Abstract
Participation in social skills therapy (SST) facilitates cognitive functioning in children with developmental disabilities. The present pilot study examined whether participation in SST was associated with enhanced encoding and 1-month delayed recall in children with Down syndrome (DS). Children were presented with novel three-step event sequences in an elicited imitation procedure. Immediate imitation was permitted as an index of encoding; long-term memory was assessed 1 month later. Parents completed questionnaires inquiring about children’s participation in SST. Participation in SST was associated with enhanced encoding of temporal order information and 1-month delayed recall of individual target actions. In addition, encoding mediated the relation between group and 1-month delayed recall. The conducted research indicates that involvement in SST may be beneficial for children with DS despite their noted strengths in imitation and social learning. As such, additional experimental work is warranted to determine causality.
Keywords: cognitive development, Down syndrome, preschoolers, quantitative methods
The ability to recall the past is at least in part socially constructed. When considering typically developing (TD) children, the manner in which parents discuss the past with their children has implications for the ways in which children discuss the past themselves. For example, Fivush and Fromhoff (1988) reported that children of mothers who were characterized as elaborative in their narrative style (i.e., those who talked more and asked more memory-related questions when conversing about past events with their children) recalled more detail about past experiences relative to children of mothers who had repetitive narrative styles (i.e., mothers who tended to repeat information). The social environment also contributes to cognitive functioning in children with developmental disabilities. As evidence of this, participation in social skills therapy (SST) has been associated with improved eye contact, joint attention, and imitation, among other abilities, in children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD; Cappadocia & Weiss, 2011; Hwang & Hughes, 2000); causality has also been confirmed in at least one report (Ozonoff & Miller, 1995). Despite these promising results, little research has been conducted to examine whether observed or parent-reported participation in SST is associated with improved cognitive outcomes in children with Down syndrome (DS). The paucity of relevant research may be explained by previous reports indicating that children with DS are relatively skilled at imitation and social interaction (Dykens & Hodapp, 2001; Libby, Powell, Messer, & Jordan, 1997; Rast & Meltzoff, 1995; Roberts & Richmond, 2015; Wright, Lewis, & Collis, 2006), thereby suggesting that such additional intervention in this domain will yield few returns. However, recent findings have demonstrated that children with DS experience impairments in long-term recall memory relative to TD children matched on developmental age (DA; Milojevich & Lukowski, 2016). Given the demonstrated effectiveness of SST for improving cognitive functioning in children with ASD, the present exploratory research was conducted to examine whether parent-reported participation in SST was associated with enhanced encoding and 1-month delayed recall by children with DS.
Understanding how cognitive development proceeds in children with DS is not only important due to the prevalence of this genetic disorder but also because of the long-term cognitive consequences associated with DS. DS is the most common autosomal chromosomal disorder worldwide, affecting approximately 1 in every 700 infants born in the United States (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2014). Although other genotypes exist, DS is most commonly characterized by an extra chromosome on the 21st pair. Children with DS experience numerous functional challenges relative to TD children, including physical impairments, issues with receptive and expressive language, and cognitive deficits (Abbeduto et al., 2001; Laws & Bishop, 2003; Selikowitz, 1997). As such, understanding the effectiveness of early intervention efforts for maximizing functional outcomes early in life and beyond should be a primary goal in the research on children with developmental disabilities.
Part of the challenge in examining associations with cognitive functioning, however, is that infants and young children cannot report on the past using language. For this reason, researchers use a behavioral paradigm known as elicited or deferred imitation to assess the emergence and development of recall capabilities in the first years of life (Lukowski & Milojevich, 2016). In one version of this procedure, children interact with novel three-dimensional stimuli during a pre-demonstration baseline period. A researcher then demonstrates how to complete a sequence of actions with the materials. Children are allowed the opportunity to imitate the actions immediately after the demonstration as an index of encoding and/or after delays ranging from minutes to months as an index of long-term memory. The data are then coded to determine whether the participants perform the demonstrated target actions (TA) and whether the completed actions are reproduced in the correct temporal order (for additional information, see Bauer, DeBoer & Lukowski, 2007). In a modification of the standard procedure just described, generalization across cues are assessed by varying the perceptual features of stimuli presented at demonstration and at test (e.g., Lukowski & Milojevich, 2013; Phung, Milojevich, & Lukowski, 2014).
