Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2020 Apr 30;15(4):e0232245. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0232245

Cost-effectiveness of psychological treatments for post-traumatic stress disorder in adults

Ifigeneia Mavranezouli 1,2,*, Odette Megnin-Viggars 1,2, Nick Grey 3,4, Gita Bhutani 5,6, Jonathan Leach 7, Caitlin Daly 8, Sofia Dias 8,¤, Nicky J Welton 8, Cornelius Katona 9,10, Sharif El-Leithy 11, Neil Greenberg 12, Sarah Stockton 2, Stephen Pilling 1,2,13
Editor: Scott McDonald14
PMCID: PMC7192458  PMID: 32353011

Abstract

Background

Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a severe and disabling condition that may lead to functional impairment and reduced productivity. Psychological interventions have been shown to be effective in its management. The objective of this study was to assess the cost-effectiveness of a range of interventions for adults with PTSD.

Methods

A decision-analytic model was constructed to compare costs and quality-adjusted life-years (QALYs) of 10 interventions and no treatment for adults with PTSD, from the perspective of the National Health Service and personal social services in England. Effectiveness data were derived from a systematic review and network meta-analysis. Other model input parameters were based on published sources, supplemented by expert opinion.

Results

Eye movement desensitisation and reprocessing (EMDR) appeared to be the most cost-effective intervention for adults with PTSD (with a probability of 0.34 amongst the 11 evaluated options at a cost-effectiveness threshold of £20,000/QALY), followed by combined somatic/cognitive therapies, self-help with support, psychoeducation, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), trauma-focused cognitive behavioural therapy (TF-CBT), self-help without support, non-TF-CBT and combined TF-CBT/SSRIs. Counselling appeared to be less cost-effective than no treatment. TF-CBT had the largest evidence base.

Conclusions

A number of interventions appear to be cost-effective for the management of PTSD in adults. EMDR appears to be the most cost-effective amongst them. TF-CBT has the largest evidence base. There remains a need for well-conducted studies that examine the long-term clinical and cost-effectiveness of a range of treatments for adults with PTSD.

Introduction

A considerable proportion of people exposed to trauma, around 5.6%, will develop post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) [1]. PTSD is a severe and disabling condition that may lead to functional impairment and reduced productivity [2]. A number of psychological interventions have been shown to be effective in the treatment of PTSD in adults, predominantly eye movement desensitisation and reprocessing (EMDR) and trauma-focused cognitive behavioural therapy (TF-CBT) [3]. However, many people with PTSD delay seeking help or are not identified by health services [4]. Given the variety of available interventions and the need for efficient use of healthcare resources, the objective of this study was to examine the cost-effectiveness of a range of psychological interventions for the treatment of PTSD in adults from the perspective of the National Health Service (NHS) and Personal Social Services (PSS) in England, using decision-analytic economic modelling.

The analysis presented here is part of the work that informed the updating of national guidance for the management of PTSD in England, published by the National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE) [5]. The guideline was developed by a guideline committee, an independent multi-disciplinary group of clinical academics, health professionals and service user and carer representatives with expertise and experience in the field of PTSD. The committee contributed to the development of the economic model by providing advice on issues relating to the natural history and treatment patterns of PTSD in the UK, and on model inputs in areas where evidence was lacking.

Methods

Population

The study population comprised adults presenting in primary care with clinically important post-traumatic stress symptoms, defined by a diagnosis of PTSD according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM), the World Health Organization (WHO) International Classification of Diseases (ICD) or similar criteria, or by clinically significant PTSD symptoms, indicated by a PTSD symptom score above threshold on a validated scale, that are present for more than 3 months after a traumatic event.

The starting age of the cohorts in the economic model was 39 years, to reflect the mean age of adults with PTSD presenting to healthcare services in the UK [6]. The percentage of women in each cohort at the start of the model was 51.6%, calculated using national statistics for the general population [7], and data on the percentage of people screened positive for PTSD by age and sex in England [4]. The starting age and gender mix of the cohorts was used to estimate mortality risks and gender-specific quality-adjusted life-years (QALYs).

Interventions

The interventions considered in the economic analysis were selected from those considered in a network meta-analysis (NMA) of randomised controlled trials (RCTs) of psychological treatments for adults with PTSD ([3]; see S2 Appendix for inclusion criteria for the NMA). We included only interventions that had been tested on at least 100 individuals in the NMA of changes in PTSD symptoms at treatment endpoint, as this was deemed the minimum adequate evidence base that would enable robust conclusions to be drawn on clinical and cost-effectiveness. Moreover, we included only interventions that had shown a higher mean effect in comparison with waitlist. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) were included in the analysis as they were relevant comparators to psychological interventions.

The economic analysis evaluated the following interventions:

  • EMDR

  • TF-CBT

  • Non-TF-CBT

  • Combined somatic/cognitive therapies

  • SSRIs

  • Combined TF-CBT/SSRIs

  • Self-help with support

  • Self-help without support

  • Counselling

  • Psychoeducation

  • No treatment, reflected in waitlist RCT arms.

TF-CBT is a broad class of psychological interventions that predominantly use trauma-focused cognitive, behavioural or cognitive-behavioural techniques and exposure approaches to treatment. Although some interventions place their main emphasis on exposure and others on cognitive techniques, most use a combination of both. TF-CBT includes therapies such as cognitive therapy, cognitive processing therapy, exposure therapy/prolonged exposure, virtual reality exposure therapy, mindfulness-based cognitive therapy and narrative exposure therapy. Although the specific interventions that make up a class do not include exactly the same content or follow the same manual, they use the same broad approach and there is considerable overlap in the proposed mechanisms. In the economic analysis that informed the NICE guideline [5] we divided the TF-CBT class by the number of sessions and format of delivery and created separate categories of TF-CBT treatment according to their intensity, as these differences in resource use comprised practical considerations that informed the guideline recommendations; in addition to different intervention costs, each TF-CBT category had its own clinical effectiveness, estimated in the guideline NMAs. However, in the analyses we present here, we considered TF-CBT as an umbrella term of interventions that share a similar approach to treatment, in order to investigate the overall performance of the TF-CBT class relative to other treatments, regardless of its mode of delivery. Resource use for TF-CBT in the economic analysis we present here was determined by the average resource use reported in the TF-CBT trials informing the analysis, considering also that their vast majority assessed individual forms of TF-CBT.

Non-TF-CBT is a class of interventions that focus on current symptoms of PTSD without re-visiting the trauma experience. Combined somatic/cognitive therapies, such as emotional freedom techniques and thought field therapy, are exposure-based therapies with both cognitive and somatic components that utilise the tapping of points on the body [8, 9]. EMDR is based on a theoretical model which posits that the dysfunctional intrusions, emotions and physical sensations experienced by trauma victims are due to the improper storage of the traumatic event in implicit memory. The EMDR procedures are based on stimulating the patient’s own information processing in order to help integrate the targeted event as an adaptive contextualised memory [10]. Counselling is a type of psychological treatment that builds on the concept of client-centred therapy by Rogers [11]. It has been described in the relevant literature as non-directive counselling, supportive counselling, supportive psychotherapy, or person-centred counselling. Individuals are helped to focus on their thoughts, feelings and behaviour; to reach clearer self-understanding; and to find and use their strengths so that they cope more effectively with their lives by making appropriate decisions, or by taking relevant action. Counselling is primarily non-directive and non-advisory, but recognises that some situations require positive guidance by means of information and advice. Psychoeducation involves the provision of information about the nature and causes of PTSD, and strategies and treatments that can help address PTSD symptoms. It can be delivered individually, but is commonly delivered to groups. Psychoeducation is usually non-directive and takes an educational didactic format. Finally, self-help therapies include interventions such as internet-based TF-CBT or other computerised psychological therapies, expressive writing and cognitive bibliotherapy. Self-help with support includes interventions in which therapist’s input is an integral part of the intervention; in self-help without support the therapist’s input is minimal or absent.

The guideline analysis included interventions tested on at least 50 people in the NMA of changes in PTSD symptoms at treatment endpoint, whereas here we used a threshold of at least 100 people to improve the robustness of the results. The impact of increasing this threshold was the omission of interpersonal psychotherapy and present-centered therapy from the analysis presented here; both interventions were shown to occupy middle-to-lower cost-effectiveness rankings in the guideline analysis, and therefore their omission had no impact on the overall conclusions of our analysis.

Economic model structure

A hybrid decision-analytic model consisting of a decision-tree followed by a three-state Markov model was constructed using Microsoft Office Excel 2013 to estimate total costs and QALYs associated with each treatment. The model structure was determined by the natural history of PTSD, its treatment patterns in the UK, and the availability of relevant clinical and epidemiological data (Fig 1).

Fig 1. Schematic diagram of the economic model.

Fig 1

The model followed hypothetical cohorts of adults with PTSD, initiated on each of the treatments assessed. The treatment duration for each of the assessed options equalled 3 months (12 weeks), according to the average duration of interventions in trials and routine clinical practice (range 4–20 weeks). Following a course of treatment, people in each cohort either remitted (entering a state of ‘no-PTSD’) or failed to remit, remaining in a ‘PTSD’ state. Those initiated on SSRIs alone or in combination were given 3 months of maintenance pharmacological therapy if they had remitted. Death was not considered during provision of interventions, as no relevant differential mortality data are available. In the next 3 months of follow-up, those who had remitted could remain in remission, relapse to PTSD or die. Those who had not remitted could remain in the ‘PTSD’ state, remit (and move to ‘no-PTSD’) or die. The length of the follow-up period immediately post-treatment was set at 3 months as this is the period for which most follow-up data are reported in RCTs of psychological interventions for PTSD.

After that point, people in each cohort entered the Markov model, run in 3-month cycles, for consistency with the duration of the two periods of the decision-tree. In each cycle, they remained in the same health state or moved between the states of ‘PTSD’ and ‘no-PTSD’ or moved to death (absorbing state). People in both the ‘PTSD’ and the ‘no-PTSD’ states received primary, community and secondary healthcare and personal social services, as relevant to their health state. A half-cycle correction was applied.

The time horizon of the analysis was 3 years (36 months), comprising 6 months in the decision tree and 2.5 years (10 x 3-month cycles) in the Markov component of the model. This time frame was deemed adequate to capture longer-term costs and effects of treatment, without making significant extrapolations and assumptions over the course of PTSD.

Effectiveness data

We obtained effectiveness data from a systematic review and NMA of psychological treatments for adults with PTSD [3]. We utilised the results of 2 NMAs of changes in PTSD symptoms: between baseline and treatment endpoint; and between baseline and 1-4-month follow-up. Details on the NMAs, including the studies and data that informed them, the selection of the effectiveness data and the transformations required for use in the economic model are provided in S1 File.

The outputs of the NMA of changes in PTSD symptoms between baseline and treatment endpoint informed the intervention effects in the model period of 0–3 months. For the 3–6 month follow-up period, the base-case economic analysis conservatively assumed that the active intervention effects were not retained and equalled the effect of no treatment; this was decided because the results of the NMA of changes in PTSD symptoms between baseline and 1-4-month follow-up were based on limited evidence and showed considerable uncertainty. Nevertheless, data from this NMA were used in a secondary analysis, to inform effects for each active intervention during 3–6 months after treatment initiation.

Baseline probability of remission

The probability of remission for no treatment (baseline) and for all model arms beyond treatment endpoint (i.e. for all treatment options during 3–6 months after treatment initiation in the base-case analysis and for all treatment options during 6–36 months after treatment initiation in both the base-case and secondary analysis) was estimated using data from an Australian national mental health survey [12]. We considered the survey participants to be representative of our study population, which was adults presenting in primary care with symptoms of PTSD. Details on the methods used for the estimation of the baseline probability of remission are provided in S2 File.

Risk of relapse

Due to lack of published evidence, an annual risk of relapse of 0.10 was assumed across all treatment arms, based on the committee’s expert opinion; this was translated into a 3-month probability of relapse of 0.026 assuming an exponential function, which was applied in the 3-month follow-up period of the decision-tree and over the whole duration of the Markov model. This assumption was tested in a sensitivity analysis.

Risk of development of side effects from SSRI treatment

The probability of developing common side effects from SSRIs (headaches, nausea or vomiting, agitation, sedation and sexual dysfunction) was estimated from a retrospective analysis of a large US managed care claims database on 40,017 people with depression who were initiated on antidepressant monotherapy, including SSRIs [13]. Serious side effects from SSRIs (such as death, attempted suicide or self-harm, falls, fractures, stroke, epilepsy/seizures) were not considered; however, their omission is not expected to have had a significant impact on the economic results, due to their low incidence in the study population [14, 15].

Mortality

PTSD is associated with an increased risk of mortality. The hazard ratio of death associated with PTSD, adjusted for confounders such as age, gender, diabetes mellitus and hypertension was obtained from a study on 637 veterans in the US [16]. This ratio was applied onto general mortality statistics for England [17] to estimate the annual mortality risk in people with PTSD over the time period they experienced PTSD symptoms. People without PTSD symptoms had the mortality risk of the general population.

Utility data

Utility scores express preferences for the health-related quality of life (HRQoL) in distinct health states; they are necessary for the estimation of QALYs. Following a systematic literature search of utility data for PTSD, the base-case economic analysis used utility scores generated from HRQoL ratings of Australian adult participants in a national mental health survey, some of whom had a diagnosis of PTSD according to DSM-4 criteria [18]. HRQoL was assessed with the Assessment of Quality of Life measure (http://www.aqol.com.au). The study provided gender-specific data for people with PTSD and people who were PTSD-free following evidence-based treatment, which corresponded directly to the model health states of interest.

In a sensitivity analysis, we used utility data derived from a sample of 808 US veterans attending primary care clinics, 97 of whom had developed PTSD, adjusted for confounders such as gender, employment status, presence of disability, and mental and physical health comorbidities [19]. HRQoL was assessed using SF-36, which was converted to utility scores using the UK algorithm [20]. Data from this study indicated a narrower utility benefit following remission from PTSD compared with the utility data used in the base-case analysis.

The mean utility decrement in people experiencing common side effects from SSRIs was estimated using EQ-5D scores of participants in a US medical expenditure survey, some of whom had depression and experienced side effects from antidepressant treatment [21]. This utility decrement was applied only over the period that people received SSRI treatment.

Resource use and cost data

The analysis included intervention costs (healthcare professional time, drug acquisition and equipment/infrastructure required for self-help interventions), costs of managing side effects from medication, and costs relating to the ‘PTSD’ and ‘no-PTSD’ health states including costs of primary, community and secondary healthcare and PSS costs.

Intervention costs (Table 1) were calculated by combining resource use reported in RCTs included in the NMA that informed the economic analysis (i.e. number and duration of therapeutic sessions and mean daily dosage of sertraline, which was the most commonly used SSRI in trials), modified to represent routine UK practice, with respective national unit costs. All psychological interventions, with the exception of self-help and psychoeducation, were assumed to be delivered in a primary care setting by Band 7 psychological therapists according to the NHS Agenda for Change (AfC) pay-scales for community-based scientific and professional staff, to reflect routine practice in the UK. Psychoeducation was assumed to be delivered by AfC band 5 Psychological Well-being Practitioners (PWPs) and self-help by AfC band 6 psychological therapists. People receiving SSRIs attended general practitioner (GP) monitoring visits and undertook routine laboratory testing. Those experiencing side effects had one extra GP contact every 3 months and received medication for their management. For self-help therapies we included the cost of the provider of digital mental health programmes, and costs of hardware and capital overheads.

Table 1. Intervention costs of treatments for adults with PTSD (2017 prices).

Intervention Resource use details Intervention cost
EMDR 6 x 1.5 hr individual sessions (9 hours) delivered by a Band 7 psychological therapist £912
TF-CBT 9 x 1.5 hr individual sessions (13.5 hours) delivered by a Band 7 psychological therapist £1,368
non-TF-CBT 9 x 1 hr individual sessions (9 hours) delivered by a Band 7 psychological therapist £912
Combined somatic/cognitive therapies 4 x 1 hr individual sessions (4 hours) delivered by a Band 7 psychological therapist £405
SSRIs (sertraline) Mean daily dosage 150mg, 4 GP visits at 0–3 months + 1 visit at 3–6 months, monitoring lab testing 0–3 months: £155
3–6 months: £39
If side effects: £37
For people experiencing side effects: 1 extra GP visit over 3 months, medication for management.
Combined TF-CBT/SSRIs Sum of the individual treatment components 0–3 months: £1,523 3–6 months: £39
Self-help with support Fixed cost of provider of digital programmes, hardware & capital overheads, 180 minutes of support by a band 6 psychological therapist £266
Self-help without support Fixed cost of provider of digital programmes, hardware & capital overheads, 40 minutes of support by a band 6 psychological therapist £98
Counselling 10 x 1 hr individual sessions (10 hours) delivered by a Band 7 psychological therapist £1,014
Psychoeducation 3 x 1 hr individual sessions (3 hours) delivered by a band 5 PWP £127
No treatment No resource use £0

EMDR: eye movement desensitisation reprocessing; IPT: interpersonal psychotherapy; PWP: psychological well-being practitioner; TF-CBT: trauma-focused cognitive behavioural therapy

Unit cost of band 7 psychological therapists: £101 per hour of direct contact with the client–see S3 File for details on estimation of unit cost

Unit cost of band 5 PWPs: £42 per hour of direct contact with the client–see S3 File for details on estimation of unit cost

Unit cost of band 6 psychological therapist: £72 per hour of direct contact with the client (mean value of unit costs of band 7 psychological therapist and band 5 PWP)

Unit cost of GP: £37 per patient contact lasting 9.22 minutes [22]

Sertraline acquisition cost: 100mg, 28 tab, £0.99 [23]– 3-month cost £1.59

Cost of monitoring lab testing (SSRIs): £5 per person (expert advice)

Cost of medication for management of side effects (SSRIs): £3 per person over 3 months (expert advice)

Fixed cost of provider of digital programmes: £36.20 per person (expert advice)

Cost of hardware & capital overheads: £14 per person [24]

Unit costs were estimated using a combination of data derived from national sources and other published evidence [2226]. Health professional unit costs included wages/salary, salary on-costs, capital and other overheads, qualification costs where available, and supervision costs. The ratio of direct (face-to-face) to indirect (preparation and administrative tasks) health professionals’ time was taken into account. Details on the methods and sources used to estimate therapists’ unit costs are reported in S3 File.

Annual costs associated with the PTSD and no-PTSD health states were estimated using predominantly NHS and PSS usage data from a national psychiatric morbidity survey conducted in England in 2014 [4], supplemented with resource use data from other published sources [2729] and expert opinion, which were subsequently combined with national unit costs [22, 23, 30]. Costs for each state included inpatient hospital stays and outpatient visits, contacts with GPs, psychiatrists, psychologists, social workers, community psychiatric and learning disability nurses, other nursing services, self-help and support groups, home help or home care, outreach or family support workers and community day-care centres. Details on the methods and data used to estimate annual costs associated with the PTSD and no-PTSD health states are provided in S4 File. These were then translated into 3-month cost figures that informed the economic model. Because the estimated health state-related costs were based to a large extent on expert opinion, a sensitivity analysis was conducted, in which PTSD costs were varied by ±50%.

Costs were expressed in 2017 prices, uplifted, where necessary, using the Hospital and Community Health Services Pay and Prices Index [22].

Discounting

Costs and QALYs were discounted at 3.5% annually as recommended by NICE [31].

Analysis

To account for the uncertainty around input parameter point estimates, a probabilistic analysis was undertaken, in which input parameters were assigned probabilistic distributions [32]. Subsequently, 10,000 iterations were performed, each drawing random values out of the distributions fitted onto the model input parameters. Mean costs and QALYs for each treatment were calculated by averaging across the 10,000 iterations. The Net Monetary Benefit (NMB) for each intervention was estimated for each iteration and averaged across the 10,000 iterations, determined by the formula

NMB=EλC

where E and C are the effects (QALYs) and costs of each intervention, respectively, and λ represents the willingness-to-pay per unit of effectiveness, set at the NICE lower cost-effectiveness threshold of £20,000/QALY [31]. The intervention with the highest NMB is the most cost-effective option [33].

The mean ranking by cost-effectiveness is reported for each intervention (out of 10,000 iterations), where a rank of 1 suggests that an intervention is the most cost-effective amongst all evaluated treatment options. The probability of the intervention with the highest NMB being the most cost-effective option is also provided, calculated as the proportion of iterations in which the intervention has had the highest NMB amongst all interventions considered in the analysis. The probability of cost-effectiveness has been estimated in a step-wise approach, according to which the most cost-effective intervention is omitted at each step, and the probability of cost-effectiveness of the next most cost-effective intervention amongst the remaining treatment options is re-calculated. The probabilities estimated following this approach reflect the uncertainty around the cost-effectiveness not only of the most cost-effective intervention, but also of the second, third, fourth, etc. most cost-effective intervention in ranking, after more cost-effective interventions have been omitted from analysis. Finally, the cost-effectiveness acceptability frontier has been plotted, showing the treatment with the highest mean NMB over different cost-effectiveness thresholds (λ), and the probability that this treatment is the most cost-effective among those assessed [33].

Table 2 reports the values of all model input parameters. Deterministic values were used in deterministic one-way sensitivity analyses. The probability distributions show the types and range of distributions assigned to each parameter; estimation of distribution ranges was based on data reported in the published sources of evidence.

Table 2. Economic model input parameters.

Input parameter Deterministic value Probability distribution (type, range) Sources–comments
Characteristics of study population
Starting age of cohort (years) 39 No distribution [6]; mean age of adults referred for assessment for possible PTSD in a UK NHS outpatient clinic
Proportion of women 0.52 No distribution Calculated using the proportion of women in the general population aged 39 years [7], and data on the percentage of people screened positive for PTSD by age and sex [4].
Odds ratios of remission versus no treatment at treatment endpoint
EMDR 42.18 95% CrI: 13.59 to 132.42 [3]; standardised mean differences converted to odds ratios according to [34]; distribution based on 300,000 samples from posterior distributions outputted from NMAs, thinned by 30 to obtain 10,000 values
TF-CBT 14.06 95% CrI: 6.76 to 29.81
non-TF-CBT 9.09 95% CrI: 2.50 to 33.62
Combined somatic/cognitive therapies 21.33 95% CrI: 3.84 to 121.63
SSRIs 7.99 95% CrI: 1.50 to 44.61
Combined TF-CBT/SSRIs 9.06 95% CrI: 1.15 to 69.34
Self-help with support 13.98 95% CrI: 2.74 to 70.74
Self-help without support 5.17 95% CrI: 1.29 to 20.27
Counselling 3.70 95% CrI: 1.12 to 12.38
Psychoeducation 8.99 95% CrI: 0.26 to 276.72
Odds ratios of remission versus no treatment at 3-month follow-up (secondary analysis only)
EMDR 7.53 95% CrI: 1.55 to 35.77 [3]; standardised mean differences converted to odds ratios according to [34]; distribution based on 300,000 samples from posterior distributions outputted from NMAs, thinned by 30 to obtain 10,000 values
3-6-month probability of remission for SSRIs assumed to equal the probability of remission of SSRIs during initial treatment (0–3 months); 3-6-month probability of remission for combined TF-CBT/SSRIs borrowed from TF-CBT.
TF-CBT 3.80 95% CrI: 1.49 to 9.72
non-TF-CBT 2.18 95% CrI: 0.37 to 12.40
Combined somatic/cognitive therapies 8.08 95% CrI: 0.41 to 155.56
SSRIs No data No data
Combined TF-CBT/SSRIs No data No data
Self-help with support 10.11 95% CrI: 2.03 to 48.96
Self-help without support 8.85 95% CrI: 0.73 to 105.43
Counselling 1.73 95% CrI: 0.37 to 8.15
Psychoeducation 2.58 95% CrI: 0.42 to 15.43
Probability of remission–no treatment (also applied to all interventions between 3–6 months in the base-case analysis & all interventions beyond 6 months in the base-case and secondary analyses)
0–3 months from PTSD onset 0.03 Beta: α = 17.26; β = 646.74 [12]. See S2 File for details
0–12 months from PTSD onset 0.15 Beta: α = 98.94; β = 565.06
0–24 months from PTSD onset 0.27 Beta: α = 176.62; β = 487.38
0–36 months from PTSD onset 0.32 Beta: α = 212.48; β = 451.52
Risk of relapse–all treatments
3-month risk 0.026 Beta: α = 2.60; β = 97.40 Expert opinion
Risk of developing common side effects from SSRIs
3-month risk 0.029 Beta: α = 687; β = 22,933 [13]
Mortality
Hazard ratio–PTSD vs no PTSD 1.77 Log-normal: 95% CI 1.02 to 3.14 [16]
Baseline mortality–general population Age/sex specific No distribution General mortality statistics for the UK population [17]
Utility values
Base-case analysis
PTSD, men 0.54 Beta: α = 26.83; β = 22.86 [18]; distribution estimated based on method of moments
PTSD, women 0.57 Beta: α = 86.75; β = 65.44
No PTSD, men 0.63 Beta: α = 5.11; β = 3.00
No PTSD, women 0.64 Beta: α = 14.11; β = 7.93
Sensitivity analysis
PTSD, all 0.61 No distribution [19]
No PTSD, all 0.64
Disutility due to side effects from SSRIs
% reduction in health state utility 10.3 Beta: α = 89.64; β = 784.07 [21]; disutility applied as a percentage onto the health state (PTSD or no PTSD) utility
Intervention costs–resource use
Number of sessions
EMDR 6 0.70: 5–6, 0.16: 4, 0.14: 3 Different probabilities assigned to different numbers of sessions for each therapy, based on resource use reported in the RCTs included in the NMAs that informed the economic analysis, supplemented by further assumptions.
Combined TF-CBT/SSRIs: resource use was the sum of the resource use of the individual treatment components.
Details on intervention costs are provided in Table 1.
TF-CBT 9 0.70: 7–9, 0.16: 5–6, 0.14: 3–4
non-TF-CBT 9 0.70: 7–9, 0.16: 5–6, 0.14: 3–4
Combined somatic/cognitive therapies 4 0.70: 4, 0.30: 2–3
Counselling 10 0.70: 8–10, 0.16: 5–7, 0.14: 3–4
Psychoeducation 3 0.70: 3, 0.16: 2, 0.14: 1
Therapist time (minutes)
Self-help with support 180 Normal: SD = 0.30*mean SD based on assumption
Self-help without support 40 Normal: SD = 0.30*mean
Number of GP contacts–SSRIs
0–3 months 4 0.70: 4, 0.30: 2–3 Different probabilities assigned to different numbers of sessions; number of visits based on expert opinion; distribution based on assumption.
3–6 months 1 0.70: 1, 0.30: 0
Treatment of side effects 1 0.80: 1, 0.20: 2
Intervention costs—unit costs
SSRI– 3-month drug acquisition £1.59 No distribution [23]
Laboratory testing–SSRIs £5 No distribution Assumption
Medication for side effects–SSRIs £3 No distribution Assumption
Self-help infrastructure £50 No distribution Fixed digital therapy provider cost based on expert advice; capital cost based on [24]
GP unit cost £37 Normal, SE = 0.05 of the mean [22]; distribution based on assumption
Band 7 clinical psychologist unit cost £101 Normal: SE = 0.05 of the mean See S3 File; distribution based on assumption
Band 5 PWP unit cost £42 Normal, SE = 0.05 of the mean See S3 File; distribution based on assumption
Band 6 therapist unit cost £72 Determined by distribution of Band 7 and Band 5 therapist unit costs Assumed to be the mean of Band 7 and Band 5 therapist unit costs
3-month NHS/PSS health state cost
PTSD £293 Gamma: SE = 0.30 of the mean Based on resource use data reported in national and other published sources [4, 2729], supplemented with expert opinion and combined with national unit costs [22, 23, 30], expressed in 2017 prices; see S4 File for details.
No-PTSD £27 Gamma: SE = 0.30 of the mean
Annual discount rate 0.035 No distribution Applied to costs and QALYs [31]

CI: confidence intervals; CrI: credible intervals; EMDR: eye movement desensitisation reprocessing; GP: general practitioner; IPT: interpersonal psychotherapy; NHS: National Health Service; PSS: personal social services; PTSD: post-traumatic stress disorder; PWP: psychological well-being practitioner; SD: standard deviation; SE: standard error; SSRI: selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor; TF-CBT: trauma-focused cognitive behavioural therapy

Two probabilistic analyses were undertaken, each using different assumptions on the effectiveness of interventions at the 3-month follow-up:

  • Base-case analysis: treatment effect between 3–6 months equalled that of no treatment

  • Secondary analysis: treatment effect between 3–6 months equalled that estimated from the NMA of changes in PTSD symptoms between baseline and 1-4-month follow-up

One-way deterministic sensitivity analyses explored the following scenarios applied onto the base-case analysis:

  • change in the annual risk of relapse between 0.05 and 0.20

  • change in the PTSD health state cost by ± 50%

  • use of alternative utility scores for the PTSD and no-PTSD states [19].

Validation of the economic model

The economic model was developed in collaboration with members of the guideline committee. All inputs and model formulae were systematically checked. The model was tested for logical consistency by setting input parameters to null and extreme values and examining whether results changed in the expected direction. Results were discussed with the committee to confirm their plausibility.

Results

Table 3 shows the results of the base-case economic analysis. Interventions have been ordered from the most to the least cost-effective. The table provides the mean number of QALYs, intervention costs and total costs per person, the mean NMB and ranking of each intervention, and its probability of being cost-effective in a step-wise approach at a threshold of £20,000/QALY.

Table 3. Base-case results of economic modelling.

Intervention Mean per person NMB (£/ person) Mean rank Prob*
QALY Intervention cost (£) Total cost (£) (at a threshold of £20,000/QALY)
EMDR 1.80 746 2,047 33,928 2.31 0.34
Combined somatic/cognitive therapies 1.77 360 1,963 33,364 3.28 0.35
Self-help with support 1.75 266 2,047 32,880 4.01 0.32
Psychoeducation 1.74 108 1,982 32,754 4.90 0.42
SSRIs 1.72 146 2,143 32,316 5.15 0.37
TF-CBT 1.74 1,058 2,854 32,042 6.38 0.26
Self-help without support 1.71 98 2,253 31,865 6.19 0.41
non-TF-CBT 1.73 705 2,670 31,860 6.79 0.50
Combined TF-CBT/SSRIs 1.73 1,204 3,140 31,451 8.19 0.48
No treatment 1.67 0 2,488 30,915 9.14 0.64
Counselling 1.69 785 3,043 30,854 9.66 1.00

EMDR: eye movement desensitisation reprocessing; NMB: net monetary benefit; Prob: probability of cost-effectiveness; SSRIs: selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors; TF-CBT: trauma-focused cognitive behavioural therapy

*estimated in a step-wise approach, according to which the most cost-effective intervention is omitted at each step, and the probability of cost-effectiveness of the next most cost-effective intervention amongst the remaining treatment options is re-calculated

EMDR was found to be the most cost-effective intervention for adults with PTSD, with the highest NMB at the cost-effectiveness threshold of £20,000/QALY. This was followed by combined somatic/cognitive therapies, self-help with support, psychoeducation, SSRIs, TF-CBT, self-help without support, non-TF-CBT, combined TF-CBT/SSRIs, no treatment and counselling. The probability of EMDR being the most cost-effective treatment amongst the 11 options assessed was only 0.34. The probabilities of cost-effectiveness for next interventions in ranking up to (and including) self-help without support did not exceed 0.42, although increasingly fewer interventions were included in the analysis, indicating uncertainty in the results. Notably, counselling was less cost-effective than no treatment; this finding was attributed to the relatively low clinical effectiveness of counselling (the lowest amongst all active treatments assessed in the economic analysis), which did not offset its relatively high intervention cost. The cost-effectiveness plane (Fig 2) depicts the mean incremental costs and QALYs of all interventions versus no treatment (placed at the origin). According to the cost-effectiveness acceptability frontier (Fig 3), combined somatic/cognitive therapies appeared to be most cost-effective amongst the 11 treatment options assessed for thresholds up to £2,500/QALY, with a low probability that reached 0.25 at maximum, whereas EMDR became the most cost-effective option at higher thresholds, with a probability that ranged from 0.19 to 0.41 amongst the 11 options assessed.

Fig 2. Cost-effectiveness plane: Base-case analysis results for 1,000 adults with PTSD.

Fig 2

Fig 3. Cost-effectiveness acceptability frontier: Base-case analysis.

Fig 3

Results of the secondary analysis, which utilised 3-month NMA follow-up data, were not very different. The top 3 interventions (EMDR, combined somatic/cognitive therapies, self-help with support) remained the same. The ranking of self-help without support improved and counselling became better than no treatment. The probabilities of cost-effectiveness of the top 5 interventions were low, ranging between 0.14 and 0.47, indicating uncertainty around the results. Self-help without support appeared to be the most cost-effective option at a zero cost-effectiveness threshold and combined somatic/cognitive therapies were most cost-effective at higher thresholds up to £18,000/QALY; EMDR was the most cost-effective option at higher thresholds, with a 0.14 probability at the threshold of £20,000/QALY. Results of the secondary analysis are provided in S5 File.

In deterministic sensitivity analyses, results were, overall, robust to changes in the risk of relapse and in the PTSD health state cost and rankings were not affected. TF-CBT was the only option that dropped (by one place) in ranking when the baseline risk of relapse was increased by 50% or the PTSD health state cost was reduced by 50%. Use of alternative utility data that assumed narrower HRQoL benefits associated with remission had a small impact on the results, with the relative cost-effectiveness of TF-CBT alone or combined with SSRIs and non-TF-CBT being reduced. However, results for other interventions were not affected. Results of deterministic sensitivity analyses are shown in S6 File. For information, results of the NICE guideline economic analysis are shown in S7 File.

Discussion

Overview of findings

EMDR appears to be the most cost-effective intervention for adults with PTSD more than 3 months after trauma, followed by combined somatic/cognitive therapies, self-help with support, psychoeducation, SSRIs, TF-CBT, self-help without support, non-TF-CBT and combined TF-CBT/SSRIs. Counselling appears to be less cost-effective than no treatment, due to its relatively low clinical effectiveness (the lowest amongst all active treatments assessed in the economic analysis), as shown in the NMAs that informed the economic analysis [3], which was not enough to offset its relatively high intervention cost. The low effectiveness found for counselling can be attributed to counselling’s non-directive person-centred approach, which is less likely to help the person overcome avoidance (which is one of the criteria for PTSD), and thus less likely to reduce PTSD symptoms. On the other hand, counselling’s effectiveness may have been underestimated to some extent due to researcher allegiance, since in the vast majority of the RCTs on counselling that informed the economic analysis, counselling served as a control treatment to other active interventions, primarily TF-CBT [3].

Results were characterised by uncertainty, as reflected in relatively low probabilities of each intervention being cost-effective amongst alternative treatment options; however they were overall robust to assumptions tested through deterministic sensitivity analyses.

Strengths and limitations

Our analysis utilised effectiveness data derived from a systematic review and NMA of changes in PTSD symptoms [3]. This methodology enabled us to consider information from direct and indirect comparisons between interventions, and allowed simultaneous comparisons across all options while preserving randomisation [35]. This approach for evidence synthesis is essential for populating model-based economic studies assessing more than two competing interventions. No inconsistency was detected between direct and indirect evidence. We used 10,000 iterations of the NMA models in the economic analysis, which represent the full uncertainty and correlation in the relative effects.

The NMA that informed the base-case economic analysis (changes in PTSD symptoms between baseline and treatment endpoint) used a large evidence base and produced robust data. The NMA of 1-4-month follow-up changes in PTSD symptoms, which informed the secondary analysis, showed considerable uncertainty for most interventions, due to the small number and size of the included studies; TF-CBT and EMDR were the only treatments in this NMA with data on at least 100 people at 1-4-month follow-up that showed evidence of sustained effect. Thus, results of this secondary economic analysis should be interpreted with caution. Both NMAs were characterised by high between-trial heterogeneity, which is likely to have been caused by heterogeneity across populations included in the trials considered in the NMAs, for example, in terms of the presence of a formal PTSD diagnosis, the severity, complexity and chronicity of PTSD symptoms, the type, extent and multiplicity of trauma exposure, the presence of comorbidity, and also the variability of interventions within each assessed option and the differences across settings, e.g. inpatient versus outpatient delivery of interventions [3]. Heterogeneity may also have been caused by the type, multiplicity and timing of previous treatments in trial participants, but relevant information was not available in the majority of the RCTs included in the NMAs; therefore, the impact of previous treatments on the effectiveness of the interventions cannot be assessed.

Regarding the potential heterogeneity across populations in the trials that informed the economic model, it should be noted that, of the 82 studies that were included in the NMAs that informed the economic analysis, 50 (61%) recruited people with a formal PTSD diagnosis whereas the remaining 32 (39%) recruited people with clinically important PTSD symptoms, as indicated by baseline scores above a predefined threshold on a validated PTSD symptom scale. The percentage of trials recruiting people with a formal PTSD diagnosis was relatively high (range 70–100%) among trials assessing TF-CBT alone or combined with SSRIs, EMDR and counselling; moderate (range 50–66%) among trials assessing non-TF-CBT and self-help with support; and rather low (range 25–33%) among trials assessing combined somatic/cognitive therapies, self-help without support and psychoeducation ([3]; see S5 Appendix for population characteristics of included studies in the NMA). It may be hypothesised that a formal diagnosis of PTSD is associated with more severe symptoms at baseline, resulting in interventions tested on such populations appearing to be less clinically and cost-effective compared with interventions tested on people with clinically important PTSD symptoms, who may have had less severe symptoms at baseline. Nevertheless, the NMA and economic modelling results did not confirm such a hypothesis, since, for example, EMDR and, to a lesser degree, TF-CTB, showed high clinical and cost-effectiveness, despite both having been tested in RCTs that, in their majority, recruited people with a formal PTSD diagnosis. On the other hand, it is possible that participants in the trials that recruited people with clinically important PTSD symptoms might meet criteria for (and might have received) a formal PTSD diagnosis, but they were not required to do so in order to participate in the trial, and this could well have been for pragmatic reasons related to trial management rather than an intention to recruit people with potentially lower symptom severity. Furthermore, for PTSD symptom scales that are based closely on diagnostic criteria, e.g. PTSD Checklist (PCL), scoring above the clinical threshold may be regarded as comparable to receiving a formal diagnosis of PTSD. Related to this point, we should note that all RCTs that evaluated combined somatic/cognitive therapies and recruited people with clinically important PTSD symptoms used scales that are based on diagnostic criteria (e.g. PCL and Modified PTSD Symptom Scale [MPSS-SR]); similarly, the majority of the studies that evaluated psychoeducation and recruited people with clinically important PTSD symptoms used scales based on diagnostic criteria (e.g. PCL and Davidson Trauma Scale). Consequently, the symptom severity of trial populations receiving combined somatic/cognitive therapies and psychoeducation (who, in the majority of trials, were not required to have a formal diagnosis of PTSD) was likely similar to the symptom severity of populations with a formal diagnosis of PTSD receiving other interventions considered in our NMA and economic analysis.

The strengths and limitations of the NMAs that informed the economic analyses should be considered when interpreting the cost-effectiveness results. Moreover, the quality and limitations of RCTs considered in the NMAs have unavoidably impacted on the quality of the model input parameters. Although all interventions included in the economic analysis had been tested on at least 100 trial participants for treatment endpoint, the size of the evidence base differed considerably across interventions. TF-CBT had by far the largest evidence base in both NMAs (N = 903 across 29 RCTs at treatment endpoint and N = 753 across 13 RCTs at 1-4-month follow-up), which gives us more confidence in the results on its clinical (and, consequently, cost-) effectiveness. The evidence base was more limited for other interventions included in the economic analysis (each tested on N<350 in each of the two NMAs). For comparison, EMDR, which was shown to be the most cost-effective treatment option in our economic analysis, was tested on N = 260 across 11 RCTs at treatment endpoint and N = 121 across 4 RCTs at 1-4-month follow-up in the NMAs that informed the economic analysis [3].

The economic model structure did not incorporate discontinuation due to limited data availability. However, the NMAs that informed the economic analysis utilised intention-to-treat data, where available, so that discontinuation has been implicitly considered in the economic analysis. The probabilistic analysis took into account, where possible, the completion rates of the interventions in the RCTs that informed the economic analysis, so that the number of sessions reflected, up to a degree, the attrition rates of each intervention. The time horizon of the analysis was 3 years, in order to capture longer-term effects and costs associated with a course of treatment for PTSD without significant extrapolation over the natural course of PTSD.

The baseline risk of remission was estimated from a large longitudinal study on adults with PTSD in the community [12], as the survey’s target population was deemed to be directly relevant to our study population. The risk of relapse was not available in published literature, and was therefore based on expert opinion. Utility data were derived from a systematic literature review. Costs incurred by adults with PTSD and those remitting from PTSD were based on published national survey data, supplemented with other published evidence and expert opinion, due to lack of more accurate information. Sensitivity analysis showed that results were robust to use of alternative values for the risk of relapse, utility and costs. The risk of side effects from SSRIs was based on an uncontrolled study that did not examine the rate of side effects that were attributable to SSRIs. Therefore, our economic analysis may have overestimated the impact of common side effects from SSRIs relative to other treatments and thus may have underestimated the relative cost effectiveness of SSRIs.

In conclusion, our study is characterised by different strengths and limitations, which we have considered when constructing our model and interpreting the results of our analysis. We carried out probabilistic analyses, which took into account the uncertainty around model parameters and, where possible, we conducted secondary and deterministic sensitivity analyses to address uncertainties and gaps in the evidence.

Comparison with existing economic evidence

Published economic evaluations of interventions for PTSD in adults have concluded that exposure therapy (a form of TF-CBT) is more cost-effective than no treatment [36]. TF-CBT and SSRIs are likely more cost-effective than usual care [18]; prolonged exposure (TF-CBT) has been found to be more cost-effective than SSRIs [37]. Finally, self-management was shown to be no more effective but overall less costly than psychoeducation [38]. These economic studies evaluated a limited range of interventions for adults with PTSD and made very few comparisons between active interventions; notably, EMDR, which was shown to be the most cost-effective intervention in our analysis, has not been evaluated in previously published economic literature on adults with PTSD.

Overall, our findings are in agreement with previously published evidence. Our economic analysis estimated the cost-effectiveness of a wider range of interventions available for adults with PTSD, such as EMDR, combined somatic/cognitive therapies, self-help, non-TF-CBT and counselling and allowed, for the first time, simultaneous comparisons of cost-effectiveness across interventions, and their ranking from the most to the least cost-effective.

On the other hand, an economic evaluation of psychological interventions for PTSD in children and young people, which also used efficacy data derived from a NMA and adopted a similar approach and methodology to the analysis described here, concluded that individual forms of TF-CBT were most cost-effective in the treatment of children and young people with PTSD, whereas EMDR occupied middle cost-effectiveness rankings amongst the treatment options assessed [39]. This finding was attributed to the lower effectiveness of EMDR relative to other treatments in children and young people compared with adult populations [40].

Generalisability of the results and implications of the study

Our analysis was conducted from the perspective of the NHS/PSS in England. Results may be generalisable to other settings with similar funding and structure of healthcare and personal social services and comparable care pathways for adults with PTSD. Conclusions on cost-effectiveness ultimately rely on the cost-effectiveness threshold adopted, and this depends on the policy makers’ willingness-to-pay for treatment benefits, which may vary across countries and health systems.

Our analysis estimated the resource use relating to the delivery of each intervention based on information reported in the RCTs that informed the economic analysis; for example, the mode number of hours for a course of EMDR and TF-CBT was 9 and 13.5, respectively. If the duration and therefore the cost of an intervention is considerably different from our estimates, then its relative cost-effectiveness is expected to be affected. However, reducing the number of sessions of an intervention will improve its cost-effectiveness only if its clinical effectiveness remains unaffected. In practice, a reduction in the number of sessions below a point that is critical for the optimal delivery of the intervention is expected to reduce its clinical effectiveness, too; the impact on its cost-effectiveness will depend on the trade-off between a lower intervention cost and a lower clinical effectiveness.

Based on the results of the NMAs and the economic analysis, the NICE guideline on PTSD recommended EMDR and individual TF-CBT for the treatment of adults with PTSD presenting more than 3 months after trauma [5]. Both interventions were shown to be effective in reducing PTSD symptoms post-treatment and were the only ones with sufficient evidence to suggest sustainment of effect beyond treatment. EMDR appeared to be the most cost-effective intervention amongst those assessed. TF-CBT appeared to be less cost-effective than other interventions (i.e. combined somatic/cognitive therapies, psychoeducation, self-help with support and SSRIs), but had by far the largest evidence base and the guideline economic analysis showed that brief individual TF-CBT (delivered in fewer than 8 sessions) had the highest clinical and cost effectiveness amongst all options assessed; the finding that brief individual TF-CBT had the highest clinical effectiveness was explained by inspection of the clinical data, which revealed that participants in trials of brief individual TF-CBT had less severe PTSD symptoms at baseline, and therefore were likely to have a better response to treatment, compared with participants in trials of more intensive forms of individual TF-CBT. The recommendation for EMDR was restricted to people with non-combat-related trauma, as evidence suggested a non-significant effect on people with combat-related trauma.

The NICE guideline recommendations on TF-CBT and EMDR for adults with PTSD are consistent with other published PTSD clinical practice guidelines (compared in [41]). Three more guidelines make recommendations of equal strength for TF-CBT and EMDR [4244], whereas one guideline makes a strong recommendation for TF-CBT while EMDR has been given a moderate rating [45].

Self-help with support was shown to be the third most cost-effective option amongst those assessed, owing to a combination of its high effectiveness at treatment endpoint (informed by N = 198 across 5 RCTs in the respective NMA [3]) and its low intervention cost. There was also limited evidence (N = 85 across 3 RCTs in the respective NMA [3]) that it can sustain effects beyond treatment endpoint. All 5 RCTs on self-help with support that informed the economic analysis focused on computerised TF-CBT, which is consistent with TF-CBT delivered by a therapist, and this element may have been the driver of the intervention’s clinical effectiveness. The NICE guideline committee considered the clinical and cost-effectiveness of self-help with support and, also, that of SSRIs, but noted their narrower evidence base and made weaker (‘consider’) recommendations for people who expressed a preference for these interventions, and, in the case of self-help, did not have severe PTSD symptoms and were not at risk of harm to themselves or others. Based on its middle rankings in the NMA and economic analysis, a ‘consider’ recommendation was also made for non-TF-CBT targeted at specific symptoms, for people who are unable or unwilling to engage in a trauma-focused intervention or have residual symptoms after treatment. Psychoeducation was shown to be cost-effective based on limited and inconclusive clinical evidence; therefore, it was not recommended as a stand-alone intervention, but as part of individual TF-CBT. Finally, the committee noted the evidence of high clinical and cost-effectiveness for combined somatic/cognitive therapies, but also considered their particularly limited evidence base beyond treatment endpoint and the lack of specific indications for these interventions, and decided not to recommend them but instead to make a recommendation for further research [5].

Conclusion

EMDR appears to be the most cost-effective intervention for adults with PTSD more than 3 months after trauma, followed by combined somatic/cognitive therapies, self-help with support, psychoeducation, SSRIs, TF-CBT, self-help without support, non-TF-CBT and combined TF-CBT/SSRIs. Counselling appears to be less cost-effective than no treatment. Results were characterised by uncertainty, and relatively limited evidence base for interventions other than TF-CBT. There is a need for well-conducted studies that examine the relative clinical and cost-effectiveness of a range of psychological treatments for adults with PTSD, in particular assessment of longer-term costs and effects, to reduce the uncertainty and limitations characterising current evidence.

Supporting information

S1 File. Selection of effectiveness data and transformation for use in the economic analysis.

(DOCX)

S2 File. Estimation of the baseline probability of remission.

(DOCX)

S3 File. Estimation of the unit cost of therapists delivering psychological interventions for PTSD in the British National Health Service (NHS).

(DOCX)

S4 File. Estimation of annual health and personal social service costs incurred by adults with PTSD and adults without PTSD.

(DOCX)

S5 File. Results of secondary probabilistic economic analysis [beneficial effect up to 3-months post-treatment].

(DOCX)

S6 File. Results of deterministic sensitivity analyses.

(DOCX)

S7 File. Results of the NICE guideline economic analysis.

(DOCX)

S1 Appendix. Search strategy.

(DOCX)

S2 Appendix. Study protocol.

(DOCX)

S3 Appendix. Details of the statistical analysis and WinBUGS codes for data synthesis.

(DOCX)

S4 Appendix. Details of the inconsistency checks and WinBUGS codes for inconsistency models.

(DOCX)

S5 Appendix. Characteristics of studies included in the network meta-analysis, and full references.

(DOCX)

S6 Appendix. List of excluded studies with reasons for exclusion.

(DOCX)

S7 Appendix. NMA data files.

(DOCX)

S8 Appendix. Risk of bias of studies included in the NMA.

(DOCX)

S9 Appendix. Model fit statistics.

(DOCX)

S10 Appendix. Inconsistency checks.

(DOCX)

S11 Appendix. Relative effects between all pairs of interventions: Direct, indirect and combined (NMA) results.

(DOCX)

S12 Appendix. Results of the NICE guideline NMA.

(DOCX)

S13 Appendix. Pairwise sub-analyses.

(DOCX)

S14 Appendix. References in the online supplementary material.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank other members of the Guideline Committee for the NICE guideline on ‘Post-traumatic stress disorder’ for their contributions to this work. Members of the Guideline Committee were: Steve Hajioff, Philip Bell, Gita Bhutani, Sharif El-Leithy, Neil Greenberg, Nick Grey, Cornelius Katona, Jonathan Leach, Richard Meiser-Stedman, Rebecca Regler, Vikki Touzel, and David Trickey.

Data Availability

Full details on the methods and the clinical studies included in the network meta-analysis that informed the economic analysis are provided in Mavranezouli et al., Psychol Med. 2020 Mar;50(4):542-555. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720000070. All other relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

The economic analysis presented in this paper was initiated by the National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health (NCCMH) and continued by the National Guideline Alliance (NGA) at the Royal College of Obstetricians and Gynaecologists (RCOG) from 1 April 2016, with support from the National Institute of Health and Care Excellence (NICE) Guidelines Technical Support Unit (TSU), University of Bristol, which is funded by the Centre for Guidelines (NICE). NCCMH and NGA received funding from NICE to develop clinical and social care guidelines. IM, OMG, SS and SP received support from the National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health and the National Guideline Alliance, which were in receipt of funding from the National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE), for the submitted work. CD, SD and NJW received support from the NICE Guidelines Technical Support Unit, University of Bristol, with funding from the Centre for Clinical Practice (NICE). The views expressed in this publication are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the RCOG, NGA, NCCMH or NICE. The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. All authors had full access to all the data in the study and had final responsibility for the decision to submit for publication. National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (2018) Post-traumatic stress disorder. Available from: https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng116

References

  • 1.Koenen KC, Ratanatharathorn A, Ng L, McLaughlin KA, Bromet EJ, Stein DJ, et al. Posttraumatic stress disorder in the World Mental Health Surveys. Psychol Med. 2017; 47(13):2260–74. 10.1017/S0033291717000708 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Alonso J, Angermeyer MC, Bernert S, Bruffaerts R, Brugha TS, Bryson H, et al. Disability and quality of life impact of mental disorders in Europe: results from the European Study of the Epidemiology of Mental Disorders (ESEMeD) project. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 2004; (420):38–46. 10.1111/j.1600-0047.2004.00329.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Mavranezouli I, Megnin-Viggars O, Daly C, Dias S, Welton N, Stockton S, et al. Psychological treatments for post-traumatic stress disorder in adults: a network meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2020; 50(4):542–55. 10.1017/S0033291720000070 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.McManus S, Bebbington P, Jenkins R, Brugha T (eds). Mental health and wellbeing in England: Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey 2014. Leeds: NHS Digital; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.National Institute for Health and Care Excellence. Post-traumatic stress disorder. NICE; 2018. https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng116 [PubMed]
  • 6.Ehlers A, Grey N, Wild J, Stott R, Liness S, Deale A, et al. Implementation of cognitive therapy for PTSD in routine clinical care: effectiveness and moderators of outcome in a consecutive sample. Behav Res Ther. 2013; 51(11):742–52. 10.1016/j.brat.2013.08.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Office for National Statistics. Population Estimates for UK, England and Wales, Scotland and Northern Ireland: Mid-2016. ONS; 2017. http://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/birthsdeathsandmarriages/lifeexpectancies/bulletins/nationallifetablesunitedkingdom/2015-09-23
  • 8.Church D, Hawk C, Brooks AJ, Toukolehto O, Wren M, Dinter I, et al. Psychological trauma symptom improvement in veterans using emotional freedom techniques: a randomized controlled trial. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2013; 201(2):153–60. 10.1097/NMD.0b013e31827f6351 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Robson RH, Robson PM, Ludwig R, Mitabu C, Phillips C. Effectiveness of thought field therapy provided by newly instructed community workers to a traumatized population in Uganda: A randomized trial. Curr Res Psychol. 2016; 7(1):1–11. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Shapiro F. Eye Movement Desensitisation and Reprocessing: Basic Principles, Protocols, and Procedures. 2nd ed New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Rogers CR. Client-Centered Therapy: Its Current Practice, Implications and Theory. Boston: Houghton Mifflin; 1951. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Chapman C, Mills K, Slade T, McFarlane AC, Bryant RA, Creamer M, et al. Remission from post-traumatic stress disorder in the general population. Psychol Med. 2012; 42(8):1695–703. 10.1017/S0033291711002856 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Anderson HD, Pace WD, Libby AM, West DR, Valuck RJ. Rates of 5 common antidepressant side effects among new adult and adolescent cases of depression: a retrospective US claims study. Clin Ther. 2012; 34(1):113–23. 10.1016/j.clinthera.2011.11.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Coupland C, Dhiman P, Morriss R, Arthur A, Barton G, Hippisley-Cox J. Antidepressant use and risk of adverse outcomes in older people: population based cohort study. BMJ. 2011; 343:d4551 10.1136/bmj.d4551 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Jakobsen JC, Katakam KK, Schou A, Hellmuth SG, Stallknecht SE, Leth-Moller K, et al. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors versus placebo in patients with major depressive disorder. A systematic review with meta-analysis and Trial Sequential Analysis. BMC Psychiatry. 2017; 17(1):58 10.1186/s12888-016-1173-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Ahmadi N, Hajsadeghi F, Mirshkarlo HB, Budoff M, Yehuda R, Ebrahimi R. Post-traumatic stress disorder, coronary atherosclerosis, and mortality. Am J Cardiol. 2011; 108(1):29–33. 10.1016/j.amjcard.2011.02.340 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Office for National Statistics. National Life Tables, England, 1980–82 to 2014–2016. ONS; 2017. http://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/birthsdeathsandmarriages/lifeexpectancies/bulletins/nationallifetablesunitedkingdom/2015-09-23
  • 18.Mihalopoulos C, Magnus A, Lal A, Dell L, Forbes D, Phelps A. Is implementation of the 2013 Australian treatment guidelines for posttraumatic stress disorder cost-effective compared to current practice? A cost-utility analysis using QALYs and DALYs. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2015; 49(4):360–76. 10.1177/0004867414553948 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Freed MC, Yeager DE, Liu X, Gore KL, Engel CC, Magruder KM. Preference-weighted health status of PTSD among veterans: an outcome for cost-effectiveness analysis using clinical data. Psychiatr Serv. 2009; 60(9):1230–8. 10.1176/ps.2009.60.9.1230 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Brazier J, Roberts J, Deverill M. The estimation of a preference-based measure of health from the SF-36. J Health Econ. 2002; 21(2):271–92. 10.1016/s0167-6296(01)00130-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Sullivan PW, Valuck R, Saseen J, MacFall HM. A comparison of the direct costs and cost effectiveness of serotonin reuptake inhibitors and associated adverse drug reactions. CNS Drugs. 2004; 18(13):911–32. 10.2165/00023210-200418130-00006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Curtis L, Burns A. Unit Costs of Health & Social Care 2017. Canterbury: PSSRU; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.NHS Business Services Authority, NHS Prescription Services. NHS England and Wales. Electronic Drug Tariff. Issue: March 2018. Compiled on the behalf of the Department of Health and Social Care. 2018. https://www.nhsbsa.nhs.uk/pharmacies-gp-practices-and-appliance-contractors/drug-tariff
  • 24.Kaltenthaler E, Brazier J, De NE, Tumur I, Ferriter M, Beverley C, et al. Computerised cognitive behaviour therapy for depression and anxiety update: a systematic review and economic evaluation. Health Technol Assess. 2006; 10(33) 10.3310/hta10330 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.British Association for Behavioural & Cognitive Psychotherapies. Criteria and guidelines for re-accreditation as a behavioural and/or cognitive psychotherapist. British Association for Behavioural & Cognitive Psychotherapies; 2016. http://www.babcp.com/files/Accreditation/CBP/Full/CBP-Full-Guidelines-V5-0614.pdf
  • 26.National College for Teaching and Leadership. Review of clinical and educational psychology training arrangements. NHS Health Education England; 2016.
  • 27.Kontopantelis E, Olier I, Planner C, Reeves D, Ashcroft DM, Gask L, et al. Primary care consultation rates among people with and without severe mental illness: a UK cohort study using the Clinical Practice Research Datalink. BMJ Open. 2015; 5(12):e008650 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-008650 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.NHS Digital. Hospital Episode Statistics for England. Admitted Patient Care statistics, 2016–17. NHS Digital; 2017.
  • 29.NHS Digital, Community and Mental Health Team. Psychological Therapies: Annual report on the use of IAPT services. England, 2015–16. NHS Digital; 2016.
  • 30.NHS Improvement. National Schedule of Reference Costs, 2016–17—NHS trusts and NHS foundation trusts. NHS Improvement; 2017. https://improvement.nhs.uk/resources/reference-costs/
  • 31.National Institute for Health and Care Excellence. Developing NICE guidelines: the manual (last updated October 2018). NICE; 2014. https://www.nice.org.uk/process/pmg20 [PubMed]
  • 32.Briggs A, Sculpher M, Claxton K. Decision Modelling for Health Economic Evaluation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Fenwick E, Claxton K, Sculpher M. Representing uncertainty: the role of cost-effectiveness acceptability curves. Health Econ. 2001; 10(8):779–87. 10.1002/hec.635 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Chinn S. A simple method for converting an odds ratio to effect size for use in meta-analysis. Stat Med. 2000; 19(22):3127–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Caldwell DM, Ades AE, Higgins JP. Simultaneous comparison of multiple treatments: combining direct and indirect evidence. BMJ. 2005; 331(7521):897–900. 10.1136/bmj.331.7521.897 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Tuerk PW, Wangelin B, Rauch SAM, Dismuke CE, Yoder M, Myrick H, et al. Health service utilization before and after evidence-based treatment for PTSD. Psychol Serv. 2013; 10(4):401–9. 10.1037/a0030549 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Le QA, Doctor JN, Zoellner LA, Feeny NC. Cost-effectiveness of prolonged exposure therapy versus pharmacotherapy and treatment choice in posttraumatic stress disorder (the optimizing ptsd treatment trial): A doubly randomized preference trial. J Clin Psychiatry. 2014; 75(3):222–30. 10.4088/JCP.13m08719 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Dunn NJ, Rehm LP, Schillaci J, Souchek J, Mehta P, Ashton CM, et al. A randomized trial of self-management and psychoeducational group therapies for comorbid chronic posttraumatic stress disorder and depressive disorder. J Trauma Stress. 2007; 20(3):221–37. 10.1002/jts.20214 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Mavranezouli I, Megnin-Viggars O, Trickey D, Meiser-Stedman R, Daly C, Dias S, et al. Cost-effectiveness of psychological interventions for children and young people with post-traumatic stress disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2019. Epub 2019/10/28. 10.1111/jcpp.13142 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Mavranezouli I, Megnin-Viggars O, Daly C, Dias S, Stockton S, Meiser-Stedman R, et al. Research Review: Psychological and psychosocial treatments for children and young people with post-traumatic stress disorder: a network meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2020; 61(1):18–29. 10.1111/jcpp.13094 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Hamblen JL, Norman SB, Sonis JH, Phelps AJ, Bisson JI, Nunes VD, et al. A guide to guidelines for the treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder in adults: An update. Psychotherapy (Chic). 2019; 56(3):359–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Department of Veterans Affairs and Department of Defence (VA/DoD). VA/DoD clinical practice guideline for the management of posttraumatic stress disorder and acute stress disorder. Washington, DC: VA/DoD; 2017. https://www.healthquality.va.gov/guidelines/MH/ptsd/
  • 43.Phoenix Australia Centre for Posttraumatic Mental Health. Australian Guidelines for the treatment of Acute Stress Disorder & Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Phoenix Australia Centre for Posttraumatic Mental Health; 2013. https://www.phoenixaustralia.org/resources/ptsd-guidelines/
  • 44.International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies. ISTSS PTSD Prevention and Treatment Guidelines Methodology and Recommendations. ISTSS; 2019. www.istss.org/treating-trauma/new-istss-prevention-and-treatment-guidelines [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 45.American Psychological Association. Clinical Practice Guideline for the Treatment of PTSD. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2017. https://www.apa.org/ptsd-guideline/ptsd.pdf

Decision Letter 0

Scott McDonald

25 Nov 2019

PONE-D-19-20536

Cost-effectiveness of psychological treatments for post-traumatic stress disorder in adults

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr Mavranezouli,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by Jan 09 2020 11:59PM. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Scott McDonald

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

1. When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Thank you for stating the following in the Competing Interests section:

I have read the journal's policy and the authors of this manuscript have the following competing interests: SD and NJW were co-applicants on a grant (unrelated to this work) from the MRC Methodology Research Programme which included an MRC Industry Collaboration Agreement with Pfizer Ltd, who part-funded a researcher to work on statistical methodology. GB is a co-investigator on a NIHR RfPB grant, Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing Therapy in Early Psychosis (EYES): A feasibility randomised controlled trial. NGreenberg is the Royal College of Psychiatrists Lead for Military and Veterans’ Health and is a trustee of two military charities. He is also a senior researcher with King’s College London working on a number of military mental health studies. NGrey is a member of the Wellcome Trust Anxiety Disorders Group developing, testing and disseminating Cognitive Therapy for PTSD (CT-PTSD), a trauma-focused cognitive behavioural therapy (TF-CBT). He has published papers and book chapters on CT-PTSD, and facilitates teaching workshops for which payment is received. As editor, he receives royalties from sales of a trauma book, A Casebook of Cognitive Therapy for Traumatic Stress Reactions. CK is Medical Director of the Helen Bamber Foundation (a human rights charity) and refugee and asylum mental health lead for the Royal College of Psychiatrists. He writes expert psychiatric reports in the context of asylum mental health. JL is NHS England Medical Director for Military and Veterans Health. SP receives funding from NICE for the development of clinical guidelines and is also supported by the NIHR UCLH Biomedical Research Centre. The authors report no other relationships or activities that could appear to have influenced the submitted work.

We note that you received funding from a commercial source: Pfizer Ltd,

Please provide an amended Competing Interests Statement that explicitly states this commercial funder, along with any other relevant declarations relating to employment, consultancy, patents, products in development, marketed products, etc.

Within this Competing Interests Statement, please confirm that this does not alter your adherence to all PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials by including the following statement: "This does not alter our adherence to PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials.” (as detailed online in our guide for authors http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/competing-interests).  If there are restrictions on sharing of data and/or materials, please state these. Please note that we cannot proceed with consideration of your article until this information has been declared.

Please include your amended Competing Interests Statement within your cover letter. We will change the online submission form on your behalf.

Please know it is PLOS ONE policy for corresponding authors to declare, on behalf of all authors, all potential competing interests for the purposes of transparency. PLOS defines a competing interest as anything that interferes with, or could reasonably be perceived as interfering with, the full and objective presentation, peer review, editorial decision-making, or publication of research or non-research articles submitted to one of the journals. Competing interests can be financial or non-financial, professional, or personal. Competing interests can arise in relationship to an organization or another person. Please follow this link to our website for more details on competing interests: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/competing-interests

3. We note that you have stated that you will provide repository information for your data at acceptance. Should your manuscript be accepted for publication, we will hold it until you provide the relevant accession numbers or DOIs necessary to access your data. If you wish to make changes to your Data Availability statement, please describe these changes in your cover letter and we will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This archival data study addresses an important and under-studied question, and reports that EMDR was most cost-effective although TF-CBT had the strongest evidence base amongst 11 psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy approaches for PTSD treatment in the UK National Health Service.

Overall, the paper read more like a technical report than a journal article. As a non-economist, I found it very difficult to follow and disentangle all of the detailed assumptions and metrics required to calculate cost-effectiveness. Several specific limitations also made it difficult to accept the findings as meaningful for actually prioritizing treatments:

1. The treatments were proposed to be delivered on a short-term (3-month, with 3-months follow-up for the SSRI) basis. This may be the official norm in the NHS but it is not reflective of the actual clinical picture in treating chronic PTSD, and therefore is not applicable except when PTSD is relatively acute and resolves rapidly.

2. The low overall probability of remission in the 3-month treatment period and the 1-4 month follow-up period and frequent relapse (for which the .10 probability over 3 months seems very low, even if some “experts” opined that to be the case) means that for large majorities of patients receiving any of these treatments there will need to be continuing or alternative treatment and increasing costs (including due to impairment as well as further treatment, and related healthcare and services). Thus, the cost-effectiveness results are at best potentially relevant to very short-term outcomes, and at worst greatly over-estimate the value of the treatments. Over longer periods of time, it is likely that the most inexpensive and readily available help, self-help with or without support, will be most cost-effective but it is of limited effectiveness with PTSD that is more than mild in severity.

3. The data on effectiveness drawn from the in-review network meta-analysis are probably based on randomized clinical trials or evaluation studies that do not reflect the actual effects when these treatments are delivered in standard care.

4. The inclusion of patients with no diagnosis but above threshold self-reported symptoms means that many will have at most mild PTSD and recovery will be much more likely than with full (let alone chronic) PTSD. This further weights the results toward less clinically complex cases, which are the types of cases often tested in studies of EMDR and TF-CBT, so this may bias the results in favor of those interventions.

5. The differences in the overall outcome, NMB, seem relatively small (approximately 500-1000 pounds per person) for the top four interventions, so some justification of why they are meaningful differences is needed.

6. The cost-effectiveness differences are all essentially due to costs, because the QALY estimates are almost identical except for counselling. This suggests that small changes in the number of sessions provided fort the professional-delivered interventions will greatly alter cost-effectiveness (as is tangentially noted in the Discussion with the mention of the increased cost-effectiveness of TF-CBT if delivered for 8 sessions). Thus, rather than demonstrating the superior cost-effectiveness of any intervention this analysis seems mainly to show that the more extensive the professional time/costs involved the less cost-effective are the formal interventions, which is not surprising.

7. The comment about TF-CBT’s evidence base is not relevant because that is not the focus of the analyses presented.

Even with these limitations acknowledged, and the Abstract and Discussion revised to make the conclusions more cautious in endorsing EMDR and more inclusive in noting the cost-effectiveness of self-help with and without social support (despite its low effectiveness), the paper seems better suited for a journal audience of policymakers, administrators, or behavioral economists than a broader health professional/researcher audience.

Reviewer #2: The manuscript, “Cost-effectiveness of Psychological Treatments for Post-traumatic Stress Disorder in Adults,” covers an important topic with broad systemic and individual-level implications. Moreover, this article highlights the need to find a balance between cost-effectiveness and treating patients with clinically-indicated treatments with a solid evidence base. Finally, the use of a the quality of life outcome adds to the richness of the analysis beyond symptom reduction.

Introduction/Methods:

This manuscript would benefit from greater clarification regarding the treatments examined. While many of my questions were answered within the supplemental “PTSD adult NMA manuscript,” readers would benefit from having these treatments explained. Even after reviewing the supplemental manuscript, I am unsure which treatments qualify as “counseling” and what, if anything, differentiates the practitioners of counseling from the other treatments. It is unclear to me to what extent settings differ across these treatment types and how that affects the outcomes. For example, is it the restrictiveness/intensity of the treatment setting, as in inpatient treatment, that drives the cost-effectiveness of the treatment, the level of training from the provider, or researcher allegiance effects, more so than the type of treatment?

1) What qualifies as counseling (Interventions Section, p. 11)?

2) Which therapies are considered “combined somatic/cognitive therapies”? Is this classification determined by the length of sessions and level of therapist training (4 individual sessions by a band 7 therapist) or rather eclectic treatments which would not qualify as CBT, for example? (Interventions Section, p. 11)

3) What is the role of the treatment setting on the outcomes? For example, you mention in the Interventions section (p.11), that TF-CBT was divided by number of sessions and format of delivery in the economic analysis informing the NICE guideline, but that in the analyses here it was considered one treatment option. How might the results differ based on this grouping?

Results:

4) On Table 3 (p. 24), the probability of cost effectiveness of TF-CBT is listed as .26. The manuscripts notes elsewhere that the mode number of sessions was 13.5, yet when it is delivered in fewer than eight sessions it becomes the most cost-effective treatment. It seems that number of sessions becomes a critical factor in determining the cost effectiveness. It may be helpful to have a discussion or a range of results to reflect changes by number of sessions or other important factors.

5) Counseling (p. 24) is listed as less cost effective than no treatment. Which factors do the authors believe contribute to this finding?

6) A reference would be helpful for readers to examine the evidence base for EMDR given it’s controversial nature (e.g., Shaprio & Brown, 2019; American Psychological Association, Guideline Development Panel for the Treatment of PTSD in Adults, 2017).

Conclusions/Limitations

7) The manuscripts notes (p. 30) that TF-CBT appeared to be less cost-effective than other interventions, yet had “by far the most solid evidence base.” What implications does that have on the findings? Perhaps TF-CBT yields stronger maintenance of treatment gains over time; although it appears that comparable remission rates were unavailable for most other treatments.

8) In addition to grouping together TF-CBT by number of sessions, the limitations section of the supplemental manuscript notes that “TF-CBT” includes a broad range of therapies from CPT to mindfulness-based cognitive therapy, whereas other studies (e.g., Gerger et al., 2014) assess CBT, CT, and ET both separately and as a group. Given the high variation among interventions and between-trial heterogeneity, what might be the impact this grouping?

9) How might the timeline and complexity of the trauma histories impact the findings?

10) How do the included studies compare in terms of patient’s treatment histories (e.g., history of multiple psychological treatments and the timing of these treatments)?

Overall, this was a well-written manuscript representing an important contribution to PTSD research, while highlighting important gaps in the literature (e.g., long-term follow-ups, applicability of treatments to complex traumas).

Reviewer #3: This paper presents results from a detailed and extensive programme of work. It draws on a separately reported strand of analysis on clinical effectiveness of a range of different types of interventions for adults with PTSD. It builds on that work by combining it with new estimations of the costs of each type of intervention when delivered in England, so that both clinical-effectiveness and cost (both to services and to individuals) are consdiered . The paper presents the methods used to inform the NICE guidelines for PTSD treatment in England: in England NICE guidelines are absolutely pivotal to informing NHS clinical practice and resourcing decisions.

While much of this evidence may exist (in different form) in the NICE documentation alongside the guidelines, it is so important that the research underpinning guidelines is also made available in peer review journal form – for transparency and so it is indexed and identifiable for systematic review. The analysis in this paper also used some different parameters to inclusion to the guidelines. It would be useful if the authors could comment on what impact on results increasing the minimum number of participants in included studies from 50 to 100 had on the overall results (I might have missed this).

Please note I do not have great expertise in clinical cost-effectiveness calculations and do not feel able to properly comment on those methods. The process has been overseen by expert committee (some of whom are also authors), appears detailed, and is well-documented. The figures aid interpretation of methods as well as of findings, esp fig 1.

Such costings inevitably must deal in averages. The decisions on where to anchor those averages seems sensible (although the community data finds rates of screen-positive PTSD to be highest in 16-24 year olds, while the cohort anchors initiation age at 39 to reflect the treatment population. This may well be appropriate.

However, in the limitations it would be good to acknowledge that while – overall – EMDR may be the most cost-effective, this could vary with group characteristics. E.g. some interventions may be more clinically effective (and therefore, potentially, also more cost-effective) in the youngest age group, or in those with the most (or least) severe symptoms, or those with comorbid or delayed PTSD. Just something to acknowledge. An understanding of this is important in understanding the applications of the results, and how rigidly the recommendations should be applied in practice. There may be situations where a lower ranked intervention is the most appropriate.

I would have liked a line defining each intervention, or at least describing how the interventions evaluated were assigned to the intervention classifications. For example, ‘counselling’ gets a damning verdict – but I am not clear precisely what kinds of interventions were counted here. Are they simply interventions that were so poorly done that they were unclassifiable in any of the other categories? Which could be a confounding factor explaining their poor performance? Were all interventions assigned to one category only, or was there overlap?

Abstract has a sentence which reads: ‘TF-CBT has the most solid evidence base’ – take especial care in the abstract (which will be all that many read) that it is clear what this means (the usual disentangling ‘lack of evidence’ and ‘evidence of lack’ is needed).

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes: Sarah M. Scott, Ph.D

Reviewer #3: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2020 Apr 30;15(4):e0232245. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0232245.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


8 Jan 2020

Responses to reviewers' comments:

We thank the reviewers for their comments. Please note that the manuscript pages in our responses refer to the version of the revised manuscript submitted with tracked changes.

Reviewer #1:

This archival data study addresses an important and under-studied question, and reports that EMDR was most cost-effective although TF-CBT had the strongest evidence base amongst 11 psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy approaches for PTSD treatment in the UK National Health Service.

Overall, the paper read more like a technical report than a journal article. As a non-economist, I found it very difficult to follow and disentangle all of the detailed assumptions and metrics required to calculate cost-effectiveness.

RESPONSE: we thank the reviewer for their comment. We gave details on all assumptions and metrics required for the economic analysis to ensure transparency. We provided the standard level of detail used to support a model-based economic evaluation paper, as recommended in the literature. See for example:

Husereau et al. Consolidated Health Economic Evaluation Reporting Standards (CHEERS) statement. Cost Effectiveness and Resource Allocation 2013; 11(6)

https://doi.org/10.1186/1478-7547-11-6

And

Husereau et al. Consolidated Health Economic Evaluation Reporting Standards (CHEERS)—Explanation and Elaboration: A Report of the ISPOR Health Economic Evaluation Publication Guidelines Good Reporting Practices Task Force. Value in Health 2013; 16(2): 231-50.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jval.2013.02.002

Several specific limitations also made it difficult to accept the findings as meaningful for actually prioritizing treatments:

1. The treatments were proposed to be delivered on a short-term (3-month, with 3-months follow-up for the SSRI) basis. This may be the official norm in the NHS but it is not reflective of the actual clinical picture in treating chronic PTSD, and therefore is not applicable except when PTSD is relatively acute and resolves rapidly.

RESPONSE: The economic analysis has assessed the cost-effectiveness of first-line interventions (provided, on average, over 3 months), but treatment for people with PTSD is not limited to 3-months in the model. People not remitting following first-line treatment or relapsing (i.e. all people in the PTSD health state) were assumed to receive further treatment comprising standard care, as reflected in the PTSD state costs (see page 16, lines 19-28 and S4 file, which provides annual health and personal social service use and costs incurred by people with PTSD and those without PTSD). Resource use of standard care was estimated based on a national psychiatric morbidity survey conducted in England in 2014, supplemented with resource use data from other published sources and expert opinion, which were subsequently combined with national unit costs, as reported on page 16. This longer-term further treatment was common to all arms of the economic model and included primary, community and secondary healthcare as well as personal social services. We have now added text under ‘Model structure’ to clarify this (page 8, lines 11-13).

Please note that these costs were estimated over a 3-year period, which was the time horizon of the analysis. As stated on page 8, lines 17-19, this time frame was deemed adequate to capture longer-term costs and effects of treatment, without making significant extrapolations and assumptions over the course of PTSD.

2. The low overall probability of remission in the 3-month treatment period and the 1-4 month follow-up period and frequent relapse (for which the .10 probability over 3 months seems very low, even if some “experts” opined that to be the case) means that for large majorities of patients receiving any of these treatments there will need to be continuing or alternative treatment and increasing costs (including due to impairment as well as further treatment, and related healthcare and services). Thus, the cost-effectiveness results are at best potentially relevant to very short-term outcomes, and at worst greatly over-estimate the value of the treatments. Over longer periods of time, it is likely that the most inexpensive and readily available help, self-help with or without support, will be most cost-effective but it is of limited effectiveness with PTSD that is more than mild in severity.

RESPONSE:

The probability of remission in the 3-month treatment period was estimated to be 0.044 for no treatment (which was used as baseline), based on data from a national survey, as explained in S2 file. The probability of remission in the 3-month treatment period for each of the active interventions was estimated by applying the relative effects (expressed as odds ratios) of each intervention versus no treatment, obtained from the network meta-analysis (and reported in Table 2), onto the baseline probability of remission for no treatment. This gave 3-month probabilities of remission ranging from 0.15 (counselling) to 0.65 (EMDR). Following that period, and for the remaining duration of the model, we conservatively applied probabilities of remission using the survey data across all arms of the model.

People who remitted were at risk of relapse for the whole duration of the economic model. The risk of relapse was based on advice and consensus amongst experts with clinical experience in treating people with PTSD, as we were not able to identify reliable long-term relapse data from relevant longitudinal studies. This is standard recommended source of information in order to populate economic models when relevant published literature is not available. We would be grateful if the reviewer could direct us to a reliable source of relapse rates for people with PTSD. Nevertheless, we did sensitivity analysis were the relapse risk of 0.10 was varied between 0.05 and 0.20; results of this analyses, which are reported in S6 file, show that the findings of the base-case analysis were not affected, with the exception of TF-CBT which dropped one place in the cost-effectiveness ranking. This finding has already been reported in the manuscript (page 28, lines 140-143).

As we explain in response to another comment by the reviewer, people not remitting after receiving one of the assessed interventions and people relapsing following remission (i.e. all people in the PTSD state) were assumed to receive standard care for PTSD and to incur primary, community and secondary healthcare as well as personal social service costs for the remaining duration of the economic model, the time horizon of which was 3 years.

Therefore, our cost-effectiveness results are not only relevant to very short-term outcomes, but cover a period of 3 years. We also disagree with the view that over longer periods of time it is likely that inexpensive and readily available self-help, with or without support, will be most cost-effective. Our analysis showed that self-help without support is not cost-effective over 3 years, although it was the most inexpensive intervention (regarding intervention cost); self-help with support was amongst the most cost-effective options, but this finding was partly driven by the effectiveness of self-help, which ranked 4th best intervention in terms of effectiveness, as it can be seen in Table 2. We also note that, as can be seen in Table 3, EMDR was the most cost-effective intervention although it had one of the highest intervention costs. So, it is not the cost alone that determines the cost-effectiveness of an intervention, it is the combination of its cost and effectiveness, in relation to respective combinations of cost and effectiveness for other alternative treatment options.

3. The data on effectiveness drawn from the in-review network meta-analysis are probably based on randomized clinical trials or evaluation studies that do not reflect the actual effects when these treatments are delivered in standard care.

RESPONSE: The data on relative effects were obtained from a network meta-analysis of RCTs, as the RCT design is the gold standard for estimating relative effects. However, relative effects were applied onto baseline (no treatment) absolute remission rates of PTSD derived from a national survey in the general population, to obtain estimates of treatments’ absolute effects under standard care delivery.

4. The inclusion of patients with no diagnosis but above threshold self-reported symptoms means that many will have at most mild PTSD and recovery will be much more likely than with full (let alone chronic) PTSD. This further weights the results toward less clinically complex cases, which are the types of cases often tested in studies of EMDR and TF-CBT, so this may bias the results in favor of those interventions.

RESPONSE: We do not agree with the view that patients with no formal PTSD diagnosis but who are above the threshold on a PTSD scale (which may be self-reported but may also be clinician-rated) will have necessarily mild PTSD. Also, we do not have reasons to believe a-priori that EMDR and TF-CBT have been tested on less clinically complex cases. In fact, the evidence dataset that was used to inform the economic model includes the following populations:

As we report in our NMA paper (accepted for publication in Psychological Medicine), of the 90 studies that were included in our NMAs, 58 (64%) included patients with a formal PTSD diagnosis.

Of the 84 studies that reported continuous data and were included in the NMAs that informed the economic analysis, 52 (62%) included patients with a formal PTSD diagnosis.

Regarding studies testing each intervention considered in the economic analysis, the percentage of studies with a formal PTSD diagnosis was:

• Trauma-focused CBT: 29/37 (78.3%)

• Trauma-focused CBT with SSRIs 3/3 (100%)

• EMDR 9/12 diagnosis (75%)

• Non-trauma-focused CBT 4/8 (50%)

• Combined somatic/cognitive therapies 1/4 (25%)

• Self-help with support 4/6 diagnosis (66%)

• Self-help without support: 3/11 (27%)

• Counselling: 8/11 (73%)

• Psychoeducation: 1/3 (33%)

This information is provided in Appendix 5 of our NMA paper. Note that the number of studies on this list is higher than the total number of studies (84) considered in the economic analysis because some studies assessed more than one active intervention and have been counted twice or even three times.

Studies on EMDR had one of the highest percentages of formal PTSD diagnosis (in contrast to what the reviewer had hypothesised), which, according to the reviewer’s argument, should have biased the results against EMDR, as the populations were more likely to have more complex PTSD. However, EMDR was shown to have the highest efficacy amongst treatments. On the other extreme, studies on self-help without support had one of the lowest percentages of formal PTSD diagnosis, which, according to the reviewer, would mean that the intervention was tested on milder PTSD cases and thus higher effectiveness would be expected, yet it was one of the least effective interventions in the dataset, as shown in Table 2 of the manuscript.

5. The differences in the overall outcome, NMB, seem relatively small (approximately 500-1000 pounds per person) for the top four interventions, so some justification of why they are meaningful differences is needed.

RESPONSE: A higher NMB suggests that an intervention is more cost-effective than its comparator, regardless of the magnitude of the difference between NMBs, although a difference in cost of £500-£1,000 per person over 3 years is not negligible if projected to the total population of people with PTSD receiving care. An intervention with a lower NMB is by definition less cost-effective, and its use instead of the more cost-effective option would result in inefficient use of resources. It needs to be noted that the uncertainty around cost-effectiveness, as reflected in the mean rank and in the probability of the best intervention being cost-effective, should also be taken into account when interpreting the difference in NMBs (and it may be more important than the magnitude of the absolute difference in NMBs when interpreting results). The NMB combines costs and effects (QALYs) into a single metric, to simplify judgements on cost-effectiveness. Please note that NMB represents net monetary benefit, i.e. the intervention’s benefit expressed in pounds (£) minus the intervention’s cost, hence the higher NMB an intervention has, the more cost-effective it is.

Regarding cost differences between 2 interventions, and whether they are meaningful or not: a difference in cost between two interventions of £500-£1000 per person may be considered high if the extra benefit is minimal; it is a waste of money if both interventions have the same effect; and it is a harmful expenditure if the costlier intervention has a worse outcome than its comparator.

Nevertheless, cost-effectiveness results need to be considered alongside uncertainty (as already stated), the magnitude and quality of the evidence base, the (un)suitability of some interventions for specific sub-populations, and other relevant factors; for this reason, cost-effectiveness was not the sole factor that the NICE guideline committee took into account when making recommendations, as reported on pages 33, line 284 to 35, line 323.

6. The cost-effectiveness differences are all essentially due to costs, because the QALY estimates are almost identical except for counselling. This suggests that small changes in the number of sessions provided fort the professional-delivered interventions will greatly alter cost-effectiveness (as is tangentially noted in the Discussion with the mention of the increased cost-effectiveness of TF-CBT if delivered for 8 sessions). Thus, rather than demonstrating the superior cost-effectiveness of any intervention this analysis seems mainly to show that the more extensive the professional time/costs involved the less cost-effective are the formal interventions, which is not surprising.

RESPONSE: we disagree with the view that QALY estimates are almost identical. For example, the difference between EMDR (most effective intervention, 1.80 QALYs) and psychoeducation (1.74 QALYs) was 0.06 QALYs per person over a 3-year period, or 2 QALYs (i.e. 2 years in perfect health) per 100 people in one year. We note that the difference in utility between the state of PTSD and the state of no PTSD in men is 0.09 (according to Table 2), which translates into a difference of 9 QALYs per 100 men moving from the PTSD into the no PTSD state in one year. If EMDR is provided instead of psychoeducation, this leads to 2 additional QALYs, which translates into 2 / 9 = 0.22 or 22 additional men out of 100 moving from the PTSD state into the no PTSD state, i.e. remitting, without relapse, for a year, which is not negligible in our view. It is true that in the area of PTSD treatment there are no great differences between interventions in terms of QALYs, because interventions have an impact primarily on people’s quality of life and have a very limited impact on survival.

The finding that brief individual TF-CBT (delivered in fewer than 8 sessions) was the most cost-effective intervention amongst all options assessed in the guideline analysis was not driven exclusively by the intervention’s cost; it was primarily driven by the fact that the intervention was shown to be the most effective in the respective NMA that informed the guideline analysis, and therefore produced the highest number of QALYs, as shown in S7 file. We have now added this clarification in the manuscript (page 34, lines 294-298).

We disagree with the reviewer’s conclusion that “the more extensive the professional time/costs involved the less cost-effective are the formal interventions”, because this is not what our analysis shows: EMDR costs £400 more than combined somatic/cognitive therapies, £500 more than self-help with support, £600 more than a course of SSRIs and £650 more than self-help without support; yet, it was shown to be more cost-effective than all these options. TF-CBT costs £300 more than non-TF-CBT and is still more cost-effective. On the other hand, self-help without support was by far the least costly intervention and it ranked 7th out of the 11 options assessed. This finding suggests that the conclusion that “the more extensive the professional time/costs involved the less cost-effective interventions are” does not hold. It is the combination of intervention cost and effectiveness of each of the interventions included in the assessment that determine their relative cost effectiveness.

7. The comment about TF-CBT’s evidence base is not relevant because that is not the focus of the analyses presented.

RESPONSE: we feel that our comment is very relevant because a large evidence base (please note that we replaced ‘most solid’ with ‘largest’ throughout the manuscript for clarity) determined the clinical effectiveness of TF-CBT, and a larger evidence base means less uncertainty and more confidence in the findings. It is reassuring for us (and hopefully for the readers and users of the NICE guideline recommendations) that the clinical effectiveness of TF-CBT at treatment endpoint (and, subsequently, its cost-effectiveness) was determined by 29 RCTs, which tested TF-CBT on 903 participants; we would be less confident if the effect of TF-CBT was determined based on e.g. 1 RCT that tested TF-CBT on 10 participants.

Even with these limitations acknowledged, and the Abstract and Discussion revised to make the conclusions more cautious in endorsing EMDR and more inclusive in noting the cost-effectiveness of self-help with and without social support (despite its low effectiveness), the paper seems better suited for a journal audience of policymakers, administrators, or behavioral economists than a broader health professional/researcher audience.

RESPONSE: limitations of the study have been reported on pages 30-31; we feel that the abstract and discussion report clearly the results and the underlying uncertainty, and also the limitations of the analysis and propose further research to overcome these limitations. The abstract reports the ranking of all interventions, where it is shown that self-help with support was the 3rd most cost-effective intervention while self-help without support was the 7th most cost-effective intervention. We note that self-help with support was also the 4th most clinically effective intervention (as seen in Table 2), which contributed to its high cost-effectiveness; in contrast, self-help without support was one of the least effective interventions, which apparently contributed to its low cost-effectiveness, despite its low intervention costs. We would like to clarify here that with and without support does not mean social support, but clinical support by a therapist; we have now clarified this on page 7, lines 1-2.

Whether the paper is suited for PLOS One is for the editor to judge, but in our experience, economic evaluations of healthcare interventions are widely published in medical and health service research journals and are of interest not only to policy makers and administrators, but also to health professionals who need to make choices guided by efficient use of resources in their everyday practice; they are also of interest to researchers in the area of healthcare who can be guided by the reported methods and gaps in knowledge when conducting their own research. PLOS ONE is a scientific journal that publishes primary research from any discipline within science and medicine, so we feel that our clinical and economic research methods and findings are suited for publication in the journal.

Reviewer #2:

The manuscript, “Cost-effectiveness of Psychological Treatments for Post-traumatic Stress Disorder in Adults,” covers an important topic with broad systemic and individual-level implications. Moreover, this article highlights the need to find a balance between cost-effectiveness and treating patients with clinically-indicated treatments with a solid evidence base. Finally, the use of a quality of life outcome adds to the richness of the analysis beyond symptom reduction.

RESPONSE: we thank the reviewer for their comment.

Introduction/Methods:

This manuscript would benefit from greater clarification regarding the treatments examined. While many of my questions were answered within the supplemental “PTSD adult NMA manuscript,” readers would benefit from having these treatments explained. Even after reviewing the supplemental manuscript, I am unsure which treatments qualify as “counseling” and what, if anything, differentiates the practitioners of counseling from the other treatments.

RESPONSE: we thank the reviewer for their comment. We have provided definitions of interventions in the related PTSD adult NMA manuscript, but have now also defined the interventions considered in the economic analysis in this manuscript (pages 5-6). Therapists in counselling were not different in the RCTs included in the NMAs that informed the economic analysis. This is also reflected in our estimation of intervention costs, where “all psychological interventions, with the exception of self-help and psychoeducation, were assumed to be delivered in a primary care setting by Band 7 psychological therapists […], to reflect routine practice in the UK” (page 11 line 26 to page 12 line 3).

It is unclear to me to what extent settings differ across these treatment types and how that affects the outcomes. For example, is it the restrictiveness/intensity of the treatment setting, as in inpatient treatment, that drives the cost-effectiveness of the treatment, the level of training from the provider, or researcher allegiance effects, more so than the type of treatment?

RESPONSE: for the economic analysis, all treatments were assumed to be provided in primary care, and this was reflected in the costing of interventions. We have added text to clarify this point (page 11, line 27). All psychological interventions, with the exception of self-help and psychoeducation, were assumed to be delivered by Band 7 psychological therapists, to reflect routine practice in the UK; no difference in training costs were assumed across interventions delivered by Band 7 therapists. Psychoeducation was assumed to be delivered by AfC band 5 Psychological Well-being Practitioners (PWPs) and self-help by AfC band 6 psychological therapists (page 12, lines 3-4). So, the setting did not affect the cost ‘element’ of cost-effectiveness. It needs to be clarified that people not responding to treatment and those relapsing were assumed to receive additional standard care, comprising community, primary and secondary care (and incur relevant costs), which, however, was not determined by the type of intervention received at the start of the model.

Regarding the setting across RCTs included in the NMAs that informed in the economic analysis, in some RCTs, interventions might have been delivered in an inpatient setting, and this may have contributed to the heterogeneity between trials (this has now been discussed on page 30, lines 188-194). However, there was no systematic difference in the setting (inpatient or outpatient) across interventions considered in the RCTs.

Some researcher allegiance effects may have been responsible for the low effectiveness of counselling, as this served as the control intervention in a number of studies assessing other active interventions. However, we argue that it is the mechanism of counselling that was primarily responsible for its effect. This has now been discussed on page 29, lines 155-165.

Overall, the difference in cost effectiveness is attributed to the difference in the type of treatment (rather than confounding factors), which translates into different clinical effectiveness and intervention cost.

1) What qualifies as counseling (Interventions Section, p. 11)?

RESPONSE: we have now given the definition of counselling in the paper (page 6, lines 14-22).

2) Which therapies are considered “combined somatic/cognitive therapies”? Is this classification determined by the length of sessions and level of therapist training (4 individual sessions by a band 7 therapist) or rather eclectic treatments which would not qualify as CBT, for example? (Interventions Section, p. 11)

RESPONSE: we have now provided a definition for these interventions on page 6, lines 7-9. As it can be seen, this classification is determined by the content of the interventions and not by delivery characteristics (such as the length of sessions and level of therapist training).

3) What is the role of the treatment setting on the outcomes? For example, you mention in the Interventions section (p.11), that TF-CBT was divided by number of sessions and format of delivery in the economic analysis informing the NICE guideline, but that in the analyses here it was considered one treatment option. How might the results differ based on this grouping?

RESPONSE: the treatment setting had no impact on the outcomes. However, creating different TF-CBT categories by number of sessions and format had an impact on results because each category had its own clinical effectiveness and intervention cost, as now explained on page 5, lines 23-25. Brief individual TF-CBT was shown to be the most clinically and cost-effective treatment option in the guideline analysis, as reported on page 34, but this result (highest clinical effectiveness) was due to the populations recruited in these trials. We have reported this information on page 34, lines 293-298. Full results of the guideline economic analysis are provided in S7 file.

Results:

4) On Table 3 (p. 24), the probability of cost effectiveness of TF-CBT is listed as .26. The manuscripts notes elsewhere that the mode number of sessions was 13.5, yet when it is delivered in fewer than eight sessions it becomes the most cost-effective treatment. It seems that number of sessions becomes a critical factor in determining the cost effectiveness. It may be helpful to have a discussion or a range of results to reflect changes by number of sessions or other important factors.

RESPONSE: Brief individual TF-CBT was shown to be the most cost-effective option in the guideline analysis not only because it was the lowest number of sessions, but also because it was shown to be the most clinically effective intervention in the guideline NMA. This finding was explained by inspection of the clinical data, which revealed that participants in trials of brief individual TF-CBT had less severe PTSD symptoms at baseline, and therefore were likely to have a better response to treatment, compared with participants in trials of more intensive forms of individual TF-CBT. We have now added this information in the manuscript (page 34, lines 293-298).

Reducing the number of sessions may improve the cost-effectiveness of an intervention if its effectiveness remains unaffected. However, reducing the number of sessions below a ‘critical’ point will also likely reduce its clinical effectiveness, and therefore its cost-effectiveness will be determined by the trade-off between a lower intervention cost and a lower clinical effectiveness. This point has been added on page 33, lines 277-282.

5) Counseling (p. 24) is listed as less cost effective than no treatment. Which factors do the authors believe contribute to this finding?

RESPONSE: a brief explanation for this finding has now been added under results on page 27, lines 114-116 and in more detail under discussion on page 29, lines 155-165.

6) A reference would be helpful for readers to examine the evidence base for EMDR given it’s controversial nature (e.g., Shaprio & Brown, 2019; American Psychological Association, Guideline Development Panel for the Treatment of PTSD in Adults, 2017).

RESPONSE: we do not feel that the evidence base for EMDR is controversial. The clinical evidence for EMDR was derived, as for all interventions, from our systematic review and NMA, described in a related paper cited in the economic manuscript. The NMA paper includes references to other reviews. Nevertheless, we have now included an overview of recommendations on the use of TF-CBT and EMDR in other PTSD clinical practice guidelines in the manuscript, and included reference to the American Psychological Association guidelines (page 34, lines 302-306).

Conclusions/Limitations

7) The manuscripts notes (p. 30) that TF-CBT appeared to be less cost-effective than other interventions, yet had “by far the most solid evidence base.” What implications does that have on the findings? Perhaps TF-CBT yields stronger maintenance of treatment gains over time; although it appears that comparable remission rates were unavailable for most other treatments.

RESPONSE: we have now replaced ‘solid’ by ‘large’ for clarity. A larger evidence base give us more confidence in the findings on clinical effectiveness for TF-CBT; this point has been clarified on page 30, lines 205-206. It is also true that TF-CBT and EMDR were the only interventions with evidence of effect at follow up, and this information has now been added in the manuscript (page 29, lines 185-187).

8) In addition to grouping together TF-CBT by number of sessions, the limitations section of the supplemental manuscript notes that “TF-CBT” includes a broad range of therapies from CPT to mindfulness-based cognitive therapy, whereas other studies (e.g., Gerger et al., 2014) assess CBT, CT, and ET both separately and as a group. Given the high variation among interventions and between-trial heterogeneity, what might be the impact this grouping?

RESPONSE: we have now added text on this issue in our NMA paper, to justify our decision to analyse together different therapies within the TF-CBT class. We have noted that other studies have assessed some of these interventions, for which evidence was adequate, separately; the majority of these studies have found evidence on the effectiveness of all interventions within the TF-CBT class but none of the studies reported any evidence on differential effects between different types of TF-CBT. Gerger et al. found no difference in the effectiveness of different interventions within the TF-CBT class, which supports our decision to consider TF-CBT interventions together, as one class, in our analysis. Moreover, we did an exploratory post-hoc sub-analysis by specific TF-CBT intervention for all studies including a waitlist control, which suggests no significant sub-group difference between specific TF-CBT intervention types. These results are provided in our related clinical [NMA] paper.

9) How might the timeline and complexity of the trauma histories impact the findings?

RESPONSE: as we have acknowledged in our NMA paper, our NMA analyses were characterised by high between-trial heterogeneity, which may have been caused by heterogeneity across populations included in the trials considered in our analysis, for example, in terms of the presence of a formal PTSD diagnosis, the baseline severity and complexity of PTSD symptoms, the type, extent and multiplicity of trauma exposure, the chronicity of symptoms and the presence of comorbidity. Moreover, the vast majority of the studies included in the NMAs did not distinguish between PTSD and complex PTSD, because our review was undertaken before ICD-11 (and the distinction between PTSD and complex PTSD) was released. Trials are likely to have varied widely in the proportion of participants with complex PTSD and this may have had an impact on the effectiveness of assessed interventions in each study and the heterogeneity across studies.

We have conducted exploratory sub-analyses by multiplicity of index trauma for the TF-CBT versus waitlist comparison for the PTSD symptom change scores between baseline and endpoint outcome; our results showed no significant subgroup differences for single compared to multiple incident index trauma. This information has been added in our NMA paper.

We have also added a paragraph on the factors that may have contributed to the high heterogeneity in the NMA results that informed the economic analysis (page 30, lines 188-198).

10) How do the included studies compare in terms of patient’s treatment histories (e.g., history of multiple psychological treatments and the timing of these treatments)?

RESPONSE: such information (multiplicity and timing of previous psychological treatment) was not reported in most RCTs included in the NMAs, and thus this information was not possible to extract and assess. Nevertheless, we have added discussion on this issue (and its potential impact on heterogeneity) on page 30, lines 194-198.

Overall, this was a well-written manuscript representing an important contribution to PTSD research, while highlighting important gaps in the literature (e.g., long-term follow-ups, applicability of treatments to complex traumas).

RESPONSE: we thank the reviewer for their comment.

Reviewer #3:

This paper presents results from a detailed and extensive programme of work. It draws on a separately reported strand of analysis on clinical effectiveness of a range of different types of interventions for adults with PTSD. It builds on that work by combining it with new estimations of the costs of each type of intervention when delivered in England, so that both clinical-effectiveness and cost (both to services and to individuals) are considered . The paper presents the methods used to inform the NICE guidelines for PTSD treatment in England: in England NICE guidelines are absolutely pivotal to informing NHS clinical practice and resourcing decisions.

While much of this evidence may exist (in different form) in the NICE documentation alongside the guidelines, it is so important that the research underpinning guidelines is also made available in peer review journal form – for transparency and so it is indexed and identifiable for systematic review. The analysis in this paper also used some different parameters to inclusion to the guidelines. It would be useful if the authors could comment on what impact on results increasing the minimum number of participants in included studies from 50 to 100 had on the overall results (I might have missed this).

RESPONSE: we thank the reviewer for their comment. We have now added this information on page 7, lines 6-10.

Please note I do not have great expertise in clinical cost-effectiveness calculations and do not feel able to properly comment on those methods. The process has been overseen by expert committee (some of whom are also authors), appears detailed, and is well-documented. The figures aid interpretation of methods as well as of findings, esp fig 1.

Such costings inevitably must deal in averages. The decisions on where to anchor those averages seems sensible (although the community data finds rates of screen-positive PTSD to be highest in 16-24 year olds, while the cohort anchors initiation age at 39 to reflect the treatment population. This may well be appropriate.

RESPONSE: as stated on page 4, lines 10-12, determining the starting age of the cohort served only in estimating mortality of the cohort over time (as mortality changes with age) and had no impact on the effectiveness of the interventions. The community data refer to populations that are screened positive for PTSD, but they do not necessarily seek treatment for this condition. In contrast, as we explain in the manuscript (page 4, lines 6-7), the age of 39 years reflects the mean age of adults with PTSD presenting to healthcare services in the UK, which was the population of interest in our study.

However, in the limitations it would be good to acknowledge that while – overall – EMDR may be the most cost-effective, this could vary with group characteristics. E.g. some interventions may be more clinically effective (and therefore, potentially, also more cost-effective) in the youngest age group, or in those with the most (or least) severe symptoms, or those with comorbid or delayed PTSD. Just something to acknowledge. An understanding of this is important in understanding the applications of the results, and how rigidly the recommendations should be applied in practice. There may be situations where a lower ranked intervention is the most appropriate.

RESPONSE: we have now acknowledged that the NMAs that informed the economic analysis showed high between-study heterogeneity, which is likely to have been caused by heterogeneity across populations included in the trials considered in the NMAs, for example, in terms of the presence of a formal PTSD diagnosis, the severity, complexity and chronicity of PTSD symptoms, the type, extent and multiplicity of trauma exposure, the presence of comorbidity, the variability of interventions within each assessed option and the differences across settings, e.g. inpatient versus outpatient delivery of interventions (page 30, lines 188-198). However, we have no indication that some interventions may be more clinically and cost-effective in people with more or less severe PTSD, or those with comorbid or delayed PTSD. We do have evidence that EMDR is less clinically and cost-effective in younger populations, compared with other treatments, and this information has now been added in the manuscript (page 32, lines 256-263). We also have evidence that EMDR has a non-significant effect in people with combat-related trauma, which has had an impact on NICE recommendations on EMDR for adults with PTSD; this has been reported on page 34, lines 299-300. Furthermore, the NICE recommendations on adults with PTSD acknowledge that lower ranked interventions may be more suitable for some people, hence there are more recommendations on self-help with support, non-TF-CBT and SSRIs; these recommendations have been summarised in the manuscript (page 34, lines 309-318). Ultimately, the choice of treatment should be a joined decision between the clinician and the patient, and this is a wider issue underlying all NICE guidelines and not specific to PTSD guideline recommendations.

I would have liked a line defining each intervention, or at least describing how the interventions evaluated were assigned to the intervention classifications. For example, ‘counselling’ gets a damning verdict – but I am not clear precisely what kinds of interventions were counted here. Are they simply interventions that were so poorly done that they were unclassifiable in any of the other categories? Which could be a confounding factor explaining their poor performance? Were all interventions assigned to one category only, or was there overlap?

RESPONSE: we have now added definitions of all treatment options assessed in the economic analysis on pages 5-6. We can confirm that each intervention was assigned to one category only.

Abstract has a sentence which reads: ‘TF-CBT has the most solid evidence base’ – take especial care in the abstract (which will be all that many read) that it is clear what this means (the usual disentangling ‘lack of evidence’ and ‘evidence of lack’ is needed).

RESPONSE: we thank the reviewer for their comment. We have now amended ‘solid’ to ‘largest’ for clarity.

Attachment

Submitted filename: PTSD adult HE model Responses to reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Scott McDonald

10 Mar 2020

PONE-D-19-20536R1

Cost-effectiveness of psychological treatments for post-traumatic stress disorder in adults

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr Mavranezouli,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

I am enthusiastic about your submission and would like to recommend it for acceptance for publication. Please respond to the reviewer's comments.

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by Apr 24 2020 11:59PM. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Scott McDonald

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors' revisions are responsive and their rebuttals are generally persuasive. A few points of clarification would be helpful. 1. The much lower percentages of studies requiring a PTSD diagnosis evaluating somatic and psychoeducation interventions (25-33%) vs. in EMDR or TF-CBT (75-78%), and the very few studies (3-4) for each of the former modalities, makes conclusions for those modalities quite uncertain. From their NMA, can the authors provide any information (briefly) to help readers determine if the PTSD symptom severity was comparable at baseline for those studies versus for the studies of TF-CBT and EMDR? This would be helpful in judging whether this is really an apples-to-apples comparison of cost effectiveness that should favor somatic and psychoeducation modalities over TF-CBT. 2. The finding that self-help plus support had a relatively strong QUALY outcome and cost-effectiveness, with a relatively large number of studies most of which required a PTSD diagnosis, deserves a bit more highlighting in the Discussion.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes: Sarah M. Scott, Ph.D

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2020 Apr 30;15(4):e0232245. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0232245.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


3 Apr 2020

Responses to reviewers' comments:

Reviewer #1

The authors' revisions are responsive and their rebuttals are generally persuasive. A few points of clarification would be helpful.

We thank the reviewer for their useful comments.

1. The much lower percentages of studies requiring a PTSD diagnosis evaluating somatic and psychoeducation interventions (25-33%) vs. in EMDR or TF-CBT (75-78%), and the very few studies (3-4) for each of the former modalities, makes conclusions for those modalities quite uncertain. From their NMA, can the authors provide any information (briefly) to help readers determine if the PTSD symptom severity was comparable at baseline for those studies versus for the studies of TF-CBT and EMDR? This would be helpful in judging whether this is really an apples-to-apples comparison of cost effectiveness that should favor somatic and psychoeducation modalities over TF-CBT.

Response: we agree that the very few studies for somatic and psychoeducation interventions make conclusions for those modalities quite uncertain, and this is reflected in the Discussion of the paper on NICE recommendations:

“Psychoeducation was shown to be cost-effective based on limited and inconclusive clinical evidence; therefore, it was not recommended as a stand-alone intervention, but as part of individual TF-CBT. Finally, the committee noted the evidence of high clinical and cost-effectiveness for combined somatic/cognitive therapies, but also considered their particularly limited evidence base beyond treatment endpoint and the lack of specific indications for these interventions, and decided not to recommend them but instead to make a recommendation for further research” (page 36, lines 365-371).

Regarding inclusion criteria on PTSD diagnosis versus clinically important PTSD symptoms: We have already acknowledged this issue as a potential factor contributing to the high heterogeneity of the NMA: “Both NMAs were characterised by high between-trial heterogeneity, which is likely to have been caused by heterogeneity across populations included in the trials considered in the NMAs, for example, in terms of the presence of a formal PTSD diagnosis, the severity, complexity and chronicity of PTSD symptoms…” (page 30, lines 187-190).

Unfortunately it was not possible to determine if severity was comparable across trials at baseline using the mean PTSD symptom severity scores, because the studies have used a range of different PTSD symptom scales and no mapping function that could allow comparison of severity levels across the scales is available. We have attempted to assess comparability of populations by looking at the % of the mean score over cut-off or the % of mean score out of the total score across studies, but we concluded that this was not reliable because it is expected that different symptom scales will have different levels of redundancy built in.

However, we note that the categorisation of diagnosis versus clinically important symptoms is based on inclusion criteria so it is possible that most of the participants in the ‘clinically important symptom’ studies could have a diagnosis, they were just not required to do so in order to participate in the trial, and this could well have been for pragmatic reasons to do with trial management rather than an indication of differing severity. It is also the case that for scales that are based closely on diagnostic criteria, e.g. PTSD Checklist (PCL), scoring above the clinical threshold may be regarded as comparable to receiving a diagnosis. The studies that evaluate combined somatic and cognitive therapies all use scales that are based on diagnostic criteria (e.g. PCL and Modified PTSD Symptom Scale [MPSS-SR]), and the majority of the studies evaluating psychoeducation also use scales based on diagnostic criteria (e.g. PCL and Davidson Trauma Scale).

We have now added the above points in the Discussion section (page 30, lines 199 to page 31, line 235).

2. The finding that self-help plus support had a relatively strong QUALY outcome and cost-effectiveness, with a relatively large number of studies most of which required a PTSD diagnosis, deserves a bit more highlighting in the Discussion.

Response: we have now added discussion on the cost-effectiveness of self-help with support (page 36, lines 350-58). We have covered discussion on the issue of formal diagnosis of PTSD vs clinically important PTSD symptoms and the potential impact on the comparability of populations regarding baseline symptom severity – please refer to our response to your previous comment.

Attachment

Submitted filename: PTSD adult HE model Responses to reviewers R2.docx

Decision Letter 2

Scott McDonald

13 Apr 2020

Cost-effectiveness of psychological treatments for post-traumatic stress disorder in adults

PONE-D-19-20536R2

Dear Dr. Mavranezouli,

We are pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it complies with all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you will receive an e-mail containing information on the amendments required prior to publication. When all required modifications have been addressed, you will receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will proceed to our production department and be scheduled for publication.

Shortly after the formal acceptance letter is sent, an invoice for payment will follow. To ensure an efficient production and billing process, please log into Editorial Manager at https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the "Update My Information" link at the top of the page, and update your user information. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, you must inform our press team as soon as possible and no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

With kind regards,

Scott McDonald

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Scott McDonald

20 Apr 2020

PONE-D-19-20536R2

Cost-effectiveness of psychological treatments for post-traumatic stress disorder in adults

Dear Dr. Mavranezouli:

I am pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

For any other questions or concerns, please email plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE.

With kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Scott McDonald

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 File. Selection of effectiveness data and transformation for use in the economic analysis.

    (DOCX)

    S2 File. Estimation of the baseline probability of remission.

    (DOCX)

    S3 File. Estimation of the unit cost of therapists delivering psychological interventions for PTSD in the British National Health Service (NHS).

    (DOCX)

    S4 File. Estimation of annual health and personal social service costs incurred by adults with PTSD and adults without PTSD.

    (DOCX)

    S5 File. Results of secondary probabilistic economic analysis [beneficial effect up to 3-months post-treatment].

    (DOCX)

    S6 File. Results of deterministic sensitivity analyses.

    (DOCX)

    S7 File. Results of the NICE guideline economic analysis.

    (DOCX)

    S1 Appendix. Search strategy.

    (DOCX)

    S2 Appendix. Study protocol.

    (DOCX)

    S3 Appendix. Details of the statistical analysis and WinBUGS codes for data synthesis.

    (DOCX)

    S4 Appendix. Details of the inconsistency checks and WinBUGS codes for inconsistency models.

    (DOCX)

    S5 Appendix. Characteristics of studies included in the network meta-analysis, and full references.

    (DOCX)

    S6 Appendix. List of excluded studies with reasons for exclusion.

    (DOCX)

    S7 Appendix. NMA data files.

    (DOCX)

    S8 Appendix. Risk of bias of studies included in the NMA.

    (DOCX)

    S9 Appendix. Model fit statistics.

    (DOCX)

    S10 Appendix. Inconsistency checks.

    (DOCX)

    S11 Appendix. Relative effects between all pairs of interventions: Direct, indirect and combined (NMA) results.

    (DOCX)

    S12 Appendix. Results of the NICE guideline NMA.

    (DOCX)

    S13 Appendix. Pairwise sub-analyses.

    (DOCX)

    S14 Appendix. References in the online supplementary material.

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: PTSD adult HE model Responses to reviewers.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: PTSD adult HE model Responses to reviewers R2.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    Full details on the methods and the clinical studies included in the network meta-analysis that informed the economic analysis are provided in Mavranezouli et al., Psychol Med. 2020 Mar;50(4):542-555. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720000070. All other relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES