Abstract
The original provocative formulation of the ‘geometric module’ hypothesis was based on a working-memory task in rats which suggested that spontaneous reorientation behavior is based solely on the environmental geometry and is impervious to featural cues. Here, we retested that claim by returning to a spontaneous navigation task with rats and domestic chicks, using a single prominent featural cue (a striped wall) within a rectangular arena. Experiments 1 and 2 tested the influence of geometry and features separately. In Experiment 1, we found that both rats and chicks used environmental geometry to compute locations in a plain rectangular arena. In Experiment 2, while chicks failed to spontaneously use a striped wall in a square arena, rats showed a modest influence of the featural cue as a local marker to the goal. The critical third experiment tested the striped wall inside the rectangular arena. We found that although chicks solely relied on geometry, rats navigated based on both environmental geometry and the featural cue. While our findings with rats are contrary to classic claims of an impervious geometric module, they are consistent with the hypothesis that navigation by boundaries and features may involve distinct underlying cognitive computations. We conclude by discussing the similarities and differences in feature-use across tasks and species.
Subject terms: Animal behaviour, Psychology, Spatial memory
Introduction
Thirty-five years ago, a remarkable set of experiments showed that disoriented rats relied on the shape of the test arena to return to a food source, while ignoring landmarks or other “featural” cues such as patterned panels and distinctive odors1,2. These results motivated the claim that reorientation is based exclusively on environmental geometry and is independent from the control of other featural cues3. Because the possibility of a purely geometric module provided new and important insights into the cognitive processes underlying spatial behavior, many studies on reorientation have followed in a wide variety of species, including humans4–6.
Across this large literature, however, results have been inconsistent and interpretations keenly contested. One fundamental reason for the debate is that there are no clear standards for what should be taken as evidence for or against modularity. In earlier works2,7 the failure to use featural cues in conjunction with geometry to guide spatial behavior was presented as the strongest demonstration of encapsulation8. Other experiments showed that although animals and human children can learn to use features to confine their searches to the goal location, their success often relied only on those cues that were placed directly near the goal; that is, when local features at the goal were removed, animals no longer showed a preference for the goal based on spatial relationships with respect to the remaining featural cues (e.g., rats2; chicks9; children10). Arguments for or against modularity have often been made based on interactions (or lack thereof) between geometry and features11. In nonhuman animals, such studies have mainly adhered to reference-memory paradigms, often involving extensive training and reinforcement of choice behavior at a single goal location4. A wide range of studies have demonstrated the associative effects of feature learning on geometry-based spatial mapping in these tasks; overshadowing12,13, blocking14,15, learned attentional changes16,17, within-compound associations between geometry and features18–20, conditioned inhibition21 and potentiation of geometry learning by featural cues15,21–24 have all shown that non-geometric features can interact with the progression of geometry learning. However, successful use of featural cues in these paradigms has been argued to reflect the engagement of associative processes that do not recruit the specialized cognitive computations engaged in the use of geometric cues following disorientation. That is, reference memory tasks such as those discussed above recruit general learning systems parallel to the specialized computations employed in the use of boundary geometry. Reference memory tasks, therefore, may mask detection of the modular nature of boundary-based spatial mapping by recruitment of associative processes. For such reasons, some researchers emphasize the importance of using a working memory task in observing distinctions between geometry and feature use25,26. It is important to note, however, that even in working memory or spontaneous tasks, researchers have found varying degrees of feature use depending on the task (e.g., aversive vs. appetitive tasks in rats27; verbal cuing in children28) or environment (e.g., large vs. small environments29,30, circular/octagonal environments10,31). Because alternative explanations for the observation of interactions between geometry and features are available, it seems that evidence for or against independent computation of geometric and featural cues rests not on whether a cue influences behavior, but on how it influences behavior25. Such differences in function may additionally lead to hypotheses about both the cognitive computations and neural representations underlying spatial navigation.
A recent study with fish provides a clear demonstration of the importance of both the task and the available environmental cues in disoriented spatial behavior32,33. The two types of tasks (working memory and reference memory) were tested across three conditions: geometry-only, features-only, and a combination of geometry and features. As in previous studies, environmental geometry immediately guided navigation in both tasks, and features were reliably learned in the reference memory task (although they learned local features significantly faster than the distal ones). In contrast, in the working memory type task, visual landmarks acquired perceptive salience and attracted the fish but without serving as a spatial landmark when they were located far from the target location. Interestingly, however, when provided simultaneously with environmental geometry, the featural cues – whether those cues were distinctive corner panels or a single uniquely-colored wall33 - led the fish to limit their choices to the correct corner significantly more often, even in the absence of reinforced training.
Similarly, behavioral studies with mice have independently tested the use of geometry and landmarks in both a working memory task and a reference memory task34. While mice reoriented by geometry from the very start in both tasks, a featural cue (i.e. striped wall) was successfully used only as a local marker (but without any sense of left versus right) in the working-memory task. With reinforcement in the reference-memory task, however, mice became increasingly accurate in identifying the one target corner. Interestingly, genetically modified mouse models of neurological disorders showed selective learning of features and geometry, depending on their hippocampal dysfunction or attentional deficit35. In those studies, however, there was no simultaneous testing of geometry with a featural cue, making it unclear whether the presence of a geometric structure enhances feature-use in rodents the way it did in fish, particularly in the spontaneous “working-memory” task.
The present study tests the generalizability of such findings across different species of animals through a working-memory test of rats and domestic chicks, presenting geometry and features in isolation (Experiments 1 and 2) and in conjunction (Experiment 3). The task involved observing disoriented animals’ corner preferences (in the absence of reward) after having previously found food in one corner of an arena. Experiment 1 examined the use of rectangular environmental geometry using a uniformly black arena; Experiment 2 examined feature-use in a square arena with a single striped wall; Experiment 3 tested the simultaneous use of geometry and features using a rectangular arena with a single striped wall. Given the potential effects of arena size, we kept our rectangular arena’s dimensions the same as the original Cheng study2. However, instead of corner panels, we chose to implement a single striped wall, in accord with the above studies on mice and based on single landmark control over head direction cell responses36,37, using a spatial frequency based on considerations of rodent visual acuity38,39, which is lower than that of chicks40. Different subjects were used for each experiment. These arenas were set in well-controlled testing rooms designed to offer no cues to orientation other than those in the arenas themselves.
Results
Experiment 1: Geometry
Experiment 1 tested the use of environmental geometry using a uniformly black-colored rectangular arena (see Fig. 1a). Due to rotational symmetry, searches to both the correct corner and the diametrically opposite corner were recorded as being geometrically correct. Importantly, if the subject is successfully disoriented, and no uncontrolled odor cues mark the correct location, the correct and geometrically identical corners should not be distinguishable. Eleven rats (mean = 5.7 trials) and ten chicks (mean = 4.4 trials) were observed. Results from one other chick were omitted due to signs of distress.
Rats
The proportion of time spent at the geometrically correct corners was significantly greater than a chance level of 0.5 (t(10) = 4.59, p = 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.38, see Fig. 1b). Importantly, there was no sign of preference for the correct corner over its geometric equivalent (t < 1, p > 0.25, Cohen’s d = 0.23), indicating an effective disorientation procedure with no lingering reward-odor cues in our working-memory paradigm.
Chicks
The proportion of time spent at the geometrically correct corners was significantly greater than a chance level of 0.5 (t(9) = 2.64, p = 0.03, Cohen’s d = 0.95, Fig. 1). Again, importantly, there was no preference for the correct corner over its geometric equivalent (t(9) = 1.34, p = 0.21, Cohen’s d = 0.19).
Both rats and chicks searched in accord with boundary geometry. This result is consistent with past findings in rats2 and in chicks41. The equal preference between the correct corner and the geometrically equivalent corner indicates an effective disorientation procedure and provides us with an internal control that ensures the absence of cues that might make the correct corner uniquely identifiable.
Experiment 2: Feature
Experiment 2 tested reorientation by a feature in a square arena with a single striped wall (see Fig. 2a). If animals distinguished all four corners with respect to the feature (i.e., relative directions or “sense”), there should be a preference for the correct corner over all others. A more limited use of the feature would be if animals used the striped wall as a direct local feature, without using it to compute sense relations. In this case, for instance, we should detect a preference for the two striped corners when the goal was at a striped corner. Nine rats (mean = 5 trials) and eight chicks (mean = 5 trials) were observed.
Rats
The proportion of time spent at the correct corner was not significantly different from a chance value of 0.25 (t(8) = 1.64, p = 0.14, Cohen’s d = 0.55). This indicates a failure to compute ‘sense’ (i.e., left/right-ness) from the featural cue. Nevertheless, there was a clear statistical preference for featurally correct over featurally incorrect corners (t(9) = 3.27, p = 0.01, Cohen’s d = 1.09, see Fig. 2b). Interestingly, however, further inspection showed that this effect was driven by trials in which the goal was near the striped wall: when the goal was at a striped corner, rats spent significantly more time at the two striped corners (72%: t(8) = 4.21, p = 0.003, Cohen’s d = 1.40) but when the goal was at an all-black corner, rats did not exhibit a preference for the black corners (46%: t < 1, p > 0.25, Cohen’s d = 0.16). In both cases, rats did not distinguish the correct corner from the symmetric corner (t’s < 1, p’s> 0.25), indicating a complete failure to use the striped wall to compute directional relationship between locations. In summary, Experiment 2’s results indicated a clear, if relatively modest, influence of the featural cue; the rats could not use it to extract ‘sense’ but leveraged a ‘goal proximity’ benefit from it, consistent with use of the feature as a beacon.
Chicks
The proportion of time spent at the correct corner was not statistically different from chance of 0.25 (t < 1, p > 0.25, Cohen’s d = 0.27); whether the corner was striped or black did not make a significant difference in performance (t(7) = 1.03, p = 0.34, Cohen’s d = 0.39, Fig. 2). Moreover, chicks did not respond to the striped feature as a cue to distinguish between the two striped corners and the two black corners, whether the goal was at a striped or all-black corner (all t’s <1, p’s> 0.25). Although this null effect should not be over-interpreted, the fact that chicks were not significantly influence by the featural cue cannot be attributed to an inability to perceive it, given their clear success in learning to associate it with a goal when provided with reinforcement (see Exp. 4 in Supplementary Materials); in the free-exploration probe trial, chicks (n = 6) spent 91.9% of their time on the side of the arena with the stripes (t(5) = 8.06, p < 0.001).
Experiment 3: Geometry and Feature
Experiment 1 demonstrated that both chicks and rats spontaneously used boundary geometry to compute spatial locations. Experiment 2 showed that although rats did not compute relative positions with respect to the feature, they used it as a local feature to guide search. In contrast, the chicks showed no sign of feature-use with respect to the striped wall and explored the corners at random. If both geometry and features are present simultaneously, will one of those cues dominate over the other? Or, will both of them influence behavior? Experiment 3 tested the combined use of environmental geometry and a feature when both were present, in a rectangular arena with one striped wall (see Fig. 3a). Nine rats (mean = 5.7 trials) and eight chicks (mean = 5.8 trials) were observed.
Rats
As in Experiment 1, the combined proportion of time spent at the two geometrically correct corners was significantly higher than a chance level of 0.5, (t(8) = 3.05, p = 0.02, Cohen’s d = 1.02, see Fig. 3b). However here, unlike Experiment 1, rats tended to prefer the correct corner over the geometrically identical diagonal corner (t(8) = 2.27, p = 0.05, Cohen’s d = 0.76). There was no significant difference in accuracy between goals at the striped and all-black corners (t(8) = 1.39, p = 0.20). In summary, contrary to some interpretations of the original geometric module hypothesis, search time was immediately guided by the use of both geometric and featural information. Interestingly, use of both sets of information occurred at the level of individual rats: e.g., the majority of the nine rats showed a pattern whereby search at the correct corner was at least 12% higher than at the rotationally equivalent corner and at least 9% higher than at the featurally similar corner (fifth-ranked rat scored: 36% at correct corner, 24% at rotationally equivalent corner, 27% at featurally similar corner, 13% at error corner). In fact, the group average of 17% at the error corner was significantly lower than chance (t(8) = −3.11, p = 0.015)
Chicks
Similarly to the rats, the combined proportion of time spent at the correct corner and its geometric equivalent was significantly higher than a chance level of 0.5 (t(7) = 3.19, p = 0.02, Cohen’s d = 1.13, Fig. 3); but, in contrast to the rats, there was no discrimination between the correct corner and its geometric equivalent (t < 1, p > 0.25). The proportion of time spent at the correct corner was not significantly different from chance (t < 1, p > 0.25, Cohen’s d = 0.27)).
In summary, rats were able to use both environmental geometry and a feature to guide search at the correct goal, while chicks reoriented only on the basis of environmental geometry, despite their tendency to rely on visual features when trained to do so9 (see Exp. 4, Supplementary Materials).
Discussion
The present experiments shed new light on the decades-old debate over the geometric module by accomplishing the following: First, Experiment 1 replicated the main finding that environmental geometry guides spatial navigation in both mammals and birds in the absence of path integration (or positional tracking). Rats and chicks spontaneously used geometry in a working-memory task with varied goal locations and did not need repeated training at one rewarded location to compute spatial relationships among the arena boundaries. This is consistent with the wealth of existing studies demonstrating that spatial mapping relies on a neurocognitive representation of boundary geometry25,26,42–45.
Second, Experiments 2 and 3 show, for the first time, that rats can use a featural cue in spatial reorientation. Although chicks did not seem to do so in the present experiment, rats successfully leveraged a featural cue to guide their search in a working-memory task, which, importantly like Cheng’s, tapped ‘spontaneous’ behavior without requiring extensive training. In Experiment 3, they jointly (in one trial) used two sets of information to guide their behavior, one based on environmental geometry, the other based on the featural cue.
These findings clearly weaken the empirical basis underlying a ‘strong’ version of modularity that is deterministic at the level of behavior (i.e. that the output of a geometric module assumed control over behavior without the influence of other processes such as landmark-use). In the critical Experiment 3, the effect size of greater search in the correct than rotationally equivalent corner is Cohen’s d = 0.76, which is far from negligible. In other words, disoriented animals are not limited only to environmental geometry in guiding initial search under disorientation. Overall, comparison of effect sizes in our findings is consistent with a greater influence of environmental geometry over features in reorientation, but it is clear that rats can also take features into consideration when performing such tasks. Nevertheless, the way in which the feature was used in this working-memory task was limited to a local-marker of the goal (apparent in their preference of locations based on the presence of the striped cue), unlike the relative spatial relationships that were computed with respect to the environmental geometry. This is in line with past findings of disoriented spatial behavior in mice, fish, as well as human children, using features and geometry in isolation10,25,32–35.
The results taken together describe an underlying spatial representation of environmental geometry which, in the absence of repeated reinforcement, operates alongside a separate feature-detector with an associative bias (direct-marking)25,33,42. This suggests that while the computations involved in the use of geometry and features are different, the output of a single behavioral choice (i.e., in Exp. 3) may involve an adaptive weighting of cues according to properties such as salience and experienced validity4,46. However, the patterns of behavior we observed here (and in the studies mentioned above) directly contradict a global image-matching strategy38, which would allow animals to distinguish between the two striped corners.
What could explain the rats’ use of the feature as a local cue, given the past findings of failure and the failure on the part of the chicks? First, it may be important to point out that there were several differences between the original methods from Cheng’s2 study and ours. First, we used a different strain of rats (Lister Hooded) from those tested in Cheng’s study (Sprague Dawley), although both studies used male rats. Lister Hooded rats have better vision than Sprague Dawleys, but not all the featural cues in Cheng’s study were visual. More importantly, when designing our apparatus, we chose to keep the same size apparatus as Cheng’s but to simplify the featural cue, based upon knowledge of cues used to orient spatial cells36,37. Instead of having multiple visual cues and odors, which might introduce issues related to multiple cue discrimination and recognition, we chose a simple, prominent striped pattern with clearly visible contrast edges, at a spatial frequency that rats were sure to perceive, perhaps even more easily than a uniformly white wall38,40. We also chose to limit our target locations to corner feeders in an empty arena (that could be cleaned to get rid of odor cues), rather than having the targets be anywhere in a sandbox and allowing rats to dig for the reward. The clear visibility of the feature within our well-lit room and its salience against an otherwise black arena, along with the clear distinction between possible choices of feeders near and far from the feature, may have prompted rats in our study to use the featural cue.
Along the same logic, it is possible that our across-species standardization of the striped cue may have favored rats over chicks, given chicks’ innate preference for smaller visual features47. We note, however, that the chicks used that very same cue quite proficiently in a reinforced reference-memory task (Exp. 4, Supplementary materials). Another possibility is that, given the difference in their body size, by using the same-sized apparatus for both chicks and rats we may have inadvertently tested the chicks in comparatively larger environments. However, according to previous studies, larger environments favor feature-use30,31, which makes this an unlikely explanation for the species differences in this task. One other possibility is that our task protocol in having the rats sample the target location twice (see Methods) provided them with a better representation of the environment. However, even if that were somehow true, it still would not be able to explain the fact that the chicks and rats performed so similarly in their use of boundary geometry (61% vs. 62% geometrically correct preference in Exp. 1), yet so differently in their use of the feature (46% vs. 68% featurally correct preference in Exp. 2).
Study after study, the variability in performance across tasks and species is with respect to feature-use (or the competition between features pitted against geometry), not about the use of geometry itself. Perhaps the crucial point is that variation across species, whether it is due to differences in their perceptual system or other ecological factors4, affects the representation of featural landmarks to a greater extent than the representation of environmental structure.
For the past few decades, the geometric module hypothesis has provided a powerful theoretical framework for understanding the central role of environmental boundaries in navigation and spatial mapping. During that time, spatial navigation research has witnessed tremendous progress and a wealth of scientific knowledge that is likely unparalleled in any other area of cognition research, and the study of environmental boundaries and landmarks in spatial representation and behavior has made a significant contribution to that end. Behavioral evidence in a wide range of animal species suggests that computations of environmental geometry involve representations of distance and directions with respect to three-dimensional boundary layouts48,49. Evidence from human neuroimaging studies suggests that learning locations with respect to boundaries and landmarks preferentially recruits the hippocampus and dorsal striatum, respectively50; a similar distinction has been found in the avian51 and rodent brains52–54. At the level of single neurons, geometry-based navigation may be supported by boundary-coding neurons such as boundary vector cells in the hippocampal formation55–57. Prior to their discovery42, boundary vector cells were hypothesized as a major input to hippocampal place cells, which in turn form allocentric “maps” of the surrounding environment in a manner sensitive to environmental geometry58–60. Direct visualization of neurons responding to a single-shot experience of environmental boundary transformations in young domestic chicks61 and intracranial recordings of boundary-specific increases in theta power from human epilepsy patients performing a computer-based navigation task62 suggest that such neural representation of boundaries may be a commonly shared underlying neural correlate of boundary-based navigation. Arguably, a vital challenge now is to combine investigation of these and other types of hippocampal spatial neurons36 with reorientation behavior43,44 across various species.
Thirty-five years after Ken Cheng’s2 formulation of the geometric module, we find ourselves in a new era of research on spatial cognition. We envisage thirty more fruitful years of research to successfully integrate behavioral and neural evidence on the representations of boundaries and features, to provide deeper insight as to how and at what level such representations interact, and to better characterize the principles that are shared and distinct across vertebrate species that ultimately give rise to behavior.
Methods
Subjects
Rats
Twenty-nine adult male Lister Hooded rats (Rattus norvegicus, Harlan Olac, Bicester, England) were housed in groups of five with continuous access to water. They were held on a 12-hr light/dark cycle; testing occurred during the light phase. Rats were on a restricted diet of 15 g of food per animal, beginning two days prior to testing. No animals dropped below 95% of free-feeding weight.
Chicks
Twenty-six newly-hatched male chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus, Agricola Berica, Vicenza, Italy) were singly housed, with a 13-hr/11-hr light/dark cycle; testing occurred during the light phase. Water and food were supplied ad libitum.
Apparatus
Rats
The arena was raised 86 cm above the ground, centered within a 2.5 m diameter circle of gray curtains to block extra-maze cues. Suspended 64 cm above the arena was a 1m-wide circular array of eight 45 W spotlights. At the center of the lights was a video camera. Arenas (rectangular: 120 ×60 cm; square: 85 ×85 cm; height 50 cm) were constructed from black medium-density fiberboard. The features were 0.5 cm thick polyurethane panels, in alternating black and white stripes (thickness 10 cm). In each corner of the arena was a white ceramic cylindrical feeder (diameter 8 cm, height 4.2 cm). Chocolate chips were used as a reward. During disorientation, animals were placed inside a light-tight box.
Chicks
The testing arenas were identical to those used for the rats. The feeders were metallic and rectangular (7 ×9 ×5 cm). The arena was placed 68 cm above the floor, and was lit by a single 35 W bulb suspended 90 cm above the arena. The arena was placed at the center of a 2 m diameter circle of thick vinyl curtains. Mealworms (Tenebrio molitor) were used as reward, and the disorientation chamber was cylindrical (diameter 10 cm, height 13 cm) and lifted from above to release the chick.
Design
Testing lasted two days, three trials per day, with an inter-trial interval of approximately 45 minutes. Each corner of each arena served as a goal at least once, and goals for the remaining two trials were chosen pseudo-randomly to balance the total number of times each corner served as the goal. No goals were repeated on consecutive trials, in order to minimize learning across trials (t < 1.1, p > 0.25). The animals’ release points were chosen randomly from the four arena walls. For the unrewarded probe phase of each trial, animals were allowed to explore the arena for 60 seconds. The time spent within 5 cm of each of the feeders was recorded.
Procedures
Rats
One day before testing, rats were provided with some chocolate chips in their cages. Before the first trial of each day, animals received a familiarization trial during which they were given three minutes to explore the arena and eat one chocolate chip placed at its center. For each test trial, chocolate chips were added to one feeder. The animal was allowed to explore until it had eaten a piece of chocolate, at which point it was removed for 15 seconds, before being placed back in to the arena from the same starting point. The animal was allowed to explore the arena until it had eaten a second piece of chocolate. This was to discourage the use of an alternation strategy, documented in rats as a method of foraging63. The animal was then disorientated for 30 seconds by placing it in a dark, covered box and rotating the box. Disorientation involved clockwise and then anticlockwise rotations (at least 720° in each direction). During this time, the feeder containing chocolate was removed from the arena and replaced with an identical, but empty, feeder. The arena was cleaned with 15% ethanol and rotated 90° clockwise to counteract the use of possible uncontrolled extra-maze cues. The animal was placed back into the arena from a randomly selected wall and allowed to search for 60 seconds. Trials in which the rat was unresponsive (no feeder approaches) were omitted; if a rat was unresponsive for more than three of the six trials, data from that rat were omitted entirely64.
Chicks
After being moved from the incubator to its home cage, each chick was given 2–3 mealworms. For the next two days, chicks were individually taken to the testing room and placed in a plain square arena (devoid of informative landmarks or geometry) with all black walls and identical black feeders at the corners (for familiarization). An object, identical to the imprinting object, was fixed to the arena floor. On the first day, chicks ate mealworms from a feeder at the center of the arena, and then in a corner. On the second day, chicks found worms in one corner, before being disoriented. Chicks were then released from the center of a randomly chosen wall and given 60 seconds to search for mealworms.
Test trials were administered for two days following familiarization. Before the first trial of each day, the chick was given a 60-second familiarization period inside the testing arena with a feeder containing mealworms at the center. For the test trials, the chick was released from the center of a randomly chosen wall. It was allowed to explore until it had eaten a mealworm, removed from the arena, and disorientated by rotating clockwise and anticlockwise for 30 seconds. During this time, the feeder containing mealworms was removed from the arena and replaced with an identical, but empty, feeder. The arena was wiped and rotated 90° clockwise. The chick was placed back into the arena from the center of a randomly selected wall and allowed to search for 60 seconds. Trials in which the chick was unresponsive (no feeder approaches) or distressed (made two attempts to jump out of the arena) were omitted. If a chick was unresponsive or distressed for more than three of the six trials, then data from that chick were omitted entirely.
All husbandry and experimental procedures complied with European Legislation for the Protection of Animals used for Scientific Purposes (Directive 2010/63/EU). Experiments with rats were carried out in the Psychology Department of Durham University, in accordance with the U.K. Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act of 1986; all experiments were approved by the university internal review board on animal testing. Experiments with chicks were carried out in the Animal Cognition and Neuroscience Laboratory of the Center for Mind/Brain Sciences at the University of Trento and were previously authorized by the University of Trento’s Ethics Committee for the Experiments on Living Organisms, and by the Italian Ministry of Health (auth. num. 201/2013-B). All experiments were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Caterina Trainito, Jlenia Marisa, Silvia Turri, and Lisa Freise for their help conducting and coding these experiments. We also would like to acknowledge Rachel Bhushan for her help in data collection (methods included in her unpublished graduate thesis, University of Trento). This work was funded by research grants to SAL from KAIST (G04170038), to GV from the Provincia Autonoma di Trento and the Fondazione Cassa di Risparmio di Trento e Rovereto, and to CL and AM from the BBSRC (BB/G01342X/2, BB/M008975/1, BB/F013094/1).
Author contributions
S.A.L., J.M.A., V.A.S., G.V. A.M. and C.L. conceived and designed the study; S.A.L. and J.M.A. conducted the experiment; S.A.L. and J.M.A. analyzed the data; S.A.L., J.M.A., C.L., A.M. drafted the manuscript and S.A.L., J.M.A., V.A.S., G.V., A.M. and C.L. contributed to further revisions.
Data availability
The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-020-64366-w.
References
- 1.Cheng, K. & Gallistel, C. R. Testing the geometric power of an animal’s spatial representation. In “Animal cognition: Proceedings of the Harry Frank Guggenheim conference” (Edited by H. L. Roitblat, T. G. Bever, & H. S. Terrace). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum (1984).
- 2.Cheng K. A purely geometric module in the rat’s spatial representation. Cognition. 1986;23:149–178. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(86)90041-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Gallistel, C. R. The Organization of Learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (1990).
- 4.Cheng K, Huttenlocher J, Newcombe NS. 25 years of research on the use of geometry in spatial reorientation: a current theoretical perspective. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2013;20:1033–1054. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0416-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Vallortigara, G. Animals as natural geometers. In “Cognitive Biology: Evolutionary and Developmental Perspectives on Mind, Brain and Behavior” (Edited by L. Tommasi, L. Nadel & M. Peterson). MIT Press, Cambridge, MA (2009).
- 6.Tommasi L, Chiandetti C, Pecchia T, Sovrano VA, Vallortigara G. From natural geometry to spatial cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2012;36:799–824. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Hermer L, Spelke ES. A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children. Nature. 1994;370:57–59. doi: 10.1038/370057a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Fodor, J. A. The modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1983).
- 9.Vallortigara G, Zanforlin M, Pasti G. Geometric modules in animals’ spatial representations: a test with chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus) J. Comp. Psychol. 1990;104:248–254. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.104.3.248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Lee SA, Shusterman A, Spelke ES. Reorientation and landmark-guided search by young children: evidence for two systems. Psychol. Sci. 2006;17(7):577–82. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01747.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Pearce JM. The 36th Sir Frederick Bartlett Lecture: An associative analysis of spatial learning. Quart. J. Exp. Psychol. 2009;62:1665–1684. doi: 10.1080/17470210902805589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Gray ER, Bloomfield LL, Ferrey A, Spetch ML, Sturdy CB. Spatial encoding in mountain chickadees: features overshadow geometry. Biol. Lett. 2005;1:314–317. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2005.0347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Kosaki Y, Austen JM, McGregor A. Overshadowing of geometry learning by discrete landmarks in the water maze: Effects of relative salience and relative validity of competing cues. J. Exp. Psychol.: An. Behav. Proc. 2013;39:126–139. doi: 10.1037/a0031199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Horne MR, Pearce JM. A landmark blocks searching for a hidden platform in an environment with a distinctive shape after extended pretraining. Learn. Behav. 2009;37:167–178. doi: 10.3758/LB.37.2.167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Pearce JM, Graham M, Good MA, Jones PM, McGregor A. Potentiation, overshadowing, and blocking of spatial learning based on-the shape of the environment. J. Exp. Psychol.: An. Behav. Proc. 2006;32:201–214. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.32.3.201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Buckley MG, Smith AD, Haselgrove M. Shape shifting: Local landmarks interfere with navigation by, and recognition of, global shape. J. Exp. Psychol: Learning, Memory and Cogn. 2014;40:492–510. doi: 10.1037/a0034901. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Cuell SF, Good MA, Dopson JC, Pearce JM, Horne MR. Changes in attention to relevant and irrelevant stimuli during spatial learning. J. Exp. Psychol.: An. Behav. Proc. 2012;38:244–254. doi: 10.1037/a0028491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Austen JM, McGregor A. Revaluation of geometric cues reduces landmark discrimination via within-compound associations. Learn. Behav. 2014;42(4):330–336. doi: 10.3758/s13420-014-0150-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Horne MR, Pearce JM. Between-cue associations influence searching for a hidden goal in an environment with a distinctive shape. J. Exp. Psych.: An. Behav. Proc. 2009;35:99–107. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.35.1.99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Rhodes SEV, Creighton G, Killcross AS, Good M, Honey RC. Integration of geometric with luminance information in the rat: Evidence from within-compound associations. J. Exp. Psychol.: An. Behav. Proc. 2009;35:92–98. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.35.1.92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Horne MR, Pearce JM. Potentiation and overshadowing between landmarks and environmental geometric cues. Learn. Behav. 2011;39(4):371–82. doi: 10.3758/s13420-011-0032-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Austen JM, Kosaki Y, McGregor A. Within-compound associations explain potentiation and failure to overshadow learning based on geometry by discrete landmarks. J. Exp. Psychol.: An. Behav. Process. 2013;39:259–272. doi: 10.1037/a0032525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Cole MR, Gibson L, Pollack A, Yates L. Potentiation and overshadowing of shape by wall color in a kite-shaped maze using rats in a foraging task. Learning and Motivation. 2011;42:99–112. doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2010.11.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kelly DM. Features enhance the encoding of geometry. An. Cogn. 2010;13:453–462. doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0296-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Lee SA, Spelke ES. Two systems of spatial representation underlying navigation. Exp. Brain Res. 2010;206:179–188. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2349-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Gallistel CR, Matzel LD. The neuroscience of learning: Beyond the Hebbian synapse. Annual Rev. Psychol. 2013;64:169–200. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Golob. EJ, Taube JS. Differences between appetitive and aversive reinforcement on reorientation in a spatial working memory task. Behav. Brain Res. 2002;136:309–316. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00184-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Shusterman A, Lee SA, Spelke ES. Cognitive effects of language on human navigation. Cognition. 2011;120:186–201. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Learmonth AE, Newcombe NS, Sheridan N, Jones M. Why size counts: Children’s spatial reorientation in large and small enclosures. Dev. Sci. 2008;11:414–426. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00686.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Sovrano VA, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G. Animals’ use of landmarks and metric information to reorient: Effects of the size of the experimental space. Cognition. 2005;97:121–133. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Newcombe NS, Ratliff KR, Shallcross WL, Twyman AD. Young children’s use of features to reorient is more than just associative: further evidence against a modular view of spatial processing. Dev. Sci. 2008;13:213–220. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00877.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Lee SA, Vallortigara G, Ruga V, Sovrano VA. Independent uses of geometry and landmark in a spontaneous reorientation task: A study of two species of fish. An. Cogn. 2012;15:861–870. doi: 10.1007/s10071-012-0512-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Sovrano VA, Baratti G, Lee SA. The role of learning and environmental geometry in landmark-based spatial reorientation of fish (Xenotoca eiseni) PLOS ONE. 2020;15(3):e0229608. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0229608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Lee SA, Tucci V, Sovrano VA, Vallortigara G. Working-memory and reference-memory tests of spatial navigation in mice (Mus musculus) J. Comp. Psychol. 2015;129:189–197. doi: 10.1037/a0039129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Lee SA, Tucci V, Vallortigara G. Spatial impairment and memory in genetic disorders: Insights from mouse models. Brain Sci. 2017;7(2):17. doi: 10.3390/brainsci7020017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hartley T, Lever C, Burgess N, O’Keefe J. Space in the brain: how the hippocampal formation supports spatial cognition. Phil. Trans. Royal Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 2014;369:20120510. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0510. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Taube J. The head direction signal: origins and sensory-motor integration. Annual Rev. Neurosci. 2007;30:181–207. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Sheynikhovich D, Chavarriaga R, Strösslin T, Arleo A, Gerstner W. Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? Insights from a rodent navigation model. Psychol. Rev. 2009;116:540–566. doi: 10.1037/a0016170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Girman, S. V., Sauvé, Y., Lund, R. D. Receptive field properties of single neurons in rat primary visual cortex. J. Neurophysiol.82, 301–311 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 40.Over, R. & Moore, D. Spatial acuity of the chicken. Brain Research211(2), 424–426 (1981). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 41.Lee SA, Spelke ES, Vallortigara G. Chicks, like children, spontaneously reorient by three-dimensional environmental geometry, not by image matching. Biol. Lett. 2012;8:492–494. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2012.0067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Lee SA. A boundary-based view of spatial cognition: A synthesis. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2017;16:58–65. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Barry C, et al. The boundary vector cell model of place cell firing and spatial memory. Rev. Neurosci. 2006;17:71–97. doi: 10.1515/REVNEURO.2006.17.1-2.71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Keinath AT, Julian JB, Epstein RA, Muzzio IA. Environmental geometry aligns the hippocampal map during spatial reorientation. Curr. Biol. 2017;27:309–317. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.11.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Poulter SL, Hartley T, Lever C. The neurobiology of navigation. Curr. Biol. 2018;28:1023–1042. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.05.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Xu Y, Regier T, Newcombe NS. An adaptive cue combination model of human spatial reorientation. Cognition. 2017;163:56–66. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.02.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Fantz, R. L. Form preferences in newly hatched chicks. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology50(5), 422–430 (1957). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 48.Lee SA, Sovrano VA, Spelke ES. Navigation as a source of geometric knowledge: Young children’s use of length, angle, distance, and direction in a reorientation task. Cognition. 2012;123:144–161. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.12.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Lee SA, Vallortigara G, Flore M, Spelke ES, Sovrano VA. Navigation by environmental geometry: the use of zebrafish as a model. J. Exp. Biol. 2013;216:3693–3699. doi: 10.1242/jeb.088625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Doeller CF, King JA, Burgess N. Parallel striatal and hippocampal systems for landmarks and boundaries in spatial memory. Proceed. National Academy Sci. 2008;105:5915–5920. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0801489105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Mayer U, Pecchia T, Bingman V, Vallortigara G. Hippocampus and Medial Striatum Dissociation during Goal Navigation by Geometry or Features in the Domestic Chick: An Immediate Early Gene Study. Hippocampus. 2016;26:27–40. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Kosaki Y, Poulter SL, Austen JM, McGregor A. Dorsolateral striatal lesions impair navigation based on landmark-goal vectors but facilitate spatial learning based on a “cognitive map”. Learning & Memory. 2015;22:179–191. doi: 10.1101/lm.037077.114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.McGregor A, Hayward AJ, Pearce JM, Good MA. Hippocampal lesions disrupt navigation based on the shape of the environment. Behav. Neurosci. 2004;118:1011–1021. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.5.1011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Pearce JM, Roberts AD, Good MA. Hippocampal lesions disrupt navigation based on cognitive maps but not heading vectors. Nature. 1998;396:75–77. doi: 10.1038/23941. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Solstad T, Boccara CN, Kropff E, Moser MB, Moser EI. Representation of geometric borders in the entorhinal cortex. Science. 2008;322:1865–1868. doi: 10.1126/science.1166466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Lever C, Burton S, Jeewajee A, O’Keefe J, Burgess N. Boundary vector cells in the subiculum of the hippocampal formation. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:9771–9777. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1319-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Stewart S, Jeewajee A, Wills TJ, Burgess N, Lever C. Boundary coding in the rat subiculum. Philosoph. Trans. Royal Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 2014;369:20120514. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.O’Keefe J, Burgess N. Geometric determinants of the place fields hippocampal neurons. Nature. 1996;381(6581):425–8. doi: 10.1038/381425a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Hartley T, Burgess N, Lever C, Cacucci F, O’Keefe J. Modeling place fields in terms of the cortical inputs to the hippocampus. Hippocampus. 2000;10:369–379. doi: 10.1002/1098-1063(2000)10:4<369::AID-HIPO3>3.0.CO;2-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Lever C, Wills T, Cacucci F, Burgess N, O’Keefe J. Long-term plasticity in the hippocampal place cell representation of environmental geometry. Nature. 2002;416:90–94. doi: 10.1038/416090a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Mayer U, Bhushan R, Vallortigara G, Lee SA. Representation of environmental shape in the hippocampus of domestic chicks (Gallus gallus) Brain Stucture and Function. 2018;223:941–953. doi: 10.1007/s00429-017-1537-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Lee SA, et al. Electrical signatures of spatial boundaries in the human subiculum. J. Neurosci. 2018;38:3265–3272. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3216-17.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Olton DS, Schlosberg P. Food-searching strategies in young rats: Win-shift predominates over win-stay. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1978;92:609–618. doi: 10.1037/h0077492. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Bhushan, R. Spatial representation from birth to old age: Insights from comparative neurobiology and behavioral genomics (University of Trento, 2018).
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.