Although a significant literature has been established on the conditions that best support recall in TD infants and children, few studies have used the elicited imitation paradigm to examine recall memory in children with DS. The research that has been conducted indicates that children with DS successfully imitate demonstrated actions both immediately and after delays ranging from minutes to months (e.g., Milojevich & Lukowski, 2016; Rast & Meltzoff, 1995). In one study, for example, young children with DS imitated single- and multi-step pretend-play actions after watching an adult model (Libby et al., 1997). Other research indicates that children with DS encode TA and temporal order information at levels comparable with TD children; children with DS also recall individual TA after a 1-month delay. Although these findings have been taken to indicate that children with DS experience relative strengths in social interaction and imitation (Dykens & Hodapp, 2001; Libby et al., 1997; Rast & Meltzoff, 1995; Wright et al., 2006), children with DS also experience impairments in long-term memory. That is, whereas TD children recall the temporal order of three-step event sequences after a 1-month delay, children with DS do not (Milojevich & Lukowski, 2016).
One possible means of improving cognitive functioning in children with DS may be through the use of early interventions, particularly those that target the development of social skills. As a result of the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA; 20 U.S.C. § 1400 et seq), a variety of federally mandated educational and intervention services are available to children with DS, with the overarching goal of enhancing functional outcomes across a variety of domains (National Down Syndrome Society, 2012). Although there is no research to our knowledge to indicate whether participation in SST is associated with enhanced cognition in children with DS, other forms of early intervention promote cognitive functioning (Connolly, Morgan, Russell, & Richardson, 1980; Connolly, Morgan, Russell, & Fulliton, 1993; Feeley, Jones, Blackburn, & Bauer, 2011). For example, longitudinal studies have demonstrated that children who participate in early intervention programs score significantly higher on tests of intelligence and general cognitive functioning relative to age-matched children with DS who have not received early intervention (Connolly et al., 1980). Importantly, these early intervention benefits are seen both immediately and over time, extending into adolescence (Connolly et al., 1993). Furthermore, research on children with other developmental delays has repeatedly demonstrated that SST is associated with (Cappadocia & Weiss, 2011; Hwang & Hughes, 2000)—and even directly enhances (Ozonoff & Miller, 1995)—cognitive functioning. In particular, Hwang and Hughes (2000) reviewed 16 empirical studies that examined the impact of SST on young children with ASD. Based on their review of the literature, they concluded that involvement in SST improves eye contact, joint attention, and imitation, among other cognitive and social skills, in children with ASD. Whether children with DS would experience any cognitive benefits associated with their involvement in SST is unknown.
The primary goal of the present study was to collect pilot data from a small sample of children with DS to examine whether participation in SST was associated with enhanced encoding and 1-month delayed recall memory and generalization across cues. In exploratory analyses, we examined the associations between participation in SST and cognitive functioning in particular because (a) previous work has documented the importance of the social context in shaping recall memory (e.g., Fivush & Fromhoff, 1988) and (b) SST interventions are not commonly experienced by children with DS. Because children with DS have demonstrated strengths in imitation and social learning (Dykens & Hodapp, 2001; Libby et al., 1997; Rast & Meltzoff, 1995; Roberts & Richmond, 2015; Wright et al., 2006), we predicted that children would show evidence of encoding the demonstrated actions relative to baseline regardless of their intervention history. However, given the social nature of the elicited imitation procedure (Heimann, Laberg, & Nordoen, 2006), we expected that children who had participated in SST would perform more TA and pairs of actions (PA) at immediate imitation relative to children who had not participated in SST. When considering long-term memory, we predicted that participation in SST would also be associated with recall performance, particularly when considering memory for TA relative to temporal order information, as previous research indicates that children with DS do not recall temporal order information after a 1-month delay (Milojevich & Lukowski, 2016). Finally, we used mediation models to examine the process by which participation in SST might impact encoding and 1-month delayed recall memory.
Method
Participants
Nineteen children diagnosed with DS (mean age = 33 months, 9 days; range from 18 months, 12 days to 53 months, 1 day; 11 girls) served as participants. Two additional children were tested but were later excluded from data analysis: One child experienced seizure activity shortly after birth, whereas the other child was not developing as expected based on parent report. These participants were recruited to participate in a study examining recall memory and related abilities in children with DS along with TD children matched to the children with DS on DA. Portions of the data collected in the larger study have been published (Lukowski & Milojevich, 2017; Milojevich & Lukowski, 2016) or are currently under review (Lukowski, Slonecker, & Milojevich, 2019; Milojevich, Slonecker, & Lukowski, 2019), but none of this work addresses the exploratory research questions presented herein.
Participants were recruited from local early intervention centers, organizations that provided educational and support services to children with DS and their families, and through snowball sampling. Interested families provided the researchers with their phone number and email addresses and were contacted with additional information about participating in the study.
Of the 19 participants, 53% were Caucasian, 21% were Asian, 5% were African American, and 16% were of mixed race; the race of 5% of the participants was unknown. Thirty-seven percent of the children were of Hispanic ethnicity. Eighty-four percent of the children were exclusively or primarily exposed to English at home, 5% were exposed to English less than half of the time at home, and 11% did not provide information sufficient to allow for categorization. Seventy-nine percent of mothers had obtained at least a 4-year college degree. Parents received US$30 in appreciation of their participation and children received either a small toy or a “junior scientist” t-shirt.
Materials and Measures
Questionnaires.
Parents provided demographic information, including child race and ethnicity, parent education, and family income, among other things. They also completed an early intervention questionnaire (Ly, 2013) that was developed to examine participation in interventions by children with ASD. The questionnaire inquired as to whether the child was participating in various interventions at the time of the study, had previously participated in the listed interventions but was not participating in them at the time of the study, or had never participated in the listed interventions (e.g., occupational therapy, physical therapy, psychotherapy, and speech therapy). Additional space was provided for parents to write in other interventions experienced by the child. For those interventions with which the child had experience, parents also recorded the number of months the intervention was in place.
Bayley Scales of Infant Development-III (BSID-III; Bayley, 2006).
The mental dimension of the BSID-III was administered at the first session to determine each child’s DA.
Elicited imitation.
Children were presented with six three-step event sequences across two study sessions; four of these sequences are featured in the present report (two presented at the first and second sessions and two presented at the second session only). Each event sequence was constrained by enabling relations, such that the three steps had to be completed in the correct temporal order for the sequence end-state to become apparent (although the actions could be physically completed in any order). We used sequences constrained by enabling relations to allow children with DS the best opportunity for immediate imitation and long-term delayed recall, as work with TD children has demonstrated that children younger than 20 months perform at chance on sequences that are arbitrarily ordered (Wenner & Bauer, 1999).
As shown in Figure 1, each event sequence was created in an analog form to allow an assessment of delayed generalization across cues (e.g., Lukowski, Wiebe, & Bauer, 2009; Phung et al., 2014). The analog sequences differed from the original in color and shape but not in size or texture. The sequences were blocked into groups of two and counterbalanced across event types (memory and generalization) and conditions (familiar or control); the sequence order within event types and conditions was randomized across sessions.
Figure 1. Example of the three-step event sequence Make a Shaker.
Note. Each event sequence was created in two forms that were perceptually distinct but functionally identical to one another. The left panels show the first step of putting the block into one of the wooden cups; the middle panels show the second step of assembling the wooden cups; the right panels show the third steps shaking the assembled apparatus (although infants also received credit for shaking only one of the wooden cups when the shaker was not fully assembled).
Source. Figure and text used with permission from Phung, Milojevich, & Lukowski (2014).
Procedure
The study was approved by the relevant Institutional Review Boards. For the questionnaire portion of the study, a waiver of written informed consent was granted; parents signed informed consent statements at the first session indicating their willingness to allow their child to participate in the behavioral portion of the study. All children were tested by the first author. Sessions were video recorded to allow for protocol checks and offline data coding.
Questionnaires.
Parents received the questionnaires by mail, completed them at home, and returned them at the first session. Parents who did not have the questionnaires completed at that time returned them at the second session.
BSID-III (Bayley, 2006).
The child was seated across the researcher at an adultsized table. After a brief warm-up procedure (e.g., Bauer & Lukowski, 2010; Bauer, Wenner, Dropik & Wewerka, 2000; Lukowski, Phung, & Milojevich, 2015; Phung et al., 2014), the researcher administered the mental portion of the BSID-III (Bayley, 2006). After the assessment was complete, parents were given a pamphlet describing chronological age-appropriate developmental milestones along with the recommendation to discuss any concerns with their child’s pediatrician.
Elicited imitation.
Following administration of the BSID-III (Bayley, 2006), children were presented with four novel three-step event sequences in turn; two of these sequences are the focus of the present report. The researcher presented each sequence in turn along with a general verbal prompt (“What can you do with this stuff?”). This baseline period was child-controlled such that this phase of testing ended when children engaged in repetitive or off-task behaviors (e.g., repeatedly banging the props on the table or dropping them on the floor; Bauer et al., 2000; Bauer & Lukowski, 2010; Lukowski et al., 2015; Phung et al., 2014). After the baseline period, the researcher demonstrated each sequence of actions twice in succession with narration.
Immediate imitation was assessed for two of the sequences as an index of encoding (Bauer & Lukowski, 2010; Bauer et al., 2000; Lukowski & Milojevich, 2013; Lukowski et al., 2015; Lukowski et al., 2005; Phung et al., 2014). The researcher returned the sequence materials to the children along with the name of the event (“You can use this stuff to Make a Shaker. How do you Make a Shaker just like I did?”). The imitation period ended when children engaged in repetitive or off-task behaviors.
Approximately 1 month later (mean delay = 31 days; range from 27 to 41 days), children returned to the laboratory for an assessment of delayed recall memory or generalization across cues. Children tested in the memory condition (n = 10) were provided with the same event sequences that they had seen at the first session, whereas children in the generalization condition (n = 9) were presented with event sequences that were functionally identical to, but perceptually distinct from, those they had seen at the first session. Children were also presented with two novel control sequences that were not previously demonstrated by the researcher. These sequences were included because the participants were expected to have developed more mature problem-solving abilities over the 1-month delay (Bauer et al., 2000). As such, increased performance at delayed recall relative to baseline may result from increased spontaneous production of the TA rather than memory per se. For this reason, the most rigorous test of long-term memory occurs when performance on previously modeled familiar sequences are compared with performance on novel event sequences presented at the same session (Bauer et al., 2000; Lukowski et al., 2005).
The researcher presented the children with the familiar and novel control sequences in turn along with the name of each event. The event name was expected to serve as a retrieval cue for familiar events and as a suggestion of activities that could be completed with the novel sequence materials (“You can use this stuff to Make a Shaker. How do you Make a Shaker with this stuff?”) (Bauer et al., 2000). The delayed recall period ended when children engaged in the repetitive or off-task behaviors described earlier.
Data Reduction
Questionnaires.
The data from the intervention questionnaire (Ly, 2013) were reduced to determine the percent of children involved in various interventions at the time of the study, previously but not at the time of the study, or never. We also recorded the number of months the intervention was in place for those children who had participated in each intervention.
BSID-III (Bayley, 2006).
The data obtained from the BSID-III were reduced as described in the administration manual so as to yield a DA for each child.
Elicited imitation.
The average number of TA (maximum = 3) and PAs produced in the correct temporal order (maximum = 2) were coded and reduced separately by event type (memory or generalization), phase (baseline and immediate imitation at the first session), and condition (delayed recall and performance on novel control sequences at the second session). TA were coded when children performed any of the three actions demonstrated by the researcher; PAs were coded by recording the order in which the TA were produced. As the most conservative measure of recall, only the first occurrence of each TA was coded thus reducing the likelihood of providing credit for behaviors produced by chance or trial-and-error problem-solving (Bauer et al., 2000; Lukowski & Milojevich, 2013; Lukowski et al., 2015; Lukowski et al., 2005; Phung et al., 2014).
Data were coded by an undergraduate research assistant who was trained on data coding procedures using an existing corpus of data prior to participant testing and was unaware of the hypotheses of the study. The first author then independently recoded the data for 25% of the sample (n = 5). Mean percent agreement on the production of TA and their order was 97% (range from 90% to 100%).
Results
Twenty-one percent (n = 4) of the children were participating in SST at the time of their involvement in the study, 5% (n = 1) had previously participated in SST but were not involved at the time of the study, and 74% (n = 14) of the children had never participated in SST. Given the small percentages of children who ever experienced SST, we grouped children into those who had ever participated in SST (the SST+ group) and those who had not (the SST− group).
Demographic Information
One-way ANOVAs and Fisher’s exact tests (FET) were conducted to determine whether children differed by group on potentially relevant demographic characteristics. As shown in Table 1, group differences were not found when considering chronological age, DA, child sex, child ethnicity, child exposure to English in the home, maternal education, or the percent of children who had ever participated in occupational therapy, physical therapy, psychotherapy, or speech therapy.
Table 1.
Differences in Demographic Characteristics by Group.
SST+ (n = 5) | SST− (n = 14) | Statistic | p-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Chronological age (months) | 35.36 ± 5.36 | 32.55 ± 3.20 | F = 0.20 | .66 |
Developmental age (months) | 23.00 ± 2.22 | 21.14 ± 1.33 | F = 0.52 | .48 |
Child sex (% girls) | 60% (n = 3) | 57% (n = 8) | — | 1.00 |
Child ethnicity (% Hispanic) | 60% (n = 3) | 29% (n = 4) | — | .31 |
Child language (% exposed to English at least 50% of the time at home) | 100% (n = 5) | 92% (n = 12) | — | .706 |
Maternal education (% with at least a 4-year degree) | 100% (n = 5) | 79% (n = 11) | — | .53 |
Occupational therapy (% that ever participated) | 100% (n = 5) | 100% (n = 14) | — | — |
Physical therapy (% that ever participated) | 80% (n = 4) | 86% (n = 12) | — | 1.00 |
Psychotherapy (% that ever participated) | 40% (n = 2) | 0% (n = 0) | — | .06 |
Speech therapy (% that ever participated) | 100% (n = 5) | 86% (n = 12) | — | 1.00 |
Note. Group differences for categorical values were examined using Fisher’s exact tests due to the small sample sizes. The p-value associated with two-sided effects is reported. SST = social skills therapy.
Preliminary Analyses
As delayed recall memory and generalization across cues were assessed in a between-subjects design at the second session, we conducted preliminary analyses to examine whether performance differed by event type (delayed recall memory or generalization across cues) at each study session.
Encoding.
We examined the impact of event type on encoding at the first session by conducting two 2 (event type) × 2 (phase: baseline and immediate imitation) mixed ANOVAs on the production of individual TA and PA completed in the correct temporal order. Main effects of phase were found on both dependent measures TA, F(1, 17) = 25.92, p < .0001, r = .78 and PA, F(1, 17) = 16.07, p < .001, r = .70, such that children performed more TA and PA at immediate imitation (TA: 1.71 ± .25; PA: .86 ± .19) relative to baseline (TA: .71 ± .15; PA: .11 ± .05). There were no main effects or interactions involving event type (null effects that were expected as the event type manipulation was not imposed until the second session).
Delayed recall.
We then examined the impact of event type on mnemonic performance after the 1-month delay by conducting two 2 (event type) × 2 (condition: familiar and novel event sequences) on TA and PA. As at the first session, the analyses revealed only main effects of condition on TA, F(1, 17) = 11.50, p = .003, r = .64, and PA, F(1, 17) = 5.71, p = .03, r = .50, such that children performed more TA and PA on familiar events (TA: 1.37 ± .20; PA: .53 ± .12) relative to novel control sequences (TA: .87 ± .17; PA: .31 ± .11). There were no main effects or interactions involving event type.
These analyses indicate that, as a group, children with DS did not perform differently by event type at encoding or after the 1-month delay. Whereas a more stringent analytic approach would have included participation in SST as a between-subjects factor, such analyses could not be conducted due to the naturally occurring distribution of children in each group. Among children in the SST+ group, four children were tested on memory events and four children were tested on generalization events. Among children in the SST− group, six children were tested on memory events and one child was tested on generalization events. Because only one child in the SST− group was tested on generalization events, formal statistical analyses could not be conducted to determine whether involvement in SST was differentially associated with mnemonic performance by event type at each session. In addition, there is no existing literature to suggest whether participation in SST would be differentially associated with delayed recall memory and generalization across cues in children with DS. For these reasons, we collapsed across event type in the following analyses and adopted the conservative approach of including event type as a categorical covariate type (0 = delayed recall memory or 1 = generalization across cues) in all analyses concerning mnemonic performance at the second session (when the event type manipulation was imposed).
Mnemonic Performance and Participation in SST
Encoding.
We examined whether participation in SST was associated with encoding by conducting two 2 (group) x 2 (phase) mixed ANOVAs on the production of TA and their order. A main effect of phase was found for TA, F(1, 17) = 34.45, p = .0001, r = .82, such that children performed more TA at immediate imitation (2.00 ± 0.25) relative to baseline (0.80 ± 0.16). A main effect of group was also apparent, F(1, 17) = 4.76, p = .04, r = .46, as children in the SST+ group performed more TA across phases (1.80 ± 0.32) relative to those in the SST− group (1.00 ± 0.19).
When considering memory for temporal order information, main effects of phase, F(1, 17) = 26.27, p = .0001, r = .78, and group, F(1, 17) = 8.65, p = .009, r = .58, were qualified by an interaction with one another: F(1, 17) = 5.24, p = .04, r = .49. These data are shown in Figure 2(a). Follow-up pairwise comparisons revealed that children performed more PA at immediate imitation relative to baseline regardless of their involvement in SST (ps < .01). Comparisons conducted by phase, however, indicated that children in the SST+ group performed more PA at immediate imitation relative to children in the SST− group (ps < .01); group differences were not found when considering performance at baseline.
Figure 2.
(a) Significant Group x Phase interaction for pairs of actions at the first session. Although children in both groups produced more pairs of actions at immediate imitation relative to baseline, children in the SST+ group produced more pairs of actions at immediate imitation relative to children in the SST− group. (b) Significant Group x Phase interaction for target actions at the second session. Delayed recall was found only for children in the SST+ group; in addition, children in the SST+ group produced more target actions on familiar events relative to children in the SST− group. SST = social skills therapy.
Delayed recall.
We examined whether participation in SST was associated with mnemonic performance at the 1-month delay by conducting two 2 (group) × 2 (condition) mixed ANOVAs on the production of individual TA and their order controlling for event type. The analysis for TA yielded main effects of condition, F(1, 16) = 10.10, p = .006, r = .62, and group, F(1, 16) = 6.10, p = .03, r = .53 that were further qualified by an interaction with one another, F(1, 16) = 5.96, p = .03, r = .52. These data are shown in Figure 2(b). Follow-up pairwise comparisons conducted by group revealed that children in the SST+ group produced more TA on familiar sequences relative to novel control events (p < .001), whereas differences were not found by condition for children in the SST− group. Pairwise comparisons conducted by condition revealed that children in the SST+ group performed more TA on familiar events relative to children in the SST− group (p < .005); group differences were not found on novel control sequences. No significant effects were found when examining PA.
Encoding as a Potential Mediator
Because the ANOVAs revealed that participation in SST was associated with increased encoding of temporal order information and better memory for TA after a 1-month delay, we examined whether performance at immediate imitation mediated the relation between participation in SST and 1-month delayed recall of familiar events. Mediation models were examined using the regression-based approach outlined in Hayes (2013), in which evidence of mediation is obtained when a bias-corrected confidence interval (k = 10,000 resamples) does not include 0. Separate analyses were conducted for TA and PA. In both analyses, event type was included as a categorical covariate.
Comparable findings were obtained across the two tested models. Participation in SST (0 = SST− group or 1 = SST+ group) was positively associated with encoding (TA: a = 1.27, p = .03; PA: a = 1.04, p = .02). Encoding was also positively associated with 1-month delayed recall (TA: b = .39, p = .03; PA: b = .44, p = .004). The direct effect of participation in SST on 1-month delayed recall was not significant for either dependent measure (TA: c’ = .78, p = .07; PA: c’ = .14, p = .58), whereas the indirect effect of encoding on the association between participation in SST and 1-month delayed recall was significant for both TA and temporal order information (TA: ab = .50, confidence interval from .0074 to 1.2969; PA: ab = .4,542, confidence interval from .0351 to 1.1440).
Given these significant findings, we then examined the specificity of the effects by conducting comparable analyses on the number of TA and PA performed on novel control sequences at the second session. Neither the direct nor indirect effects were significant.
Discussion
Although participation in SST has been associated with improved cognitive outcomes in children with ASD (Cappadocia & Weiss, 2011; Hwang & Hughes, 2000; Ozonoff & Miller, 1995), there is a lack of research examining the potential benefits of this intervention on cognitive functioning in children with DS. The limited attention to the potential benefits of this therapeutic technique likely results from previous studies indicating that children with DS experience relative strengths in imitation and social interaction (Dykens & Hodapp, 2001; Libby et al., 1997; Rast & Meltzoff, 1995; Roberts & Richmond, 2015; Wright et al., 2006). Such competencies are also evident in research conducted using the elicited imitation paradigm, as children with DS encoded TA and temporal order at levels comparable with TD children matched on DA. Despite this strength in encoding, long-term memory for temporal order information was impaired in the children with DS. That is, whereas TD children recalled the temporal order of three-step event sequences after a 1-month delay, children with DS did not (Milojevich & Lukowski, 2016). The present study was conducted to collect pilot data from a small sample of children with DS to examine whether participation in SST was associated with enhanced encoding and 1-month delayed recall. We chose to examine the potential benefits of participating in SST in particular, as the social context shapes the manner in which TD infants recall the past (Lukowski et al., 2005; Phung et al., 2014) and children (e.g., Fivush & Fromhoff, 1988).
Consistent with previous research (Rast & Meltzoff, 1995; Roberts & Richmond, 2015), the results revealed that children with DS encoded the TA and their order relative to baseline independent of their involvement in SST. Similarly, children in the SST+ and SST− groups demonstrated encoding of temporal order information relative to baseline. However, participation in SST was associated with enhanced encoding of temporal order, such that children in the SST+ group performed more PA at immediate imitation relative to children in the SST− group. This finding indicates that although participation in SST was not necessary for children to encode the demonstrated TA and their order, participation was associated with more robust encoding of temporal order information in particular.
Contrary to what was observed at the first session, involvement in SST was necessary to support memory for individual TA after a 1-month delay (children did not show evidence of remembering temporal order information over the long term). In addition, examination of performance on familiar and novel events revealed findings that varied by group and condition. Participation in SST was only associated with performance on familiar events, as children in the SST+ group performed more TA on familiar events relative to children in the SST− group. The lack of group differences on novel events indicates that participation in SST was preferentially associated with memory and did not result in increased problem-solving abilities or general interaction with the sequence materials.
Given the demonstrated association between involvement in SST and mnemonic performance, mediation analyses were conducted to examine the process through which involvement in SST might facilitate recall. The results revealed that participation in SST was not directly associated with memory after the 1-month delay. Instead, encoding mediated the association between involvement in SST and 1-month delayed recall. This finding was observed when considering memory for individual TA and PA for previously modeled events but was not observed when considering performance on novel control events presented at the second session. The results pertaining to familiar events are reminiscent of other work conducted with TD infants in which encoding manipulations promoted evidence of long-term recall. For example, allowing infants additional exposures to to-be-remembered information (Bauer, Wiebe, Waters, & Bangston, 2001) or the opportunity to imitate the demonstrated TA before the imposition of an extended delay (e.g., Lukowski et al., 2005) has been associated with enhanced memory over lengthy delays.
Limitations and Future Directions
The present research represents proof of the concept that participation in SST is associated with enhanced encoding and 1-month delayed recall in children with DS and supports the notion that additional empirical attention should be devoted to understanding the potential benefits of participating in SST for children with DS. In addition to focusing on causal relations and additional description of the mechanism underlying the observed associations between involvement in SST and recall memory, subsequent research on this topic should account for some of the limitations of the present study. Given the preliminary nature of this research, we did not ask parents to report on the particular elements of SST experienced by children in the SST+ group. Future researchers should ask parents to report on the particular characteristics of the interventions experienced by their children so as to identify the specific elements of SST that are associated with encoding and delayed recall. In addition, although generally acceptable for pilot research and proof of concept studies, one of the primary limitations of this work is the rather small sample of children with DS who had participated in SST. However, this limitation reflects the natural state of participation in SST, as this intervention is not commonly experienced by children with DS. Despite this shortcoming, reported effect sizes for the conducted statistical analyses were quite large (most rs ≥ .50; Rosenthal & Rosnow, 1984), suggesting that future research with larger samples should yield similar findings.
Implications for Research and Practice
Although exploratory, the conducted research advances our understanding of the utility of SST for children with DS by revealing associations with encoding and 1-month delayed recall memory. These findings are meaningful in that they draw attention to the potential benefits of an intervention that is not commonly experienced by children with DS. Based on these preliminary results, future research should also determine whether SST facilitates performance on declarative memory tasks in older individuals and under test conditions that are less socially focused (such as the ability to recall previously presented word pairs; for a review of other hippocampus-dependent memory processes, see Eichenbaum, 2004). Experimental work is also needed to determine which elements of SST are causally associated with improvements in declarative memory. Better understanding of the impact of social skills interventions on children with DS through future empirical research may be instrumental in promoting cognitive competencies in these individuals. Specifically, such findings would have significant implications for promoting learning and retention in children with DS in educational and therapeutic contexts, thereby facilitating adaptive functioning in the short term as well as over extended delays.
Acknowledgments
Funding
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This research was funded in part by the Undergraduate Research Opportunities Program (UROP) at the University of California, Irvine.
Biography
Helen M. Milojevich is a postdoctoral fellow at the Center for Developmental Science at UNC-Chapel Hill. Her research focuses on understanding how experiences of early adversity impact children’s development. She is particularly interested in understanding how experiences of early adversity lead to risk for psychopathology through differences in emotional competence.
Emily M. Slonecker is a third year doctoral student in the Department of Psychological Science at UC Irvine. Her research examines the intersection of child narrative skills and autobiographical memory development. Specifically, she is interested in exploring how gender socialization impacts child testimony within the legal system.
Angela F. Lukowski is an Associate Professor and Vice Chair in the Department of Psychological Science at UC Irvine. Her research primarily focuses on cognitive development in infancy and early childhood with the goal of identifying how context impacts cognitive functioning in typically developing children and children with Down syndrome.
Footnotes
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
References
- Abbeduto L, Pavetto M, Kesin E, Weissman MD, ’Karadottir S, O‘Brien A, & Cawthon S (2001). The linguistic and cognitive profile of Down syndrome: Evidence from a comparison with fragile X syndrome. Down Syndrome: Research and Practice, 7, 9–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bauer PJ, DeBoer T, & Lukowski AF (2007). In the language of multiple memory systems: Defining and describing developments in long-term declarative memory (pp. 240–270). In Oakes LM & Bauer PJ (Eds.), Short- and long-term memory in infancy and early childhood: Taking the first steps towards remembering. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Bauer PJ, & Lukowski AF (2010). The memory is in the details: Relations between memory for the specific features of events and long-term recall during infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 1–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bauer PJ, Wenner JA, Dropik PL, & Wewerka SS (2000). Parameters of remembering and forgetting in the transition from infancy to early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 65, 1–204. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bauer PJ, Wiebe SA, Waters JM, & Bangston SK (2001). Reexposure breeds recall: Effects of experience on 9-month-olds’ ordered recall. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 174–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bayley N (2006). Bayley scales of infant and toddler development-III. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment. [Google Scholar]
- Cappadocia MC, & Weiss JA (2011). Review of social skills training groups for youth with Asperger syndrome and high functioning autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 70–78. [Google Scholar]
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2014). Birth defects: Data and statistics on Down syndrome. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/birthdefects/downsyndrome/data.html
- Connolly B, Morgan S, Russell FF, & Richardson B (1980). Early intervention with Down syndrome children: Follow-up report. Physical Therapy, 60, 1405–1408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Connolly BH, Morgan SB, Russell FF, & Fulliton WL (1993). A longitudinal study of children with Down syndrome who experienced early intervention programming. Physical Therapy, 73, 170–179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dykens EM, & Hodapp RM (2001). Research in mental retardation: Toward an etiologic approach. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 42, 49–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eichenbaum H (2004). Hippocampus: Cognitive processes and neural representations that underlie declarative memory. Neuron, 44, 109–120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feeley KM, Jones EA, Blackburn C, & Bauer S (2011). Advancing imitation and requesting skills in toddlers with Down syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 2415–2430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fivush R, & Fromhoff FA (1988). Style and structure in mother-child conversations about the past. Discourse Processes, 11, 337–355. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes AF (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Heimann M, Laberg KE, & Nordoen B (2006). Imitative interaction increases social interest and elicited imitation in non-verbal children with autism. Infant and Child Development, 15, 297–309. [Google Scholar]
- Hwang B, & Hughes C (2000). The effects of social interactive training on early social communicative skills of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 331–343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laws G, & Bishop DVM (2003). The comparison of language abilities in adolescents with Down syndrome and children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 1324–1339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Libby S, Powell S, Messer D, & Jordan R (1997). Imitation of pretend play acts by children with autism and Down syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 365–383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukowski AF, & Milojevich HM (2013). Sleeping like a baby: Examining relations between habitual infant sleep, recall memory, and generalization across cues at 10 months. Infant Behavior & Development, 36, 369–376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukowski AF, & Milojevich HM (2016). Examining recall memory in infancy and early childhood using the elicited imitation paradigm. Journal of Visualized Experiments, 110, Article e53347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukowski AF, & Milojevich HM (2017). Sleep problems and temperament in young children with Down syndrome and typically developing controls. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 61, 221–232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukowski AF, Phung JN, & Milojevich HM (2015). Language facilitates event memory in early childhood: Child comprehension, adult-provided linguistic support and delayed recall at 16 months. Memory, 23, 848–863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukowski AF, Slonecker EM, & Milojevich HM (2019). Sleep problems and recall memory in children with Down syndrome and typically developing controls. Manuscript submitted for publication. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukowski AF, Wiebe SA, & Bauer PJ (2009). Going beyond the specifics: Generalization of single actions, but not temporal order, at 9 months. Infant Behavior & Development, 32, 331–335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukowski AF, Wiebe SA, Haight JC, Deboer T, Nelson CA, & Bauer PJ (2005). Forming a stable memory representation in the first year of life: Why imitation is more than child’s play. Developmental Science, 8, 279–298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ly AR (2013). It’s all in the family: Marital and coparenting quality in families of children with and without autism spectrum disorders (Doctoral dissertation). Available from ProQuest Information and Learning [Google Scholar]
- Milojevich HM, & Lukowski AF (2016). Recall memory in children with Down syndrome and typically developing peers matched on developmental age. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 60, 89–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- National Down Syndrome Society. (2012). Early intervention. Retrieved from http://www.ndss.org/Resources/Therapies-Development/Early-Intervention/
- Ozonoff S, & Miller JN (1995). Teaching theory of mind: A new approach to social skills training for individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25, 415–433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phung JN, Milojevich HM, & Lukowski AF (2014). Adult language use and infant comprehension of English: Associations with encoding and generalization across cues at 20 months. Infant Behavior & Development, 37, 465–479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rast M, & Meltzoff AN (1995). Memory and representation in young children with Down syndrome: Exploring deferred imitation and object permanence. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 393–407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts LV, & Richmond JL (2015). Preschoolers with Down syndrome do not yet show the learning and memory impairments seen in adults with Down syndrome. Developmental Science, 3, 404–419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenthal R, & Rosnow RL (1984). Essentials of behavioral research: Methods and data analysis. New York, NY: McGraw Hill. [Google Scholar]
- Selikowitz M (1997). Down syndrome: The facts (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Wenner JA, & Bauer PJ (1999). Bringing order to the arbitrary: One to two-year-olds’ recall of event sequences. Infant Behavior & Development, 22, 585–590. [Google Scholar]
- Wright I, Lewis V, & Collis GM (2006). Imitation and representational development in young children with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56, 429–450. [Google Scholar]