentary behaviour in older adults? The international journal of behavioral nutrition and physical activity. 2011. June 16;8:62 10.1186/1479-5868-8-62 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 3130631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Tudor-Locke C, Burkett L, Reis JP, Ainsworth BE, Macera CA, Wilson DK. How many days of pedometer monitoring predict weekly physical activity in adults? Preventive medicine. 2005. March;40(3):293–8. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.06.003 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Jhamb M, Weisbord SD, Steel JL, Unruh M. Fatigue in patients receiving maintenance dialysis: a review of definitions, measures, and contributing factors. American journal of kidney diseases: the official journal of the National Kidney Foundation. 2008. August;52(2):353–65. 10.1053/j.ajkd.2008.05.005 . Pubmed Central PMCID: 2582327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Luigi Lavorgna

16 Mar 2020

PONE-D-19-30554

Harnessing Digital Health to Objectively Assess Cognitive Impairment in People undergoing Hemodialysis Process: The Impact of Cognitive Impairment on Mobility Performance Measured by Wearables

PL

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by Apr 30 2020 11:59PM. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Luigi Lavorgna

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

1. When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

 

Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. In your Data Availability statement, you have not specified where the minimal data set underlying the results described in your manuscript can be found. PLOS defines a study's minimal data set as the underlying data used to reach the conclusions drawn in the manuscript and any additional data required to replicate the reported study findings in their entirety. All PLOS journals require that the minimal data set be made fully available. For more information about our data policy, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability.

 

Upon re-submitting your revised manuscript, please upload your study’s minimal underlying data set as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and include the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers within your revised cover letter. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories. Any potentially identifying patient information must be fully anonymized.

 

Important: If there are ethical or legal restrictions to sharing your data publicly, please explain these restrictions in detail. Please see our guidelines for more information on what we consider unacceptable restrictions to publicly sharing data: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions. Note that it is not acceptable for the authors to be the sole named individuals responsible for ensuring data access.

 

We will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide in your cover letter.

3. Thank you for stating in your Funding Statement:

"Partial support was provided by the Qatar National Research Foundation (Award number: NPRP 7-1595-3-405 and NPRP 10-0208-170400).".

i) Please provide an amended statement that declares *all* the funding or sources of support (whether external or internal to your organization) received during this study, as detailed online in our guide for authors at http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submit-now.  Please also include the statement “There was no additional external funding received for this study.” in your updated Funding Statement.

ii) Please include your amended Funding Statement within your cover letter. We will change the online submission form on your behalf.

4. Thank you for stating the following in the Competing Interests section:

"None".

We note that one or more of the authors are employed by a commercial company: 'Hamad Medical Corporation'.

1.     Please provide an amended Funding Statement declaring this commercial affiliation, as well as a statement regarding the Role of Funders in your study. If the funding organization did not play a role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript and only provided financial support in the form of authors' salaries and/or research materials, please review your statements relating to the author contributions, and ensure you have specifically and accurately indicated the role(s) that these authors had in your study. You can update author roles in the Author Contributions section of the online submission form.

Please also include the following statement within your amended Funding Statement.

“The funder provided support in the form of salaries for authors [insert relevant initials], but did not have any additional role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. The specific roles of these authors are articulated in the ‘author contributions’ section.”

If your commercial affiliation did play a role in your study, please state and explain this role within your updated Funding Statement.

2. Please also provide an updated Competing Interests Statement declaring this commercial affiliation along with any other relevant declarations relating to employment, consultancy, patents, products in development, or marketed products, etc.  

Within your Competing Interests Statement, please confirm that this commercial affiliation does not alter your adherence to all PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials by including the following statement: "This does not alter our adherence to  PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials.” (as detailed online in our guide for authors http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/competing-interests) . If this adherence statement is not accurate and  there are restrictions on sharing of data and/or materials, please state these. Please note that we cannot proceed with consideration of your article until this information has been declared.

* Please include both an updated Funding Statement and Competing Interests Statement in your cover letter. We will change the online submission form on your behalf.

Please know it is PLOS ONE policy for corresponding authors to declare, on behalf of all authors, all potential competing interests for the purposes of transparency. PLOS defines a competing interest as anything that interferes with, or could reasonably be perceived as interfering with, the full and objective presentation, peer review, editorial decision-making, or publication of research or non-research articles submitted to one of the journals. Competing interests can be financial or non-financial, professional, or personal. Competing interests can arise in relationship to an organization or another person. Please follow this link to our website for more details on competing interests: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/competing-interests

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The study reported in this manuscript is a well-done clinical trial, the conduction of which has followed all the recommended steps by checklists and guidelines, including the registration of its protocol. The statistical analyses are robust and scientifically sound. I would only recommend authors to streamline introduction, that, as it is now, reads quite long.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2020 Apr 20;15(4):e0225358. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0225358.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


31 Mar 2020

Journal Requirements

C1. When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf.

Response: Done

C2. In your Data Availability statement, you have not specified where the minimal data set underlying the results described in your manuscript can be found. PLOS defines a study's minimal data set as the underlying data used to reach the conclusions drawn in the manuscript and any additional data required to replicate the reported study findings in their entirety. All PLOS journals require that the minimal data set be made fully available. For more information about our data policy, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability.

Upon re-submitting your revised manuscript, please upload your study’s minimal underlying data set as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and include the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers within your revised cover letter. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories. Any potentially identifying patient information must be fully anonymized.

Important: If there are ethical or legal restrictions to sharing your data publicly, please explain these restrictions in detail. Please see our guidelines for more information on what we consider unacceptable restrictions to publicly sharing data: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions. Note that it is not acceptable for the authors to be the sole named individuals responsible for ensuring data access.

We will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide in your cover letter.

Response: The de-identified data was uploaded in a public repository: Data Archiving and Networking Services (DANS). https://doi.org/10.17026/dans-xy5-n8c8

Relevant DOI has been added into the revised cover letter.

C3. Please provide an amended statement that declares *all* the funding or sources of support (whether external or internal to your organization) received during this study, as detailed online in our guide for authors at http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submit-now. Please also include the statement “There was no additional external funding received for this study.” in your updated Funding Statement.

Response: Done

Page 22 Line 435: “Support was provided by the Qatar National Research Foundation (Award number: NPRP 7-1595-3-405 and NPRP 10-0208-170400). There was no additional external funding received for this study. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the sponsor.”

C4. Please include your amended Funding Statement within your cover letter. We will change the online submission form on your behalf.

Response: Done.

C5. We note that one or more of the authors are employed by a commercial company: 'Hamad Medical Corporation'.

Please provide an amended Funding Statement declaring this commercial affiliation, as well as a statement regarding the Role of Funders in your study. If the funding organization did not play a role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript and only provided financial support in the form of authors' salaries and/or research materials, please review your statements relating to the author contributions, and ensure you have specifically and accurately indicated the role(s) that these authors had in your study. You can update author roles in the Author Contributions section of the online submission form.

Please also include the following statement within your amended Funding Statement.

“The funder provided support in the form of salaries for authors [insert relevant initials], but did not have any additional role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. The specific roles of these authors are articulated in the ‘author contributions’ section.”

If your commercial affiliation did play a role in your study, please state and explain this role within your updated Funding Statement.

Response: Please note that Hamad Medical Corporation (HMC) is a non-profit public healthcare provider in the State of Qatar (https://www.hamad.qa/EN). None of the co-authors, including authors affiliated with HMC, claimed any conflict of interest.

C6. Please also provide an updated Competing Interests Statement declaring this commercial affiliation along with any other relevant declarations relating to employment, consultancy, patents, products in development, or marketed products, etc.

Within your Competing Interests Statement, please confirm that this commercial affiliation does not alter your adherence to all PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials by including the following statement: "This does not alter our adherence to PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials.” (as detailed online in our guide for authors http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/competing-interests) . If this adherence statement is not accurate and there are restrictions on sharing of data and/or materials, please state these. Please note that we cannot proceed with consideration of your article until this information has been declared.

* Please include both an updated Funding Statement and Competing Interests Statement in your cover letter. We will change the online submission form on your behalf.

Response: As indicated above, none of the co-authors claimed any conflict of interest. HMC is the principal public healthcare provider in the State of Qatar. It is a non-profit organization.

Reviewer 1

R1C1. I would only recommend authors to streamline introduction, that, as it is now, reads quite long.

Response: We refined the Introduction section in the revised manuscript to be more focusing.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response Letter_2020.3.26.docx

Decision Letter 1

Luigi Lavorgna

3 Apr 2020

Harnessing Digital Health to Objectively Assess Cognitive Impairment in People undergoing Hemodialysis Process: The Impact of Cognitive Impairment on Mobility Performance Measured by Wearables

PONE-D-19-30554R1

Dear Dr. Bijan Najafi,

We are pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it complies with all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you will receive an e-mail containing information on the amendments required prior to publication. When all required modifications have been addressed, you will receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will proceed to our production department and be scheduled for publication.

Shortly after the formal acceptance letter is sent, an invoice for payment will follow. To ensure an efficient production and billing process, please log into Editorial Manager at https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the "Update My Information" link at the top of the page, and update your user information. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, you must inform our press team as soon as possible and no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

With kind regards,

Luigi Lavorgna

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Luigi Lavorgna

8 Apr 2020

PONE-D-19-30554R1

Harnessing Digital Health to Objectively Assess Cognitive Impairment in People undergoing Hemodialysis Process: The Impact of Cognitive Impairment on Mobility Performance Measured by Wearables

Dear Dr. Najafi:

I am pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

For any other questions or concerns, please email plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE.

With kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Luigi Lavorgna

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response Letter_2020.3.26.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    The minimal data set is available from the Data Archiving and Networking Services (DANS) public repository (DOI: https://doi.org/10.17026/dans-xy5-n8c8).


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES