Communication between the frontal and medial temporal lobes is impaired in schizophrenia. Adams et al. find that when retrieving memories, subjects with schizophrenia have lower theta phase oscillatory coupling between these areas, and lower α-GABAA receptor availability in the hippocampal area where coupling is lost.
Keywords: schizophrenia, hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, theta, spatial memory
Abstract
Frontotemporal dysconnectivity is a key pathology in schizophrenia. The specific nature of this dysconnectivity is unknown, but animal models imply dysfunctional theta phase coupling between hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). We tested this hypothesis by examining neural dynamics in 18 participants with a schizophrenia diagnosis, both medicated and unmedicated; and 26 age, sex and IQ matched control subjects. All participants completed two tasks known to elicit hippocampal-prefrontal theta coupling: a spatial memory task (during magnetoencephalography) and a memory integration task. In addition, an overlapping group of 33 schizophrenia and 29 control subjects underwent PET to measure the availability of GABAARs expressing the α5 subunit (concentrated on hippocampal somatostatin interneurons). We demonstrate—in the spatial memory task, during memory recall—that theta power increases in left medial temporal lobe (mTL) are impaired in schizophrenia, as is theta phase coupling between mPFC and mTL. Importantly, the latter cannot be explained by theta power changes, head movement, antipsychotics, cannabis use, or IQ, and is not found in other frequency bands. Moreover, mPFC-mTL theta coupling correlated strongly with performance in controls, but not in subjects with schizophrenia, who were mildly impaired at the spatial memory task and no better than chance on the memory integration task. Finally, mTL regions showing reduced phase coupling in schizophrenia magnetoencephalography participants overlapped substantially with areas of diminished α5-GABAAR availability in the wider schizophrenia PET sample. These results indicate that mPFC-mTL dysconnectivity in schizophrenia is due to a loss of theta phase coupling, and imply α5-GABAARs (and the cells that express them) have a role in this process.
Introduction
Schizophrenia—characterized by delusions, hallucinations, and motivational and cognitive impairments—has long been hypothesized to be a disorder of functional brain connectivity (Friston, 1999). Dysconnectivity between the left medial temporal lobe (mTL)—in particular, the hippocampus—and prefrontal cortex is heavily implicated in the disorder (Weinberger et al., 1992; Ellison-Wright and Bullmore, 2009).
Most direct projections from anterior hippocampus to prefrontal cortex in humans (Croxson et al., 2005) and non-human primates (Barbas and Blatt, 1995) terminate in medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). This direct, unidirectional hippocampal-mPFC projection is heavily implicated in functions relevant to psychiatric disorders, e.g. working memory, context-dependent memory, planning, decision-making, and contextual regulation of fear (Godsil et al., 2013). Moreover, both participants with a diagnosis of schizophrenia and their unaffected relatives are substantially impaired at spatial working memory tasks (Park et al., 1995; Fleming et al., 1997; Glahn et al., 2003).
Several rodent (Colgin, 2011) and human (Anderson et al., 2010; Guitart-Masip et al., 2013) electrophysiology studies have shown that functional hippocampal-PFC coupling is strongest in the theta band (∼4–10 Hz in rodents, ∼1–8 Hz in humans) (Jacobs, 2014). Crucially, the strength of theta phase coupling between these regions in rodents also correlates with memory performance for spatial, contextual and reward contingency associations (Jones and Wilson, 2005; Benchenane et al., 2010; Hyman et al., 2010; Kim et al., 2011).
Hippocampal theta is generated by medial septal projections and local interactions between pyramidal cells and interneurons (Colgin, 2013). Oriens-lacunosum/moleculare (OLM) cells (a type of hippocampal interneuron) express somatostatin and fire spontaneously in the theta range, unlike parvalbumin-positive interneurons (Huh et al., 2016). Inhibiting somatostatin (but not parvalbumin) interneurons in mPFC during encoding impairs spatial working memory and also hippocampal-mPFC theta synchrony (Abbas et al., 2018). Importantly, Magnin et al. (2019) showed that somatostatin (but not parvalbumin) interneurons in CA1 express γ-aminobutyric acid type A receptors containing the α5 subunit (α5-GABAARs)—these are abundant in hippocampus and mPFC (Burt et al., 2018)—and that α5-GABAARs modulate spatial memory. Moreover, subjects with schizophrenia show greater post-mortem reductions in somatostatin interneurons than other kinds (at least in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) (Fung et al., 2010), and a deficit in GABA release in mTL (especially if antipsychotic-naïve) (Frankle et al., 2015).
Intriguingly, rodent models of schizophrenia [Df(16)A+/– and maternal immune activation (MIA)] exhibit impaired hippocampal-mPFC theta coupling (Dickerson et al., 2010; Sigurdsson et al., 2010). Crucially, these frequency-specific changes can potentially be linked to biological mechanisms (Uhlhaas and Singer, 2010; Colgin, 2013; Hunt et al., 2017) and thus drug targets. The Df(16)A+/– mouse models the 22q11.2 microdeletion in humans that causes schizophrenia in ∼30% of cases (Karayiorgou et al., 2010). During memory guided decision-making, these animals show decreased hippocampal-mPFC theta coherence, and pretraining theta coherence correlates with subsequent learning speed (Sigurdsson et al., 2010). Similarly, MIA rats—modelling prenatal infection, an environmental risk factor for schizophrenia—have lower hippocampal-mPFC coherence in theta (and other bands), and reduced phase locking of mPFC neuron firing to hippocampal theta (Dickerson et al., 2010). Whether similar deficits in task-related theta coupling between mPFC and hippocampus exist in schizophrenia, however, is currently unknown, as functional MRI and EEG lack the temporal and spatial resolution, respectively to determine this, and no such magnetoencephalography (MEG) study has yet (to our knowledge) been performed.
Here, we addressed this question in participants with diagnoses of schizophrenia, almost half of whom were unmedicated. Control subjects were matched for age, gender and IQ. Our participants performed two tasks, chosen because they are both known to elicit hippocampal-mPFC theta coupling in humans. First, during MEG, participants performed a spatial memory task in which the cued retrieval of object locations has been shown to increase mPFC-mTL theta phase coupling (Kaplan et al., 2014). Second, outside the scanner, participants performed a memory integration task in which the (hippocampal-dependent) (Bunsey and Eichenbaum, 1996) inference of unseen associations (i.e. A-C) between overlapping, observed pairs (i.e. A-B, B-C) is associated with increased mPFC-mTL theta phase coupling during encoding (Backus et al., 2016) and hippocampal-mPFC blood oxygen level-dependent correlations during retrieval (Zeithamova and Preston, 2010). Given the potential importance of somatostatin interneurons in mediating theta coherence, we also measured α5-GABAAR availability (expressed on somatostatin interneurons) in a partly overlapping set of participants with schizophrenia and control subjects using 11C-Ro15-4513 PET—reported in full elsewhere (Marques et al., 2020). 11C-Ro15-4513 is an inverse agonist and has ∼10 times the affinity for α5-GABAARs than for other GABAARs such as α1-GABAARs (Huang et al., 2000) (most commonly expressed on parvalbumin interneurons in sensory areas) (Burt et al., 2018).
To summarize, we predicted that subjects with schizophrenia would exhibit decreased mPFC-mTL theta phase coupling during cued spatial memory retrieval, and that subjects with schizophrenia would be impaired during spatial memory retrieval and memory inference.
Methods and materials
Participants
The study was approved by an NHS research ethics board (REF: 17/LO/0027). All participants gave written informed consent and were compensated for their time. We recruited 18 participants (15 male, three female) who met DSM-IV criteria for schizophrenia from local NHS trusts (11 from early psychosis services). We recruited 35 healthy control subjects (via UCL subject databases and local advertising), matching their age, sex and IQ (at group level) to the schizophrenia group.
General exclusion criteria were: current anticonvulsant or benzodiazepine use, metal in the head or upper body, age >60 years, poor vision even when corrected, and not having been educated in English (for IQ estimation). Control subjects were excluded if they had any history of neurological or psychiatric illness, and subjects with schizophrenia if they had another psychiatric diagnosis, including affective psychosis. Diagnoses were determined by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR axis I disorders (SCID) (First et al., 2007). Participants were asked not to smoke or consume caffeinated drinks on the day of testing.
We recorded the age, sex, handedness, years of education, IQ (Wechsler Test of Adult Reading, WTAR) and Digit Span (forward and backward) of all participants. In subjects with schizophrenia, we also recorded diagnosis, Positive and Negative Symptoms Scale (PANSS) (Kay et al., 1987), antipsychotic medication, and a saliva cannabis test (Supplementary Table 1). Nine controls’ MEG scans were unusable: four had large artefacts, three suffered nausea and stopped, one fell asleep, and in one subject the fiducial markers fell off: leaving 26 controls (19 male, seven female). All schizophrenia participants’ MEG data were usable. One schizophrenia subject did not perform the memory integration task.
Behavioural tasks
The spatial memory task was first used by Doeller et al. (2008) and our version was based on an adaptation of that task by Kaplan et al. (2014). During the task, participants were asked to encode and retrieve the locations of different objects within a series of desktop virtual reality environments (Fig. 1A). The task was created using the Unity game engine (Unity Technologies Ltd). Each virtual reality environment was a flat 100 m sided square arena with a textured ground surface surrounded by a boundary wall and external landmarks projected at infinity. To locate oneself, orientation cues from distant landmarks must be combined with proximity to the boundaries.
Figure 1.
Task structure and behavioural findings. (A) The spatial memory task. In the training phase, participants navigate around the environment using a keypad and must remember the locations of four objects, only one of which is visible in each trial. In the test phase, the subject fixates a grey screen before being shown an image of one object (the cue period). They must then navigate to the remembered location of that object, starting from a random point, and press a button, at which point the object appears in its actual location to provide feedback on their performance. During their navigation to the location, the object is displayed at the top left corner of the screen, to ensure participants don’t forget what they are trying to locate. The next test trial starts when they ‘run over’ the visible object. (B) Left: Individual schizophrenia (Scz) participants’ average performance across all objects was numerically but not significantly worse than controls’ (Con). Right: A histogram showing the (normalized) frequency of average performance centiles for each object location (averaged across three sessions) for subjects with schizophrenia and controls, using translucent bars. Achievement of near ceiling performance is more common in controls than schizophrenia subjects—the proportion of subjects with schizophrenia above the 90th centile of performance was significantly lower than the proportion of controls (χ2 = 7.6, P = 0.006): this comparison was also significant at other high performance thresholds (e.g. 80th or 85th centiles). (C) The memory integration task. During the training phase, participants were first asked to passively view successive pairs of stimuli displayed as text on screen and imagine those stimuli interacting as vividly as possible. Unknown to participants, they viewed two overlapping pairs (e.g. A-B, B-C) on non-consecutive trials that were drawn from a series of 12 events comprising a famous person, location and object. In the test phase, participants were first asked if each stimulus (randomly interleaved with an equal number of novel stimuli) had been presented earlier (testing recognition memory), and if it had been, which other stimulus it had been associated with. Importantly, some associations had been directly experienced (e.g. A-B, B-C) and some had to be inferred (e.g. A-C). (D) Performance on the memory integration task. Top left: Hits, misses, correct rejections and false alarms in the recognition memory test. Bottom left: d’ (or sensitivity) and response bias. Top right: Accuracy on direct and indirect associations, with best fit lines across conditions. Bottom right: Accuracy on indirect associations split by the number of direct associations from that event that the subject answered correctly (i.e. if a subject correctly recalled the direct association ‘Madonna-Bus stop’ but not ‘Bus stop-Knife’, their performance on indirect association ‘Madonna-Knife’ would contribute to the ‘1’ bar). The schizophrenia group are impaired (relative to controls) at indirect associations even when both direct associations have been recalled correctly. This indicates their reduction in indirect accuracy does not simply result from schizophrenia subjects remembering fewer direct associations. *P < 0.05; **P < 0.005.
In the training phase, participants navigate around the environment using a keypad and must remember the locations of four objects (per session), only one of which is visible in each trial. When the participant sees the object, they must approach and collide with (run over) it and then look around to try to encode their location; at this point the current object disappears and a new object appears. All four objects are seen twice each during the training phase.
In the test phase, the subject fixates a grey screen for 3 s (the baseline period) before being shown an image of one object for 3 s (the cue period). They must then navigate to the remembered location of that object, starting from a random point, and press a button, at which point the object appears in its actual location to provide feedback on their performance. The next test trial starts when they navigate to and collide with the visible object. Object locations were drawn pseudorandomly from 16 possible locations: each block contained an object near a corner, one near the middle of a boundary, and two closer to the middle of the space.
Participants completed up to three task sessions that made use of different objects and environments with up to eight test trials for each object in each session, providing a maximum of 32 test trials per session and 96 test trials in total for analysis. Each session lasted 15–30 min, depending on the subject’s speed, with short breaks in between sessions. Some participants did not complete all 32 trials in each session as MEG recording duration was capped at 30 min.
Performance was measured as the distance between the response location and true location of the object, normalized as the percentage of all possible distance errors for that object that were greater than the observed distance error, to account for the fact that objects near boundaries could produce larger errors than those near the centre (Miller et al., 2018).
The memory integration task (Fig. 1C) was adapted from a version used previously (Horner and Burgess, 2013). During the training phase, participants passively viewed pairs of stimuli displayed as white text on a grey background for 6 s, with a 2-s intertrial interval. Participants were instructed to remember these associations by imagining the stimuli interacting as vividly as possible. Unknown to participants, they viewed two overlapping pairs (i.e. A-B, B-C)—on non-consecutive trials—drawn from 12 ‘events’ that comprised a famous person, object and location.
In the test phase, participants were presented with each stimulus, interleaved with 36 new stimuli, and asked whether they had appeared during training (OLD) or not (NEW)—testing recognition memory. Irrespective of their answer, if the stimulus had been presented in the training phase, they were then asked to select which of six stimuli it had been paired with. Importantly, this could test either directly viewed (i.e. A-B, B-C) or indirectly inferred (i.e. A-C) associations. Participants were given 12 s to respond to each question. They performed two blocks, each with a training phase of 24 encoding pairs and a test phase of 54 recognition questions, of which 36 were OLD items, of which 12 were tested on indirect associations. Thus participants were tested on 48 direct and 24 indirect associations in total, lasting ∼30 min including a break. Participants were instructed that they would find the test phase difficult and should make their best guess if they didn’t remember. They were not told that some associations would be indirect.
MEG acquisition, preprocessing and analysis
MEG recordings were made using a 275-channel axial gradiometer system (CTF Omega, VSM MedTech) sampling at 480 Hz while participants sat upright in a magnetically shielded room. Head position coils were attached to nasion and left and right pre-auricular sites for anatomical co-registration. Eye movements were recorded using an Eyelink 1000 eye tracker (SR Research). The data were imported into SPM12 (Litvak et al., 2011) (Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, London, UK) and downsampled to 200 Hz. Eye blink and heartbeat artefacts were identified by ICA and removed using FieldTrip (Oostenveld et al., 2011) and EEGLAB (Delorme and Makeig, 2004). High-pass (1 Hz) and notch (48–52 Hz) filters were applied and the data were epoched and merged across sessions. All SPM images were smoothed using a 12 × 12 × 12 mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel. Trials containing signal artefacts (on average, 14/90 control and 20/88 schizophrenia trials) were identified and removed by visual inspection, resulting in 76 [standard deviation (SD) = 15] trials per control and 68 (SD = 19) per schizophrenia subject.
MEG source localization was conducted using the linearly constrained minimum variance (LCMV) beamformer in SPM12 with a single-shell forward model to generate maps of the mean source power difference between conditions on a 5 mm grid (Barnes and Hillebrand, 2003) co-registered to MNI coordinates (Kaplan et al., 2014). Theta phase in each voxel was extracted by applying the Hilbert transform to the 1–8 Hz band-pass filtered time series generated by the beamformer.
We focused our analyses on a 1–8 Hz theta band because hippocampal theta occupies a broader frequency band than the traditional 3–7 Hz often used in cortical M/EEG studies. In particular, (i) invasively-recorded oscillations in human hippocampus appear to show both low (2–5 Hz) and high (6–9 Hz) theta bands (Bush et al., 2017); (ii) those in the lower theta frequency range seem functionally equivalent to rodent theta (4–10 Hz) (Jacobs, 2014), given their relation to spatial memory encoding (Lega et al., 2012) and retrieval (Watrous et al., 2013; Bohbot et al., 2017); and (iii) Df(16)A+/– mice show abnormal hippocampal-mPFC coupling in both the 1–4 Hz and 4–6 Hz but not the 7–10 Hz ranges. In any case, our group differences were clearly not confined to the 3–7 Hz range (Fig. 2).
Figure 2.
Analysis of sensor-level power in the spatial memory task. (A) Power changes, averaged across all sensors, between the first 1.5 s of the cue and the final 1.5 s of the baseline periods. Differences between groups were apparent at P < 0.0125 (Bonferroni correction for four frequency bands) in the theta band (1–8 Hz, indicated with a box). (B) Scalp plot showing differences in the 1–8 Hz theta band between groups during the same period. Highlighted sensors show significant differences at P < 0.001 (uncorrected). (C) Left: Time frequency plots showing power changes across the sensors highlighted in (B) during the same period following removal of cue period eye movement variance and the average event-related field across all trials. This illustrates the loss of 1–8 Hz theta power (black square) in schizophrenia (Scz) subjects during the cue period. Right: Average 1–8 Hz theta band power changes during the same period across participants following removal of the event-related field and eye movement regression.
All MEG analyses described here compared the first 1.5 s of the cue period with the last 1.5 s of the preceding fixation period (Fig. 2C). As both event-related fields and/or eye movements evoked by the visual cue can confound this comparison, we attempted to eliminate their effects in key analyses. First, the event-related field was removed by subtracting the average waveform across trials from single trial waveforms prior to power analyses. Second, any effect of the eye movement signal identified by ICA was removed by linear regression across trials, with subsequent power and coupling analyses performed on the residual values (Kaplan et al., 2014).
Theta coupling between voxels was estimated using the phase locking value (Lachaux et al., 1999) (PLV), which is defined as the resultant vector length of phase differences between source and seed voxels over time: a larger value indicating reduced variance in the phase difference (and therefore greater phase coupling) between two signals. In previous phase coupling analyses, the seed voxel was chosen to be that with the greatest theta power increase from baseline to cue within 20 mm of the group maximum (Kaplan et al., 2014). Here, we did the same, using the overall average peak (after regressing out eye movements) of (8 52 8). This voxel is in the right medial part of Brodmann area (BA)10 (superior rostral gyrus), and the 20-mm radius encompasses parts of BA10, BA9 and BA32 on both sides. Given we had more control subjects than participants with schizophrenia, the overall average peak could potentially favour positive findings in controls, if it were closer to the average peak location of control subjects rather than participants with schizophrenia. This was not the case, however, as the subjects with schizophrenia average cue-related theta power increase [at (3 53 13)] was slightly closer to the overall average than the controls’ average [at (−2 53 9)]. We also preferred to use the overall average peak rather than group-specific peaks, because (i) the control’s average was much more inferior and thus closer to potential eye-related theta power artefacts; (ii) the overall average was close to the (0 58 22) group peak found in controls in Kaplan et al. (2014); and (iii) we had no reason to suspect different projection sites in controls and subjects with schizophrenia, so it seemed best to pool the data to obtain the most accurate estimate.
Although PLV measures phase coherence, it can be biased by concurrent changes in power that affect the signal-to-noise ratio (Muthukumaraswamy and Singh, 2011). We sought to eliminate this potential confound by linear regression, removing any effect of trial-by-trial variance in (mean-corrected) power across baseline (i.e. fixation) and cue periods and then contrasting the residual PLV values (Kaplan et al., 2014). We removed any linear effect of head movements in each trial on PLV values using the same method.
To identify potential correlations between theta power or PLV and trial-by-trial performance, we extracted beta coefficients for the linear relationship between distance error (rather than the normalized centile measure of performance in Fig. 1B, as these distributions were very skewed) and power (or PLV) and then examined the distribution of beta coefficients in each voxel, across participants, using standard SPM analyses. All mTL SPM results are small volume corrected for a dilated (by one voxel in each direction) bilateral hippocampal and parahippocampal mask (Fig. 3B) generated using the WFU Pick Atlas toolbox (Maldjian et al., 2003).
Figure 3.
Analysis of source localized power in the spatial memory task. All results are overlaid on a canonical Montreal Neurological Institute T1 image. (A) Source localized 1–8 Hz theta power increases during the first 1.5 s of the cue period, compared to an immediately preceding 1.5 s baseline period, for controls and schizophrenia subjects (after regression of eye movement variance). Both groups exhibit significant increases centred on mPFC [controls: peak (14 54 −4), Z = 7.18, PFWE < 0.001; schizophrenia subjects: peak (22 58 12), Z = 4.73, PFWE = 0.003]. The crosshairs in each plot are at (8 52 8), the group peak and seed chosen for subsequent phase coupling analysis. Images are thresholded at PFWE < 0.05. (B) The dilated bilateral hippocampal-parahippocampal gyrus (HC + PHG) mask (Maldjian et al., 2003) used for all mTL small volume correction analyses. (C) Source localized 1–8 Hz theta power increases during the cue period within the HC + PHG mask. Controls showed a large increase in anterior mTL theta power bilaterally (following eye movement regression), particularly on the left [left peak at (−12 0 −12), Z = 5.15, PFWE(SVC) < 0.001, right peak (20 18 −34), Z = 5.42, PFWE(SVC) < 0.001]. Image is thresholded at PFWE < 0.05. Subjects with schizophrenia showed a much smaller increase on the right side (not shown); and control subjects showed a significantly greater increase than schizophrenia subjects on the left [peak (−14 8 −18), Z = 3.21, PFWE(SVC) = 0.019]. The image is thresholded at Punc < 0.005 for display purposes. (D) Relationship between frontal lobe cue-related 1–8 Hz power increase and performance on the more demanding (backward) condition of the Digit Span across participants.
To compute between-subject correlations of behavioural performance and average (not trial-by-trial) cue-related power or PLV changes within a given area, we used SPM to calculate the eigenvariate (in this context, similar to the average) of power or PLV within a mask (defined by the WFU Pick Atlas or by an SPM contrast) for each subject.
PET imaging
Six controls and 11 subjects with schizophrenia from the MEG study also underwent a PET scan using the radiotracer 11C-Ro15-4513 at the Invicro Centre for Imaging Sciences, London. They were a subsample from a larger PET study (29 controls and 31 schizophrenia subjects), whose protocol is reported in Marques et al. (2020). Briefly, all subjects were male and matched in age, and were not taking any psychotropic medication other than antipsychotics; 10 participants with schizophrenia were unmedicated. Dynamic PET scans were acquired over 90 min using a Siemens ECAT EXACT HR scanner (CTI/Siemens, model 962). During the PET acquisition, arterial blood data were sampled via the radial artery, in order to compute blood time-activity curves. High resolution T1-weighted volumes were also acquired using a 3 T MR scanner (Magneton Trio Syngo MR B13 Siemens 3T; Siemens AG), and dynamic PET data were corrected for inter-frame motion and aligned with the individual’s structural T1 MRI by minimizing a mutual information cost function (SPM8). A neuroanatomical atlas was co-registered to each subject’s image space (using MIAKAT, www.miakat.org) and applied to the dynamic PET data to derive regional time-activity curves. α5-GABAAR availability was indexed using the distribution volume (Vt) of 11C-Ro15-4513. As previously (Myers et al., 2012; Horder et al., 2018), Vt was calculated using a metabolite-free arterial plasma input function and the two-tissue compartmental model (2TCM) solved with non-linear least squares at the regional level and the Logan graphical method at the voxel level.
Data availability
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, upon reasonable request.
Results
Behavioural results
Schizophrenia and control groups performed two tasks known to elicit mPFC-hippocampal theta coupling: a spatial memory task during MEG and a memory integration task (without MEG). The groups were well-matched in age, sex and IQ (Supplementary Table 2), although the controls spent more years in education [15.9 versus 14.2, t(42) = 3.3, P = 0.003] and, despite IQ matching, performed better on the more difficult backward condition of the digit span (6.8 versus 5.4, rank test Z = 2.2, P = 0.03).
On the spatial memory task, controls and participants with schizophrenia showed no difference in overall performance (rank test, Z = 1.49, P = 0.14; Fig. 1B, left plot), although fewer schizophrenia subjects than controls performed near ceiling (90th centile) levels for individual object locations (χ2 = 7.6, P = 0.006; Fig. 1B, right plot). The right plot in Fig. 1B is a histogram showing the probability a participant’s average performance (across three sessions) on each of four object locations falls into the given percentile ranges of possible distance errors (see ‘Materials and methods’ section). Given the reasonable performance of the schizophrenia group, we could interpret the imaging results with confidence that they were performing the task similarly to the controls subjects.
Conversely, the schizophrenia group were significantly impaired at the memory integration task. During recognition, they showed increased false alarms [t(41 = 3.2, P = 0.002, Hedges' g = 1.0, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.3–1.6; Fig. 1D] which, despite no increase in misses [t(41 = 1.5, P = 0.16], resulted in lower d′ [t(41 = 3.0, P = 0.006, Hedges' g = 0.9, 95% CI 0.2–1.5; Fig. 1D]—i.e. less separation of ‘signal’ and ‘noise’ distributions. Importantly, this was unlikely to arise from an increased response bias (i.e. participants with schizophrenia always responding ‘OLD’), which was numerically lower than controls’ (Fig. 1D).
In addition, schizophrenia subjects showed impaired memory for both direct and inferred associations [main effect of group, F(1,41) = 12.4, P = 0.001, η2 = 0.23; Fig. 1D], but especially the latter: there was a group × association interaction [F(1,41 = 9.8, P = 0.003, η2 = 0.19] caused by a larger group difference in the inferred [t(41) = 4.0, P = 0.0003, Hedges' g = 1.2, 95% CI 0.5–1.9] than the direct association [t(41) = 2.6, P = 0.013, Hedge’s g = 0.8, 95% CI 0.1–1.4]. Indeed, despite IQ-matching, the schizophrenia group were no better than chance at inferring associations [t(16) = 0.8, P = 0.4], whereas the control group were substantially better [t(25) = 6.2, P = 10−6, Hedges' g = 1.2, 95% CI 0.7–1.7]. Importantly, the schizophrenia group’s impairment at inferred associations was not simply due to poorer memory for direct associations: even if two direct associations (A-B, B-C) were recalled correctly, the schizophrenia group were still worse than the controls at inferring A-C [t(36) = 2.9, P = 0.006, Hedges' g = 0.9, 95% CI 0.2–1.6] and no better than chance [t(14) = 1.8, P = 0.1; Fig. 1D].
MEG power results
We examined task-related changes in theta power and phase coupling during cued spatial memory retrieval (Kaplan et al., 2014). Specifically, we contrasted oscillatory power or coupling during the first 1.5 s of each cue period with the immediately preceding 1.5 s of the fixation period.
First, we found that participants with schizophrenia showed lower cue versus baseline theta power increases across all sensors [t(42) = 2.9, P = 0.006, Hedges' g = 0.9, 95% CI 0.2–1.5; Fig. 2A]. Conversely, there were no significant group differences in alpha (8–14 Hz), beta (15–30 Hz) or gamma (40–90 Hz) after Bonferroni correction. Subsequent analysis of power changes across the scalp revealed significant group differences in theta power located over frontotemporal and temporo-occipital sensors, respectively (Fig. 2B). Time-frequency plots, averaged over these groups of sensors, demonstrate that induced theta power increases were prominent throughout the first 1 s of the cue period in the control group, but much less prominent and sustained in the schizophrenia subjects (Fig. 2C). Importantly, theta power differences could not be accounted for by an event-related field evoked by the visual cue, or by more eye movement during the cue period (see ‘Materials and methods’ section).
Next, we attempted to localize the source of these cue-related theta power increases. We found that both controls and schizophrenia subjects showed robust 1–8 Hz theta power increases with maxima around mPFC [controls: peak (14 54 −4), Z = 7.18, PFWE < 0.001; schizophrenia: peak (22 58 12), Z = 4.73, PFWE = 0.003; Fig. 3A]. Using a dilated bilateral hippocampal and parahippocampal mask (Fig. 3B), we found that controls also showed a robust increase in 1–8 Hz theta power in bilateral mTL [left peak at (−12 0 −12), Z = 5.15, PFWE(SVC) < 0.001, right peak (20 18 −34), Z = 5.42, PFWE(SVC) < 0.001; Fig. 3C], whereas the schizophrenia group only showed a power increase on the right at a very low threshold [peak (30 12 −22), Z = 2.47, Punc = 0.007]. Importantly, the increase in 1–8 Hz theta power in left anterior mTL was significantly higher in controls than schizophrenia subjects [peak (−14 8 −18), Z = 3.21, PFWE(SVC) = 0.019; Fig. 3C).
We assessed whether the smaller increase in theta power during the cue period in the schizophrenia group could be attributed to greater power during the baseline period in that group. When comparing controls and schizophrenia subjects theta power during the baseline period only, no voxels within the mask were significant at Punc < 0.005.
Finally, we asked whether there was any correlation between frontal or mTL theta power increases and performance on the spatial memory task. The controls’ robust mTL theta power increase did not correlate with trial-by-trial performance [Punc > 0.005], and neither did mTL theta power in the schizophrenia group. Schizophrenia subjects’ performance weakly correlated with theta power in cingulate cortex only [peak (−6 22 24), Z = 2.90, Punc = 0.002]. Controls’ performance correlated with theta power in visual and parietal areas (not reported), but not cingulate cortex.
Interestingly, however, the frontal lobe theta power increase correlated with performance on the more demanding (backward) part of the Digit Span across all participants (r = 0.49, P = 0.008; Fig. 3D) and within controls (r = 0.48, P = 0.014) but was not statistically significant within schizophrenia subjects (r = 0.38, P = 0.12). However, frontal theta had no relationship with performance on the spatial memory task.
MEG phase coupling results
Second, we examined changes in theta phase coupling between a subject-specific mPFC seed showing robust power increases during the cue period and the rest of the brain. In controls, two mTL regions showed robust PLV increases (Fig. 4A): left anterior mTL [peak (−26 10 −24), Z = 4.07, PFWE(SVC) = 0.003] and left posterior hippocampus [peak (−36 −44 −2), Z = 3.51, PFWE(SVC) = 0.018]. Conversely, in schizophrenia subjects, no areas within the mask were significant at Punc < 0.005.
Figure 4.
MPFC theta phase coupling analysis. All images are thresholded at Punc < 0.005 for display purposes unless otherwise stated. (A) Controls showed significant increases in 1–8 Hz theta phase coupling (analysed using the PLV) during the cue period in two regions—one in posterior hippocampus [peak (−36 −42 −2), Z = 3.51, PFWE(SVC) = 0.018] and one in anterior mTL [peak (−26 10 −24), Z = 4.07, PFWE(SVC) = 0.003]. Subjects with schizophrenia did not show a significant theta increase in either mTL region. (B) Controls showed greater 1–8 Hz theta PLV than schizophrenia subjects in left anterior mTL, after correcting for trial-by-trial variance in both theta power and head movement and use of antipsychotic medication [peak (−26 10 −22), Z = 3.37, PFWE(SVC) = 0.024]. (C) Eigenvariates (similar to the average) of cue-related PLV increases in the significant region shown in (B) (after theta power regression). The schizophrenia group are divided according to antipsychotic medication use. It is clear that antipsychotics cannot be causing the schizophrenia group’s reduction in PLV as only the schizophrenia group OFF medication show a significant difference in PLV from control subjects (Hedges' g = 1.3, 95% CI 0.4–2.2). The comparison between schizophrenia subjects ON and OFF medication was underpowered and not significant (Hedges' g = 0.8, 95% CI −0.2–1.8). (D) Cue-related theta PLV increase in controls correlated positively with performance in right anterior mTL [peak (32 4 −24), Z = 3.1, PFWE(SVC) = 0.057, Punc = 0.001]. Note: The colour bar scale corresponds to t-statistics in the SPM: here, of beta coefficients resulting from a linear regression of trial-wise performance on PLV on in each voxel.
As predicted, we saw a loss of theta phase coupling with mPFC in schizophrenia subjects: the control versus schizophrenia group difference was significant in left anterior mTL [peak (−26 10 −22), Z = 3.37, PFWE(SVC) = 0.024; Fig. 4B], and was the strongest effect in the whole brain: nowhere else was significant at Punc < 0.001. Moreover, this effect could not be accounted for by trial-by-trial variance in theta power, head movement or antipsychotic medication usage, each of which were included as nuisance regressors. There was also no significant difference in PLV in mTL between control subjects and schizophrenia subjects during only the baseline period [at Punc < 0.005].
Interestingly, however, the difference in mPFC-mTL theta coupling between controls and schizophrenia subjects was weaker when nuisance regressors were omitted from this analysis [PFWE(SVC) = 0.053]. The regressor contributing most to this change was antipsychotic medication. Figure 4C shows the eigenvariates (roughly, averages) of participants’ PLV (after theta power regression) within the left anterior mTL region that showed the peak PLV difference between controls and schizophrenia subjects (Fig. 4B). Only the comparison between controls and participants with schizophrenia OFF medication was significant [t(32) = 3.4, P = 0.002, Hedges' g = 1.3, 95% CI 0.4–2.2; controls versus schizophrenia ON medication t(34) = 0.9, P = 0.4]. Schizophrenia subjects ON versus OFF medication did not show a significant difference in PLV [t(16) = 1.8, P = 0.086, Hedges' g = 0.8, 95% CI −0.2–1.8], although this comparison was clearly underpowered. Importantly, the overall difference in PLV between controls and all schizophrenia subjects (irrespective of medication status) is still large: Hedges' g = 0.7 (95% CI 0.1–1.3). Very similar results were obtained without regression of theta power changes from the PLV.
We asked whether changes in mPFC theta phase coupling during the cue period correlated with task performance. In control subjects, PLV in right mTL increased with performance [peak (32 4 −24), Z = 3.10, PFWE(SVC) = 0.06, Punc = 0.001; Fig. 4D]. This relationship fell far short of statistical significance in the schizophrenia group, however [peak (22 −10 −36), Z = 1.87, Punc = 0.031]. An exploratory analysis revealed the largest cortical PLV increase in schizophrenia subjects arose in a mid-cingulate area [peak (8 −12 30), Z = 3.87, Punc < 0.001], adjacent to the region showing a theta power correlation with performance (Fig. 5A and B). In schizophrenia subjects, across the whole brain, the strongest correlation with performance was in mid-cingulate cortex [peak (8 8 42), Z = 3.13, Punc = 0.001; Fig. 5C]: and this effect was stronger in schizophrenia subjects than in control subjects [peak (2 4 36), Z = 2.81, Punc = 0.002]. Although tentative, this may imply subjects with schizophrenia use the mid-cingulate region (part of the frontoparietal control network A) (Schaefer et al., 2018) to compensate for hippocampal-mPFC circuit dysfunction.
Figure 5.
Exploratory whole-brain analyses revealed theta power and phase coupling effects in a mid-cingulate region in schizophrenia subjects. (A) In a whole-brain analysis, schizophrenia subjects’ (Scz) performance correlated with 1–8 Hz theta power in cingulate cortex only [peak (−6 22 24), Z = 2.90, Punc = 0.002]. The image is thresholded at Punc = 0.005 for display purposes. (B) A whole-brain PLV analysis of the schizophrenia group found the most significant increase in a mid-cingulate area [peak (8 −12 30), Z = 3.87, Punc < 0.001], close to the cingulate area showing a theta power correlation to performance in subjects with schizophrenia. (C) In schizophrenia subjects, PLV increase and performance also correlated in mid-cingulate cortex [peak (6 2 38), Z = 2.82, Punc = 0.002]; the PLV-performance relationship here was stronger in schizophrenia subjects than in controls [peak (2 4 36), Z = 2.81, Punc = 0.002]. Subjects with schizophrenia may thus be using this node of the frontoparietal control network to compensate for their hippocampal-mPFC circuit dysfunction in this task.
We tested whether the schizophrenia group’s ability to increase mPFC-mTL theta phase coupling during the cue period correlated with task performance on the memory integration task. The measure of interest in the latter task was ‘direct condition performance – inferred condition performance’, as this isolates hippocampal function from overall memory impairment. In the schizophrenia group there was a negative relationship between this measure and cue-related PLV increase in the spatial memory task, but it didn’t reach significance (r = −0.41, P = 0.10); there was no relationship between them in controls (r = 0.03, P = 0.9).
We assessed the specificity of mTL phase coupling differences to the theta band by repeating the analyses in the alpha (8–14 Hz), beta (15–30 Hz) and low gamma (40–60 Hz) bands (high gamma being hard to resolve at deep sources). There were no significant group PLV differences even at a liberal significance threshold [Punc < 0.005], and the interaction of group × frequency was significant in the left mTL in a two-way ANOVA [peak (−10 −34 0), Z = 3.16, Punc = 0.001].
We assessed whether symptom severity rather than antipsychotic exposure could underlie the possible influence of medication on theta coupling, as symptoms were numerically but not significantly lower in subjects with schizophrenia ON medication (PANSS total mean = 46, SD = 10) than OFF medication (PANSS total mean = 57, SD = 23). PANSS and PLV did not correlate, however (r = −0.11, P = 0.7; Supplementary Fig. 2). Cannabis use did not appear to affect PLV either: the PLV of the schizophrenia group testing negative (n = 9) was almost identical to that of those testing positive (n = 9) [t(16) = 0.02, P = 1; Supplementary Fig. 3].
PET results
In an attempt to identify a potential molecular mechanism underlying the difference in mTL theta power and mPFC-mTL phase coupling between controls and subjects with schizophrenia, we turned to PET imaging of α5-GABAARs, found on hippocampal interneurons expressing somatostatin. Marques et al. (2020) found a significant reduction in α5-GABAAR availability in a hippocampal region of interest in an overlapping group of schizophrenia subjects (see ‘Materials and methods’ section); the hippocampus was the only region in the brain where α5-GABAAR availability was different in schizophrenia subjects versus controls. We performed a voxel-wise analysis of their whole sample (31 subjects with schizophrenia, 29 control subjects) to see if reduced α5-GABAAR availability in their schizophrenia group overlapped with mTL regions showing reduced theta power and PLV in our schizophrenia group. For this exploratory analysis, we used a permissive significance threshold [Punc < 0.05] for the controls versus schizophrenia subjects α5-GABAAR binding contrast to see how widespread the α5-GABAAR reduction might be. Within the relatively large hippocampal-parahippocampal mask area (8315 voxels, Fig. 3B), the schizophrenia group showed ‘hotspots’ of reduced α5-GABAAR availability, mainly in anterior mTL (Fig. 6A), which overlapped with the areas of reduced theta power (Fig. 3C) and PLV (Fig. 4B) in schizophrenia subjects. The latter was unlikely to be due to chance: for the theta PLV group contrast, 106/146 voxels overlapped with the 1133 voxels showing reduced α5-GABAAR availability (permutation testing: P = 0.013; Fig. 6B). In the theta power group contrast, 39/140 voxels overlapped (P = 0.15). Cue-related change in PLV in the mTL area showing control on versus schizophrenia phase coupling differences (Fig. 4B) did not correlate significantly with α5-GABAAR availability in left hippocampus, however (r = −0.39, P = 0.13), although there were only six controls and 11 subjects with schizophrenia who had both PET and MEG.
Figure 6.
PET analysis. (A) A reanalysis of PET data presented by Marques et al. (2020), who found reduced α5-GABAAR availability in hippocampus in subjects with schizophrenia (Scz). We performed a voxel-wise analysis of these data to look for any overlap between areas of α5-GABAAR reduction and cue-related PLV decrease. At reduced threshold [Punc < 0.05], the schizophrenia group had four areas of reduced α5-GABAAR availability >100 voxels in size: two in right anterior mTL [peak (32 −12 −20), Z = 3.30, Punc < 0.001; peak (28 18 −34), Z = 2.75, Punc = 0.003], one in left anterior mTL [peak (−20 4 −26), Z = 2.40, Punc = 0.008] and one in posterior hippocampus [peak (−20 −34 −12), Z = 2.15, Punc = 0.016]. The third (anterior mTL) area overlapped with the areas of reduced theta power (Fig. 3C, right) and PLV (Fig. 4B) in schizophrenia subjects: the crosshairs are placed at the peak contrast voxel from Fig. 4B. (B) The probabilities of the overlaps between the Ro-15 PET controls > schizophrenia contrast in (A) and the theta power and PLV controls > schizophrenia contrasts in Figs 3C and 4B were assessed using permutation testing (10 000 permutations). This histogram plots the number of overlapping voxels expected by chance: the theta PLV effect overlap is unlikely to be due to chance (P = 0.013).
Discussion
We have shown that mPFC-mTL dysconnectivity during memory retrieval in schizophrenia is characterized by a loss of theta phase coupling. Summarizing our findings, we observed reduced mTL theta power and mPFC-mTL theta phase coupling in subjects with schizophrenia versus controls, specific to the 1–8 Hz frequency band, not caused by antipsychotic medication and unrelated to cannabis use. Furthermore, schizophrenia subjects exhibited mild behavioural impairments in spatial memory performance, but more profound deficits in a hippocampal-dependent memory integration task. Notably, the mTL region showing reduced theta phase coupling in our MEG study coincided with areas of reduced α5-GABAAR availability in an overlapping schizophrenia sample (measured with PET).
Hippocampal-prefrontal coupling in schizophrenia
This study demonstrates a disruption of oscillatory coupling in schizophrenia between mTL and the principle hippocampal-prefrontal projection zone (mPFC), primarily in the (1–8 Hz) theta band (Hedges' g = 0.8).
One might question why the schizophrenia group did not show more marked impairments in the spatial memory task, given their loss of mPFC-mTL coupling during that task, and their profound impairment at the indirect inference in the memory integration task, which is known to depend on mPFC-mTL coupling. One reason may be that the indirect inference is very dependent on recurrent hippocampal circuitry (Horner et al., 2015; Banino et al., 2016), whereas neighbouring mTL areas (or even mid-cingulate cortex; Fig. 5) can contribute to spatial memory task performance. Indeed, epilepsy patients with hippocampal resections can still perform a similar spatial memory task (Vikbladh et al., 2019). Interestingly, patients with hippocampal lesions are more impaired at precise positional memory (d > 1) than general object-location memory (d < 0.5) (Kessels et al., 2001): a pattern shared by our schizophrenia group, who were under-represented in the highest centiles of accuracy (Fig. 1B).
Another reason for the schizophrenia performance differences between tasks may be that spatial tasks are more reliant on right hippocampus (Kessels et al., 2001)—as the correlation of our controls’ performance with PLV in right mTL suggests—whereas the memory integration effect is strongest in left hippocampus (Backus et al., 2016). Although hippocampal dysfunction is likely bilateral in schizophrenia subjects (Hanlon et al., 2011), hippocampal pathology is greater on the left (Harrison, 2004). If mPFC-mTL communication is worse on the left in schizophrenia, as our results imply, this could also explain the performance differences in schizophrenia subjects on these tasks, and the lack of relationship between memory integration performance and mPFC-mTL coupling in the other task.
We had predicted that mPFC-mTL coupling would correlate with spatial memory performance in both groups: this relationship was only significant in controls, however. This may be because the schizophrenia group compensated by using other circuits to perform the task: we found some preliminary evidence of this, in that their performance correlated with cue-related theta power and PLV in a mid-cingulate control area. Controls did not seem to use this area to perform the task.
Consistent with our results, reduced hippocampal-mPFC functional connectivity is seen in resting state functional MRI data in subjects with schizophrenia (Zhou et al., 2008), and hippocampal-prefrontal functional connectivity is reduced in antipsychotic-naïve first episode schizophrenia but normalizes following treatment (Blessing et al., 2019). Also, a resting state MEG study found reduced right hippocampal coupling (in theta) to the rest of the medial temporal network in healthy participants with the schizophrenia risk alleles of the ZNF804A polymorphism (Cousijn et al., 2015).
There is also a large literature on abnormal interactions between hippocampus and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. These areas display functional abnormalities during working memory tasks in schizophrenia subjects, high-risk populations, healthy participants with schizophrenia risk alleles, and animal models (Bähner and Meyer-Lindenberg, 2017). Indeed, subjects with the 22q11.2 microdeletion show abnormal hippocampal-frontoparietal network functional connectivity in resting state functional MRI (Schleifer et al., 2019).
Numerous EEG studies have found task-related theta power loss in schizophrenia, e.g. during Go-No Go tasks (Simmonite et al., 2015) and in oddball responses (Javitt et al., 2018), which involve hippocampal-prefrontal areas in MEG (Recasens et al., 2018). Reduced theta coupling has also been found in schizophrenia subjects, e.g. between fronto-temporal regions during talking (Ford et al., 2002) and recognition memory (Thézé et al., 2019), between fronto-parietal regions in a working memory task (Griesmayr et al., 2014), within PFC during the continuous performance task (Ryman et al., 2018), and between cingulate and sensorimotor cortex during the Stroop task (Popov et al., 2019). Our findings advance this literature by demonstrating impaired theta coupling with a deep mTL source that is key to schizophrenia pathology and present in animal models, but which EEG cannot resolve.
Interestingly, resting state EEG studies often show increased theta coupling in schizophrenia (Andreou et al., 2015; Di Lorenzo et al., 2015; Gomez-Pilar et al., 2018; Krukow et al., 2018)—although not always (Tauscher et al., 1998; Koenig et al., 2001)—indicating that task-baseline theta connectivity differences in schizophrenia may be due to both an increased baseline and reduced effects of the task. We did not find increased theta power or coupling in mTL at baseline in schizophrenia subjects, however.
Comparing schizophrenia results with animal studies
Our findings in subjects with schizophrenia recapitulate key aspects of the Df(16)A+/– mouse model: both show a loss of mTL-mPFC coupling during retrieval in the low end of the broader theta (1–8 Hz) range, rather than a broadband loss of coupling (as in the MIA model). Nevertheless, the MIA (and neonatal ventral hippocampal lesion) models suggest that antipsychotics may rescue hippocampal-mPFC theta coherence (Dickerson et al., 2012; Lee et al., 2014). Our underpowered comparison of schizophrenia ON and OFF antipsychotics was not statistically significant, but the numerical difference in coupling between these groups was in the same direction as that found in these animal models. The main difference between the Df(16)A+/– and schizophrenia results was that only the latter showed a loss of task-related theta power in mTL.
Hippocampal-mPFC theta coupling correlates with spatial memory performance in rodents (Jones and Wilson, 2005) and here we demonstrated this in humans for the first time: a previous attempt to do so may have lacked sufficient power (Kaplan et al., 2014). The Df(16)A+/– mice are impaired in the task acquisition phase (Stark et al., 2008), whereas schizophrenia subjects’ performance is less frequently at ceiling levels (similar to reward learning tasks in which performance differences between subjects with schizophrenia and controls are greatest at the easiest conditions, when controls perform at ceiling) (Gold et al., 2012).
Other schizophrenia rodent models have also shown disturbances of hippocampal theta, e.g. in theta synchrony within hippocampus (Kaefer et al., 2019), loss of theta phase coupling with PFC (Del Pino et al., 2013), or in ‘hyper-coupling’ to PFC (Hartung et al., 2016; Bygrave et al., 2019), or decoupling during sleep (Phillips et al., 2012), indicating theta dysfunction in this circuit may be common to many genetic and environmental risk factors for schizophrenia and other psychiatric disorders (Godsil et al., 2013).
The co-localization of phase coupling loss with reduced α5-GABAAR availability in a wider schizophrenia sample warrants further investigation. One key question—if they are related—is whether α5-GABAAR reduction is pathological or compensatory. Theoretically it could be either: α5-GABAARs phasically inhibit somatostatin interneurons but also tonically inhibit pyramidal neurons, and blocking the latter in mice improves spatial memory (Magnin et al., 2019). Marques et al. (2020) found that α5-GABAAR availability in unmedicated schizophrenia, while reduced overall, correlated positively with total symptoms (PANSS).
Recognition and associative memory in schizophrenia
The schizophrenia group showed a striking impairment (Hedges' g = 1.2) in indirect inference in the memory integration task, which may depend on hippocampal-mPFC theta coupling (Backus et al., 2016), although we could not establish such a relationship across tasks. The indirect ‘A-C’ inference is impaired in chronic (Armstrong et al., 2012a, b) and first episode schizophrenia (Armstrong et al., 2018) (although our sample is the first IQ-matched comparison), and relational memory impairments in schizophrenia relate to hippocampal dysfunction (Ragland et al., 2015). Other hippocampal-dependent cognitive operations, such as transitive inference, are also impaired in schizophrenia subjects and in unaffected relatives (Titone et al., 2004; Onwuameze et al., 2016) although not necessarily in early psychosis (Williams et al., 2012).
The higher false alarm rate (but unchanged hit rate) in subjects with schizophrenia (Hedges' g = 1.0) in the recognition memory test matches findings in recognition (Weiss et al., 2004) and spatial working memory (Mayer and Park, 2012; Starc et al., 2017). In a population sample performing an auditory perceptual task, false alarm rate correlated with auditory hallucinations (de Boer et al., 2019). Specific increases in false alarms are predicted by disinhibition in neural network models (Murray et al., 2014) and GABA blockade in PFC in rats selectively increases false alarms in an attentional task (Auger et al., 2017) similar to schizophrenia (Demeter et al., 2013).
Limitations
We attempted to exclude any potential confounds of antipsychotic medication or cannabis use, and remove variance associated with linear effects of eye movements (on theta power) and both theta power and head movements (on theta phase coupling). Nevertheless, non-linear effects of these variables will remain. Furthermore, the hippocampus also contains nicotinic cholinergic receptors (nAChRs) (Dannenberg et al., 2017), which can modulate theta power in hippocampal slices (Lu and Henderson, 2010) and following brainstem stimulation in anaesthetized mice (Stoiljkovic et al., 2016). Thus it is possible—given schizophrenia subjects generally smoke more than controls, and that we asked subjects to refrain from smoking prior to testing—that the schizophrenia group might have had more nicotine-withdrawal effects than the control subjects, which may have contributed to group differences in theta power. Whether nAChRs can also modulate theta coupling is unknown, but future studies should attempt to counterbalance nicotine effects.
Volume conduction is also a concern in source-reconstructed data (e.g. whether ‘hippocampal’ theta actually originates in PFC), but this paradigm and source localization using both MEG and functional MRI in the same subjects confirmed the localization of hippocampal activity (Kaplan et al., 2012). Increases in hippocampal theta power in this paradigm have also been seen using intracranial EEG (Bush et al., 2017).
The numbers of subjects with both PET and MEG imaging was small: the exploratory finding of overlap in areas of reduced α5-GABAAR availability and reduced PLV in mTL in schizophrenia needs to be replicated in a larger sample of subjects who have all had both MEG and PET investigations. It is also unclear whether the relationship between these findings is causal.
Conclusion
We have shown that mPFC-mTL dysconnectivity during memory retrieval in schizophrenia is characterized by a loss of theta (1–8 Hz) phase coupling. This coupling loss in schizophrenia was not explained by the co-existing loss of mTL theta power, head movements, antipsychotic medication or cannabis use. The schizophrenia group were also impaired at two memory tasks involving mPFC-mTL interaction, although more work is needed to understand the relationships between performance and theta coupling in schizophrenia. These findings are important as they were predicted by a genetic schizophrenia mouse model, which could be used for translational studies of pharmacological targets that modulate theta and thus cognition. As the site of coupling differences overlapped with areas of reduced α5-GABAAR availability in a larger schizophrenia sample, and as these receptors participate in the generation of theta oscillations, the relationship between them warrants further investigation.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
The authors are very grateful to Faith Borgan, Ilinca Angelescu, Andrew Watson, Nicholas Green, and Lynis Lewis (Noclor) for their help in recruiting participants with a schizophrenia diagnosis, and also to the imaging support staff at the Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, in particular Daniel Bates and David Bradbury.
Funding
R.A.A. is an MRC Skills Development Fellow (MR/S007806/1) and has also been supported by the Academy of Medical Sciences (AMS-SGCL13-Adams), the National Institute of Health Research (CL-2013-18-003), and the NIHR UCLH Biomedical Research Centre. R.K. was supported by a Sir Henry Wellcome Fellowship (101261/Z/13/Z). The PET study was funded by grants from the Medical Research Council UK (MC-A656-5QD30), Maudsley Charity (no. 666), Brain and Behavior Research Foundation, and Wellcome Trust (094849/Z/10/Z) to O.D.H. N.B. is a Wellcome Principal Research Fellow (202805/Z/16/Z) and is supported by an ERC advanced grant (NEUROMEM).
Competing interests
T.R.M. has received honoraria for speaking and chairing from Lundbeck, Jansenn and Astellas, and received honoraria to participate in advisory boards organized by Angelini Pharmaceuticals. O.D.H. has received investigator-initiated research funding from and/or participated in advisory/speaker meetings organized by Astra-Zeneca, BMS, Eli Lilly, Jansenn, Lundbeck, Lyden-Delta, Servier, and Roche. Neither O.D.H. nor his family have been employed by or have holdings/a financial stake in any biomedical company.
Glossary
- MEG =
magnetoencephalography
- mPFC =
medial prefrontal cortex
- mTL =
medial temporal lobe
- PANSS =
Positive and Negative Symptoms Scale
- PLV =
phase locking value
References
- Abbas AI, Sundiang MJM, Henoch B, Morton MP, Bolkan SS, Park AJ, et al. Somatostatin interneurons facilitate hippocampal-prefrontal synchrony and prefrontal spatial encoding. Neuron 2018; 100: 926–39.e3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson KL, Rajagovindan R, Ghacibeh GA, Meador KJ, Ding M.. Theta oscillations mediate interaction between prefrontal cortex and medial temporal lobe in human memory. Cereb Cortex 2010; 20: 1604–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andreou C, Leicht G, Nolte G, Polomac N, Moritz S, Karow A, et al. Resting-state theta-band connectivity and verbal memory in schizophrenia and in the high-risk state. Schizophr Res 2015; 161: 299–307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Armstrong K, Avery S, Blackford JU, Woodward N, Heckers S.. Impaired associative inference in the early stage of psychosis. Schizophr Res 2018; 202: 86–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Armstrong K, Kose S, Williams L, Woolard A, Heckers S.. Impaired associative inference in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 2012a; 38: 622–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Armstrong K, Williams LE, Heckers S.. Revised associative inference paradigm confirms relational memory impairment in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 2012b; 26: 451–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Auger ML, Meccia J, Floresco SB.. Regulation of sustained attention, false alarm responding and implementation of conditional rules by prefrontal GABAA transmission: comparison with NMDA transmission. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2017; 234: 2777–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Backus AR, Schoffelen J-M, Szebényi S, Hanslmayr S, Doeller CF.. Hippocampal-prefrontal theta oscillations support memory integration. Curr Biol 2016; 26: 450–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bähner F, Meyer-Lindenberg A.. Hippocampal-prefrontal connectivity as a translational phenotype for schizophrenia. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2017; 27: 93–106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Banino A, Koster R, Hassabis D, Kumaran D.. Retrieval-based model accounts for striking profile of episodic memory and generalization. Sci Rep 2016; 6: 31330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbas H, Blatt GJ.. Topographically specific hippocampal projections target functionally distinct prefrontal areas in the rhesus monkey. Hippocampus 1995; 5: 511–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes GR, Hillebrand A.. Statistical flattening of MEG beamformer images. Hum Brain Mapp 2003; 18: 1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benchenane K, Peyrache A, Khamassi M, Tierney PL, Gioanni Y, Battaglia FP, et al. Coherent theta oscillations and reorganization of spike timing in the hippocampal- prefrontal network upon learning. Neuron 2010; 66: 921–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blessing EM, Murty VP, Zeng B, Wang J, Davachi L, Goff DC.. Anterior hippocampal-cortical functional connectivity distinguishes antipsychotic naïve first-episode psychosis patients from controls and may predict response to second-generation antipsychotic treatment. Schizophr Bull 2019. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbz076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Boer JN, Linszen MMJ, de Vries J, Schutte MJL, Begemann MJH, Heringa SM, et al. Auditory hallucinations, top-down processing and language perception: a general population study. Psychol Med 2019; 49: 2772–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bohbot VD, Copara MS, Gotman J, Ekstrom AD.. Low-frequency theta oscillations in the human hippocampus during real-world and virtual navigation. Nat Commun 2017; 8: 14415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bunsey M, Eichenbaum H.. Conservation of hippocampal memory function in rats and humans. Nature 1996; 379: 255–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burt JB, Demirtaş M, Eckner WJ, Navejar NM, Ji JL, Martin WJ, et al. Hierarchy of transcriptomic specialization across human cortex captured by structural neuroimaging topography. Nat Neurosci 2018; 21: 1251–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bush D, Bisby JA, Bird CM, Gollwitzer S, Rodionov R, Diehl B, et al. Human hippocampal theta power indicates movement onset and distance travelled. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2017; 114: 12297–302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bygrave AM, Jahans-Price T, Wolff AR, Sprengel R, Kullmann DM, Bannerman DM, et al. Hippocampal-prefrontal coherence mediates working memory and selective attention at distinct frequency bands and provides a causal link between schizophrenia and its risk gene GRIA1. Transl Psychiatry 2019; 9: 142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colgin LL. Oscillations and hippocampal-prefrontal synchrony. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2011; 21: 467–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colgin LL. Mechanisms and functions of theta rhythms. Annu Rev Neurosci 2013; 36: 295–312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cousijn H, Tunbridge EM, Rolinski M, Wallis G, Colclough GL, Woolrich MW, et al. Modulation of hippocampal theta and hippocampal-prefrontal cortex function by a schizophrenia risk gene. Hum Brain Mapp 2015; 36: 2387–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Croxson PL, Johansen-Berg H, Behrens TEJ, Robson MD, Pinsk MA, Gross CG, et al. Quantitative investigation of connections of the prefrontal cortex in the human and macaque using probabilistic diffusion tractography. J Neurosci 2005; 25: 8854–66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dannenberg H, Young K, Hasselmo M.. Modulation of hippocampal circuits by muscarinic and nicotinic receptors. Front Neural Circuits 2017; 11: 102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Del Pino I, García-Frigola C, Dehorter N, Brotons-Mas JR, Alvarez-Salvado E, Martínez de Lagrán M, et al. Erbb4 deletion from fast-spiking interneurons causes schizophrenia-like phenotypes. Neuron 2013; 79: 1152–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delorme A, Makeig S.. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods 2004; 134: 9–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demeter E, Guthrie SK, Taylor SF, Sarter M, Lustig C.. Increased distractor vulnerability but preserved vigilance in patients with schizophrenia: evidence from a translational Sustained Attention Task. Schizophr Res 2013; 144: 136–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Lorenzo G, Daverio A, Ferrentino F, Santarnecchi E, Ciabattini F, Monaco L, et al. Altered resting-state EEG source functional connectivity in schizophrenia: the effect of illness duration. Front Hum Neurosci 2015; 9: 234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickerson DD, Restieaux AM, Bilkey DK.. Clozapine administration ameliorates disrupted long-range synchrony in a neurodevelopmental animal model of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2012; 135: 112–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickerson DD, Wolff AR, Bilkey DK.. Abnormal long-range neural synchrony in a maternal immune activation animal model of schizophrenia. J Neurosci 2010; 30: 12424–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doeller CF, King JA, Burgess N.. Parallel striatal and hippocampal systems for landmarks and boundaries in spatial memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2008; 105: 5915–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellison-Wright I, Bullmore E.. Meta-analysis of diffusion tensor imaging studies in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2009; 108: 3–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- First MB, Williams JB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M.. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I disorders, clinical trials version (SCID-CT). N Y Biom Res N Y State Psychiatr Inst 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Fleming K, Goldberg TE, Binks S, Randolph C, Gold JM, Weinberger DR.. Visuospatial working memory in patients with schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1997; 41: 43–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ford JM, Mathalon DH, Whitfield S, Faustman WO, Roth WT.. Reduced communication between frontal and temporal lobes during talking in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2002; 51: 485–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frankle WG, Cho RY, Prasad KM, Mason NS, Paris J, Himes ML, et al. In vivo measurement of GABA transmission in healthy subjects and schizophrenia patients. Am J Psychiatry 2015; 172: 1148–59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ. Schizophrenia and the disconnection hypothesis. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1999; 99: 68–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fung SJ, Webster MJ, Sivagnanasundaram S, Duncan C, Elashoff M, Weickert CS.. Expression of interneuron markers in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the developing human and in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167: 1479–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glahn DC, Therman S, Manninen M, Huttunen M, Kaprio J, Lönnqvist J, et al. Spatial working memory as an endophenotype for schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2003; 53: 624–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Godsil BP, Kiss JP, Spedding M, Jay TM.. The hippocampal-prefrontal pathway: the weak link in psychiatric disorders? Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2013; 23: 1165–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gold JM, Waltz JA, Matveeva TM, Kasanova Z, Strauss GP, Herbener ES, et al. Negative symptoms and the failure to represent the expected reward value of actions: behavioral and computational modeling evidence. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2012; 69: 129–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gomez-Pilar J, de Luis-García R, Lubeiro A, de la Red H, Poza J, Núñez P, et al. Relations between structural and EEG-based graph metrics in healthy controls and schizophrenia patients. Hum Brain Mapp 2018; 39: 3152–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griesmayr B, Berger B, Stelzig-Schoeler R, Aichhorn W, Bergmann J, Sauseng P.. EEG theta phase coupling during executive control of visual working memory investigated in individuals with schizophrenia and in healthy controls. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2014; 14: 1340–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guitart-Masip M, Barnes GR, Horner A, Bauer M, Dolan RJ, Duzel E.. Synchronization of medial temporal lobe and prefrontal rhythms in human decision making. J Neurosci 2013; 33: 442–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanlon FM, Houck JM, Pyeatt CJ, Lundy SL, Euler MJ, Weisend MP, et al. Bilateral hippocampal dysfunction in schizophrenia. Neuroimage 2011; 58: 1158–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrison PJ. The hippocampus in schizophrenia: a review of the neuropathological evidence and its pathophysiological implications. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004; 174: 151–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartung H, Cichon N, De Feo V, Riemann S, Schildt S, Lindemann C, et al. From shortage to surge: a developmental switch in hippocampal-prefrontal coupling in a gene-environment model of neuropsychiatric disorders. Cereb Cortex 2016; 26: 4265–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horder J, Andersson M, Mendez MA, Singh N, Tangen Ä, Lundberg J, et al. GABAA receptor availability is not altered in adults with autism spectrum disorder or in mouse models. Sci Transl Med 2018; 10. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.aam8434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horner AJ, Bisby JA, Bush D, Lin W-J, Burgess N.. Evidence for holistic episodic recollection via hippocampal pattern completion. Nat Commun 2015; 6: 7462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horner AJ, Burgess N.. The associative structure of memory for multi-element events. J Exp Psychol Gen 2013; 142: 1370–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang Q, He X, Ma C, Liu R, Yu S, Dayer CA, et al. Pharmacophore/receptor models for GABA(A)/BzR subtypes (alpha1beta3gamma2, alpha5beta3gamma2, and alpha6beta3gamma2) via a comprehensive ligand-mapping approach. J Med Chem 2000; 43: 71–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huh CYL, Amilhon B, Ferguson KA, Manseau F, Torres-Platas SG, Peach JP, et al. Excitatory inputs determine phase-locking strength and spike-timing of CA1 stratum oriens/alveus parvalbumin and somatostatin interneurons during intrinsically generated hippocampal theta rhythm. J Neurosci 2016; 36: 6605–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunt MJ, Kopell NJ, Traub RD, Whittington MA.. Aberrant network activity in schizophrenia. Trends Neurosci 2017; 40: 371–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyman JM, Zilli EA, Paley AM, Hasselmo ME.. Working memory performance correlates with prefrontal-hippocampal theta interactions but not with prefrontal neuron firing rates. Front Integr Neurosci 2010; 4: 2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobs J. Hippocampal theta oscillations are slower in humans than in rodents: implications for models of spatial navigation and memory. Phil Trans R Soc B 2014; 369: 20130304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Javitt DC, Lee M, Kantrowitz JT, Martinez A.. Mismatch negativity as a biomarker of theta band oscillatory dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2018; 191: 51–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones MW, Wilson MA.. Theta rhythms coordinate hippocampal-prefrontal interactions in a spatial memory task. PLoS Biol 2005; 3: e402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaefer K, Malagon-Vina H, Dickerson DD, O’Neill J, Trossbach SV, Korth C, et al. Disrupted-in-schizophrenia 1 overexpression disrupts hippocampal coding and oscillatory synchronization. Hippocampus 2019; 29: 802–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaplan R, Bush D, Bonnefond M, Bandettini PA, Barnes GR, Doeller CF, et al. Medial prefrontal theta phase coupling during spatial memory retrieval. Hippocampus 2014; 24: 656–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaplan R, Doeller CF, Barnes GR, Litvak V, Düzel E, Bandettini PA, et al. Movement-related theta rhythm in humans: coordinating self-directed hippocampal learning. PLoS Biol 2012; 10: e1001267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karayiorgou M, Simon TJ, Gogos JA.. 22q11.2 microdeletions: linking DNA structural variation to brain dysfunction and schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci 2010; 11: 402–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opfer LA.. The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1987; 13: 261–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessels RP, de Haan EH, Kappelle LJ, Postma A.. Varieties of human spatial memory: a meta-analysis on the effects of hippocampal lesions. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 2001; 35: 295–303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim J, Delcasso S, Lee I.. Neural correlates of object-in-place learning in hippocampus and prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 2011; 31: 16991–7006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koenig T, Lehmann D, Saito N, Kuginuki T, Kinoshita T, Koukkou M.. Decreased functional connectivity of EEG theta-frequency activity in first-episode, neuroleptic-naïve patients with schizophrenia: preliminary results. Schizophr Res 2001; 50: 55–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krukow P, Jonak K, Karakuła-Juchnowicz H, Podkowiński A, Jonak K, Borys M, et al. Disturbed functional connectivity within the left prefrontal cortex and sensorimotor areas predicts impaired cognitive speed in patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 2018; 275: 28–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lachaux JP, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J, Varela FJ.. Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Hum Brain Mapp 1999; 8: 194–208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee H, Dvorak D, Fenton AA.. Targeting neural synchrony deficits is sufficient to improve cognition in a schizophrenia-related neurodevelopmental model. Front Psychiatry 2014; 5: 15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lega BC, Jacobs J, Kahana M.. Human hippocampal theta oscillations and the formation of episodic memories. Hippocampus 2012; 22: 748–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Litvak V, Mattout J, Kiebel S, Phillips C, Henson R, Kilner J, et al. EEG and MEG data analysis in SPM8. Comput Intell Neurosci 2011; 2011: 852961. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu CB, Henderson Z.. Nicotine induction of theta frequency oscillations in rodent hippocampus in vitro. Neuroscience 2010; 166: 84–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magnin E, Francavilla R, Amalyan S, Gervais E, David LS, Luo X, et al. Input-specific synaptic location and function of the α5 GABAA receptor subunit in the mouse CA1 hippocampal neurons. J Neurosci 2019; 39: 788–801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maldjian JA, Laurienti PJ, Kraft RA, Burdette JH.. An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. Neuroimage 2003; 19: 1233–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marques TR, Ashok A, Angelescu I, Borgan F, Myers J, Lingford-Hughes A, et al. GABA-A receptor differences in Schizophrenia: A Positron Emission Tomography study using [11C]Ro154513. Mol Psychiatry 2020. Available from: 10.1038/s41380-020-0711-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayer JS, Park S.. Working memory encoding and false memory in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder in a spatial delayed response task. J Abnorm Psychol 2012; 121: 784–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller J, Watrous AJ, Tsitsiklis M, Lee SA, Sheth SA, Schevon CA, et al. Lateralized hippocampal oscillations underlie distinct aspects of human spatial memory and navigation. Nat Commun 2018; 9: 2423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray JD, Anticevic A, Gancsos M, Ichinose M, Corlett PR, Krystal JH, et al. Linking microcircuit dysfunction to cognitive impairment: effects of disinhibition associated with schizophrenia in a cortical working memory model. Cereb Cortex 2014; 24: 859–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthukumaraswamy SD, Singh KD.. A cautionary note on the interpretation of phase-locking estimates with concurrent changes in power. Clin Neurophysiol 2011; 122: 2324–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers JFM, Rosso L, Watson BJ, Wilson SJ, Kalk NJ, Clementi N, et al. Characterisation of the contribution of the GABA-benzodiazepine α1 receptor subtype to [(11)C]Ro15-4513 PET images. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 2012; 32: 731–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Onwuameze OE, Titone D, Ho B-C.. Transitive inference deficits in unaffected biological relatives of schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Res 2016; 175: 64–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, Schoffelen J-M.. FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput Intell Neurosci 2011; 2011: 156869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park S, Holzman PS, Goldman-Rakic PS.. Spatial working memory deficits in the relatives of schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995; 52: 821–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips KG, Bartsch U, McCarthy AP, Edgar DM, Tricklebank MD, Wafford KA, et al. Decoupling of sleep-dependent cortical and hippocampal interactions in a neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia. Neuron 2012; 76: 526–33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Popov T, Kustermann T, Popova P, Miller GA, Rockstroh B.. Oscillatory brain dynamics supporting impaired Stroop task performance in schizophrenia-spectrum disorder. Schizophr Res 2019; 204: 146–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ragland JD, Ranganath C, Harms MP, Barch DM, Gold JM, Layher E, et al. Functional and neuroanatomic specificity of episodic memory dysfunction in schizophrenia: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of the relational and item-specific encoding task. JAMA Psychiatry 2015; 72: 909–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Recasens M, Gross J, Uhlhaas PJ.. Low-frequency oscillatory correlates of auditory predictive processing in cortical-subcortical networks: a MEG-study. Sci Rep 2018; 8: 14007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryman SG, Cavanagh JF, Wertz CJ, Shaff NA, Dodd AB, Stevens B, et al. Impaired midline theta power and connectivity during proactive cognitive control in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2018; 84: 675–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaefer A, Kong R, Gordon EM, Laumann TO, Zuo X-N, Holmes AJ, et al. Local-global parcellation of the human cerebral cortex from intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Cereb Cortex 2018; 28: 3095–114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schleifer C, Lin A, Kushan L, Ji JL, Yang G, Bearden CE, et al. Dissociable disruptions in thalamic and hippocampal resting-state functional connectivity in youth with 22q11.2 deletions. J Neurosci 2019; 39: 1301–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sigurdsson T, Stark KL, Karayiorgou M, Gogos JA, Gordon JA.. Impaired hippocampal-prefrontal synchrony in a genetic mouse model of schizophrenia. Nature 2010; 464: 763–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simmonite M, Bates AT, Groom M, Hollis C, Liddle PF.. Reduced event-related low frequency EEG activity in patients with early onset schizophrenia and their unaffected siblings. Psychiatry Res 2015; 232: 51–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Starc M, Murray JD, Santamauro N, Savic A, Diehl C, Cho YT, et al. Schizophrenia is associated with a pattern of spatial working memory deficits consistent with cortical disinhibition. Schizophr Res 2017; 181: 107–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stark KL, Xu B, Bagchi A, Lai W-S, Liu H, Hsu R, et al. Altered brain microRNA biogenesis contributes to phenotypic deficits in a 22q11-deletion mouse model. Nat Genet 2008; 40: 751–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoiljkovic M, Kelley C, Nagy D, Leventhal L, Hajós M.. Selective activation of α7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors augments hippocampal oscillations. Neuropharmacology 2016; 110: 102–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tauscher J, Fischer P, Neumeister A, Rappelsberger P, Kasper S.. Low frontal electroencephalographic coherence in neuroleptic-free schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychiatry 1998; 44: 438–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thézé R, Manuel AL, Pedrazzini E, Chantraine F, Patru MC, Nahum L, et al. Neural correlates of reality filtering in schizophrenia spectrum disorder. Schizophr Res 2019; 204: 214–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Titone D, Ditman T, Holzman PS, Eichenbaum H, Levy DL.. Transitive inference in schizophrenia: impairments in relational memory organization. Schizophr Res 2004; 68: 235–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W.. Abnormal neural oscillations and synchrony in schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci 2010; 11: 100–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vikbladh OM, Meager MR, King J, Blackmon K, Devinsky O, Shohamy D, et al. Hippocampal contributions to model-based planning and spatial memory. Neuron 2019; 102: 683–93.e4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watrous AJ, Tandon N, Conner CR, Pieters T, Ekstrom AD.. Frequency-specific network connectivity increases underlie accurate spatiotemporal memory retrieval. Nat Neurosci 2013; 16: 349–56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinberger DR, Berman KF, Suddath R, Torrey EF.. Evidence of dysfunction of a prefrontal-limbic network in schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance imaging and regional cerebral blood flow study of discordant monozygotic twins. Am J Psychiatry 1992; 149: 890–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss AP, Zalesak M, DeWitt I, Goff D, Kunkel L, Heckers S.. Impaired hippocampal function during the detection of novel words in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2004; 55: 668–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams LE, Avery SN, Woolard AA, Heckers S.. Intact relational memory and normal hippocampal structure in the early stage of psychosis. Biol Psychiatry 2012; 71: 105–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeithamova D, Preston AR.. Flexible memories: differential roles for medial temporal lobe and prefrontal cortex in cross-episode binding. J Neurosci 2010; 30: 14676–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou Y, Shu N, Liu Y, Song M, Hao Y, Liu H, et al. Altered resting-state functional connectivity and anatomical connectivity of hippocampus in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2008; 100: 120–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, upon reasonable request.