Previously termed “somatization,” conditions that are now referred to as medically unexplained (e.g., irritable bowel, chronic fatigue syndrome, fibromyalgia) are characterized by chronic symptoms in which the cause is unknown or contested (Page & Wessely, 2003, p. 223). As described in the lead article of this Major Contribution (Authors, first paper under review for this issue), counseling psychologists can play multiple roles to improve the lives of patients with medically unexplained symptoms (MUS).
Our major role is, of course, a clinical one. Physicians often refer patients with MUS for cognitive-behavioral interventions. Indeed, cognitive-behavioral treatment is recommended as the first-line treatment for MUS (e.g., van Dessel et al., 2014). While some patients gratefully accept a referral for mental health care, others see the referral as an invalidation of their illness (Page & Wessely, 2003). In cases like these, the psychologist who receives the “soft hand-off” from a physician needs to listen, understand, and empathize with the patient’s frustration before explaining the potential benefits of a psychosocial treatment.
Understandably, when diagnostic testing has been inconclusive and multiple treatments have been tried unsuccessfully, both physicians and patients are likely to be frustrated (Epstein et al., 2006). For this reason, psychologists can play two additional important roles in the care of these patients: (1) training and/or consulting with medical providers about how to talk with patients about their chronic physical symptoms and, to guide these efforts, (2) researching best communication practices for relating to patients with chronic MUS (McAndrew et al., 2019).
At present, most of the literature on communicating and developing relationships with patients with MUS is clinical opinion or, if empirical, qualitative rather than quantitative (cf. van Ravenzwaaij et al., 2010). Overall, authors emphasize the need to reassure and empower rather than reject patients with MUS who are frustrated over what they see as poor medical care (e.g., Page & Wessely, 2003). Patients want their providers to believe that their physical symptoms are real and to treat them respectfully. Exemplifying this point, one qualitative study (Nordin et al., 2006) found that for some patients with MUS, improving their relationships with medical providers was the top concern, even exceeding their desire for functional improvement.
McAndrew, et al. (2019) pointed out that counseling psychologists can also contribute meaningfully to the treatment of patients with MUS by researching the predictors and outcomes of effective patient-provider relationships. To further this objective, it is first necessary to identify the kinds of communications that characterize strong relationships.
In line with this thinking, we are conducting a federally-funded project to study aspects of effective observable physician-patient relationships in primary care consultations with military veterans who have Gulf War Illness, one type of MUS. For these veterans, being believed by their physicians about the etiology and reality of their symptoms is of paramount importance. The literature on concordance in medical care informed the plan of this project. As a concept, concordance refers to interactions in which patient and provider listen to one another’s beliefs about the illness or condition, negotiate a shared understanding of these beliefs (Royal Pharmaceutical Society of Great Britain, 1997), and come to an agreement about how to move forward. Essentially, concordance is at the heart of what Epstein et al. (2005) termed patient-centered communication, i.e., patient and physician reaching a “shared understanding of the problem and its treatment” (p. 1521).
Generally speaking, authors concur that a lack of concordance – discordance -- is exceedingly problematic (e.g., Salmon, 2007; Wileman, May, & Chew-Graham, 2002). In the context of MUS, perceived discordance is common (Engel, Adkins, & Cowan, 2002) since patients often see their MUS as physically determined (Chew-Graham, Dowrick, Wearden, Richardson, & Peters, 2010; olde Hartman et al., 2009), whereas medical providers often see these conditions as stress related (Reid, Whooley, Crayford, & Hotopf, 2001). Unfortunately, all too often discordant views wind up “pit[ting] patients and providers against each other” (McAndrew, Friedlander, Phillips, Santos, & Helmer, 2018).
So how is concordance reflected in observable patient-provider communication? In searching the relevant literature, we located no observer measure of this specific construct, despite an abundance of articles on the characteristics of relationship-centered care (e.g., Roter, 2000) and shared decision making (e.g., Makoul & Clayman, 2006). To fill this gap, we created the System for Observing Medical Alliances, or SOMA, in order to assess concordance of illness perceptions and treatment recommendations behaviorally. This article describes the development of the SOMA and an initial test of its interrater reliability.
A secondary aim of this article is to introduce TCP readers to the health psychology literature underpinning our development of the SOMA. We begin with a discussion of the concepts concordance around illness beliefs (Phillips, Leventhal, & Leventhal, 2011; Royal Pharmaceutical Society of Great Britain, 1997), which informed the SOMA’s operational definition of concordance, and shared decision-making in medical care (e.g., Makoul & Clayman, 2006), which informed our development of a pool of patient and provider behaviors that may facilitate concordance.
Concordance around Illness Beliefs
To identify specific behavioral indicators of concordance for the SOMA, we began by reviewing Leventhal and colleagues’ (2003) Common-Sense Model of Self-Regulation (Hale, Treharne, & Kitas, 2007; Leventhal, Weinman, Leventhal, & Phillips, 2008; McAndrew et al., 2008). In this model, people’s medical self-management efforts, including when to seek treatment and whether to follow a provider’s suggestions, are guided by five aspects of beliefs about the health threat (McAndrew et al., 2018): (1) the identity or nature of the underlying condition, (2) its cause and (3) consequence, (4) the timeline for symptom relief or cure, and (5) the degree of perceived control over the problematic symptoms and possible treatments (McAndrew, Martin et al., 2017; McAndrew, Schneider, Burns, & Leventhal, 2007).
From this perspective, people’s self-management efforts are reflected in how they make behavioral adjustments in response to attempts to relieve a problematic health condition (McAndrew et al., 2008). Consider, for example, sudden, unusual and acute back pain. The belief that the pain is due to back strain (identity) after lifting a heavy object (cause) prompts the person to apply a topical cream. If the pain continues unabated, the person’s next self-management attempt might be to use a heating pad, get a massage, or take an analgesic. If all these control attempts fail, the person is likely to make an appointment with a medical provider, believing that the pain has become chronic (timeline) and may result in serious debilitation (consequence).
Furthermore, suffering from unexplained symptoms is sustained by these kinds of illness beliefs in a circular fashion (van Ravenzwaaij et al., 2010). That is, the onset of symptoms typically requires a person to make some sense of them, but this increased sensitivity to the symptoms and consequent avoidance of physical and social activity may exacerbate the symptoms, leading to further attempts to find a cause, with increasingly more avoidance and greater functional impairment.
To interrupt this vicious cycle, providers need to negotiate a shared understanding of the health threat with their patients before recommending interventions to improve quality of life (McAndrew et al., 2018). Supporting this point, a survey of patients seeking care at an internal medicine clinic found that perceived concordance with physicians around illness beliefs predicted participants’ adherence to their physicians’ recommendations in the subsequent month (Phillips et al., 2011). By contrast, when patients and providers disagree about the nature or cause of a health condition, not only does their relationship tend to suffer (Hahn, 2001; Ring, Dowrick, Humphris, & Salmon, 2004; Smith et al., 2006; Wileman et al., 2002), but also the physician’s recommendations tend to be dismissed or, if passively accepted, not adhered to.
In short, providers who recognize the importance of concordance purposefully engage patients in a discussion of illness beliefs in order to negotiate a singular view of the health condition that will align with the proposed treatment. In this sense, patient-provider concordance around illness beliefs is the outcome of shared decision making, a more longstanding term in the health field.
Shared Decision Making
Interest in viewing patients as partners in their health care has burgeoned over the past 35 years since the concept of shared decision making was mentioned in a 1982 Presidential Commission report on biomedical ethics (Makoul & Clayman, 2006). Contemporary authors concur that medical care has moved away from what has been called the traditional, “paternalistic” approach to patients (Charles, Gafney, & Whelan, 1999, p. 651) to a more equitable sharing of treatment decisions. This movement was no doubt fueled by an exponential increase in treatment options for even the most serious diseases, and the recognition that (a) benefits need to be balanced against risks when choosing one treatment option over another (Charles et al., 1999) and (b) patients differ in the value they place on risk taking (Ford, Schofield, & Hope, 2003).
Whereas the benefit of involving patients in decision making has been amply demonstrated in terms of enhanced patient satisfaction, better adherence to treatment, reduction of physical symptoms, improved psychological well-being (Ford et al., 2003) and retention in therapy for post-traumatic stress disorder (Mott, Stanley, Street, Grady, & Teng, 2014), less is known about behaviors that contribute to effective decision making. Addressing this question, Ford et al. (2003) conducted a qualitative study in the U.K. to discover aspects of informed decision making that people see as essential. Analysis of semi-structured interviews with patients, providers, and lay individuals showed no clear consensus on the amount of guidance medical personnel should offer patients, but the provision of up-to-date health information and a strong patient-provider relationship emerged as important themes. With respect to qualities of the relationship, most participants mentioned “trust, respect, honesty and partnership building” (p. 595), all of which seem essential for developing concordance.
Using a different methodology, Smith, Pandit, Rush, Wolf, and Simon (2016) surveyed 3,400 adults to understand the behavioral and demographic factors associated with patients’ preferences for shared decision making. Respondents who viewed this process as most beneficial were significantly older, more highly educated, had more income and were in better health overall, with fewer chronic conditions. When demographic characteristics were controlled for, viewing shared decision making as desirable was positively associated with what Smith et al. called patient activation, or “the degree to which an individual possesses knowledge, motivation, skills, and confidence to make effective health-related decisions” (p. 68). The authors recommended that providers need to prepare patients, particularly those younger, with low incomes, and less education, how to engage in meaningful discussions of their medical care.
In short, the literature on shared decision making highlights two important aspects of provider behavior that informed our creation of the SOMA: facilitating the patient’s (1) trust and (2) active engagement in the consultation process. These aspects brought to mind the working alliance in psychotherapy, which Fuertes and colleagues (2007, 2009) adapted to be a closer fit to physical medicine by defining it as “patient-provider trust and agreement about the nature of the treatment” (2007, p. 30). For this reason, we reviewed the literature on patients’ and providers’ perceptions of the medical working alliance to develop specific items for the SOMA.
Perceptions of the Medical Working Alliance
The working alliance construct, having originated in the psychotherapy literature, has recently received a fair amount of attention in the fields of physical rehabilitation (Hall, Ferreira, Maher, Latmer, & Ferreira, 2010) and oncology (e.g., Mack et al., 2009; Trevino, Maciejewski, Epstein, & Prigerson, 2015). As is well known in our field, perceptions of the working alliance have demonstrated significant, substantive associations with client outcomes in literally hundreds of studies of individual (Flückiger, Del Re, & Horvath, 2018) and couple/family (Friedlander, Escudero, Welmers van-der Poll, & Heatherington, 2018) therapies.
In adapting the shortened version (Tracey & Kokotovic, 1989) of the Working Alliance Inventory (Horvath & Greenberg, 1989) to physical medicine, Fuertes et al. (2007) discussed the failure of other medical relationship measures to adequately assess the alliance construct as defined by Bordin (1979): a strong emotional bond and negotiated agreements on the goals and tasks of treatment. Fuertes et al.’s (2007) 12-item Physician-Patient Working Alliance Inventory (P-PWAI) operationalizes the bond as liking, trust, understanding and confidence in the physician, and agreement as shared ideas about (a) the medical problem and treatment plan (goals) and (b) health improvement methods (tasks). From this definition, we considered that the trust aspect of alliance in medical care seems less personal than the emotional bond aspect of the alliance in psychotherapy, and the agreement aspect is more specific to concordance of illness beliefs and treatment recommendations.
Fuertes et al.’s (2007, 2009) studies with the P-PWAI showed significant associations between patients’ perceptions of the alliance and their actual adherence to treatment recommendations, adherence self-efficacy, perceptions of their physicians’ empathy, and satisfaction with medical treatment. However, since the self-report items on the P-PWAI are phenomenological (e.g., “I believe that my doctor likes me”) rather than behavioral, we searched the observational communication literature to discover whether Fuertes et al.’s (2007, 2009) two aspects of the medical alliance (trust and agreement) had been operationalized behaviorally. As described below, compared to agreement/concordance, the relational aspect of patient-provider communication has received the bulk of attention in the health care literature.
Observing Relationship-Centered Care
The construct relationship-centered care, characterized as optimal in terms of mutuality between patient and provider (Roter, 2000), set the stage for health researchers’ development of various observer measures of physicians’ communication behaviors. Describing what she called a building-block approach to studying these behaviors, Roter (2000) delineated five aspects of physician communication: information giving (content and manner), question asking (format and content), partnership-building, rapport-building, and socioemotional talk. All five of these aspects are represented in the Roter Interaction Analysis System (RIAS; Roter, 1977; 1995; Roter & Larson, 2002), the most comprehensive and widely used observer measure of patient-physician communication. Compared with other measures that only categorize the behavior of physicians (e.g., Elwyn et al., 2003; Krupat, Frankel, Stein, & Irish, 2006), the RIAS codes for both patient and physician take into account nonverbal as well as verbal behavior.
While the RIAS does not measure concordance, use of this measure in a study of contingent physician/patient behaviors (Eide, Quera, Graugaard, & Finset, 2003) informed our conceptualization of the SOMA. Specifically, Eide et al. conducted a lag sequential analysis of the RIAS categories during cancer and hematology consultations to investigate how patient behaviors may have acted as cues for physician behaviors, and vice versa. The purpose of the analysis was to identify specific pairs of behavior that might be significantly and substantively associated. Results showed that in both samples, behaviors indicative of emotional support (e.g., affirmation) on the part of the physician significantly preceded patients’ expressions of concern about their condition. Similarly, patient expressions of concern were significantly followed by physicians’ emotional support. The authors noted the importance of these relational aspects of communication since in previous literature (e.g., Lo, 1995; Maguire, Faulkner, Booth, Elliott, & Hillier, 1996), questioning was described as the best method for prompting patients to become actively involved in the consultation process. Results of this sequential study confirmed our decision to include dimensions of patient engagement and trust in the SOMA.
Overview and Development of the SOMA
Alliance Dimensions and Items
The SOMA model has three alliance dimensions: Engagement in the Consultation Process (ENGAGEMENT), Trust in the Provider (TRUST), and Concordance of Illness Beliefs and Treatment Recommendations (CONCORDANCE). Based on their observations of patient and provider behavior (nonverbal as well as verbal), trained raters use these indicators to arrive at five ratings of a medical consultation: patient ENGAGEMENT, provider contribution to patient ENGAGEMENT, patient TRUST, provider contribution to patient TRUST, and patient-provider CONCORDANCE. The various operationally defined indicators, clustered within each dimension, reflect patients’ positive or facilitative (+) and negative or hindering (−) behaviors related to engaging with and trusting their providers; providers’ behaviors reflecting their contributions to (+) or detractions from (−) their patients’ engagement and trust; and patient-provider interactions that reflect concordance (+) and discordance (−).
We developed the original pool of verbal and nonverbal behavioral indicators from four sources: (1) studies of concordance of illness beliefs, shared decision making and relationship-centered health care referenced earlier; (2) Fuertes et al.’s (2007, 2009) self-report measure of the medical alliance, the P-PWAI; (3) various observer systems for coding patient-physician communication (Del Piccolo, Putnam, Mazzi, & Zimmermann, 2004; Elwyn et al., 2003; Krupat et al., 2006; Roter & Larson, 2001); and (4) Friedlander et al.’s (2006) SOFTA-o1, an observer measure of alliance-related behaviors in psychotherapy. Although the behavioral indicators in the SOMA system reflect medical working alliances in physical healthcare, we do not assume that this set of indicators reflects the universe of alliance-related behaviors, but rather is indicative of common occurrences in consultations with diverse kinds of medical providers.
We intentionally wrote the behavioral indicators (italicized below) to require little inference on the part of raters. In order to further minimize subjectivity, raters are directed to refer to an indicator’s operational definition when in doubt about its presence. As an example, a negative indicator of patient TRUST, patient refuses or is reluctant to respond to the provider’s questions, is operationally defined as:
This item is checked each time the patient purposefully does not respond or react, verbally or nonverbally, to a comment, question, or request made by the provider. This item may also be checked if the provider has to repeat the comment or question multiple times in order to elicit a response (with the exception of patients who have difficulty hearing, etc.), or if the patient only responds when explicitly invited by the provider (“What do you think about that?” “Did you hear my question?”). The patient may also express reluctance by saying, for example, “I don’t want to talk about it,” or “I don’t care,” or “It’s none of your business.” Moments of silence in which the patient is thinking should not be checked. Additionally, if the patient nods or shake his/her head, but does not respond verbally, this item should not be checked.
Below are the definitions of each SOMA dimension, with sample behaviors, and Table 1 presents hypothetical patient and provider dialogue for several indicators. The complete list of behaviors and their operational definitions can be found in the online supplement to this article.
Table 1.
Examples of SOMA Indicators and Illustrative Quotes by Alliance Dimension
Dimension | Sample Behavioral Indicators | Hypothetical Quotes |
---|---|---|
ENGAGEMENT | ||
Patient | Patient asks the provider to explain the treatment recommendations | “So you think more exercise will help? So how often should I be exercising?” |
Provider | Provider encourages the patient to take an active role in the consultation | “I’m really curious to hear your thoughts about what would be most helpful for you right now.” |
TRUST | ||
Patient | Patient expresses confidence in the provider’s knowledge/expertise or ability to help | “You’ve always helped me in the past, and you’re the one with the medical degree and experience, so I’ll do whatever you think I should try next.” |
Provider | Provider offers reassurance or normalizes the patient’s reactions or emotions | “It is really hard to live with constant pain, and I’m aware that many people with your condition often feel frustrated or even hopeless at times.” |
CONCORDANCE | Provider and patient agree on the etiology of the patient’s health/medical concerns | Patient: “I think I’m short of breath because I’ve been smoking for so long.” Provider: “Yeah, I think you are probably right about that.” |
ENGAGEMENT in the Consultation Process:
The patient’s view of the medical consultation as meaningful and potentially helpful; a sense of involvement in the process; a sense that improvement in the medical condition or the patient’s health is possible. Examples of behavioral indicators of patient ENGAGEMENT include patient asks the provider to explain the treatment recommendations (+) and patient shows a lack of involvement or indifference toward the consultation process (−). Some indicators of provider ENGAGEMENT include provider encourages the patient to take an active role in the consultation (+), and provider criticizes the patient’s lack of involvement in the consultation process (−).
TRUST in the Provider:
The patient’s sense that the provider is trustworthy, respectful, genuinely concerned and cares about the patient’s welfare; a sense that the provider has the requisite knowledge, skills, and expertise to help the patient; a sense of comfort, safety and lack of defensiveness. Some patient indicators include patient expresses confidence in the provider’s knowledge/expertise or ability to help (+), and patient avoids eye contact with the provider throughout much of the consultation (−). Examples of provider indicators include provider offers reassurance or normalizes the patient’s reactions or emotions (+), and provider does not attend to overt expressions of patient vulnerability (−).
CONCORDANCE of Illness Perceptions and Treatment Recommendations:
The patient and provider having the sense of working collaboratively to understand and address the patient’s symptoms and improve the patient’s health, well-being, and quality of life; a sense of mutuality or solidarity (“we’re in this together”), or a felt unity between patient and provider. Some patient-provider interactions indicative of concordance (+) and discordance (−) include provider and patient agree on the etiology of the patient’s health/medical concerns (+), provider and patient negotiate or compromise on how the patient will follow the treatment recommendation(s) (+), provider and patient disagree on the etiology of the patient’s health/medical concerns (−) and provider and patient disagree that a proposed treatment plan can achieve the desired health outcome(s) (−).
Assessments of Face and Content Validity
To refine our initial 84-item pool of indicators based on an assessment of face and content validity, we surveyed two panels, identified through professional networks. First, 23 medical professionals (physicians, registered nurses, and dentists) were asked to indicate whether each behavioral indicator reflected a facilitative or hindering attitude toward the medical consultation on the part of patient/provider. Second, 12 health psychology researchers were asked to rate to which of the three SOMA dimensions each indicator belonged, its clarity (on a 3-point scale from not clear to clear) and representativeness (on a 3-point scale from not representative to highly representative). Panelists were also asked to suggest clarifications and additional behaviors that they viewed as particularly relevant to the medical alliance.
Based on the responses of both panels, we eliminated eight indicators due to unclear positive or negative valence, a lack of clarity or representativeness, or an unclear match with the SOMA dimension. We also modified three TRUST and ENGAGEMENT indicators and shifted them to CONCORDANCE. The resulting behaviors were used in the first assessment of the SOMA, described below.
SOMA Rating Procedure
Similar to the rating procedure for Friedlander et al.’s (2006) observer measure of the psychotherapeutic alliance, trained SOMA raters independently record the key behaviors that are clustered within each dimension as they are observed in a medical consultation session, referring to the operational definitions as needed. Next, each rater considers the frequency of the tallied behaviors, their valence (+ or -) and contextual meaningfulness in order to arrive at a global rating (for the entire consultation session) for each of the 5 SOMA dimensions (patient ENGAGEMENT and TRUST, provider ENGAGEMENT and TRUST, and patient-provider CONCORDANCE).
The ratings for each of the five dimensions can range from −3 (extremely problematic) to +3 (extremely strong). Based on specific guidelines in the rating manual, when only positive indicators are observed, the rating must be between +1 and +3; when only negative indicators are observed, the rating must be between −1 and −3; and when both positive and negative indicators are observed, the rating must be between −2 and +2. According to the guidelines, a dimensional rating of 0 (unremarkable/ neutral) is required when none of the behaviors in that dimension is observed.
Further Item Refinement and Interrater Reliabilities
In an initial test of the SOMA’s interrater reliability, 5 raters (3 female and 2 male PhD students in counseling psychology) used the SOMA to rate 33 audiotapes of primary and specialty care consultations with Gulf War veterans, all of whom had been deployed to the Persian Gulf between August, 1990, and July, 1991, and were formally diagnosed with Chronic Multisymptom Illness (i.e., MUS), commonly known as Gulf War Illness. These veterans (92% male; M = 51.33 years, SD = 4.59) identified as African American (29.17%), White/Caucasian (62.5%), Hispanic/Latinx (4.17%), or multiracial (4.17%). The providers either saw the veterans in primary care or at a specialty center, the War Related Illness and Injury Study Center (WRIISC), a national program in the Veteran’s Administration Post Deployment Health Services. The consultations were conducted by physicians from various medical specialties (e.g., internal medicine, gerontology, physical medicine/rehabilitation, occupational medicine, pulmonary/chronic care, etc.) who reviewed the veterans’ medical concerns (e.g., joint pain, irritable bowel syndrome) and conducted semi-structured interviews to take the veterans’ personal health histories and inquire about their exposures to chemicals, dust, contaminated water, and so on, prior to making treatment recommendations.
To maximize rater independence, one three-person team rated patient ENGAGEMENT and TRUST, and a different two-person team rated provider ENGAGEMENT and TRUST. All five judges rated CONCORDANCE. The audiotapes were rated independently, followed by team negotiation of discrepant ratings. By keeping track of the timing of each observed behavior using the tape counter, team members were easily able to discuss discrepancies while reviewing the audio recordings.
Since this was the first application of the SOMA, the raters conferred with the first author about adding or modifying specific indicators to more fully capture the consultation process. Specifically, two indicators were added to patient ENGAGEMENT, one indicator was added to provider TRUST, one indicator was deleted from patient TRUST, and one patient ENGAGEMENT indicator was modified. After these modifications, patient and provider ENGAGEMENT each had 13 indicators, patient and provider TRUST each had 14 indicators, and the CONCORDANCE dimension had 15 indicators. The operational definitions of the new and modified items were adjusted accordingly.
Based on the five judges’ independent ratings of the development sample of audiotapes, we calculated intraclass correlation (ICC) reliabilities using a two-effects, consistency model where raters are random and effects of the measures are fixed. Results were strong: patient ICC (3,3) = .94 (ENGAGEMENT) and .89 (TRUST); provider ICC (3,2) = .79 (ENGAGEMENT) and .82 (TRUST); and CONCORDANCE ICC (3,5) = .92. Team members negotiated all discrepancies to arrive at the final ratings used in the following analyses.
Descriptive Statistics
Table 2 summarizes the means, standard deviations, skewness, kurtosis, and intercorrelations of the five SOMA dimensions based on the sample of 33 audiotapes. As shown in the table, the indices were normally distributed, and all mean ratings were moderately positive (recall that ratings can range from −3 to +3). Patient ENGAGEMENT had the highest ratings, M = 2.08, SD = 0.82, and CONCORDANCE had the lowest ratings, M = 1.26, SD = 1.02.
Table 2.
SOMA Descriptive Statistics and Interrater Reliabilities
Dimension | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Patient | |||||
1. Engagement | -- | ||||
2. Trust | 0.45** | -- | |||
Provider | |||||
3. Engagement | 0.12 | −0.01 | -- | ||
4. Trust | 0.07 | 0.32* | 0.65*** | -- | |
5. Patient-Provider Concordance | 0.46** | 0.58*** | 0.13 | 0.28† | -- |
M | 2.08 | 1.42 | 0.92 | 1.40 | 1.26 |
SD | 0.82 | 1.15 | 0.85 | 0.83 | 1.02 |
Skewness | −0.54 | −0.62 | 0.20 | −0.56 | −0.57 |
Kurtosis | −0.74 | 1.35 | 0.15 | 1.47 | −0.00 |
Minimum, Maximum | 0, +3 | −2, +3 | −1, +3 | −1, +3 | −1, +3 |
ICC | 0.94a | 0.89a | 0.79b | 0.82b | 0.92c |
Notes. Ns = 33 (descriptive statistics), 16–23 (reliabilities). SOMA ratings can range from −3 (extremely problematic) to +3 (extremely strong), where 0 = unremarkable or neutral. ICC = Intraclass correlation coefficient.
Team 1 (3 raters).
Team 2 (2 raters).
Teams 1 and 2 (5 raters).
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
p = .065.
We anticipated moderately positive correlations between the two patient dimensions and between the two provider dimensions. Based on the development sample, this prediction was supported, r = .45, p < .01 (patient ENGAGEMENT and TRUST) and r = .65, p < .001 (provider ENGAGEMENT and TRUST). Notably, only the patient ratings were significantly correlated with CONCORDANCE, patient ENGAGEMENT r = .46, p < .01, and TRUST r = .58, p < .001, although the relation between CONCORDANCE and provider TRUST approached significance, r = .28, p = .065. Whereas patient TRUST was significantly associated with provider TRUST, r = .32, p < .05, patient and provider ENGAGEMENT were minimally correlated, r = .12, ns.
Prediction of Patient-Provider Concordance
For exploratory purposes, we regressed CONCORDANCE on the four other alliance dimensions. Results were significant, F(4, 26) = 4.36, p = .008, R2 = .40. Examination of the beta weights indicated that only patient TRUST accounted for significant unique variance, b = 0.45, t = 2.31, p = .03, in the prediction of CONCORDANCE.
Discussion
Our immediate objective in creating the System for Observing Medical Alliances was to create a behavioral measure of the relationship between patients with medically unexplained symptoms, or MUS, and their medical providers. Due to the contested nature of MUS, we anticipated that strong patient-provider relationships would be reflected in observed concordance about the nature of the illness and recommended treatment(s). This prediction was based on a study (Phillips & McAndrew, 2019) in which self-reported concordance about MUS between patients and providers was associated with greater patient satisfaction, and interviews with patients and providers who described discordance about the nature of MUS as an indicator of a poor medical alliance (Ring et al., 2004; Smith et al., 2006).
In the present initial assessment of the SOMA, we sampled health care consultations with Gulf War veterans suffering from MUS. Results indicated that as anticipated, ENGAGEMENT and TRUST were positively associated within each perspective (i.e., patient and provider). Notably, only the two patient dimensions were significantly associated with patient-provider CONCORDANCE, a finding that reflects (a) the importance of active patient involvement in the health care literature (e.g., Smith et al., 2016); and (b) Fuertes and colleagues’ (2007, 2009) adaptation of the working alliance to the medical encounter, in which patient trust figures prominently.
This is not to say that provider behavior is unrelated to concordance. Rather, it seems likely that providers’ attempts to enhance their patients’ trust may indirectly facilitate concordance. Although this reasoning awaits further testing, we found a significant correlation between patient and provider TRUST, and patient TRUST was the strongest unique contributor to CONCORDANCE. The latter finding extends the literature on facilitation of concordance around treatment recommendations. In particular, the result is consistent with studies indicating that trust-building behaviors on the part of providers are crucial aspects of relationship-centered care (Eide et al., 2004), particularly for patients who have MUS (Nordin et al., 2006; Page & Wessely, 2003). The authors of the first paper of this Major Contribution, for example, found that 30% of veterans suffering from MUS indicated that having a provider validate their experience was the most helpful aspect of the encounter.
Interestingly, the nonsignificant association between patient and provider ENGAGEMENT indicates that these two perspectives on engagement were distinct (i.e., patients’ behavior reflective of their level of engagement versus providers’ behavioral contributions to their patients’ engagement). Alternatively, since patient ENGAGEMENT was, on average, quite high compared to provider ENGAGEMENT and provider TRUST, the physicians in our sample may not have considered it as necessary to foster their patients’ engagement as it was to enhance their trust. This explanation is consistent with the results of an observational study of supportive communication by medical providers (Street, O’Malley, Cooper, & Haidet, 2008), in which patient engagement was self-initiated 84% of the time. Similarly, Street, Gordon, Ward, Krupat, and Kravitz, (2005) concluded that supportive communication is infrequent, but when providers do provide support, it tends to facilitate greater patient engagement.
Some authors suggested that active patient engagement, with little encouragement on the part of providers, can challenge traditional expectations for medical care (Anderson & Funnell, 2010). In support of this claim, a qualitative study concluded that patients with MUS are seen as demanding when they pressure their providers for medical interventions (Salmon, Ring, Dowrick, & Humphris, 2005). In this regard, the high average patient ENGAGEMENT rating in our military sample is noteworthy, since veterans with MUS have had to empower themselves to manage their condition (Iversen, Chalder, & Wessely, 2007; Zavestoski et al., 2004). For most of the 25 years since Operation Desert Storm/Shield (Persian Gulf War), veterans with MUS have been marginalized by the medical community, told by physicians that their symptoms are either “all in [their] head” or due to post-traumatic stress (Brown et al., 2001). In response, these veterans have advocated for better treatments, resulting in the Veterans Administration and Department of Defense investing hundreds of millions of dollars to improve the treatment of Gulf War Illness.
Although we created the SOMA to study war-related MUS, the measure can be used to investigate medical alliances in various physical care settings with diverse populations to advance knowledge about how concordance is reflected in observable patient and provider behavior. Indeed, our conceptualization of concordance as a critical component of the medical alliance reflects the growing trend in health care in which the term compliance with treatment was first replaced by adherence to treatment, and then more recently by concordance (Bell, Airaksinen, Lyles, Chen, & Aslani, 2007).
In developing the operational definition of concordance for the SOMA, we included patient-provider agreement on illness perceptions as well as on treatment recommendations. This definition contributes to the concordance literature, which mainly focuses on the shared decision process regarding treatment options (Joosten et al., 2008). However, patient and provider also need to agree on the nature and etiology of the problem. That is, when patient and provider do not share an understanding of the problem, self-management is likely to be compromised, since the daily management of chronic conditions like MUS relies heavily on the patient’s investment.
In reviewing the health psychology literature, we found that despite growing theoretical, empirical and clinical interest in concordance, there is little understanding of its behavioral manifestation. We believe that our identification of key behavioral indicators of concordance for the SOMA can be helpful for psychologists in their consultations with physicians who are having difficulty reaching concordance with their patients. Since medical providers tend to overestimate their patients’ understanding, satisfaction and adherence intentions (Phillips et al., 2011; Shannon, Mitchell, & Cain, 2002), estimation levels may improve when providers are introduced to the SOMA’s behavioral indicators of concordant versus discordant communication, patient indicators of engagement and trust, and specific behaviors providers can use to facilitate patient engagement and trust. In this sense, the SOMA’s contribution is potentially as important for the training of medical students and residents as it is for researching patient-physician interactions.
For future study, one strength of the SOMA is its relative ease of use. Raters do not require extensive training, and unlike some other coding systems, a transcript of the medical encounter is not needed. We found that within a few hours of working with the measure, our raters, none of whom is a medical provider, could code in time with the audio recording. In other words, SOMA rating of a consultation only takes as long as its recording.
Future studies should examine if concordance is optimized when the levels of both patient and provider engagement are high. It also seems important to examine the longitudinal development of patient TRUST and ENGAGEMENT to understand their temporal relations to patient-provider CONCORDANCE. With a large sample, researchers could determine whether patient ENGAGEMENT mediates the relation between provider TRUST and CONCORDANCE. In other words, particularly for patients with complex conditions, is concordance stronger when providers intentionally make the consultation process as comfortable as possible in order to activate patient involvement?
Since the present assessment of the SOMA was based on a sample of primary and specialty care consultations with veterans who have MUS, future research could determine whether the measure is equally reliable for studying encounters between patients with other physical health conditions and diverse medical providers, such as physician assistants, dentists or chiropractors. Studies could also determine the degree to which ratings of the three SOMA dimensions – TRUST, ENGAGEMENT and CONCORDANCE – are most predictive of patient compliance with the negotiated treatment recommendations. For example, does high CONCORDANCE predict patient follow through on health promotion behaviors like exercise and diet?
The SOMA is the first observational measure of the working alliance tailored specifically for patient-provider relationships in physical healthcare. In introducing this measure to readers of TCP, we wanted to provide an example of how as counseling psychologists we can leverage our expertise in relational psychotherapeutic processes to enhance the care of medical patients through research, consultation and training. As the SOMA’s development was part of a large, federally-funded study of patient-provider relationships for veterans with MUS, a secondary aim of this article is to encourage counseling psychologists to collaborate with physicians to conduct grant-funded research in areas that speak to our expertise (cf. Burrow-Sánchez, Martin, & Imel, 2016), such as developing and sustaining strong interpersonal relationships.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by #1K2HX001369 and #101HX001678 from the United States (U.S.) Department of Veterans Affairs, Veterans Health Administration, Office of Research and Development, Health Services Research and Development Program. It was also supported by the VA NJ War Related Illness and Injury Study Center. The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the position or policy of the Department of Veterans Affairs or the United States government
Footnotes
Based on the reasoning described earlier, we adapted some ENGAGEMENT and TRUST indicators from the SOFTA-o to the SOMA and created additional indicators to better reflect the alliance in physical medicine.
Contributor Information
Myrna L. Friedlander, University at Albany
Kelsey Kangos, University at Albany.
Kieran Maestro, University at Albany.
Hannah Muetzelfeld, University at Albany.
Scott T. Wright, University at Albany
Nicole Da Silva, University at Albany.
Justin Kimber, University at Albany.
Drew A. Helmer, Veterans Affairs New Jersey Health Care System
Lisa M. McAndrew, Veterans Affairs New Jersey Health Care System, University at Albany
References
- McAndrew LM, Friedlander M, Litke D, Phillips LA, Kimber J, & Helmer DA (2019). Medically unexplained physical symptoms: What they are and why counseling psychologists should care about them. The Counseling Psychologist. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips LA & McAndrew LM, (2019). An empirical evaluation of veterans’ perceived non-concordance with the provider regarding medically unexplained symptoms. The Counseling Psychologist. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson RM, & Funnell MM (2010). Patient Empowerment: Myths and Misconceptions. Patient Education and Counseling, 79, 277–282. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2009.07.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bell JS, Airaksinen MS, Lyles A, Chen TF, & Aslani P (2007). Concordance is not synonymous with compliance or adherence. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 64, 710–711. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2125.2007.02971_1.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bordin ES (1979). The generalizability of the psychoanalytic concept of the working alliance. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research & Practice, 16, 252–260. doi: 10.1037/h0085885 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Brown P, Zavestoski S, McCormick S, Linder M, Mandelbaum J, & Luebke T (2001). A gulf of difference: Disputes over Gulf War-related illnesses. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 42, 235–257. doi: 10.2307/3090213 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burrow-Sánchez JJ, Martin JL, & Imel ZE (2016). Applying for grant funding as a counseling psychologist: From thought to action. The Counseling Psychologist, 44, 479–524. doi: 10.1177/0011000015626272 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Charles C, Gafni A, & Whelan T (1999). Decision-making in the physician–patient encounter: Revisiting the shared treatment decision-making model. Social Science & Medicine, 49, 651–661. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(99)00145-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chew-Graham C, Dowrick C, Wearden A, Richardson V, & Peters S (2010). Making the diagnosis of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome/Myalgic Encephalitis in primary care: A qualitative study. BMC Family Practice, 11, 1–7. doi: 10.1186/1471-2296-11-16 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Del Piccolo L, Putnam SM, Mazzi MA, & Zimmermann C (2004). The biopsychosocial domains and the functions of the medical interview in primary care: Construct validity of the Verona Medical Interview Classification System. Patient Education and Counseling, 53, 47–56. doi: 10.1016/s0738-3991(03)00115-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eide H, Quera V, Graugaard P, & Finset A (2004). Physician–patient dialogue surrounding patients’ expression of concern: Applying sequence analysis to RIAS. Social Science & Medicine, 59, 145–155. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2003.10.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elwyn G, Edwards A, Wensing M, Hood K, Atwell C, & Grol R (2003). Shared decision making: Developing the OPTION scale for measuring patient involvement. BMJ Quality & Safety, 12, 93–99. doi: 10.1136/qhc.12.2.93 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engel CC, Adkins JA, & Cowan DN (2002). Caring for medically unexplained physical symptoms after toxic environmental exposures: Effects of contested causation. Environmental Health Perspectives, 110, 641–647. doi: 10.1289/ehp.02110s4641 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein RM, Franks P, Fiscella K, Shields CG, Meldrum SC, Kravitz RL, &Duberstein PR (2005). Measuring patient-centered communication in patient–physician consultations: Theoretical and practical issues. Social Science and Medicine, 61, 1516–1528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein RM, Shields CG, Meldrum SC, Fiscella K, Carroll J, Carney PA, & Duberstein PR (2006). Physicians’ responses to patients’ medically unexplained symptoms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68, 269–276. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000204652.27246.5b [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flückiger Del Re, Wampold BE., & Horvath AO. (2018). The alliance in adult psychotherapy: A meta-analytic synthesis. Psychotherapy, 55, 326–340. doi: 10.1037/pst0000172 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ford S, Schofield T, & Hope T (2003). What are the ingredients for a successful evidence-based patient choice consultation? A qualitative study. Social Science and Medicine, 56, 589–602. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(02)00056-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedlander ML, Escudero V, Horvath AO, Heatherington L, Cabero A, & Martens MP (2006). System for Observing Family Therapy Alliances: A tool for research and practice. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 214–225. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.53.2.214 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Friedlander ML, Escudero V, Welmers van-der Poll M, & Heatherington L (2018). Meta-analysis of the alliance-outcome relation in couple and family therapy. Psychotherapy, 55, 356–371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuertes JN, Boylan LS., & Fontanella JA. (2009). Behavioral indices in medical care outcome: The working alliance, adherence, and related factors. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 24, 80–85. doi: 10.1007/s11606-008-0841-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuertes JN, Mislowack A, Bennett J, Maury L, Gilbert TC, Fontan G, & Boylan LS (2007). The physician-patient working alliance. Patient Education and Counseling, 66, 29–36. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2006.09.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahn SR (2001). Physical symptoms and physician-experienced difficulty in the physician-patient relationship. Annals of Internal Medicine, 134, 897–904. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-134-9_part_2-200105011-00014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hale ED, Treharne GJ, & Kitas GD (2007). The common-sense model of self-regulation of health and illness: How can we use it to understand and respond to our patients’ needs? Rheumatology, 46, 904–906. doi: 10.1093/rheumatology/kem060 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall AM, Ferreira PH, Maher CG, Latmer J, & Ferreira ML (2010). The influence of the therapist-patient relationship on treatment outcome in physical rehabilitation: A systematic review. Physical Therapy, 90, 1099–1110. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20090245 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horvath AO, & Greenberg LS (1989). Development and validation of the Working Alliance Inventory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36, 223–233. doi: 10.1037//0022-0167.36.2.223 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Iversen A, Chalder T, & Wessely S (2007). Gulf War Illness: Lessons from medically unexplained symptoms. Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 842–854. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.07.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joosten EA, DeFuentes-Merillas L, De Weert G, Sensky T, Van Der Staak C, & de Jong CA (2008). Systematic review of the effects of shared decision-making on patient satisfaction, treatment adherence and health status. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 77, 219–226. doi: 10.1159/000126073 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krupat E, Frankel R, Stein T, & Irish J (2006). The Four Habits Coding Scheme: Validation of an instrument to assess clinicians’ communication behavior. Patient Education and Counseling, 62, 38–45. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2005.04.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maguire P, Faulkner A, Booth K, Elliott C, & Hillier V (1996). Helping cancer patients disclose their concerns. European Journal of Cancer, 32A, 78–81. doi: 10.1016/0959-8049(95)00527-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makoul G, & Clayman ML (2006). An integrative model of shared decision making in medical encounters. Patient Education and Counseling, 60, 301–312. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2005.06.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McAndrew LM, Friedlander ML, Santos S, Phillips LA, & Helmer DA (2018). Concordance of illness perceptions: The key to improving care of medically unexplained symptoms. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 111, 140–142. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2018.05.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McAndrew LM, Martin JL, Friedlander ML, Shaffer K, Breland JY, Slotkin S, & Leventhal H (2018). The common sense of counseling psychology: introducing the Common-Sense Model of self-regulation. Counselling Psychology Quarterly, 34, 497–512. doi: 10.1080/09515070.2017.1336076 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McAndrew LM, Musumeci-Szabo TJ, Mora PA, Vileikyte L, Burns E, Halm EA, Leventhal EA, Leventhal H (2008). Using the common sense model to design interventions for the prevention and management of chronic illness threats: From description to process. British Journal of Health Psychology, 13, 195–204. doi: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McAndrew L, Schneider SH, Burns E, & Leventhal H (2007). Does patient blood glucose monitoring improve diabetes control? The Diabetes Educator, 33, 991–1011. doi: 10.1177/0145721707309807 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mott JM, Stanley MA, Street RL Jr., Grady RH, & Teng EJ (2014). Increasing engagement in evidence-based PTSD treatment through shared decision-making: A pilot study. Military Medicine, 179, 143–149. doi: 10.7205/milmed-d-13-00363 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nordin TA, Hartz AJ, Noyes R, Anderson MC, Rosenbaum ME, James PA, Ely JW, Agarwal N, & Levy BT (2006). Empirically identified goals for the management of unexplained symptoms. Family Medicine, 38, 476–482. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- olde Hartman TC, Borghuis MS, Lucassen PL, van de Laar FA, Speckens AE, & van Weel C (2009). Medically unexplained symptoms, somatisation disorder and hypochondriasis: Course and prognosis. A systematic review. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 66, 363–377. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.09.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Page LA, & Wessely S (2003). Medically unexplained symptoms: Exacerbating factors in the doctor-patient encounter. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 96, 223–227. doi: 10.1258/jrsm.96.5.223 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips LA, Leventhal EA, & Leventhal H (2011). Factors associated with the accuracy of physicians’ predictions of patient adherence. Patient Education and Counseling, 85, 461–467. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2011.03.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reid S, Whooley D, Crayford T, & Hotopf M (2001). Medically unexplained symptoms--GPs’ attitudes towards their cause and management. Family Practice, 18, 519–523. doi: 10.1093/fampra/18.5.519 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ring A, Dowrick C, Humphris G, & Salmon P (2004). Do patients with unexplained physical symptoms pressurise general practitioners for somatic treatment? A qualitative study. British Medical Journal, 328, 1057–1060. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38057.622639.ee [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roter D (1995). Advancing the physician’s contribution to enhancing compliance. Journal of Pharmacoepidemiology, 3, 37–48. doi: 10.1300/j055v03n02_03 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Roter DL, & Larson S (2001). The relationship between residents’ and attending physicians’ communication during primary care visits: An illustrative use of the Roter Interaction Analysis System. Health Communication, 13, 33–48. doi: 10.1207/s15327027hc1301_04 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roter D, & Larson S (2002). The Roter Interaction Analysis System (RIAS): Utility and flexibility for analysis of medical interactions. Patient Education and Counseling, 46, 243–251. doi: 10.1016/s0738-3991(02)00012-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Royal Pharmaceutical Society of Great Britain (1997). From compliance to concordance: Achieving shared goals in medicine taking. London, England: Royal Pharmaceutical Society. [Google Scholar]
- Salmon P (2007). Conflict, collusion or collaboration in consultations about medically unexplained symptoms: The need for a curriculum of medical explanation. Patient Education and Counseling, 67, 246–54. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2007.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salmon P, Ring A, Dowrick CF, & Humphris GM (2005). What do general practice patients want when they present medically unexplained symptoms, and why do their doctors feel pressurized? Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 59, 255–260. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.03.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shannon SE, Mitchell PH, & Cain KC (2002). Patients, nurses, and physicians have differing views of quality of critical care. Journal of Nursing Scholarship, 34, 173–179. doi: 10.1111/j.1547-5069.2002.00173.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith RC, Lyles JS, Gardiner JC, Sirbu C, Hodges A, Collins C, . . . Goddeeris J (2006). Primary care clinicians treat patients with medically unexplained symptoms: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 21, 671–677. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2006.00460.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SG, Pandit A, Rush SR, Wolf MS, & Simon CJ (2016). The role of patient activation in preferences for shared decision making: Results from a national survey of US adults. Journal of Health Communication, 21, 67–75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Street RLJ, Gordon HS, Ward MM, Krupat E, & Kravitz RL (2005). Patient participation in medical consultations: Why some patients are more involved than others. Medical Care, 43, 960–969. doi: 10.1097/01.mlr.0000178172.40344.70 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Street RL, O’Malley KJ, Cooper LA, & Haidet P (2008). Understanding concordance in patient-physician relationships: personal and ethnic dimensions of shared identity. The Annals of Family Medicine, 6, 198–205. doi: 10.1370/afm.821 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tracey TJ, & Kokotovic AM (1989). Factor structure of the Working Alliance Inventory. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1, 207–210. [Google Scholar]
- Trevino KM, Maciejewski PK, Epstein AS, & Prigerson HG (2015). The lasting impact of the therapeutic alliance: Patient-oncology alliance as a predictor of caregiver bereavement adjustment. Cancer, 121, 3534–3542 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Dessel N, den Boeft M, van der Wouden JC, Kleinstaeuber M, Leone SS, Terluin B, . . . van Marwijk H. (2014). Non‐pharmacological interventions for somatoform disorders and medically unexplained physical symptoms (MUPS) in adults. Cochrane Database of Systemic Reviews, 1–163. doi: 10.1002/14651858.cd011142.pub2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Ravenzwaaij J, olde Hartman TS, van Ravesteijn H, Everleigh R,van Rijswijk E, & Lucassen PLBJ. (2010). Explanatory models of medically unexplained symptoms: A qualitative analysis of the literature. Mental Health in Family Medicine, 7, 223–231. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wileman L, May C, & Chew-Graham CA (2002). Medically unexplained symptoms and the problem of power in the primary care consultation: A qualitative study. Family Practice, 19, 178–182. doi: 10.1093/fampra/19.2.178 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zavestoski S, Brown P, McCormick S, Mayer B, D’Ottavi M, & Lucove JC (2004). Patient activism and the struggle for diagnosis: Gulf War illnesses and other medically unexplained physical symptoms in the U.S. Social Science and Medicine, 58, 161–175. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(03)00157-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.