Abstract
Objective
To evaluate the effects of bilingualism on the emergence of Alzheimer’s clinical syndrome.
Background
Studies have proposed an increase of cognitive and neural reserve from management and control of two languages, with a consequent delayed expression of dementia.
Methods
In a clinic with a large immigrant population, we identified 253 patients with Probable Alzheimer’s disease (AD) with intermediate or high evidence of AD pathophysiological process. These patients were reviewed for demographic variables, native language (L1) other than English, ages of onset and presentation, Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), digit spans, word fluencies, naming, and memory.
Results
Among these patients, 74 (29.2%) were bilinguals with various L1s (Farsi, Spanish, Chinese, Tagalog, Arabic, others). When compared to the 179 monolingual AD patients, those who were bilingual had significant delays in ages of onset and presentation of approximately four years (p=0.003). These delays persisted despite bilinguals having worse MMSE scores on presentation. There were no significant group differences on other variables except that for worse naming in English among bilinguals vs. monolinguals. Caregiver/informants reported that 66 (89.2%) of the 74 bilingual AD patients had gradually regressed to predominant use of their L1.
Conclusions
In line with published reports worldwide, we found that bilingualism delays the expression of Alzheimer’s clinical syndrome. We also found frequent reversion to the first learned language. These findings suggest that, among bilinguals, the availability of an L1 “back-up” either facilitates compensation or masks emergence of the early symptoms of dementia.
Keywords: age of onset, language, dementia, bilingualism, Alzheimer’s disease
INTRODUCTION
Alzheimer’s disease (AD), the most common dementia, is an age-related disorder that has reached epidemic proportions with the aging of populations. Despite the increasing prevalence of AD and the burden of this disorder to patients, families, and society, there has been a lack of success in developing disease-modifying therapies. Consequently, there is increasing emphasis on prevention or identifying modifiable risk factors that can prevent, or at least retard, the development of this disorder [1].
The frequent use of two or more languages could be one of the most important modifiable risk factors for delaying the expression of Alzheimer’s clinical syndrome [2]. Much of the world is bilingual, and economically advanced societies are experiencing an influx of immigrant populations with different native languages (L1s). Elderly bilinguals, or those who can use two languages in everyday life and are at risk for AD, can clarify whether there is a “bilingual advantage” in the onset of dementia symptoms. Some, but not all, studies suggest that bilingualism promotes cognitive and neural resilience and delays the onset of the symptoms of dementia [3–7]. If learning or knowing a second language could postpone dementia symptoms even for a few years, this could mean major benefits, not only for quality of life in later years but also for decreasing related healthcare costs [8].
The literature on the relationship between bilingualism and AD remains controversial as bilingualism is complex and the diagnosis of AD can be imprecise. Variations in bilingualism include age of acquisition, proficiency in the second language (L2), and differences in contextual use. Moreover, AD is not equivalent to the typical amnestic-predominant clinical syndrome of dementia. Accordingly, from a university referral program, we evaluated all our sequential bilinguals (L2 acquisition after age 5 and historically able to use both L1 and L2 in everyday life with L2 primarily outside of home) and diagnosed with biomarker-supported Alzheimer’s clinical syndrome uncomplicated by other potential neuropathology. The bilingual patients from our program varied in their original language, but all had English as their second language. We hypothesized that, compared to monolinguals with AD, bilinguals with AD have a significant delay in onset of symptoms of dementia and suggest potential mechanisms for this effect.
METHODS
The participants in this study presented for evaluation, over a 15-year period, to a university clinical program located in a U.S. urban area with a large immigrant population. The program is also known as a center for early-onset dementias and a high proportion of our referrals are for early-onset AD. All patients had the insidious onset and progression of cognitive changes and underwent a comprehensive evaluation, laboratory assessment, and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI); many also had fluorodeoxy-glucose positron emission tomography (FDG-PET) of the brain. A total of 253 patients met National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association (NIA-AA) Criteria for “Probable AD with evidence of AD pathophysiological process”, either Intermediate (primarily FDG-PET) or High (primarily low cerebrospinal fluid [CSF] amyloid-β levels) [9,10]. NIA-AA criteria were re-assessed retrospectively for this study. Patients tended to have either FDG-PET or CSF studies (all CSF studies included total tau and phospho-tau). Only three had amyloid PET imaging and none had tau PET (hence, we did not use AT(N) research criteria) [11]. Patients with evidence of vascular changes on MRI or other potential neuropathology were excluded from consideration. All patients had “moderate dementia” based on “Extensive functional impact on daily life with impairment in basic activities. No longer independent and requires frequent assistance with daily life activities” [11]. The participants for this study were part of an institutional review board approval for this research.
From their intake presentation, we abstracted demographic and examination information and documentation of the presence of bilingualism. Among all patients, we abstracted ages of onset and presentation, sex, handedness, years of education, and Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores. A Neurobehavioral Status Examination (NBSE) available in most patients and included digit span forward and backward, numbers of “F” words/minute and category (animals)/minute, the 15-item mini-Boston Naming Test (mBNT), and a 10-item (5 registration trials) verbal learning test with 15-minute delayed recall and 20-item recognition [12]. If bilingualism was present, we further abstracted available information on language use in the first five years of life, the later acquisition of English, immigrant status, the historical ability to use both languages on a daily basis, whether English was the predominant language at work or outside the home, and any change in current language use.
Statistical Analysis
The demographic and neurobehavioral characteristics of the two dementia groups were compared using t-tests for continuous variables and Fisher’s exact test or χ2 for sex and handedness. For both age of onset and age or presentation, we performed logistic regressions with groups as dependent variable and significant or near significant non-language variables as predictors. These were followed with analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) controlling for dementia severity as measured by MMSE. The statistical analysis was performed using SPSS (version 23; SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA).
RESULTS
Of the 253 patients with Probable AD, 74 (29.2%) were sequential bilinguals who were previously fluent in English, and, of these bilingual patients, 66 (88.2%) were reported as having regressed to predominant or exclusive use of L1 both inside and outside the home. All the bilingual participants were immigrants, and they varied in their original language. The L1s in our immigrant population were Farsi (22), Spanish (21), Mandarin Chinese (5), Tagalog (4), Arabic (4), Korean (3), Hebrew (3), Hindi (3), French (2), Russian (2), and one each Bengali, Croatian, Greek, Hausa, and Hungarian.
The bilingual AD patients were older in age of onset (t=−2.99; df 251, p=0.003) and presentation (t=−3.03; df 251, p=0.003). They also had lower MMSE scores consistent with more advanced disease (t=3.38; df 251; p=0.001) (See Table 1). Although there was quite a difference in numbers of males vs. females between the two groups, this did not reach statistical significance (p=0.052 Fisher’s Exact Test or χ2=3.63, df 1, p=0.057). There were not significant group differences on the remaining neurobehavioral variables except for worse confrontational naming (mBNT) among the bilingual Probable AD patients (t=1.98; df 215, p=0.049).
TABLE 1:
Bilingual n=74 | Monolingual n=179 | |
---|---|---|
Ages OnsetA | 62.4 (10.5) | 58.0 (10.7) |
Age of PresentationA | 66.0 (10.5) | 62.1 (8.8) |
Male/Female | 46/28 | 85/92 |
Education (years) | 14.8 (4.8) | 14.5 (4.2) |
Handedness R/L | 70/4 | 171/17 |
MMSE scoresA,B | 18.1 (8.1) | 21.6 (6.3) |
Neurobehavior Status ExaminationC | ||
Digit Span: Forward | 5.22 (1.11) | 5.37 (1.36) |
Digit Span: Backward | 2.87 (1.63) | 2.84 (1.18) |
“F” Word Fluency | 7.21 (6.12) | 7.67 (5.57) |
Category Fluency | 9.22 (4.55) | 10.26 (6.07) |
mBNTA,D | 8.55 (4.08) | 9.96 (4.83) |
Delayed verbal recall | 1.74 (2.08) | 2.06 (2.42) |
Memory recognition | 14.95 (2.98) | 15.44 (2.85) |
Statistically significant group differences
MMSE=Mini-Mental State Examination
Available on 58 bilinguals and 159 monolinguals
mBNT=mini-Boston Naming Test.
Logistic regression was performed for group (bilingual vs. monolingual Probable AD) with non-language variables that were significant or near significant (ages of onset or presentation, MMSE, and sex). The results showed that increased ages of onset and presentation and worse severity of dementia per MMSE scores predicted membership in the bilingual Probable AD group. Subsequent ANCOVAs controlling for MMSE scores, did not have overall significant models but bilingual group membership remained significant for ages of onset and presentation (See Table 3). Notably, there were no group*gender interaction effects.
TABLE 3:
Dependent Variable: Age at Onset with MMSE score as covariate | |||||
Source | Sum of Squares | df | Mean Square | F | Sig. |
Corrected Model | 951.856B | 4 | 237.964 | 2.109 | .081 |
Intercept | 76820.636 | 1 | 76820.636 | 680.864 | .000 |
MMSE score | 11.365 | 1 | 11.365 | .101 | .751 |
GroupA | 887.208 | 1 | 887.208 | 7.863 | .006 |
Gender | 52.808 | 1 | 52.808 | .468 | .495 |
Group*Gender | .062 | 1 | .062 | .001 | .981 |
Error | 23581.098 | 209 | 112.828 | ||
Total | 760232.750 | 214 | |||
Corrected Total | 24532.954 | 213 | |||
Dependent Variable: Age at Presentation with MMSE as covariate | |||||
Source | Sum of Squares | df | Mean Square | F | Sig. |
Corrected Model | 763.430B | 4 | 190.857 | 2.324 | .058 |
Intercept | 90376.846 | 1 | 90376.846 | 1100.475 | .000 |
MMSE score | 3.270 | 1 | 3.270 | .040 | .842 |
GroupA | 686.759 | 1 | 686.759 | 8.362 | .004 |
Gender | 1.905 | 1 | 1.905 | .023 | .879 |
Group*Gender | 5.595 | 1 | 5.595 | .068 | .794 |
Error | 17410.561 | 212 | 82.125 | ||
Total | 866768.000 | 217 | |||
Corrected Total | 18173.991 | 216 |
Group: Bilingual vs. Monolingual Probable AD
R2=0.039 (Adjusted R2=0.02)
Group: Bilingual vs. Monolingual Probable AD
R2=0.042 (Adjusted R2=0.024)
DISCUSSION
We found evidence to support a delay in the onset of dementia symptoms among well-characterized patients with Alzheimer’s clinical syndrome who were bilingual in different languages. Like other reports in the literature, the bilingual Probable AD patients had the onset and presentation of their dementia symptoms about 4 years later than comparable monolingual patients with AD despite worse dementia severity (i.e., MMSE scores) on presentation. The more advanced MMSE scores suggested that bilingualism compensates for cognitive decline as well as for the neuropathology of AD in everyday life so that disease is more advanced on reported onset and presentation.
In 2007, Bialystok and colleagues reported that bilingual patients with probable AD were diagnosed about four years later than monolinguals with probable AD [13]. Since then, investigators and most retrospective studies from around the world have suggested that bilingualism delays clinical symptoms of dementia by 4–5 years [3–7,13–40] (See Table 4). In addition, pervasive multilingualism could be a factor in the low prevalence of cognitive complaints and dementia among older adults in Luxembourg [41], and, in the Lothian Birth Cohort (Scotland), those who learned a second language manifested better cognition in late life [42]. Others report that the more languages spoken, the better the cognitive performance in the aged [43].
TABLE 4:
Bialystok E, et al. 2007 [13] | Canada; 184 Patients with dementia, 51% bilingual which showed dementia symptoms 4–5 years later than monolinguals. |
Craik FI, et al.2010 [14] | Canada: follow-up of the above study; 211 new patients with probable AD, bilinguals (n=102) were diagnosed 4.3 years later + symptom onset 5.1 years later than monolinguals (n=109). |
Zheng Y, et al. 2018 [15] | China; Cantonese-Mandarin bilinguals was older at AD onset, and older at first clinic visit than monolinguals. |
Alladi S, et al. 2016, 2017 [17,18] | India; bilinguals(n=390) developed clinical dementia 4.5 years later, delay in post-stroke cognitive impairment and bvFTD (but not other forms of FTLD or PSP) than monolinguals (n=257). |
Woumans E, et al. 2015 [19] | Belgium; 134 patients with probable AD, bilinguals (n=69) had first symptoms (4.6 years) and diagnosis (4.8 years) later after controlling for demographics. |
There are several other supportive studies, as well as those that question the relationship of bilingualism and dementia. Among patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI), a frequent prelude of dementia, one report found that bilinguals were several years older at age of symptom onset [24]. Others found that multilinguals had a lower risk for amnestic MCI and higher temporal tissue density [25–29]. Another study found bilingualism associated with later age-of-diagnosis only among those with a low (<11 years) educational level [30]. Much disagreement appears to stem from reports of a lack of effect of bilingualism on incidence of dementia or AD [4,5,35–37,44,45]. However, assessing for an effect on new incident cases of dementia is distinct from retrospectively assessing for an effect on age of onset or delay in clinical dementia.
In this study, the Probable AD patients showed a “bilingual advantage” in the expression of Alzheimer’s clinical syndrome. Bilingualism could increases cognitive reserve through the constant management of two simultaneously active and competing languages [13,41,46–58]. Better reserve, with better compensation or masking of worsening cognition, could explain the worse dementia severity per MMSE scores on presentation for bilinguals. Furthermore, increased cognitive and neural reserve from bilingualism may be beneficial when there is cognitive decline from neuropathology such as that from AD [13,17,48,50,51]. In the presence of greater cognitive reserve and optimal compensatory use of brain resources, there may be a need for more advanced AD pathology in order to express dementia symptoms [49]. A few studies show that bilinguals with dementia may have more advanced temporal neuropathology than monolinguals with dementia [7,14,29,53,59–62]. Computerized tomography scans of bilingual and monolingual AD patients who were matched for level of cognitive performance showed that the bilingual patients had more disease (greater mesotemporal atrophy-radial width of the temporal horn and the temporal horn ratio) than the monolingual patients [59]. Others report similar or lower tissue density in medial temporal areas in multilingual patients with AD versus monolingual patients with AD despite comparable impairment [29]. Consistent with these structural changes, cerebral hypometabolism was more extensive among AD bilinguals vs. AD monolinguals, indicating more advanced disease [61].
As with other research on bilingualism in dementia [63], this study has potential limitations. One of the most important factors is the social diversity of language use including adaptations to interactional contexts [64]. Bilingualism’s effects may depend on whether they are in a home or familial vs. formal or work context, along with social situation and psychological distancing effects [42,65–67]. The bilinguals in this report used English outside the home and at work, but often their native language at home. Another important issue is the objective determination of L2 proficiency without relying on self-reports [68,69]. In this study there was collateral verification of “routine daily use” proficiency among the bilinguals. Bilingualism in dementia may also be affected by factors such as immigration status, as well as acculturation, education, specific L1 language groups, and other variables [16,31,42,63,65–73]. All of the bilingual participants in this study were immigrants. There were more males among the bilinguals, possibly because of immigrant status (but gender differences did not meet significance). Finally, many studies fail to fully characterize the dementia as consistent with Alzheimer’s clinical syndrome, as is done in this study.
In conclusion, bilingualism appears to delay presentation with Alzheimer’s clinical syndrome, possibly from compensation or masking of cognitive decline. Learning a second language, even in older age, may be an important mechanism for promoting compensatory reserve and for delaying a functional decline from early dementia [74,75]. Even subsequent regression to L1 with advancing dementia may suggest ongoing compensation from better retained semantic access in L1 [76]. The findings of this study can lead to more research and investigation of the use of two languages as an important delaying factor for dementia symptom onset.
Table 2:
Dependent Variable: Group (Bilingual Probable AD vs. Monolingual Probable AD) | ||||||
B | S.E. | Wald | df | Sig. | Exp(B) | |
Age of Onset | .047 | .017 | 7.543 | 1 | .006 | 1.048 |
MMSE score | −.072 | .022 | 10.516 | 1 | .001 | .930 |
Sex | .572 | .331 | 2.990 | 1 | .084 | 1.771 |
Constant | −2.676 | 1.120 | 5.704 | 1 | .017 | .069 |
B | S.E. | Wald | df | Sig. | Exp(B) | |
Age at Presentation | .048 | .018 | 7.551 | 1 | .006 | 1.049 |
MMSE score | −.071 | .022 | 10.207 | 1 | .001 | .931 |
Sex | .571 | .329 | 3.004 | 1 | .083 | 1.769 |
Constant | −2.954 | 1.214 | 5.922 | 1 | .015 | .052 |
Nagelkerke R2 0.14
Acknowledgements
The authors acknowledge the support of Elvira Jimenez, Ph.D., in managing that supervising the overall research activities of our department.
Funding Sources Statement
This research was supported by U.S. National Institute on Aging Grant 1RF1AG050967 (M. F. Mendez PI).
Footnotes
Disclosure Statement
M. F. Mendez has no conflicts of interest. D. Chavez has no conflicts of interest. G. Akhlaghipour has no conflicts of interest.
Statement of Ethics
This research was conducted ethically in accordance with the World Medial Association Declaration of Helsinki and the appropriate guidelines for human studies. The procedures followed were assessed by the UCLA Institutional Review Board and given approval for anonymized, de-identified abstraction of retrospective clinical information.
REFERENCES
- 1.Sommerlad A, Livingston G: Preventing Alzheimer’s dementia. BMJ 2017;359:j5667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Mendez MF: Bilingualism and Dementia: Cognitive Reserve to Linguistic Competency. J Alzheimers Dis 2019;71:377–388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Atkinson AL: Does bilingualism delay the development of dementia? J Eur Psychol Stud 2016;7:43–50. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Klimova B, Valis M, Kuca K: Bilingualism as a strategy to delay the onset of Alzheimer’s disease. Clin Interv Aging 2017;12:1731–1737. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Mukadam N, Sommerlad A, Livingston G: The Relationship of Bilingualism Compared to Monolingualism to the Risk of Cognitive Decline or Dementia: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. J Alzheimers Dis 2017;58:45–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Woumans E, Versijpt J, Sieben A, Santens P, Duyck W: Bilingualism and Cognitive Decline: A Story of Pride and Prejudice. J Alzheimers Dis 2017;60:1237–1239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Grundy JG, Anderson JAE: Commentary: The Relationship of Bilingualism Compared to Monolingualism to the Risk of Cognitive Decline or Dementia: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Front Aging Neurosci 2017;9:344. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bialystok E, Abutalebi J, Bak TH, Burke DM, Kroll JF: Aging in two languages: Implications for public health. Ageing Res Rev 2016;27:56–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack CR Jr., Kawas CH, Klunk WE, Koroshetz WJ, Manly JJ, Mayeux R, Mohs RC, Morris JC, Rossor MN, Scheltens P, Carrillo MC, Thies B, Weintraub S, Phelps CH: The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 2011;7:263–269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Jack CR Jr., Albert MS, Knopman DS, McKhann GM, Sperling RA, Carrillo MC, Thies B, Phelps CH: Introduction to the recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 2011;7:257–262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Jack CR Jr., Bennett DA, Blennow K, Carrillo MC, Dunn B, Haeberlein SB, Holtzman DM, Jagust W, Jessen F, Karlawish J, Liu E, Molinuevo JL, Montine T, Phelps C, Rankin KP, Rowe CC, Scheltens P, Siemers E, Snyder HM, Sperling R, Contributors: NIA-AA Research Framework: Toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 2018;14:535–562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Daffner KR, Gale SA, Barrett AM, Boeve BF, Chatterjee A, Coslett HB, D’Esposito M, Finney GR, Gitelman DR, Hart JJ Jr., Lerner AJ, Meador KJ, Pietras AC, Voeller KS, Kaufer DI: Improving clinical cognitive testing: report of the AAN Behavioral Neurology Section Workgroup. Neurology 2015;85:910–918. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bialystok E, Craik FI, Freedman M: Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia 2007;45:459–464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Craik FI, Bialystok E, Freedman M: Delaying the onset of Alzheimer disease: bilingualism as a form of cognitive reserve. Neurology 2010;75:1726–1729. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Zheng Y, Wu Q, Su F, Fang Y, Zeng J, Pei Z: The Protective Effect of Cantonese/Mandarin Bilingualism on the Onset of Alzheimer Disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2018;45:210–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Alladi S, Bak TH, Duggirala V, Surampudi B, Shailaja M, Shukla AK, Chaudhuri JR, Kaul S: Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology 2013;81:1938–1944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Alladi S, Bak TH, Mekala S, Rajan A, Chaudhuri JR, Mioshi E, Krovvidi R, Surampudi B, Duggirala V, Kaul S: Impact of Bilingualism on Cognitive Outcome After Stroke. Stroke 2016;47:258–261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Alladi S, Bak TH, Shailaja M, Gollahalli D, Rajan A, Surampudi B, Hornberger M, Duggirala V, Chaudhuri JR, Kaul S: Bilingualism delays the onset of behavioral but not aphasic forms of frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia 2017;99:207–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Woumans E, Santens P, Sieben A, Versijpt J: Bilingualism delays clinical manifestations of Alzheimer’s disease.. Biling Lang Cogn 2015;18:568–574. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Clare L, Whitaker CJ, Craik FI, Bialystok E, Martyr A, Martin-Forbes PA, Bastable AJ, Pye KL, Quinn C, Thomas EM, Gathercole VC, Hindle JV: Bilingualism, executive control, and age at diagnosis among people with early-stage Alzheimer’s disease in Wales. J Neuropsychol 2016;10:163–185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Perani D, Abutalebi J: Bilingualism, dementia, cognitive and neural reserve. Curr Opin Neurol 2015;28:618–625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Freedman M, Alladi S, Chertkow H, Bialystok E, Craik FI, Phillips NA, Duggirala V, Raju SB, Bak TH: Delaying onset of dementia: are two languages enough? Behav Neurol 2014;2014:808137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Klein RM, Christie J, Parkvall M: Does multilingualism affect the incidence of Alzheimer’s disease?: A worldwide analysis by country. SSM Popul Health 2016;2:463–467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Bialystok E, Craik FI, Binns MA, Ossher L, Freedman M: Effects of bilingualism on the age of onset and progression of MCI and AD: evidence from executive function tests. Neuropsychology 2014;28:290–304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Perquin M, Vaillant M, Schuller AM, Pastore J, Dartigues JF, Lair ML, Diederich N, MemoVie G: Lifelong exposure to multilingualism: new evidence to support cognitive reserve hypothesis. PLoS One 2013;8:e62030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Ramakrishnan S, Mekala S, Mamidipudi A, Yareeda S, Mridula R, Bak TH, Alladi S, Kaul S: Comparative Effects of Education and Bilingualism on the Onset of Mild Cognitive Impairment. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2017;44:222–231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Ossher L, Bialystok E, Craik FI, Murphy KJ, Troyer AK: The effect of bilingualism on amnestic mild cognitive impairment. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2013;68:8–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Wilson RS, Boyle PA, Yang J, James BD, Bennett DA: Early life instruction in foreign language and music and incidence of mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology 2015;29:292–302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Duncan HD, Nikelski J, Pilon R, Steffener J, Chertkow H, Phillips NA: Structural brain differences between monolingual and multilingual patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease: Evidence for cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 2018;109:270–282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Gollan TH, Salmon DP, Montoya RI, Galasko DR: Degree of bilingualism predicts age of diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease in low-education but not in highly educated Hispanics. Neuropsychologia 2011;49:3826–3830. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Chertkow H, Whitehead V, Phillips N, Wolfson C, Atherton J, Bergman H: Multilingualism (but not always bilingualism) delays the onset of Alzheimer disease: evidence from a bilingual community. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2010;24:118–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Lawton DM, Gasquoine PG, Weimer AA: Age of dementia diagnosis in community dwelling bilingual and monolingual Hispanic Americans. Cortex 2015;66:141–145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Padilla C, Mendez MF, Jimenez EE, Teng E: Bilingualism in older Mexican-American immigrants is associated with higher scores on cognitive screening. BMC Geriatr 2016;16:189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Crane PK, Gruhl JC, Erosheva EA, Gibbons LE, McCurry SM, Rhoads K, Nguyen V, Arani K, Masaki K, White L: Use of spoken and written Japanese did not protect Japanese-American men from cognitive decline in late life. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2010;65:654–666. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Yeung CM, St John PD, Menec V, Tyas SL: Is bilingualism associated with a lower risk of dementia in community-living older adults? Cross-sectional and prospective analyses. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2014;28:326–332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Sanders AE, Hall CB, Katz MJ, Lipton RB: Non-native language use and risk of incident dementia in the elderly. J Alzheimers Dis 2012;29:99–108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Zahodne LB, Schofield PW, Farrell MT, Stern Y, Manly JJ: Bilingualism does not alter cognitive decline or dementia risk among Spanish-speaking immigrants. Neuropsychology 2014;28:238–246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Bialystok E, Craik FIM, Binns MA, Ossher L, Freedman M: Effects of bilingualism on the age of onset and progression of MCI and AD: evidence from executive function tests. Neuropsychology 2014;28:290–304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Mendez MF, Perryman KM, Ponton MO, Cummings JL: Bilingualism and dementia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1999;11:411–412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Ratnavalli E JN, Ramanan S, Chandrashekar S PS: Bilingualism does ot delay the age at onset of dementia - A study from memory clinic in South India.. Neurology 2015;84(14 Supplement):P6.206. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Perquin M, Diederich N, Pastore J, Lair ML, Stranges S, Vaillant M, MemoVie G: Prevalence of Dementia and Cognitive Complaints in the Context of High Cognitive Reserve: A Population-Based Study. PLoS One 2015;10:e0138818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Bak TH, Nissan JJ, Allerhand MM, Deary IJ: Does bilingualism influence cognitive aging? Ann Neurol 2014;75:959–963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kave G, Eyal N, Shorek A, Cohen-Mansfield J: Multilingualism and cognitive state in the oldest old. Psychol Aging 2008;23:70–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Mukadam N, Jichi F, Green D, Livingston G: The relationship of bilingualism to cognitive decline: The Australian Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2018;33:e249–e256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Liu YC, Liu YY, Yip PK, Meguro M, Meguro K: Speaking one more language in early life has only minor effects on cognition in Taiwanese with low education level: the Taishan Project. Psychogeriatrics 2017;17:256–261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Lombardi G, Polito C, Berti V, Bagnoli S, Nacmias B, Pupi A, Sorbi S: Contribution of Bilingualism to Cognitive Reserve of an Italian Literature Professor at High Risk for Alzheimer’s Disease. J Alzheimers Dis 2018;66:1389–1395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.DeLuca V, Rothman J, Bialystok E, Pliatsikas C: Redefining bilingualism as a spectrum of experiences that differentially affects brain structure and function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2019;116:7565–7574. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Guzman-Velez E, Tranel D: Does bilingualism contribute to cognitive reserve? Cognitive and neural perspectives. Neuropsychology 2015;29:139–150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Groot C, van Loenhoud AC, Barkhof F, van Berckel BNM, Koene T, Teunissen CC, Scheltens P, van der Flier WM, Ossenkoppele R: Differential effects of cognitive reserve and brain reserve on cognition in Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2018;90:e149–e156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Bialystok E, Craik FI, Luk G: Bilingualism: consequences for mind and brain. Trends Cogn Sci 2012;16:240–250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Kroll JF, Dussias PE: The Benefits of Multilingualism to the Personal and Professional Development of Residents of The US. Foreign Lang Ann 2017;50:248–259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Marian V, Shook A: The cognitive benefits of being bilingual. Cerebrum 2012;2012:13. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Estanga A, Ecay-Torres M, Ibanez A, Izagirre A, Villanua J, Garcia-Sebastian M, Iglesias Gaspar MT, Otaegui-Arrazola A, Iriondo A, Clerigue M, Martinez-Lage P: Beneficial effect of bilingualism on Alzheimer’s disease CSF biomarkers and cognition. Neurobiol Aging 2017;50:144–151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Khachatryan E, Camarrone F, Fias W, Van Hulle MM: ERP Response Unveils Effect of Second Language Manipulation on First Language Processing. PLoS One 2016;11:e0167194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Kroll JF, Bobb SC, Misra M, Guo T: Language selection in bilingual speech: evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2008;128:416–430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Thierry G, Wu YJ: Brain potentials reveal unconscious translation during foreign-language comprehension. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2007;104:12530–12535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Meuter R, Allport A: Bilingual language switcning in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection. J Mem Lang 1999;40:25–40. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Paulmann S, Elston-Guttler KE, Gunter TC, Kotz SA: Is bilingual lexical access influenced by language context? Neuroreport 2006;17:727–731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Schweizer TA, Ware J, Fischer CE, Craik FI, Bialystok E: Bilingualism as a contributor to cognitive reserve: evidence from brain atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex 2012;48:991–996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Gold BT: Lifelong bilingualism and neural reserve against Alzheimer’s disease: a review of findings and potential mechanisms. Behav Brain Res 2015;281:9–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Perani D, Farsad M, Ballarini T, Lubian F, Malpetti M, Fracchetti A, Magnani G, March A, Abutalebi J: The impact of bilingualism on brain reserve and metabolic connectivity in Alzheimer’s dementia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2017;114:1690–1695. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Bak TH, Robertson I: Biology enters the scene-a new perspective on bilingualism, cognition, and dementia. Neurobiol Aging 2017;50:iii–iv. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Watson CW, Manly JJ, Zahodne LB: Does bilingualism protect against cognitive aging?: Methodological issues in research on bilingualism, cognitive reserve, and dementia incidence. Linguist Approaches Biling 2016;6:590–604. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Green DW, Abutalebi J: Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. J Cogn Psychol (Hove) 2013;25:515–530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Woumans E, Duyck W: The bilingual advantage debate: Moving toward different methods for verifying its existence. Cortex 2015;73:356–357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Caldwell-Harris CL, Aycicegi-Dinn A: Emotion and lying in a non-native language. Int J Psychophysiol 2009;71:193–204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Hayakawa S, Costa A, Foucart A, Keysar B: Using a Foreign Language Changes Our Choices. Trends Cogn Sci 2016;20:791–793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Gasquoine PG: Effects of bilingualism on vocabulary, executive functions, age of dementia onset, and regional brain structure. Neuropsychology 2016;30:988–997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Altarriba J, Basnight-Brown DM: Methodological considerations in performing semantic- and translation-priming experiments across languages. Behav Res Methods 2007;39:1–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Fuller-Thomson E, Milaszewski D, Abdelmessih SA: The importance of untangling the role of nativity and bilingualism in Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex 2013;49:1441; discussion 1442–1443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Fuller-Thomson E, Kuh D: The healthy migrant effect may confound the link between bilingualism and delayed onset of Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex 2014;52:128–130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Manly JJ, Espino DV: Cultural influences on dementia recognition and management. Clin Geriatr Med 2004;20:93–119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Gollan TH, Slattery TJ, Goldenberg D, Van Assche E, Duyck W, Rayner K: Frequency drives lexical access in reading but not in speaking: the frequency-lag hypothesis. J Exp Psychol Gen 2011;140:186–209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Antoniou M, Wright SM: Uncovering the Mechanisms Responsible for Why Language Learning May Promote Healthy Cognitive Aging. Front Psychol 2017;8:2217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Schlegel AA, Rudelson JJ, Tse PU: White matter structure changes as adults learn a second language. J Cogn Neurosci 2012;24:1664–1670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Ellajosyula R, Narayanan J, Patterson K: Striking loss of second language in bilingual patients with semantic dementia. J Neurol 2019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]