Skip to main content
Elsevier - PMC COVID-19 Collection logoLink to Elsevier - PMC COVID-19 Collection
. 2020 May 6;34:179–184. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.03.008

Polarization in America: two possible futures

Gordon Heltzel 1, Kristin Laurin 1
PMCID: PMC7201237  PMID: 32391408

Highlights

  • Polarization, that is, the separation and clustering of political attitudes, is good for democracy in small doses.

  • However, excessive polarization leads people to disregard views different from their own, making it hard to achieve democratic solutions to societal problems.

  • America has reached record-high levels of polarization, and is now excessively polarized.

  • Theory and evidence suggest two possible futures: polarization decreases and stabilizes, or it self-reinforces, increasing further.

  • Interventions should correct misperceptions of polarization resulting from stereotypes, extremists, poll bias, and negativity.

Abstract

The rise of polarization over the past 25 years has many Americans worried about the state of politics. This worry is understandable: up to a point, polarization can help democracies, but when it becomes too vast, such that entire swaths of the population refuse to consider each other’s views, this thwarts democratic methods for solving societal problems. Given widespread polarization in America, what lies ahead? We describe two possible futures, each based on different sets of theory and evidence. On one hand, polarization may be on a self-reinforcing upward trajectory fueled by misperception and avoidance; on the other hand it may have recently reached the apex of its pendulum swing. We conclude that it is too early to know which future we are approaching, but that our ability to address misperceptions may be one key factor.


Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2020, 34:179–184

This review comes from a themed issue on Emotion, motivation, personality and social sciences *political ideologies*

Edited by John T Jost, Eran Halperin and Kristin Laurin

For a complete overview see the Issue and the Editorial

Available online 6th May 2020

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.03.008

2352-1546/© 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Recall the last time you heard a news story about political foes disrespecting and ignoring each other. Now recall the last time you heard a news story about political foes respectfully listening to each other. Many Americans find the latter increasingly difficult, as news stories document the negative effects of rising political polarization in recent decades [1]. As polarization has risen, so have Americans’ worries: 90% believe their country is divided over politics and 60% feel pessimistic about their country overcoming these divisions to solve its biggest problems [2,3]. What does the future hold? We argue that, at its current level, polarization threatens the stability of American democracy [4], then offer two alternative predictions for its trajectory.

Current polarization

Political polarization occurs when subsets of a population adopt increasingly dissimilar attitudes toward parties and party members (i.e., affective polarization; [5]), as well as ideologies and policies (ideological polarization; [6]).1 With little-to-no polarization, most people support a mixture of liberal and conservative stances across issues, and they can support one party without disliking others. With very high polarization, large, separate clusters of the population endorse ideologically consistent stances across all issues, and love their own party while loathing the other(s).

Polarization recently reached an all-time high in the US [7]. In the last half century, members of both parties have reported increasingly extreme ideological views [8], a trend more pronounced among Republicans than Democrats, especially in the last decade [9, 10, 11]. More than ever, Americans endorse their party’s stance across all issues [7]. Since the 1990s, Americans’ liking for their own party and dislike for opponents have both increased [5,12]. For example, 80% of Americans today feel unfavorable towards their partisan foes, and the portion feeling very unfavorable has nearly tripled since 1994 [13]. These trends have led scholars to speculate that politics is a unique intergroup domain wherein people’s hate for opponents exceeds their affinity for co-partisans [12,14].

Polarization and democracy

Does polarization help or hurt democracies?

Political scientists continue to debate the costs and benefits of polarization. At its best, polarization can be benign, and produce more effective, stable democracies. It encourages civic engagement: Polarized citizens more often vote, protest, and join political movements, all of which are necessary for functioning democracy [15] and help disrupt undesirable status quos [16]. Polarization also entails pluralistic policy alternatives [17]; this is crucial for democracies, which rely on citizens being able to consider multiple policies and have thorough, constructive debates between them [18]. Ideally, this kind of engagement and pluralism ultimately produce effective, stable government: It helps societies identify policies that are both optimal for solving their biggest problems [89], and unlikely to be overturned when a new party takes power since they are mutually agreed-upon [85].

At its worst, polarization is pernicious, posing a challenge to the democratic process [4]. Highly polarized citizens often refuse to engage with each other, reactively dismissing out of hand both potential flaws in their own views and potential merits of their other opponents’ [16,19]. Under these conditions, constructive debates are impossible and mutually acceptable policies elusive.

Of course, people might feel morally compelled to polarize, even to this pernicious degree. For example, if one half of a society begins to embrace morally abhorrent ideas (i.e., white supremacy; Neo-Nazi ideologies), the other half might be justified in polarizing away from them, refusing to engage with or consisder their views. A full philosophical discussion of the morality of polarization falls beyond the scope of this paper (see Refs. [20, 21, 22]). Nonetheless, in a world where one half of a population refuse to engage with the other, even if this is the most morally correct choice, democratic processes can no longer operate effectively [23,24]. The only policies considered are those loved by one party and despised by the other; one side eventually ekes out a narrow victory, leaving the other desperate to delegitimize it [25]. In short, when polarization inspires revulsion, democracies run the risk of breaking down.

Is contemporary polarization helping or hurting American democracy?

Recent research in psychology has primarily highlighted the negative consequences of polarization in America. Americans accept smaller paychecks to avoid listening to opposing partisans [26], move to new places to surround themselves with ideologically similar residents [27], and swipe left on people with whom they disagree politically [28]. Polarized Americans are more willing to exclude people with opposing political beliefs than to exclude people of other races [29,30]—a jarring comparison considering the prevalence of race-based exclusion [31].

Likewise, Americans have trouble critically evaluating the flaws and merits of policies [32]. Instead, they seek information that confirms their partisan preferences [91] and disregard facts that counter them [33,34]. Out of loyalty [35], they treat core party issues as immune to debate [19,36,37] and suppress their opponents’ views [38, 39, 40].

In short, recent psychological findings suggest that Americans are refusing to interact with politically dissimilar others [19], and are motivated to overlook both the inadequacies in policies they support and the merits of opposing policies. Even if they feel—even if they are—morally justified in both avoiding opponents and their beliefs while doubling down on their own, this carries pragmatic risks [41]. In a system where the two polarized parties represent sizeable portions of the population, democratic processes may lead to suboptimal, oft-overturned policies that inadequately address societal problems [25].

Future polarization

Given the current state of polarization, what lies ahead for America? Extant theorizing leads us to consider two alternative futures.

Possible future #1: polarization is a self-reinforcing cycle that will continue to increase

Polarization may be bound to increase, owing to a self-reinforcing cycle. This cycle could take many forms (e.g., [42,43]), one of which is described in this very issue [44]. Drawing from these sources, we briefly review evidence that Americans overperceive polarization then reactively distance themselves from opponents, thereby increasing actual polarization; from here, they will again over perceive this now-elevated polarization, creating a self-perpetuating upward spiral.

Americans overestimate the extremity of both their opponents’ and co-partisans’ views [45,46], to the point where they perceive partisan opinion gaps to be twice their true size [47]. They also perceive vast partisan differences in moral values [48], even though both liberals and conservatives endorse similar core moral values (i.e., care, fairness; [49]) and disavow harm to others [50].

There are at least three sources contributing to these overestimates. First, biased polling measures may be inviting evidence for polarization with division-inciting questions [51]. For example, Republicans report more polarized attitudes toward ‘the opposing party’ than ‘the Democratic Party’ [52], and divisive policy terms increase partisan opinion discrepancies (e.g., ‘global warming’ versus ‘climate change’; [53]).

Second, though fewer than 10% of Americans identify as extremely liberal or conservative [7], this minority pervades political discourse: News stories cover their views more often [54,55], they are twice as likely to post about politics on social media [1], and because they use negative, angry language to morally condemn opponents [56], their messages are more likely to spread through social networks [57,58]. This disproportionately vocal minority may skew people’s perceptions of the modal views on each side.

Third, the psychological weight of bad news leads Americans to overestimate polarization [59]. Negative political content (e.g., stories of disrespect and close-mindedness, distressing poll results, extremists’ messages) grabs attention, dwells in memory, and colors our impressions of politics more than equally positive content [60].

Compounding these three processes, routinely exaggerated political polarization likely engenders a self-perpetuating cycle. When citizens overestimate polarization, they often dislike and avoid their opponents [61,62], which can, in turn, increase actual polarization: Disliking opponents may cause people to adopt preferences even further from those of the opponents [63], and avoiding opponents creates political echo chambers (especially among conservatives; [64,90]) that reinforce partisans’ pre-existing views. Likewise, when partisans overestimate how much they are hated by their opponents, they feel licensed to hate their opponents more in response [95]. Thus, people tend to overestimate polarization, which leads them to gradually shift further and further away from who they perceive their opponents to be.

Possible future #2: polarization is a pendulum that has reached its apex

Alternatively, though, polarization may have reached its peak, owing to Americans’ growing resentment for polarization and its consequences. Their resentment has grown for two reasons. First (and most directly), polarization leads to more extreme policy alternatives [17], which Americans find unappealing, even when they come from their own party [65].

Second (and more indirectly), Americans disapprove of polarization’s consequences. They feel that the quality of political discussion has deteriorated, featuring too many insults and not enough factual debate [3,5], and they are embarrassed about their current politicians’ antagonistic behavior [3,66]. Rather than applauding party representatives who berate opponents, they prefer civil, respectful political relations [67,68]; this is especially true among liberals [69]. Likewise, they believe political closed-mindedness is unintelligent and morally wrong [70,71], and reject co-partisans who refuse to consider opposing views [19,37], even socially excluding these dogmatic co-partisans [72].

When polarization leads fellow partisans to become disrespectful and close-minded, Americans respond by detaching from their parties and beliefs, resulting in weaker polarization. For example, upon seeing co-partisans disrespect opponents and ignore their views, Americans disidentify with their parties [1,73], instead moving toward more moderate positions [74,75].

Which future is most likely?

Existing empirical findings provide mixed evidence as to which of the possible futures is in fact emerging. First, we consider evidence of polarization from public polls. On one hand, polls in the past decade show flat or even decreased rates of polarization. Despite 2016’s contentious election, Americans showed no change in their preference for their own party over the opposing party between 2014 and 2017 [76]. Although dislike for political opponents increased sharply starting in 1994, since 2012 this trend has barely fluctuated [15]. Across four polls from 2011 to 2017, Pew gathered Democrats’ and Republicans’ attitudes on ten different issues; Partisans’ attitudes have either converged or remained stable across five issues (government business regulation; government waste; corporate profits; homosexuality; immigration; [13]).

On the other hand, partisans’ attitudes have grown further apart across the other five issues (welfare; helping the needy; addressing inequalities for Black people; military strength; environmental policy; [13]). And although polarization remained stable before and after Trump’s election, upcoming elections could highlight and exacerbate partisan divides. Moreover, infectious diseases typically evoke prejudice against groups whose norms oppose one’s own [96], so the current COVID-19 pandemic could further exacerbate already high levels of affective polarization.

Turning to behavioral indicators of polarization, on one hand, despite concerns from scholars about sustained, record-high polarization [14], many consequences of polarization have not manifested. For example, Americans in 2017 were no more likely than Americans in 2014 to suppress unfavorable news about their party, to exclude political opponents, or to support criminal investigations of opposing politicians [76]. Likewise, even today’s most fervent partisans would rather help their party than harm opponents [77]. For example, most partisans would rather allocate money to both co-partisans and opposing partisans than to co-partisans exclusively [78], and would rather publish favorable news about their own party than disparaging news about opponents [76,79].

On the other hand, and more troublingly, polarization’s most destructive consequences have worsened in recent years. For example, Americans’ support for tear gassing counter-party protesters has risen since 2012 [76], and 5–15% of partisans support violence against political opponents [92]. Likewise, politically motivated hate crimes and aggression have increased recently, especially among the alt-right [93]. For example, after Trump’s election in 2017, the United States witnessed ∼1600 more hate crimes than its annual average [80].

Conclusion

Extant theory and evidence paint two different pictures of the future: Polarization may continue to rise in a self-perpetuating cycle, or it may have reached its peak and even begun its downward arc. In fact, both processes may be at work simultaneously. One key factor in determining which will win out may be whether political and media institutions are able combat misperceptions of polarization. To the extent they do so successfully, this might intercept polarization’s self-perpetuating cycle, and help re-establish the existence of at least some common ground between the parties [46,61,81, 82, 83, 84].

Conflict of interest statement

Nothing declared.

References and recommended reading

Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review, have been highlighted as:

  • • of special interest

Footnotes

1

Polarization occurs everywhere to varying degrees, among both citizens and elected officials. We focus on polarization among American citizens (for elite polarization, see Refs. [86,44]) because of the wealth of empirical studies examining this population over time. With American polarization at record highs, the prevalence and effects described here are likely milder elsewhere; moreover, unique political and cultural factors (e.g., multi-party systems, democratic versus other political systems, media availability) likely influence how polarization manifests [87,88,94].

References

  • 1.Klar S., Krupnikov Y., Ryan J.B. The New York Times. 2019. Polarized, or sick of politics?https://www.nytimes.com/2019/04/12/opinion/polarization-politics-democrats-republicans.html p. 21. Retrieved from. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.PRRI/TheAtlantic . 2019. American Democracy in Crisis: The Fate of Pluralism in a Divided Nation.https://www.prri.org/research/american-democracy-in-crisis-the-fate-of-pluralism-in-a-divided-nation/ Retrieved from. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Pew Research Center . Pew Research Center; 2019. Public Highly Critical of State of Political Discourse in the U.S. Available at https://www.people-press.org/2019/06/19/public-highly-critical-of-state-of-political-discourse-in-the-u-s/ (Accessed October 2019) [Google Scholar]
  • 4•.McCoy J., Rahman T., Somer M. Polarization and the global crisis of democracy: common patterns, dynamics, and pernicious consequences for democratic polities. Am Behav Sci. 2018;62:16–42. [Google Scholar]; McCoy et al. review suggest generalizable processes and consequences of polarization based on historic cases of polarization in multiple countries. They also consider polaraization’s compatibility with democratic processes, suggesting that the few democratic benefits of polarization are outweighed by its pernicious consequences.
  • 5•.Iyengar S., Lelkes Y., Levendusky M., Malhotra N., Westwood S.J. The origins and consequences of affective polarization in the United States. Ann Rev Polit Sci. 2019;22:129–146. [Google Scholar]; Iyengar et al. review evidence of affective (identity and party-based) polarization, charting its growth, causes, and effects.
  • 6.Fiorina M.P., Abrams S.J. Political polarization in the American public. Ann Rev of Polit Sci. 2008;11:563–588. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Abramowitz A.I., Saunders K.L. Is polarization a myth? J Polit. 2008;70:542–555. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Levendusky M.S. University of Chicago Press; 2009. The Partisan Sort: How Liberals Became Democrats and Conservatives Became Republicans. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bacon P.Jr. 2018. The Republican Party has Changed Dramatically Since George H. W. Bush Ran it. Available at https://fivethirtyeight.com/features/the-republican-party-has-changed-dramatically-since-george-h-w-bush-ran-it/ (Accessed February 2020) [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Smith Tom W., Davern Michael, Freese Jeremy, Morgan Stephen. General Social Surveys, 1972–2018 [Machine-Readable Data File] /Principal Investigator, Smith, Tom W.; Co-Principal Investigators, Michael Davern, Jeremy Freese, and Stephen Morgan; Sponsored by National Science Foundation. --NORC ed.-- Chicago: NORC; NORC at the University of Chicago [Producer and Distributor]; 2018. Data accessed from the GSS Data Explorer website at. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Pew Research Center . Pew Research Center; 2014. Political Polarization in the American Public.https://www.people-press.org/2014/06/12/political-polarization-in-the-american-public/ Available at. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Iyengar S., Krupenkin M. The strengthening of partisan affect. Polit Psychol. 2018;39:201–218. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Pew Research Center . 2017. The Partisan Divide on Political Values Grows Even Wider. Available at https://www.people-press.org/2017/10/05/the-partisan-divide-on-political-values-grows-even-wider/ (Accessed October 2019) [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Parker M.T., Janoff-Bulman R. Lessons from morality-based social identity: the power of outgroup “hate,” not just ingroup “love”. Soc Justice Res. 2013;26:81–96. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Pew Research Center . Pew Research Center; 2019. Political Independents: Who They Are, What They Think. Available at https://www.people-press.org/2019/03/14/political-independents-who-they-are-what-they-think/ (Accessed October 2019) [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Stavrakakis Y. Paradoxes of polarization: democracy’s inherent division and the (anti-) populist challenge. Am Behav Sci. 2018;62:43–58. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Campbell J.E. Princeton University Press; 2018. Polarized: Making Sense of a Divided America. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Shi F., Teplitskiy M., Duede E., Evans J.A. The wisdom of polarized crowds. Nat Hum Behav. 2019;3:329. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0541-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Skitka L.J., Washburn A.N., Carsel T.S. The psychological foundations and consequences of moral conviction. Curr Opin Psychol. 2015;6:41–44. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Brown W. Princeton University Press; 2009. Regulating Aversion: Tolerance in the Age of Identity and Empire. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Popper K.R. Routledge & Kegan Paul; 1957. The Open Society and its Enemies: The Spell of Plato. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Rawls J. Harvard university press; 1971. A Theory of Justice. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Lozada M. Us versus them: social representation and the imaginaries of other in Venezuela. Pap Soc Represent. 2014;23:21.1–21.16. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.McCoy J., Diez F. U.S. Institute of Peace; Washington DC: 2001. International Mediation of Political Conflict in Venezuela. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Carothers T., O’Donohue A., editors. Democracies Divided: The Global Challenge of Political Polarization. Brookings Institution Press; 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 26•.Frimer J.A., Skitka L.J., Motyl M. Liberals and conservatives are similarly motivated to avoid exposure to one another’s opinions. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2017;72:1–12. [Google Scholar]; Frimer et al. demonstrated the lengths partisans will go to avoid hearing political views they disagree with. They also find that this occurs partially because of anticipated dissonance and the fear of creating interpersonal conflict.
  • 27.Motyl M., Iyer R., Oishi S., Trawalter S., Nosek B.A. How ideological migration geographically segregates groups. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2014;51:1–14. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Huber G.A., Malhotra N. Political homophily in social relationships: evidence from online dating behavior. J Polit. 2017;79:269–283. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Haidt J., Rosenberg E., Hom H. Differentiating diversities: moral diversity is not like other kinds. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2003;33:1–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Iyengar S., Westwood S.J. Fear and loathing across party lines: new evidence on group polarization. Am J Polit Sci. 2015;59:690–707. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Pager D., Shepherd H. The sociology of discrimination: racial discrimination in employment, housing, credit, and consumer markets. Ann Rev Sociol. 2008;34:181–209. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.33.040406.131740. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Baron J. Social norms for citizenship. Soc Res. 2018;85:229–253. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Kahan D.M., Peters E., Dawson E.C., Slovic P. Motivated numeracy and enlightened self-government. Behav Public Policy. 2017;1:54–86. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Van Bavel J.J., Pereira A. The partisan brain: an identity-based model of political belief. Trends Cogn Sci. 2018;22:213–224. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Clark C.J., Winegard B., Liu B.S., Ditto P.H. Tribalism is human nature tribalism is human nature. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2019;28:587–592. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Baron J. Actively open-minded thinking in politics. Cognition. 2019;188:8–18. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Tetlock P.E. Thinking the unthinkable: Sacred values and taboo cognitions. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003;7:320–324. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00135-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Brandt M.J., Reyna C., Chambers J.R., Crawford J.T., Wetherell G. The ideological-conflict hypothesis. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2014;23:27–34. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Crawford J.T. Ideological symmetries and asymmetries in political intolerance and prejudice toward political activist groups. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2014;55:284–298. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.van Prooijen J.W., Krouwel A.P.M., Boiten M., Eendebak L. Fear among the extremes: how political ideology predicts negative emotions and outgroup derogation. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2015;41:485–497. doi: 10.1177/0146167215569706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Somer M., McCoy J. Transformations through polarizations and global threats to democracy. Ann Am Acad Pol Soc Sci. 2019;681:8–22. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Ahler D.J. Self-fulfilling misperceptions of public polarization. J Polit. 2014;76:607–620. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Klein E. Avid Reader Press; New York: 2020. Why we’re Polarized. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Wilson AE, Parker V, Feinberg M: Polarization in the contemporary political and media landscape. Curr Opin Behav Sci. (this issue).
  • 45.Clifford S. Compassionate democrats and tough republicans: how ideology shapes partisan stereotypes. Polit Behav. 2019:1–25. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Ahler D.J., Sood G. The parties in our heads: misperceptions about party composition and their consequences. J Polit. 2018;80:964–981. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Van Boven L., Judd C.M., Sherman D.K. Political polarization projection: social projection of partisan attitude extremity and attitudinal processes. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2012;103:84–100. doi: 10.1037/a0028145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Graham J., Nosek B.A., Haidt J. The moral stereotypes of liberals and conservatives: exaggeration of differences across the political spectrum. PLoS One. 2012;7 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0050092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Graham J., Nosek B.A., Haidt J., Iyer R., Koleva S., Ditto P.H. Mapping the moral domain. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2011;101:366–385. doi: 10.1037/a0021847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Schein C., Gray K. The unifying moral dyad: liberals and conservatives share the same harm-based moral template. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2015;41:1147–1163. doi: 10.1177/0146167215591501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Berinsky A.J. Measuring public opinion with surveys. Ann Rev Polit Sci. 2017;20:309–329. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Westfall J., Van Boven L., Chambers J.R., Judd C.M. Perceiving political polarization in the United States: party identity strength and attitude extremity exacerbate the perceived partisan divide. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015;10:145–158. doi: 10.1177/1745691615569849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Schuldt J.P., Roh S., Schwarz N. Questionnaire design effects in climate change surveys: implications for the partisan divide. Ann Am Acad Polit Soc Sci. 2015;658:67–85. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Graber D.A., Dunaway J. Cq Press; 2017. Mass Media and American Politics. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Sobieraj S., Berry J.M. From incivility to outrage: political discourse in blogs, talk radio, and cable news. Polit Commun. 2011;28:19–41. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Frimer J.A., Brandt M.J., Melton Z., Motyl M. Extremists on the left and right use angry, negative language. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2018;45:1216–1231. doi: 10.1177/0146167218809705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Brady W.J., Gantman A.P., Van Bavel J.J. Attentional capture helps explain why moral and emotional content go viral. J Exp Psychol: Gen. 2019;149:746–756. doi: 10.1037/xge0000673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58•.Brady W.J., Wills J.A., Jost J.T., Tucker J.A., Van Bavel J.J. Emotion shapes the diffusion of moralized content in social networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2017;114:7313–7318. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1618923114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Brady et al. show that adding emotional content to morally charged tweets increases the likelihood that those tweets are shared on social media. The most emotionally charged tweets are shared almost twice as often.
  • 59.Rozin P., Royzman E.B. Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2001;5:296–320. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Soroka S., Fournier P., Nir L. Cross-national evidence of a negativity bias in psychophysiological reactions to news. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2019;116:18888–18892. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1908369116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61•.Dorison C.A., Minson J.A., Rogers T. Selective exposure partly relies on faulty affective forecasts. Cognition. 2019;188:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Dorison et al. find that people opt to avoid opposing political views because they expect to feel upset when hearing these views. When an intervention corrected these misperceptions, people were more willing to listen to opponents.
  • 62.Enders A.M., Armaly M.T. The differential effects of actual and perceived polarization. Polit Behav. 2018;41:815–839. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Heider F. Wiley; New York: 1958. The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Mutz D.C. Cambridge University Press; 2006. Hearing the Other Side: Deliberative Versus Participatory Democracy. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Pew Research Center . 2019. Partisan Antipathy: More Intense, More Personal. Available at https://www.people-press.org/2019/10/10/the-partisan-landscape-and-views-of-the-parties/ (Accessed October 2019) [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Paulus F.M., Müller-Pinzler L., Meshi D., Peng T.-Q., Martinez Mateo M., Krach S. The politics of embarrassment: considerations on how norm-transgressions of political representatives shape nation-wide communication of emotions on social media. Front Commun. 2019;4:11. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2019.00011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 67•.Frimer J.A., Skitka L.J. The Montagu principle: incivility decreases politicians’ public approval, even with their political base. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2018;115:845. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Using congressional records and real tweets, Frimer and Skitka show that people across the political spectrum prefer political civility and disapprove of aggressive, insulting politicians.
  • 68.Mutz D.C., Reeves B. The new videomalaise: effects of televised incivility on political trust. Am Polit Sci Rev. 2005;99:1–15. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Fridkin K., Kenney P. Oxford University Press; 2019. Taking Aim at Attack Advertising: Understanding the Impact of Negative Campaigning in US Senate Races. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.John L.K., Jeong M., Gino F., Huang L. The self-presentational consequences of upholding one’s stance in spite of the evidence. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 2019;154:1–14. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Ståhl T., Zaal M.P., Skitka L.J. Moralized rationality: relying on logic and evidence in the formation and evaluation of belief can be seen as a moral issue. PLoS One. 2016;11:1–38. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0166332. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72•.Heltzel G. University of British Columbia; 2019. Seek and ye shall be fine: attitudes towards political perspective-seekers (Doctoral Dissertation) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; In this ongoing work, we have examined cross-national online and lab data to examine people’s feelings towards co-partisans who seek out opposing perspectives. We find a strong preference for fellow group members who seek out opposing views rather than avoiding them, even among extreme partisans and across highly contentious issues.
  • 73.Klar S., Krupnikov Y., Ryan J.B. Affective polarization or partisan disdain? Untangling a dislike for the opposing party from a dislike of partisanship. Public Opin Q. 2018;82:379–390. [Google Scholar]
  • 74•.Druckman J.N., Gubitz S.R., Levendusky M.S., Lloyd A.M. How incivility on partisan media (de)polarizes the electorate. J Polit. 2019;81:291–295. [Google Scholar]; Druckman et al. find that Americans identify less with their party and hold more moderate positions when they see news reports about their party insulting political opponents.
  • 75•.Shafranek R.M. Political consequences of partisan prejudice. Polit Psychol. 2019;41:35–51. [Google Scholar]; Shafranek finds that Americans feel less identified with their party and hold more moderate positions when they see fellow partisans discriminate against their political opponents.
  • 76•.Westwood S.J., Peterson E., Lelkes Y. Are there still limits on partisan prejudice? Public Opin Q. 2019;83:584–597. [Google Scholar]; Westwood et al. compare partisan animosity in recent years using national surveys and four experiments. Comparing national polls from 2012 and 2016, their results suggest affective polarization has not changed in recent years. Additionally, three of their four experiments showed no increase in behavioral consequences of polarization.
  • 77.Lelkes Y., Westwood S.J. The limits of partisan discrimination. J Polit. 2017;79:485–501. [Google Scholar]
  • 78•.Tappin B.M., McKay R.T. Moral polarization and out-party hostility in the US political context. J Soc Polit Psychol. 2019;7:213–245. [Google Scholar]; Tappin and McKay find that partisans would rather benefit both co-partisans and opposing partisans than only themselves and their fellow partisans. This holds true even among partisans who feel their party is highly moral while their opponents are highly immoral.
  • 79•.Amira K., Wright J.C., Goya-Tocchetto D. In-group love versus out-group hate: which is more important to partisans and when? Polit Behav. 2019:1–22. [Google Scholar]; Amira et al. make participants choose between their love for their party against their hate for opponents, and find that participants generally prefer to promote their party than disparage their opponents. However, when participants heard news about their own party’s immoral behavior, they respond defensively, preferring to publish unsavory stories about opponents. Thus, partisans may prefer to hurt their opponents when their own party is threatened.
  • 80.Rushin S, Edwards GS: The Effect of President Trump’s Election on Hate Crimes (January 14, 2018). Available at SSRN: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3102652 or 10.2139/ssrn.3102652. [DOI]
  • 81.Gray K. 2019. The Cabbage Roll Epiphany: Our Best Chance at Depolarizing the United States. Available at https://bigthink.com/Charles-Koch-Foundation/why-grandmas-food-tastes-better (Accessed October 2019) [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Keltner D., Robinson R.J. Imagined ideological differences in conflict escalation and resolution. Int J Confl Manag. 1993;4:249–262. [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Levendusky M.S. Americans, not partisans: can priming American national identity reduce affective polarization? J Polit. 2017;80:59–70. [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Schein C, Kubin E, Bignman Y, Gray K: Ridging Political Divides by Focusing on Shared Moral Values (unpublished manuscript).
  • 85.Barber M., McCarty N. Causes and consequences of polarization. Polit Negot: Handb. 2015;37:39–43. [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Hetherington M.J. Resurgent mass partisanship: the role of elite polarization. Am Polit Sci Rev. 2001;95:619–631. [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Skitka L.J., Liu J.H., Yang Y., Chen H., Liu L., Xu L. Exploring the cross-cultural generalizability and scope of morally motivated intolerance. Soc Psychol Pers Sci. 2012;4:324–331. [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Viciana H., Hannikainen I.R., Gaita A. The dual nature of partisan prejudice: morality and identity in a multiparty system. PLoS One. 2019;14:1–22. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0219509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Mill J.S. Prometheus Books; Buffalo, NY: 1991. Considerations on Representative Government; p. 1861. [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Barberá P., Jost J.T., Nagler J., Tucker J.A., Bonneau R. Tweeting from left to right: Is online political communication more than an echo chamber? Psychol Sci. 2015;26:1531–1542. doi: 10.1177/0956797615594620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Ditto P.H., Liu B.S., Clark C.J., Wojcik S.P., Chen E.E., Grady R.H., Celniker J.B., Zinger J.F. At least bias is bipartisan: A meta-analytic comparison of partisan bias in liberals and conservatives. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2019;14:273–291. doi: 10.1177/1745691617746796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Kalmoe N.P., Mason L. Lethal mass partisanship: Prevalence, correlates, and electoral contingencies. NCAPSA American Politics Meeting; Washington, DC; 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Womick J., Rothmund T., Azevedo F., King L.A., Jost J.T. 2019 Group-based dominance and authoritarian aggression predict support for Donald Trump in the US presidential election. Social Psychol Personality Sci. 2016;10:643–652. [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Prior M. Media and political polarization. Ann Rev Political Sci. 2013;16:101–127. [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Moore-Berg S., Karlinsky L.O.A., Hameiri B., Bruneau E. 2020. The Partisan Penumbra: Political Partisans’ Exaggerated Meta-Perceptions Predict Intergroup Hostility. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Schaller M., Neuberg S.L. Vol. 46. Academic Press; 2012. Danger, disease, and the nature of prejudice(s) pp. 1–54. (Advances in Experimental Social Psychology). [Google Scholar]

Articles from Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES