Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2020 Apr 28;10:7105. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-62917-9

The relationship between chronotype and intelligence: the importance of work timing

Péter P Ujma 1,2, Tanja G Baudson 3,4, Róbert Bódizs 1,2,, Martin Dresler 5
PMCID: PMC7188801  PMID: 32345987

Abstract

Sleep-wake patterns show substantial biological determination, but they are also subject to individual choice and societal pressure. Some evidence suggests that high IQ is associated with later sleep patterns. However, it is unclear whether the relationship between IQ and later sleep is due to biological or social effects, such as the timing of working hours. We investigated the association between habitual sleep timing during work days and work-free days, working time and membership in Mensa, an organization of highly intelligent individuals (IQ  ≥130) using a sample of 1,172 adults split between Mensa members and age- and sex-matched volunteers from a large web-based database. We found no difference in chronotype, and the later sleep timing of Mensa members on work days was fully accounted for by later work start times. Our results indicate that later sleep timing in those with higher IQs is not due to physiological differences, but rather due to later work schedules. Later working times and the resulting lower social jetlag may be one of the reasons why higher IQ is associated with lower prospective morbidity and mortality.

Subject terms: Circadian rhythms and sleep, Psychology, Intelligence

Introduction

Sleep-wake patterns depend on genetic effects, social pressure and previously accumulated sleep debt1. Previous research has established a potential link between sleep-wake timing or chronotype (phase of entrainment to the day-night cycle) and IQ, but it is unclear whether this effect is biological or social—that is, whether more intelligent people are biologically (most likely genetically) predisposed to sleep at a different time, or whether this is a consequence of environmental factors. A large study2 of 15,197 respondents from the Add Health longitudinal study showed a small but significant linear relationship between childhood IQ and self-reported bedtimes and wake-up times between the ages of 18–28 years (r = 0.013–0.053). A meta-analysis of the literature up to 20113 excluding the Kanazawa et al. study found a similar association between diurnal preference and cognitive ability (morningness: N = 2,177, r = −0.042; eveningness N = 1519, r = 0.075), with no evidence for publication bias. Some recent work, however, found weak positive associations between an earlier chronotype and intelligence in school-age children and adolescents46. Additionally, very large genome-wide association studies7,8 have also revealed that cognitive ability and morningness are in a negative genetic correlation—that is, genetic variants associated with an earlier chronotype are also associated with lower cognitive ability (rg = −0.15–0.17). Paradoxically, academic performance, itself moderately correlated with IQ9, is positively correlated with morningness3,10.

Almost all previous research was conducted with adolescents or young adults, and most studies did not systematically measure work day and free day sleep-wake preferences and lifestyle factors which may influence this relationship. Specifically, it is currently unknown whether the relationship between IQ and sleep-wake timing reflects individual differences in the biological chronotype or rather in social influences (such as different work and school schedules or differences in the use of stimulants or hypnotics). Even genetic studies are not exempt from this, due to the not always causative nature of genetic correlations11.

The goal of our study was to specify to what extent sleep time differences as a function of IQ are mediated by the different social environments of high-IQ individuals. We assessed the IQ–sleep timing relationship using a sample of working age, high-IQ adults and age- and sex-matched controls with data about both work day and free day sleep timings and work schedules. We compared the self-reported sleep-wake patterns of adult German Mensa (IQ ≥ 130) members to an age- and sex-matched random sample of the MCTQ database12 (Ntotal = 1172, mean age: 38.2 years). In doing so, we investigated differences in sleep timing on work days and weekends separately in order to separate the preferred chronotype from the effects of socially pressure on sleep-wake timing1, and specifically investigated the role of work schedules and additional lifestyle variables.

Results

We compared the self-reported sleep-wake timing of adult German Mensa (IQ > 130) members to an age- and sex-matched random sample of the MCTQ database12 (Ntotal = 1172).

Mensa members were characterized by later lights-off, sleep onset midsleep and wake-up times as well as shorter sleep latency, but only during work days. During free days, Mensa members still experienced less sleep latency and longer waking inertia (time in bed after waking up), but there was no difference in the main measures in sleep timing. Mensa members suffered from less social jetlag (but not after correcting for oversleeping) and less weekly sleep deprivation, despite having slightly more work days on average. Importantly, Mensa members had nearly a standard deviation less light exposure, both on free days and working days. Group averages, sample sizes, standard deviations as well as t-values and significance levels are reported in Table 1.

Table 1.

A comparison of Mensa and control samples in terms of chronotype-related and miscellaneous variables.

N (Control) N (Mensa) M (Control) M (Mensa) SD (Control) SD (Mensa) Cohen’s d p
Work day Age (years) 586 569 38.17 38.22 10.66 10.74 <0.01 0.94
Bedtime (hmidnight) 577 518 −0.69 −0.65 1.39 1.28 −0.03 0.67
Sleep preparation (hmidnight) 577 518 −0.56 −0.31 1.21 1.28 −0.20 <0.001
Sleep latency (min) 576 517 16.99 13.36 16.92 15.32 0.22 <0.001
Sleep onset (hmidnight) 577 517 −0.27 −0.09 1.25 1.32 −0.14 0.02
Wake time (hmidnight) 578 516 6.69 6.91 1.21 1.31 −0.17 <0.001
Waking inertia (min) 574 517 15.98 15.41 16.54 17.08 0.03 0.57
Sleep duration (h) 577 518 6.98 6.99 1.07 1.07 −0.01 0.89
Midsleep (hmidnight) 576 517 3.21 3.40 1.10 1.22 −0.17 0.01
Free day Bedtime (hmidnight) 585 518 0.44 0.28 1.74 1.44 0.10 0.11
Sleep preparation (hmidnight) 585 518 0.54 0.58 1.66 1.44 −0.02 0.69
Sleep latency (min) 580 517 13.92 11.61 14.53 13.17 0.17 0.01
Sleep onset (hmidnight) 574 517 0.71 0.76 1.51 1.48 −0.04 0.56
Wake time (hmidnight) 570 515 8.67 8.75 1.71 1.72 −0.04 0.48
Waking inertia (min) 578 512 19.40 24.19 18.87 22.58 −0.23 <0.001
Sleep duration (h) 571 518 8.03 7.98 1.48 1.32 0.03 0.59
Midsleep (hmidnight) 573 518 4.77 4.77 1.63 1.51 0.02 0.94
Chronotype (corrected midsleep, hmidnight) 551 485 4.30 4.32 1.39 1.37 −0.02 0.75
Light exposure (work day, h) 571 516 2.54 1.12 1.93 0.97 0.92 <0.001
Light exposure (free day, h) 574 517 3.37 2.06 1.86 1.31 0.81 <0.001
Social jetlag (h) 551 485 1.58 1.39 1.06 0.97 0.19 <0.001
Social jetlag (h), corrected for oversleeping 552 485 1.23 1.14 0.87 0.79 0.11 0.08
Sleep deprivation/week (h) 552 484 1.97 1.61 1.85 1.52 0.21 <0.001
Workdays/week 564 485 4.92 5.16 0.77 0.75 −0.31 <0.001

Statistical significance is derived from independent sample t-tests. Bold p-values indicate differences which were significant after correcting for multiple comparisons. Effect sizes are reported in Cohen’s d, and positive signs correspond to higher values in controls. hmidnight indicates that the value is a time point expressed in fractions of hours before (negative) or after (positive) midnight. Chronotype refers to free day midsleep corrected for oversleeping.

So far, our results were in line with some of the literature indicating generally later sleep timing among higher IQ adult participants; however, this difference was limited to work days and not present in the corrected chronotype. Next, we used linear regression models to test whether later work day sleep patterns were accounted for by non-endogenous variables, including workplace characteristics (work start and commute duration) and substance use (consumption of alcohol, cigarettes, caffeine, and hypnotics) (Table 2).

Table 2.

Results of linear regression models with work day midsleep timing as the dependent variable.

Model Independents β p
1 Mensa 0.096 0.009
2 Mensa 0.036 0.204
Work start time 0.626 <0.001
Commute duration −0.150 <0.001
3 Mensa 0.044 0.116
Work start time 0.630 <0.001
Commute duration −0.143 <0.001
Cigarettes/day 0.094 0.002
Beer/day −0.010 0.732
Glass wine/day −0.023 0.426
Liquor/day 0.032 0.252
Coffee/day 0.014 0.624
Caffeine drink/day 0.071 0.014
Tea/day −0.023 0.404
Hypnotics/day 0.023 0.403

Positive standardized regression coefficients for “Mensa” indicate later midsleep timing in Mensa members. Model 3 incorporates the self-reported number of various alcoholic beverages, coffee and other caffeinated beverages, cigarettes and hypnotics medication consumed on a typical day.

Data on all variables were available for 480 Mensa members and 271 controls. Supplementary Table S1 reports descriptive statistics for all covariates by subgroup and Table 3 reports subgroup differences. Supplementary Table S2 provides the correlation matrix of covariates.

Work start timing and commute duration were the strongest predictors of work day midsleep timing, and later work start times (MMensa = 8:35, SD = 1:18; MControl = 8:21, SD = 1:21, values indicating hours and minutes) among Mensa members alone fully mediated the association between Mensa membership and work day midsleep timing (Table 2).

In sum, we found no statistically significant differences between the chronotype of highly intelligent individuals and matched controls. Sleep timing differences on work days were fully accounted for by the later work schedules of Mensa members.

Discussion

We assessed sleep timing differences between highly intelligent subjects and matched controls. We did find later midsleep timing and waking time in Mensa members, but this was limited to work days, and even then, it was fully accounted for by later work start times. We found no subgroup difference in chronotype. Thus, it appears that at least in the current sample earlier sleep timing in those with lower IQs is not due to physiological differences, but rather a function of earlier work schedules. Much higher levels of both working day and free day light exposure was found in control participants. While our database does not allow the empirical investigation of the causes of this, we speculate that since IQ is associated with job prestige13, extremely intelligent individuals may be overrepresented in office jobs and underrepresented in outdoor jobs, such as agriculture or construction. Free-day differences might be explained by the well-documented1417 concentration of better educated or more intelligent individuals in urban areas, where outdoors activities are potentially less available or less attractive. In any case, despite large differences in self-reported light-exposure, subgroup differences in sleep timing were modest and limited to work days.

Our findings are partially at odds with some of the previous literature, which generally reported negative associations between an early chronotype and intelligence in young adults2,3, including pleiotropic genetic effects working in opposite directions18. This discrepancy may be accounted for by two study design features which were uniform in most of the previous literature but differed in ours. First, all previous studies used adolescent or very young adult (≤25 years old) samples, while in ours, participants (38 years on average) were closer to middle age. Second, all studies referenced in the Preckel et al. meta-analysis used questionnaires (typically the MEQ, the LOCI [Lark-Owl Chronotype Indicator], or the CSM) to obtain a single measure of morningness and/or eveningness. These questionnaires—while containing questions about both preferred and usual activity patterns and being correlated with other measures such as the MCTQ19—do not strictly separate work day and weekend habits, in contrast to the MCTQ used in our study. Combining work day and weekend sleep timing into a general chronotype measure offers a limited opportunity to distinguish individual differences due to biological preferences (most prominent on weekends) and those due to societal pressure (most prominent on work days). Notably, the study of Kanazawa et al.2, which enquired about work day and free day bedtimes separately, is an exception to this. This contradiction might arise from several factors, including self-report inaccuracy (as wake times were quite late, on average 10:00 AM or later on weekends and later than 7:00 AM on work days, with some inaccuracies such as frequent 12:00 PM [noon] bedtimes manually corrected by the authors) and age-specific effects as all respondents were very young (<30 years old). Further possibilities are that IQ–sleep timing associations are culture-specific, and while brighter individuals might indeed prefer sleeping later in the United States, they do not do so in Germany, supporting the role of non-endogenous environmental factors such as differences in societal pressure in creating a later chronotype in brighter individuals. A similar caveat applies to the evidence for a negative genetic correlation (rg = 0.15–0.17) between morningness and IQ18. The genetic correlation between morningness and IQ is based on summary genetic association data from a chronotype GWAS7, in which morningness was assessed by respondents’ self-report of being a “morning/evening person” on a subjective Likert-type scale. Genetic correlations do not necessarily implicate a common biological mechanism. They may arise from biological pleiotropy if the same genes are implicated in the actual biological processes regulating two phenotypes, but they may also arise from mediated pleiotropy, in which one trait is caused by an environmental influence which in turn is caused by the other, heritable trait20. Our results suggest that the negative genetic correlation between morningness and IQ may be a case of mediated rather than biological pleiotropy: genetic effects resulting in higher IQs do not cause lower morningness because these traits share their biological underpinnings, but because heritable high intelligence exposes individuals to environments – such as later or more permissive work schedules—in which an early sleep-wake pattern is less preferred. Future studies using Mendelian Randomization (MR) on genetic data may test this hypothesis, which would be supported by a unidirectional causal path from IQ-related genetic variants to sleep timing. We are aware of one such study21, which found minimal causal influence of education on chronotype using an education polygenic score which is also associated with intelligence22.

Our study suffers from a number of limitations. First, due to its cross-sectional design we had a limited ability to assess causation. Because IQ is stable throughout the lifetime23 and highly heritable24, and employees are usually not free to choose when their work day starts, we believe that it is more likely that the correlation between IQ and sleep timing as well as work start and sleep timing reflects the effect of the former on the latter than vice versa, but only longitudinal designs could test this reliably. Second, our sample may have been underpowered to detect very small associations between IQ and free day sleep timing or chronotype. Third, we did not have IQ data available from our control sample, therefore, we are unable to perform fine-grained analyses within the normal IQ range or quantitatively assess the effects of intelligence range extension resulting from comparing a highly selected sample to random volunteers. Because participation in the control group required being interested in and filling out an internet-based questionnaire, it is likely that the average IQ of our control group was above the population average as volunteer databases usually are25,26. Fourth, it must be emphasized that the relationship between sleep timing and intelligence was – in line with previous results3—modest, amounting to a weekday midsleep timing about 14 minutes later and approximately 19 minutes less social jetlag per an approximately 1–2 SD IQ difference, assuming a Mensa mean higher than 130 and a control group mean above 100 but not exceeding 115. Fifth, Mensa members may not be fully representative for all individuals with an exceptionally high IQ. However, because selection into this organization is based purely on IQ, we believe that any associations between intelligence and chronotype should show up in the comparison of this sample with controls barring the very specific case when Mensa applicants are also systematically different on some other variable which has exactly the opposite relationship with chronotype as intelligence.

Overall, our analysis of an adult sample suggests that highly intelligent individuals do not have a biological preference for a later chronotype: while they do go to bed, sleep, and wake up later on work days, this effect is fully mediated by later working times and not present on free days or in the corrected chronotype. Highly intelligent individuals also suffer lower social jetlag, although this effect did not reach statistical significance once oversleeping was controlled for. Thus, the effect of intelligence on chronotype seems to have less to do with biological differences than with different work-related environmental pressures on sleep schedules. Eveningness and specifically social jetlag has negative effects on overall morbidity and mortality, including leading causes of death27,28, high IQ, however, is associated with a lower chance of all-cause mortality29 and morbidity30,31. Our results suggest that—although the size of the effects is modest—reduced social jetlag in those with late chronotypes due to later or more permissive work schedules might be one of the factors mediating the protective role of high IQ against morbidity and mortality. Because our results indicate that these risk differences are the result of lifestyle factors rather than biological differences, policies enabling a better alignment of desired and actual work schedules could be beneficial to individuals not possessing high intelligence.

Methods

Sample

We recruited 586 members of the German Mensa Society, an organization where a standardized IQ test score at least two standard deviations above the mean (IQ ≥ 130) assessed by trained testers and evaluated by a professional psychologist is a prerequisite for membership. As control, we selected a random sample of the same number (N = 586) of age- and sex-matched controls from the main MCTQ database12,32. There were 232 females and 354 males in each subgroup with a mean age of 38.2 years (see Tables 2 and 3 for more details on subgroups and subgroup differences). The study was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Munich and all participants gave informed consent accordingly. All research was performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations.

Table 3.

Mensa vs. Control differences in covariates potentially influencing sleep timing.

t df p Mean Difference SE 95% CI
Lower Upper
Work start time −2.33 751.00 0.02 −0:13:59.63 0:06:00.957 −0:25:48.23 −0:02:11.02
Commute duration 0.97 756.00 0.33 1.41 1.46 −1.46 4.27
Cigarettes/day 3.44 806.00 0.00 1.27 0.37 0.55 2.00
Beer/day 2.57 807.00 0.01 0.17 0.07 0.04 0.31
Glass wine/day 0.69 807.00 0.49 0.02 0.03 −0.04 0.09
Liquor/day 0.05 807.00 0.96 <0.01 0.01 −0.02 0.03
Coffee/day 2.79 807.00 0.01 0.35 0.13 0.11 0.60
Caffeine drink/day −1.96 807.00 0.05 −0.16 0.08 −0.32 0.00
Tea/day −1.54 807.00 0.12 −0.16 0.11 −0.37 0.04
Hypnotic/day −1.11 807.00 0.27 −0.01 0.01 −0.03 0.01

Negative effect sizes indicate higher values in Mensa members. Commute duration is given in minutes. Respondents could provide their consumption of psychoactive substances in monthly, weekly or daily quantities which was always converted to daily values.

Materials

All participants filled out the MCTQ questionnaire32, which contains questions about basic sociodemographic data (age, sex, place of residence, height and weight), other occupational and behavioral data (number of work days per week, working hours, light exposure on work and free days) as well as about sleep habits (bedtime, time of lights-off, time to falling asleep, wake-up time and the habitual time of getting out of bed) for both work days and free days  separately. From the responses, sleep duration and midsleep (the mid-point between sleep onset and sleep end) on work and work-free days, social jetlag (absolute midsleep difference between work days and free days), weekly sleep deprivation (the difference between the weekly average sleep duration and work day sleep duration, multiplied by the number of work days) and chronotype-proxy MSFsc (free-day midsleep corrected for sleep debt accumulated during the work week) were computed. We also collected information on work start times, commute time, and the consumption of legal substances acting on the central nervous system (i.e., cigarettes, alcohol, coffee, other caffeinated beverages, and hypnotics).

Analyses

We calculated the differences between Mensa members and matched controls using independent-sample t-tests. Multiple comparisons were controlled for using the Benjamini-Hochberg method of false detection rate (FDR) correction33. IBM SPSS Statistics Version 20 was used to test the confounding effects of extraneous variables on Mensa-control differences in sleep-wake timing with hierarchical linear regression models. The absence of missing data in the MCTQ was not a prerequisite for inclusion in all analyses. For this reason, missing data were found in some subjects, and the final sample sizes for each variable are reported in the Results and Table 1.

Supplementary information

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Higher Education Institutional Excellence Program of the Ministry of Human Capacities in Hungary, within the framework of the Neurology thematic program of the Semmelweis University. We thank Mensa in Deutschland e.V. (MinD) for their help in recruiting high-IQ participants. We further thank Celine Vetter and Till Roenneberg for their help with data acquisition and their valuable comments on the manuscript.

Author contributions

M.D. and T.G.B. designed the study and performed data collection. P.U. and R.B. performed statistical analyses. All authors wrote and reviewed the manuscript.

Data availability

The datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on request.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-020-62917-9.

References

  • 1.Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M. Life between clocks: daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. Journal of biological rhythms. 2003;18:80–90. doi: 10.1177/0748730402239679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Kanazawa S, Perina K. Why night owls are more intelligent. Personality and Individual Differences. 2009;47:685–690. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.05.021. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Preckel F, Lipnevich AA, Schneider S, Roberts RD. Chronotype, cognitive abilities, and academic achievement: A meta-analytic investigation. Learning and Individual Differences. 2011;21:483–492. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.07.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Arbabi T, Vollmer C, Dorfler T, Randler C. The influence of chronotype and intelligence on academic achievement in primary school is mediated by conscientiousness, midpoint of sleep and motivation. Chronobiology international. 2015;32:349–357. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2014.980508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Demirhan E, Randler C, Beşoluk Ş, Horzum MB. Gifted and non-gifted students’ diurnal preference and the relationship between personality, sleep, and sleep quality. Biological Rhythm Research. 2018;49:103–117. doi: 10.1080/09291016.2017.1333568. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Rahafar A, Randler C, Vollmer C, Kasaeian A. Prediction of school achievement through a multi-factorial approach – The unique role of chronotype. Learning and Individual Differences. 2017;55:69–74. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.03.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Jones SE, et al. Genome-Wide Association Analyses in 128,266 Individuals Identifies New Morningness and Sleep Duration Loci. PLOS Genetics. 2016;12:e1006125. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hill, W. D. et al. A combined analysis of genetically correlated traits identifies 187 loci and a role for neurogenesis and myelination in intelligence. Molecular Psychiatry (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 9.Roth B, et al. Intelligence and school grades: A meta-analysis. Intelligence. 2015;53:118–137. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2015.09.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Tonetti L, Natale V, Randler C. Association between circadian preference and academic achievement: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Chronobiol Int. 2015;32:792–801. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2015.1049271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Deary IJ, Harris SE, Hill WD. What genome-wide association studies reveal about the association between intelligence and physical health, illness, and mortality. Current opinion in psychology. 2018;27:6–12. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Roenneberg T, Allebrandt, Karla V, Merrow M, Vetter C. Social Jetlag and Obesity. Current Biology. 2012;22:939–943. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Strenze T. Intelligence and socioeconomic success: A meta-analytic review of longitudinal research. Intelligence. 2007;35:401–426. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.09.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Alexopoulos DS. Urban vs Rural Residence and IQ. Psychological Reports. 1997;80:851–860. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1997.80.3.851. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Teasdale TW, Owen DR, SØRensen TIA. Regional differences in intelligence and educational level in denmark. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 1988;58:307–314. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1988.tb00906.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lehmann IJ. Rural-Urban Differences in Intelligence. The Journal of Educational Research. 1959;53:62–68. doi: 10.1080/00220671.1959.10882612. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Abdellaoui, A. et al. Genetic Consequences of Social Stratification in Great Britain. bioRxiv, 457515, 10.1101/457515 (2018).
  • 18.Hill, W. D. et al. A combined analysis of genetically correlated traits identifies 187 loci and a role for neurogenesis and myelination in intelligence. Molecular Psychiatry, 10.1038/s41380-017-0001-5 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 19.Zavada A, Gordijn MC, Beersma DG, Daan S, Roenneberg T. Comparison of the Munich Chronotype Questionnaire with the Horne-Ostberg’s Morningness-Eveningness Score. Chronobiol Int. 2005;22:267–278. doi: 10.1081/CBI-200053536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Deary IJ, Harris SE, Hill WD. What genome-wide association studies reveal about the association between intelligence and physical health, illness, and mortality. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2019;27:6–12. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Davies, N. M., Dickson, M., Davey Smith, G., Windmeijer, F. & van den Berg, G. J. The effect of education on adult mortality, health, and income: triangulating across genetic and policy reforms. bioRxiv, 250068, 10.1101/250068 (2018).
  • 22.Krapohl, E. et al. Multi-polygenic score approach to trait prediction. Molecular Psychiatry, 10.1038/mp.2017.163https://www.nature.com/articles/mp2017163#supplementary-information (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 23.Deary IJ, Pattie A, Starr JM. The Stability of Intelligence From Age 11 to Age 90 Years: The Lothian Birth Cohort of 1921. Psychological Science. 2013;24:2361–2368. doi: 10.1177/0956797613486487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Plomin R, Deary IJ. Genetics and intelligence differences: five special findings. Mol Psychiatry. 2015;20:98–108. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Munafò MR, Tilling K, Taylor AE, Evans DM, Davey Smith G. Collider scope: when selection bias can substantially influence observed associations. International journal of epidemiology. 2017;47:226–235. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyx206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Taylor AE, et al. Exploring the association of genetic factors with participation in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. International journal of epidemiology. 2018;47:1207–1216. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyy060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Knutson KL, von Schantz M. Associations between chronotype, morbidity and mortality in the UK Biobank cohort. Chronobiology International. 2018;35:1045–1053. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2018.1454458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Castilhos Beauvalet, J. et al. Social jetlag in health and behavioral research: a systematic review. Vol. 7 (2017).
  • 29.Calvin CM, et al. Intelligence in youth and all-cause-mortality: systematic review with meta-analysis. International journal of epidemiology. 2011;40:626–644. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyq190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Calvin, C. M. et al. Childhood intelligence in relation to major causes of death in 68 year follow-up: prospective population study. BMJ (Clinical research ed.)357, 10.1136/bmj.j2708 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 31.Wraw C, Deary IJ, Gale CR, Der G. Intelligence in youth and health at age 50. Intelligence. 2015;53:23–32. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2015.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Roenneberg T, et al. Epidemiology of the human circadian clock. Sleep medicine reviews. 2007;11:429–438. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2007.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Benjamini, Y. & Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 289–300 (1995).

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on request.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES