Abstract
kakusei is a non-coding RNA that is overexpressed in foraging bee brain. This study describes a possible role of the IEG kakusei during the daily foraging of honey bees. kakusei was found to be transiently upregulated within two hours during rewarded foraging. Interestingly, during unrewarded foraging the gene was also found to be up-regulated, but immediately lowered when food was not rewarded. Moreover, the kakusei overexpression was diminished within a very short time when the time schedule of feeding was changed. This indicates the potential role of kakusei on the motivation of learned reward foraging. These results provide evidence for a dynamic role of kakusei during for aging of bees, and eventually its possible involvement in learning and memory. Thus the kakusei gene could be used as search tool in finding distinct molecular pathways that mediate diverse behavioral components of foraging.
Introduction
Social behavior of honey bee foraging has been an attractive field of research. Further exploration of the dynamics of honey bee foraging at the molecular and cellular level could help in uncovering the complex mechanisms of social behavior. Honey bee foraging comprises several behavioral components including learning, memory, social interaction and communication. In 1973 the Austrian ethologist Karl Ritter von Frisch (Austrian ethologist) was honored with Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine for his investigations of sensory perceptions in honey bees [1]. He translated the meaning of the bee waggle dance into a particular movement of the honey bee that looks like the form of figure eight, and revealed that foraging bees of the same colony share information with the help of this dance [2–4]. Bee foraging has gained enormous attention, since it offers the opportunity to elucidate the complexity of behavioral mechanisms involved in accomplishing the foraging task. Thanks to the efforts of many researchers, today we have a substantial amount of knowledge on this subject. However, understanding of the context of molecular and cellular mechanisms underlying foraging tasks is still poor.
During foraging, bees fly back and forth several times during the day between the hive and the food location, collecting nectar/pollen and bringing it to the hive for their colony. Foraging involves highly systematic and dynamic behavioral capacities that include long distance navigation using the sun as compass, evaluation of food quality, learning and memorizing flower cues, sense of colour, and social communication/interaction for coordination in collecting nectar, pollen, and water [2,5,6]. For decades the outdoor experiments have been routinely performed by feeding bees on pollen or sucrose solution placed in a specific place in order to mimic the natural foraging environment in close proximity. Using this experimental setup, we attempted to identify foraging regulatory genes in the European honey bee species Apis mellifera while the bees were performing their daily routine foraging. For this, the immediate early genes (IEGs) were the ultimate targets to examine, as they are well known as neural markers.
IEGs are rapidly induced by a large number of stimuli, and alterations of their expression are considered as part of the general neuronal response to natural stimuli as interpreted by normal synaptic activity [7]. The products of IEGs can activate downstream targets that typically function as part of a network of constitutively expressed proteins [7]. IEGs are also known to be the first activated genes that link cellular membrane events to the nucleus [8], and the gene expression changes are required for the late neuronal response which relates to the process of learning and memory formation [9] and which is a part of everyday brain functions [10]. In addition, depending on the type of the stimuli, the IEG-encoded proteins can be individually regulated in different parts of the brain [8,11], indicating that the same or different IEGs can, regulate different functions when expressed in different parts of the brain.
In our recent work, we have demonstrated involvement of two IEGs i.e., early growth response 1 (egr-1) and, nuclear hormone receptor 38 (hr38) and their corresponding partner genes such as ecdysone receptor (ecr), dopamine/ecdysteroid receptor (dopecr), dopamine decarboxylase (ddc) and dopamine receptor 2 (dopr2) during the daily foraging of bees [12]. Other papers have reported on the involvement of egr-1 in the transition from nursing to foraging bees [13] and the role of hr38 in caste and labor division [14], while the other genes were shown to have acted as egr-1 downstream genes involved in ecdysteroid and dopamine signaling with a role in the processing of courtship memory [15] in Drosophila and in olfactory learning and memory [16] in honey bees, respectively. The IEG c-jun (also known as jun-related antigen, jra in fruit fly) and egr-1 have been used as neuronal markers for the identification in honey bees of specific brain regions involved in the time memory as well as in innate and learned behaviours [17,18,19]. Interestingly, while most of the above mentioned genes are already well known through various research studies, a recently discovered insect IEG kakusei, a noncoding RNA, has been found to have function in the patterning of neuronal activity in the foraging bees [20,21,22]. Expression of kakusei was detected during ice-induced seizures in bees awakening from anesthesia, and its expression was prominent in the Kenyon cells of the mushroom bodies [20,21,22]. Since kakusei was found as neural marker, we were interested to extend the work on this gene in our experimental design.
The present study represents further investigation of immediate early genes (IEG) that could play a role during the daily foraging of honey bees, and is a continuous work of our previous report by Singh et al [12]. We selected two potential IEGs kakusei and c-jun, and four other orthologous genes that have been reported to be involved in cognitive process in vertebrates: extracellular signal-related kinase (rk7), glutamate receptor (GluR), 5-hydroxy tryptamine (serotonin) receptor 2 alpha (5-HT2α) and dopamine receptor 1 (Dop1). Among the six genes tested, we observed only for the kakusei gene with a transient and prolonged upregulation during reward foraging and a short period of overexpression during unrewarded foraging. These findings demonstrate a potential role for kakusei during reward foraging and in learning of food reward foraging.
Materials and methods
Foraging experiment and sample collection
Honey bees were purchased from the local bee keepers, Bangalore, Karnataka, India. The bee colonies were kept inside the bee house located within the institute campus, National Centre for Biological Sciences (NCBS), TIFR, Bangalore, India. And one of the most common honey bee species Apis Mellifera was used in this experiment. Therefore, no endangered or threatened species or locations were involved in this study. The behavioral tests were performed by using an outdoor flight cage (length = 12m, height = 5m, width = 2.5m) located within the NCBS campus, Bangalore, India. The Apis mellifera colonies were kept within the outdoor flight case and bees were fed with pollen and 1 M sugar solution placed on a green and yellow plastic plate respectively. The distance of feeders was 10m from the beehive with a 1.5m gap between the two feeders. The feeding time was from 14.00 to 17.00 hrs every day. Sample collection was started after the foraging bees had visited feeders for several days. For the gene expression profiling, nectar foragers were collected and the gene expression analysis was carried out with those samples. The procedures were same as previously described [12] and the same samples have been reused in this study.
Collection procedure and sample grouping
Collection during foraging
The first arriving foragers were caught at the feeder plate before presenting the sugar solution on the feeder (0min group) and the caught bees were immediately flash frozen in liquid nitrogen for further gene expression analysis. Soon after the first collection, sugar solution was presented and the first arriving foragers were marked by paint markers. The marked bees were collected at a series of time points with 15 min intervals up to 2 hrs (15min, 30min, 45min, 60min, 75min, 90min, 105min, and 120min groups), then flash frozen immediately in liquid nitrogen. We caught paint-marked foragers as soon as they landed over the plate and before they start drinking the sugar solution at those set times during their repeated trips. The paint mark on bees was done using Uni POSCA Paint Markers (Uni Mitsubishi Pencil, UK). In one day we collected about 24 bees, 1–2 bees for each time point starting from 14.00 hours until 16.00 hours and continued in following days until we obtained 5 bees at each time point.
Collection before and after foraging
Pre-foraging bees were paint-marked while foraging and collected in the morning (09.00 hrs) of the next day in the hive before they started foraging; whereas post-foraging bees were caught in the hive in the evening (18.00 hrs) after the bees finished foraging, on the same day that the bees were paint marked. We took gentle care during the collection procedure in order that the foraging bees were not disturbed and to avoid inducing stress phenomena; thereby the interactions between the collector and the bees were considered minimal [23,24,25]. The caught bees were immediately flash frozen in liquid nitrogen.
Collection without food reward
This category included only the foraging bees collected at the empty feeder plate at different time points. The foragers were marked at 0min upon their arrival at the empty feeder and 0min samples were collected. After the collection of 0min samples, 1 M sucrose solution was presented to let the bees continue coming and the collection continued for one hour with samples at 15min, 30min, 45min and 60min. While the unmarked bees were drinking, the marked bees were caught as soon as they landed on the feeder plate; it was made sure that those bees had not touched the sucrose solution. About 2 bees were collected in each day of collection and the collection started at 14.00 hrs. The bees were immediately flash frozen in liquid nitrogen as soon as they were caught.
Collection at different feeding time
Here the procedure is same with that of (1). The only difference was that the feeder was presented at 11.00 hrs instead of the normal feeding time 14.00 hours, and the collection also started from the same time and continued for 1 hr with samples at 0min, 15min, 30min, 45min and 60min.
Brain dissection, RNA and cDNA preparation, and qPCR
The frozen bees were lyophilized at -50°C with vacuum at 0.420 mBar for 20min, using a Freeze Zone® PlusTM 4.5 liter cascade Freeze Dry System (Labconco Corporation, Kanas City). Brain dissection was performed in a glass chamber containing 100% ethanol placed on a dry ice platform. The whole brain from each bee was dissected and placed into a micro centrifuge tube separately, and 500 μl Trizol (Trizol® Reagent, ambion RNA, life technology) was added. The brain was homogenized, RNA was extracted, and cDNA was prepared from that RNA, using the kits supplied by Invitrogen (Thermo Fisher Scientific) following the manufacturer's protocols. The cDNA from each brain was subjected to qPCR using a 7900HT Fast Real Time PCR System (Applied Biosystem, Singapore) in a 10μl reaction volume containing oligonucleotide primers (Sigma Aldrich) specific to target genes and SYBR Green (KAPA Syber® FAST PCR Master Mix (2X) ABI Prism®). The qPCR cycles followed Applied Biosystem protocol and the Rp49 gene was used as endogenous control [26] in each qPCR run and analysis. The details of the target genes and the oligonucleotide primers are provided in Table 1.
Table 1. Details of primers used for quantifying the target genes.
Gene Name | NCBI Gene ID | Chromosome No. & location | Oligonucleotide primer sequence 5′ - 3′ | Amplicon size |
---|---|---|---|---|
Kakusei | 100049563 | LG2 | Fow—TGGGTAGGGTTGGTAAGGGAA | 91 bp |
NC_007071.3 | Rev—ACACGAAACCATCCTGCCAC | |||
Erk7 | 408917 | LG4 | Fow—ACCCGGTCCGAAGAAGAAAT | 67 bp |
NC_007080.3 | Rev—CAGGCCAAAAGTCTGAGAATCA | |||
c-Jun | 726289 | LG9 | Fow—CCCTTCAGCAATTTAACCTTATC | 78 bp |
NC_007078.3 | Rev—CGTGGCGGCATCCAAA | |||
GluR | 411220 | LG7 | Fow—GGGATCGCCTCATATACCCA | 71 bp |
NC_007076.3 | Rev—GAGCGAACCAAAGGCTGTTT | |||
5-HT2α | 411323 | LG9 | Fow—GTCTCCAGCTCGATCACGGT | 126 bp |
NC_007078.3 | Rev—GGGTATGTAGAAGGCGATCAGAGA | |||
Rp49 | 406099 | LG11 | Fow—CAGTTGGCAACATATGACGAG | 124 bp |
NC_007080.3 | Rev—AAAGAGAAACTGGCGTAAACC | |||
Dop1 | 406111 | LG15 | Fow—ACAGAATTCCGAGAAGCGTTCA | 79 bp |
NC_007084.3 | Rev–ATTCGCTAGTCGACGGTTCATTT |
LG: Linkage Group
Statistical analysis of qPCR
We calculated relative gene expression level using the relative standard curve method with the help of SDS 2.4 software provided with the 7900HT Fast Real system. The standard deviation was calculated following Applied Biosystem’s ‘Guide to performing relative quantification of gene expression using real-time quantitative PCR’. The fold change was determined relative to time t0, and the statistical significance was tested using one-way ANOVA with Turkey- Kramer post-hoc multiple comparison test; the analysis was carried out with the help of GraphPad InStat software [27]. Normal distribution of each group compared was tested using the D'Agostino & Pearson omnibus normality test. In order to check the differences among the independent experiments, the two-way ANOVA program of GraphPad Prism was also employed (GraphPad Software Inc. www.graphpad.com).
Results
In this study, only the immediate early gene kakusei showed a remarkable transient upregulation during the course of food reward foraging. The other five genes erk7, c-jun, glur, 5-ht2α, dop1 showed no statistically significant differences (S1 Fig).
Expression profile of kakusei during food reward foraging
The expression of kakusei was measured at eleven time points, BF (pre/before foraging), 0min, 15min, 30min, 45min, 60min, 75min, 90min, 105min, 120min and AF (post/after foraging). In order to check the consistency of the results, three experiments were performed using independent samples collected from two different bee colonies over three months. The experiment 1 and 2 were from colony 1 and experiment 3 was from colony 2. Each of the three experiments demonstrated transient increases of kakusei level during the continuous food reward foraging over the collection time of two hours. The results are summarized in Fig 1 (S1 Table).
We observed an increase of kakusei expression from 0min (start of foraging, 14.00 hrs) to 15min (P = <0.0001) and from 15min to 30min (P = <0.0001) (Fig 1 Exp 1A). While the increase held at about 30min (P30m-45m = not significant) and a decrease was apparent from 45min, there was a sustained increased in expression of kakusei further down along the time series until 120min during the reward foraging as compared to the level at 0min (Fig 1 Exp 1A and S1 Table). A similar pattern was also observed in the subsequent two experiments with independent samples (Fig 1 Exp 2A/3A and S1 Table). The kakusei level was also significantly higher (P = <0.0001) at the start of foraging (0min) when compared to the level at pre foraging (09.00 hrs) or post foraging (18.00 hrs), while there was no significant difference between the before and after foraging samples (Fig 1 Exp 1B). This observation was further validated by two further independent experiments (Fig 1 Exp 2B & 3B). We then examined whether kakusei overexpression pattern was statistically different among the three independent results. We found significantly different overexpression of kakusei among the three independent experiments (P = 0.0001). The difference in kakusei levels between Exp-1/Exp-3 vs Exp-2 (P = 0.0001) was much higher than Exp-1 vs Exp-3 (P = 0.044). This difference might be due to age differences among the group of bees in Exp-1A/1B, Exp-2A/2B and Exp-3A/3B as we did not maintain the age of foraging bees during sample collection. The graphical results of the analysis are presented in Fig 2A. Similarly, in the case of the start/pre/post foraging groups, among the three independent experiments, we observed difference between Exp-2 vs Exp-1/-3 (P = 0.0001) and no difference between Exp-1 and Exp-3 (Fig 2B).
Expression changes of kakusei during unrewarded foraging
In order to confirm whether the kakusei upregulation was solely due to the food reward, we presented the empty plate at the usual feeding time 14.00 hrs and the bees were collected without feeding following the procedure described in the methods section. Interestingly, kakusei expression from the whole bee brains showed upregulation at 15min (P = 0.0011; Fig 2C), indicating a potential effect on kakusei of the learned motivation of food reward. However, the value no longer increased after 15min (P15m-30m/P30m-45m/P45m-60m = not significant) and the level dropped by 45min (P0m-45m = not significant) (Fig 2C and S2 Table) indicating that a food reward is required in order to sustain kakusei upregulation. This result suggests that kakusei is involved in learning of food reward during foraging.
Testing for time-dependent food reward foraging effect on kakusei expression
To further examine whether the kakusei expression depends on the time of feeding, the sucrose solution was presented at 11.00 hrs, three hours ahead of the usual feeding time instead of 14.00 hrs (it may be recalled that the bees were fed every day at 14.00 hrs in the previous experiments). As before the marked bees were collected over a period of 1 hour, at 0min, 15min, 30min, 45min and 60min, as described in method section. A similar pattern of overexpression of kakusei was observed as in the previous experiment of food reward foraging in which the collection began at 14:00 hrs. The increase continued from 0min to 15min (P = <0.0001) and 15 min to 30min (P = 0.0195), and then showed no further increase (P30m-45m/P45m-60m = not significant) (Fig 2D and S2 Table). This further reveals that kakusei role is independent of feeding time during the day, but responds to the reward of food during foraging.
Discussion
The importance of using immediate early genes as tool for finding molecular and cellular mechanisms underlying neuronal network and behaviors in honey bees has been well emphasized in the recent studies [11,12,28,29]. Keeping this idea as central, this study extends our recent report [12] on the search for immediate early genes (IEGs) that could be used as research tool for finding the molecular and cellular mechanisms underlying social behavior using foraging of honey bees as model system. As the foraging of bees consists of systematically well organized social behaviors that include learning, memory, social interaction and communication etc; honey bee foraging has been extensively studied in various research objectives in connection to social behavior [2,5,6]. Noting that honey bees learn food sources, identify the food location, memorize the place, interact and communicate among the foragers, as can be clearly observed during the time of their daily foraging; it is a promising approach to find the genes whose expression is increased upon foraging and to examine their roles in these behavioral routines. In the present study the possible function of the newly discovered insect IEG kakusei is presented, suggesting its role during the foraging of bees. A possible function of the gene in learning of food reward during foraging had also been observed.
kakusei is an insect immediate early gene recently identified by Kubo and his group in the seizure induced bees by cold or CO2 treatment, and is transcribed into a non-coding neuronal RNA [20]. They found that the transcript was prominently increased in the small-type Kenyon cells of the dancers and foraging bee brains, suggesting involvement of the gene in foraging behavior. However, its involvement during the course of foraging had never been studied before. This is the first report on transient overexpression of kakusei during foraging. We have further examined the possible role of the gene before and after foraging. After food reward kakusei was immediately overexpressed over a short period of time from 15 to ca. 45 to 60 min, and then started decreasing. Interestingly, without food reward, there was a short time overexpression at 15min and no further increase of the gene level. This indicates that food reward is essential for the increase and sustaining of higher kakusei levels during foraging, and suggests a possible role of kakusei in learning and memorizing of food reward during foraging. The possibility of kakusei involvement in learning and memory was previously indicated in the earlier report by Kiya et al., 2007 [20] as they reported overrepresentation of kakusei among the re-orienting Kenyon cells of bees assumed to be foragers. This is in line with the notion that re-orientation flight was the mechanism of flight behavior that was practiced by those bees for memorizing the hive location, thus indicating a role of kakusei in the information integration during foraging flight, which is an essential part for dance communication [20]. We suspect that the kakusei gene might also be involved in social interaction and communication among the foraging bees, linking through different molecular pathways as these behaviors were clearly observed among the foragers over the two hours of foraging. Future research on this direction will be able to give a valid comment with evidence on it. Moreover, the suggestive evidence of the present study on the role of kakusei in learning needs to be rigorously tested further, using other experiments such as proboscis extension reflex (PER), in order to draw a conclusive answer which will be carried out in our further research. Our recent study by Singh et al., 2018 [12] had demonstrated involvement of two IEGs, egr-1and hr38, and downstream genes such as ecr, dopecr, ddc, and dopr2 during daily foraging. Moreover, the role of egr-1 and hr38 in learning of food reward during foraging and memory processing was also suggested. Therefore, coordination of kakusei and the two IEGs egr-1 and hr38 during foraging behavior and learning and memory of food reward during foraging is highly assumable that would be tested in our future works. Since kakusei is a non coding gene, this study also clearly supports the dynamic role of non-coding genes in daily routine behavior.
On the other hand, earlier studies on songbird (Zebra finch) showed profound involvement of egr-1 gene in learning and memory [30]. In the egr-1 transgenic cricket (Gryllus bimaculatus), behaviorally relevant neural circuits was also visualized at cellular resolution [31]. In rodent system, several studies had revealed Egr-1 roles in synaptic plasticity and memory formation in a variety of memory systems including amygdala-dependent memory consolidation processes [32]. Moreover, the functions of hr38 in neural activity in Bombyx mori (silkmoth) and Drosophila melanogaster (fruit fly) and in honey bees were also revealed [14,33]. It is highly promising to do further research with egr-1, hr38 and kakusei using honeybees as model system for further understanding on learning and memory and the finding on honeybees will be useful across animal kingdom.
Conclusion
Previous reports have already indicated involvement of immediate early genes could be used as neural marker and their association in social behaviors such as learning and memory or memory processing. Subsequently, the importance of using immediate early genes as tool for finding molecular and cellular basis of these behaviors in honey bees had been thoroughly discussed [11,12,28,29]. Our recent report by Singh et al., has provided further evidence that the two IEGs egr-1 and hr38 are immediately and transiently expressed during honey bee foraging and further that they are involved in learning and memory processing [12]. The present finding on the recently discovered IEG kakusei is an addition to the circle, thus increasing the number of IEGs in elucidating molecular and cellular signaling underlying social behaviors, using honey bee foraging as model system. In addition to the available reports from different studies, further studies on how the three IEGs egr-1, hr38 and kakusei coordinate in completing the foraging task via their specific role in the learning, memory and their roles in communication and interaction could contribute in mapping molecular and cellular pathways to these behavior. Thereby the mechanisms of social behavior in honey bees could be opened up in more details and mapped which could be further translated to cognitively complex animals and even to human [34].
Supporting information
Acknowledgments
Dr. Ned Mantei, Department of Biology, Molecular Health Sciences, ETH Zurich, carefully and thoroughly read the manuscript. He provided valuable suggestions and advices and immensely helped in editing the manuscript. It is great honor to thank Dr. Ned Mantei for his kind and wholehearted support in making the manuscript to this form.
Dr. Axel Brockmann is gratefully acknowledged for providing the facilities in his lab to work and complete this project. Dr. Brockmann also read the manuscript and provided valuable comments. Ms. Neha Tanwar and Dr. Sophia Liyang kindly supported during experimental sample preparation.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
This work was completed with the help of Research Associate Fellowship provided by Council of Scientific and Industrial Research, Govt. of India, Award no. 09/860(0167)/2015—EMR-1 and Bridging Postdoctoral Fellowship by National Centre for Biological Sciences, TIFR, India, to Dr. Asem Surindro Singh.
References
- 1.Michelsen A (2003) "Karl von Frisch lecture. Signals and flexibility in the dance communication of honey bees". Journal of Comparative Physiology A 189:165–174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Von Frisch K (1993) The Dance Language and Orientation of Bees; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Riley J, Greggers U, Smith A, Reynolds D, Menzel R (2005) "The flight paths of honey bees recruited by the waggle dance". Nature 435:205–207. 10.1038/nature03526 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Seeley TD, Visscher PK, Passino KM (2006) "Group decision making in honey bee swarms". American Scientist 94:220–229. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Seeley TD (1995) Wisdom of the Hive. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Thorpe WH (1983) "Karl von Frisch. 20 November 1886–12 June 1982". Biographical Memoirs of Fellows of the Royal Society 29: 196–200. 10.1098/rsbm.1983.0008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Perez-Cadahia B, Drobic B, Davie JR (2011) Activation and function of immediate-early genes in the nervous system. Biochem Cell Biol 89:61–73. 10.1139/O10-138 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Beckmann AM and Wilce PA (1997) Egr transcription factors in the nervous system. Neurochem Int 31:477–510; discussion 517–6. 10.1016/s0197-0186(96)00136-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Hughes P and Dragunow M (1995) Induction of immediate-early genes and the control of neurotransmitter-regulated gene expression within the nervous system. Pharmacol Rev 47:13378. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Loebrich S and Nedivi E (2009) The function of activity-regulated genes in the nervous system. Physiol Rev 89:1079–103. 10.1152/physrev.00013.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Sommerlandt FMJ, Brockmann A, Rossler W, Spaethe J (2019) Immediate early genes in social insects: a tool to identify brain regions involved in complex behaviors and molecular processes underlying neuroplasticity. Cell Mol Life Sci 76:637–651. 10.1007/s00018-018-2948-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Singh AS, Shah A, Brockmann A (2018) Honey bee foraging induces upregulation of early growth response protein 1, hormone receptor 38 and candidate downstream genes of the ecdysteroid signalling pathway. Insect Mol Biol 27:90–98. 10.1111/imb.12350 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Khamis AM, Hamilton AR, Medvedeva YA, Alam T, Alam I, Essack M et al. (2015) Insights into the transcriptional architecture of behavioral plasticity in the honey bee Apis mellifera. Sci Rep 5:11136 10.1038/srep11136 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Yamazaki Y, Shirai K, Paul RK, Fujiyuki T, Wakamoto A, Takeuchi H et al. (2006) Differential expression of HR38 in the mushroom bodies of the honey bee brain depends on the caste and division of labor. FEBS Lett 580:2667–2670. 10.1016/j.febslet.2006.04.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Ishimoto H, Wang Z, Rao Y, Wu C-F, Kitamoto T (2013) A Novel Role for Ecdysone in Drosophila Conditioned Behavior: Linking GPCR-Mediated Noncanonical Steroid Action to cAMP Signaling in the Adult Brain. PLoS Genet 9:e1003843 10.1371/journal.pgen.1003843 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Lagisz M, Mercer AR, Mouzon C, Santos LLS, Nakagawa S (2016) Association of aminereceptor DNA sequence variants with associative learning in the honey bee. Behav Genet 46:242–251. 10.1007/s10519-015-9749-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Shah A, Jain R, Brockmann A (2018) Egr-1: a candidate transcription factor involved in molecular processes underlying timememory. Front Psychol 9:865 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00865 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Alaux C and Robinson GE (2007) Alarm pheromone induces immediate–early gene expression and slow behavioral response in honey bees. J Chem Ecol 33: 1346–1350. 10.1007/s10886-007-9301-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Lutz CC and Robinson GE (2013) Activity-dependent gene expression in honey bee mushroom bodies in response to orientation flight. J Exp Biol 216:2031–2038. 10.1242/jeb.084905 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Kiya T, Kunieda T, Kubo T (2007) Increased neural activity of a mushroom body neuron subtype in the brains of forager honey bees. PLoS One 2 e371 10.1371/journal.pone.0000371 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kiya T, Kunieda T, Kubo T (2008) Inducible- and constitutive-type transcript variants of kakusei, a novel non-coding immediate early gene, in the honey bee brain. Insect Mol Biol 17:531–536. 10.1111/j.1365-2583.2008.00821.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kiya T, Ugajin A, Kunieda T and Kubo T (2012) Identification of kakusei, a Nuclear NonCoding RNA, as an Immediate Early Gene from the Honey bee, and Its Application for Neuroethological Study. Int J Mol Sci 13:15496–15509; 10.3390/ijms131215496 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Abbott KR and Dukas R (2009) Honey bees consider flower danger in their waggle dance. Anim Behav 78: 633–635. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Bateson M, Desire S, Gartside SE, Wright GA (2011) Agitated honey bees exhibit pessimistic cognitive biases. Curr Biol 21:1070–1073. 10.1016/j.cub.2011.05.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Nieh JC (2010) A negative feedback signal that is triggered by peril curbs honey bee recruitment. Curr Biol 20:310–315. 10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.060 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Ugajin A, Kunieda T, Kubo T (2013) dentification and characterization of an Egr ortholog as a neural immediate early gene in the European honey bee (Apis mellifera L.). FEBS Lett 587: 3224–3230. 10.1016/j.febslet.2013.08.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Motulsky HJ (1999) Analyzing Data with GraphPad Prism. GraphPad Software Inc, San Diego CA, www.graphpad.com. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kiya T and Kubo T (2011) Dance Type and Flight Parameters Are Associated with Different Mushroom Body Neural Activities in Worker Honeybee Brains PLoS ONE 6(4): e19301 10.1371/journal.pone.0019301 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Singh AS (2019) Immediate Early Genes as Search Tool for Finding Cellular and Molecular Events Underlying Social Behaviors of Honeybees: A Brief Commentary. Nova Medicine and Health, 2020, Advances in Medicine and Biology, Volume 153, ISBN: 978-1-53616-487-9. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Clayton DF (2013) The genomics of memory and learning in songbirds. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet 14:45–65. 10.1146/annurev-genom-090711-163809 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Watanabe T, Ugajin A, Aonuma H, (2018) Immediate-Early Promoter-Driven Transgenic Reporter System for Neuroethological Research in a Hemimetabolous Insect. eNeuro. 10.1523/ENEURO.0061-18.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Stephanie AM, Melissa SM, and Glenn ES (2011) Early growth response gene 1 (Egr-1) is required for new and reactivated fear memories in the lateral amygdale. Learn Mem 18:24–38. 10.1101/lm.1980211 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Fujita N, Nagata Y, Nishiuchi T, Sato M, Iwami M, Kiya T (2013) Visualization of neural activity in insect brains using a conserved immediate early gene, Hr38. Curr Biol 23:2063–70 10.1016/j.cub.2013.08.051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Singh AS (2014) Genetic predominance of autism spectrum disorder and finding the risk genes. OA Autism 08;2:8. [Google Scholar]