Skip to main content
PLOS Biology logoLink to PLOS Biology
. 2020 May 12;18(5):e3000674. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000674

Thalamic, cortical, and amygdala involvement in the processing of a natural sound cue of danger

Ana G Pereira 1,¤, Matheus Farias 1, Marta A Moita 1,*
Editor: Jennifer K Bizley2
PMCID: PMC7217448  PMID: 32396574

Abstract

Animals use auditory cues generated by defensive responses of others to detect impending danger. Here we identify a neural circuit in rats involved in the detection of one such auditory cue, the cessation of movement-evoked sound resulting from freezing. This circuit comprises the dorsal subnucleus of the medial geniculate body (MGD) and downstream areas, the ventral area of the auditory cortex (VA), and the lateral amygdala (LA). This study suggests a role for the auditory offset pathway in processing a natural sound cue of threat.


When others stop and silence ensues, animals respond as if threatened. This study highlights the brain areas involved in listening to the dangerous silence.

Introduction

The use of auditory alarm cues from conspecifics is widespread across the animal kingdom [14]. Most research has focused on actively emitted signals, such as alarm calls and foot stamping [1,3]. However, auditory cues generated by movement patterns of prey can play a crucial role in predator–prey interactions. Although it is well known that predators use motion cues produced by moving prey for their detection [58], less is known about the ability of prey to use these motion cues to detect impending danger. Nevertheless, it has been established that crested pigeons use distinct wing whistles produced by conspecific escape flights [2] and rats use silence resulting from freezing as alarm cues [4]. Recently, it has also been suggested that seismic waves produced by fast running in elephants promote vigilance in conspecifics [9].

The neuronal pathways underlying defensive responses triggered by sounds have been extensively studied using classical conditioning to pure tones, sweeps, or white noise [10]. However, little is known about the mechanisms by which natural sounds (that most likely shaped the auditory system through evolution) trigger defensive responses [11,12]. Previously, we found that rats use freezing by others as a cue of danger. This is a learned cue, as it required prior experience with shock. Importantly, the cessation of the movement-evoked sound resulting from the onset of freezing was the alarm cue. In playback experiments, this cue was sufficient to trigger freezing in rats [4]. Hence, in the present study, we probed the neuronal circuits involved in the processing of natural sound cues of threat by testing the response of rats to the transition from movement-evoked sound to silence.

Results

First, we exposed individual rats to unsignaled footshocks, as prior studies found that rats previously exposed to shock but not naive ones respond to the distress of others [4,13]. The following day, we tested their freezing response to the cessation of movement-evoked sound (silence test) (Fig 1A). During the test, rats were placed in a box and allowed to explore for 3 minutes during which a speaker played the recorded sound of another rat moving [4] (see Methods). After this baseline period, the sound ceased for 1 minute (silence period). We started by looking at the role of the lateral nucleus of the amygdala (LA), broadly implicated in freezing driven by learned aversive sounds [10]. To this end, we optogenetically inactivated the LA using ArchT, a genetically encoded proton pump that hyperpolarizes neurons upon illumination with green light [14]. Importantly, the inactivation started 10 seconds before the onset of silence to encompass the transition from the movement-evoked sound to silence (Fig 1C and Methods). In this experiment, we used 3 groups: ArchT + light, in which neuronal activity is manipulated; and 2 control groups, control light (Ct light) and control ArchT (Ct ArchT), in which neuronal activity was left undisturbed (see Methods). Because there was no significant difference in freezing between the 2 control groups (S1 Fig), we combined them into a single control group. During the silence test, we found virtually no freezing during the baseline period in both ArchT + light and control animals. Silence onset led to a robust increase in freezing in the control group, whereas in the ArchT + light group, there were minimal changes in rats’ freezing behavior (Fig 1C and 1D and S1 Fig). Similar results were found when analyzing the behavior of animals in the 2 control groups separately (Wilcoxon rank sum test comparing median change in freezing of ArchT + light and Ct light groups, p = 0.003, rank sum = 33; ArchT + light and Ct ArchT groups, p = 0.001, rank sum = 28) (also see S1 Fig). This result shows that activity in the LA is necessary for the display of freezing triggered by the transition from movement-evoked sound to silence.

Fig 1. Optogenetic inactivation of the LA abolishes freezing triggered by the cessation of the movement-evoked sound.

Fig 1

(A) Schematics of the behavioral paradigm. (B) Left, image of a coronal brain section from a representative rat, in which neurons in LA expressed ArchT; right, image depicting a higher magnification of the area highlighted by the white square on the left image. Pink, cell bodies labeled with DAPI. Green, cell bodies and cellular processes of cells expressing ArchT-GFP. White arrow indicates tip of the injector. Scale bar 100 μm. (C) Movement-evoked sound (indicated by black bar) was played to rats from the ArchT + light (green, n = 7) and Control (blue, n = 15 including animals from both Ct light and Ct ArchT) groups with an interval of 1 minute of silence (between 0 and 60 seconds). Green bar indicates the period during which 556-nm light was on. Line graph shows average % of freezing throughout the test session (shaded area shows dispersion of the data given by ±SEM) (S1 Data, Sheet Fig 1). (D) Individual dots represent the change in the percentage of time each animal spends freezing during the minute immediately preceding the cessation of the movement-evoked sound and the minute of silence. Horizontal bar represents the median value of the group (ArchT + light = 0.00%, Control = 71.20%, Wilcoxon rank sum test, rank sum = 33) (S1 Data, Sheet Fig 1). ArchT, archaerhodopsin from Halorubrum strain TP009; Ct ArchT, control ArchT; Ct light, control light; DAPI, 4', 6-diamidino-2-phenylindole; GFP, green fluorescent protein; LA, lateral amygdala.

The LA receives auditory information through a direct pathway originating in several subnuclei of the medial geniculate body (MGB) of the auditory thalamus [15]. These same MGB subnuclei are also the origin of an indirect pathway that involves connections through the auditory cortex as well as other cortical regions [10,15,16]. To study the auditory pathways that convey information about the cessation of movement-evoked sound to the LA, we started by asking which of MGB’s subnuclei projecting to LA both directly and indirectly are active during exposure to the cessation of movement-evoked sound (Fig 2A). To this end, we quantified the expression of the neural activity marker c-fos in 2 groups of animals: one exposed to continuous playback of sound and another exposed to the same sound but with 2 periods of silence (see Methods). As expected, we found that rats exposed to the sound of movement with silence gaps increased freezing during the two 1-minute silence periods relative to the preceding minute (median change in freezing = 40.27%). No increase was observed when comparing the same time periods for rats in the continuous sound group (median change in freezing = −1.53%; Wilcoxon rank sum test comparing median change in freezing of silence gaps and continuous sound groups, p = 0.0303, rank sum = 53.5). The quantitative analysis of c-fos expression revealed that exposure to the sound of movement with silence gaps significantly increased the number of c-fos-positive cells in the dorsal division of the MGB (MGD) in comparison to continuous sound exposure (Fig 2B). In addition, we found that the number of c-fos-positive cells increased from anterior to posterior regions of the MGD (Fig 2D). We also observe that the average number of c-fos-labeled cells in the MGD of animals exposed to the sound of movement with silence gaps was quite variable. This variability is unlikely to result from differences in anatomic distribution of c-fos-positive cells within this subnucleus (such as proximity to other regions in the MGB that may also respond to sound offset), because we systematically probed the same region of interest within it (see Methods).

Fig 2. Differential activity in the MGD triggered by the cessation of movement-evoked sound.

Fig 2

(A) Schematics of the MGB and its subnuclei. Regions circled in blue are part of both the mono- and polysynaptic pathway to the LA. (B) Average number of c-fos-positive cells in the MGD and MGM, the SG, and the PIL of individual rats from the continuous sound (light gray n = 4) and silence gaps (dark gray n = 7) group. The horizontal bar represents the median value, and Wilcoxon rank sum test was used for comparisons (MGD continuous sound = 5.87, silence gaps = 18.7, rank sum = 11; MGM continuous sound = 5.54, silence gaps = 10.50, rank sum = 21; SG continuous sound = 3.36, silence gaps = 5.95, rank sum = 18; PIL continuous sound = 3.33, silence gaps = 4.07, rank sum = 17) (S1 Data, Sheet Fig 2). (C) Representative pictures of c-fos-labeled cells in MGD. Scale bar 100 μm. (D) Line graph showing average number of c-fos-labeled cells in the MGD along the anterior-posterior axis for continuous sound and silence groups. Vertical bars represent ±SEM (n-way ANOVA main effect repeated measures AP axis p = 0.001, F = 12.69; silence versus continuous sound effect p = 0.01, F = 7.71) (S1 Data, Sheet Fig 2). LA, lateral amygdala; MGB, medial geniculate body; MGD, dorsal division of the MGB; MGM, medial division of the MGB; MGV, ventral division of the MGB; PIL, posterior intralaminar thalamic nucleus; PP, peripeduncular nucleus; SG, suprageniculate nucleus.

Our findings show that the MGD is the only subnucleus of the auditory thalamus projecting directly to LA that is more active when rats are exposed to the sound of movement with silence gaps. Prior electrophysiology studies in rodents report the presence of cells with sound offset responses throughout the MGB, with particular prevalence in the MGD (with the highest incidence in the caudal part) and the marginal zone, or shell, of the ventral division of the MGB (MGV) [1720]. This suggests that c-fos-positive cells in the MGD may have been activated by the offset of the sound of movement when silence gaps are introduced. In agreement with this hypothesis, we also observed the presence of robust c-fos labeling in the ventral shell of the MGV, in particular, in more rostral areas (see S2 Fig). Alternatively, c-fos expression may reflect a response to an increase in stimulus saliency resulting from the introduction of silence gaps in the background sound. However, as we failed to see significant changes in the other MG subnuclei studied, this alternative explanation would imply that the MDG is the subnucleus with strongest sensitivity to changes in saliency. To our knowledge, there is no evidence in this direction. Future experiments in which the activity of MGD neurons is recorded during exposure to sound of movement with either silence gaps or other changes in saliency are necessary to definitively disambiguate between the 2 possibilities.

Next, we tested whether MGD activity is required for silence-triggered freezing by optogenetically inactivating this area (Fig 3A). We found that freezing was greatly reduced upon MGD inactivation relative to controls (Fig 3C and 3D). When analyzing the behavior of animals in the 2 control groups separately, the difference between ArchT + light and Ct ArchT was again significant; however, the difference relative to Ct light failed to reach significance (Wilcoxon rank sum test comparing median change in freezing of ArchT + light and Ct light groups, p = 0.07, rank sum = 40; ArchT + light and Ct ArchT groups, p = 0.017, rank sum = 31) (also see S3 Fig). However, if outliers are removed, then all comparisons reveal significant differences across groups (Wilcoxon rank sum test comparing median change in freezing of ArchT + light and Ct light groups, p = 0.038, rank sum = 27; ArchT + light and Ct ArchT groups, p = 0.0043, rank sum = 21). Several studies addressed the anatomical and physiological properties of the MGD [15,1719,2123]. However, to our knowledge, the contribution of this subnucleus to behavior remained untested. Therefore, we asked whether the MGD is required for freezing in response to a conditioned tone, as are other MGB subnuclei that project to LA [24]. To this end, we conditioned the same rats used in the previous experiment with 3 tone–shock pairings. The following day, we tested their response to the conditioned tone while optogenetically inhibiting the MGD (Fig 3A). Inactivation of the MGD did not affect the expression of freezing triggered by the conditioned pure tone (Fig 3E). To better compare the role of the MGD in freezing triggered by a conditioned tone or the cessation of movement-evoked sound, we analyzed the increase in freezing during the initial 15 seconds of the stimulus (silence gap or tone) in both tests. With this analysis, we diminish potential confounds related with differences in the duration of the stimulus and MGD inactivation (60-second silence and 75-second inactivation versus 15-second tone and 30-second inactivation). We found that after 15 seconds of exposure to either the silence gap or the conditioned stimulus, control animals show a significant increase in freezing (S4 Fig). In contrast, when MGD is inactivated (ArchT + light group), rats increased freezing upon the 15-second conditioned tone but not to the first 15 seconds of the silence gap (S4 Fig). Moreover, the same animals (ArchT + light) increase their freezing significantly more during the tone test than during the silence test, despite optogenetic inactivation of the MGD in both cases (S4 Fig). These results show that activity in the MGD is necessary for the display of freezing triggered by the transition from movement-evoked sound to silence but not that triggered by a conditioned pure tone. In line with physiological data, this indicates that the MGD may be preferentially recruited to process the offset of sound [17,18,20]. Whether it is the offset cells in MGD that directly or indirectly drive activity in LA, leading to the expression of freezing, remains to be established. Furthermore, even though our manipulation affected mostly neurons in MGD, we cannot exclude the possibility that we have silence some neurons in suprageniculate nucleus (SG) and the dorsal region of the MGV (S3 Fig).

Fig 3. Activity in the MGD is necessary for freezing triggered by the cessation of movement-evoked sound but not by a conditioned tone cue.

Fig 3

(A) Schematics of the behavioral paradigm used for testing the role of the MGD in freezing triggered by the cessation of movement-evoked sound and 5-kHz pure tone. (B) Top left, coronal brain section from a representative rat, in which MGD neurons express ArchT; top right, image with a higher magnification of the area highlighted by the white square on the left image (see S1 Movie for detailed anatomy). Bottom, axon terminals in the tip of LA originating from ArchT-expressing cells in MGD. Pink, cell bodies labeled with DAPI. Green, cell bodies and cellular processes of cells expressing ArchT-GFP. Scale bar 100 μm. (C and D) Same as Fig 1C and 1D (respectively) for rats expressing ArchT and exposed to light illumination in the MGD (green, n = 7) and respective controls (blue, n = 13 including animals from both Ct light and Ct ArchT groups). Horizontal bar represents the median value (ArchT + light = 7.73%, Control = 51.20%, Wilcoxon rank sum test, rank sum = 43) (S1 Data, Sheet Fig 3). (E) Individual value plot displaying the change in the percentage of time animals spent freezing during the 15 seconds immediately preceding the presentation of the 5-kHz conditioned tone and the 15 seconds during which the tone was played. Horizontal bar represents the median value (ArchT + light = 73.33%; Control = 40.00%; Wilcoxon rank sum test, rank sum = 88.5) (S1 Data, Sheet Fig 3). ArchT, archaerhodopsin from Halorubrum strain TP009; Ct ArchT, control ArchT; Ct light, control light; LA, lateral amygdala; MGB, medial geniculate body; MGD, dorsal division of the MGB; DAPI, 4', 6-diamidino-2-phenylindole; GFP, green fluorescent protein.

The MGD sends direct projections to the LA [15] but also projects to discrete areas in the cortex that through polysynaptic connections can convey information to the LA. To test whether auditory processing areas downstream of the MGD are necessary for the response to the cessation of the movement-evoked sound, we focused on the 2 regions in the temporal cortex to which MGD axons project: posterodorsal (PD) and ventral area (VA) [21,25]. We inactivated PD or VA using a pharmacological approach (S5 Fig). Briefly, rats with bilateral cannulas targeting PD or VA received a microinjection of the GABA agonist muscimol, just prior to the silence test (see Methods). At the end of the experiment, animals received an injection of the retrograde tracer cholera toxin B (CTB), which allowed confirmation that the inactivated cortical region received input from MGD (Fig 4C and 4F). Inactivation of PD with muscimol did not significantly decrease freezing triggered by silence in comparison with control rats (Fig 4A and 4B). Inactivating VA, however, strongly impaired freezing during the silence period (Fig 4D and 4E). These results show that activity in VA, but not PD, is necessary for freezing triggered by the cessation of movement-evoked sound.

Fig 4. The VA but not the PD region of the auditory cortex is necessary for freezing triggered by silence.

Fig 4

(A, B) Same as Fig 1C and 1D, (respectively) but for rats with bilateral injection of muscimol (dark purple, n = 7) or PBS (light purple, n = 8) in the area PD. Horizontal bar represents the median value of the group (muscimol = 87.87%, PBS = 30.73%, Wilcoxon rank sum test, rank sum = 54). (S1 Data, Sheet Fig 4). (C) Top, coronal brain section from a representative rat in which MGD cells are labeled with CTB injected in the PD. Bottom, image with higher magnification of the area highlighted by the white square on the top image. Pink, cell bodies labeled with DAPI. Yellow, cell bodies of CTB-positive cells. (D, E) Same as (A, B) (respectively), but for rats with bilateral injections of muscimol (dark purple, n = 6) or PBS (light purple, n = 6) in VA. Horizontal bar represents the median value (muscimol = 0.00%, PBS = 86.07%, Wilcoxon rank sum test, rank sum = 57). (F) Same as (C) but for CTB injections in the VA. Scale bar 100 μm. (S1 Data, Sheet Fig 4). ArchT, archaerhodopsin from Halorubrum strain TP009; Ct ArchT, control ArchT; Ct light, control light; CTB, cholera toxin subunit B; DAPI, 4', 6-diamidino-2-phenylindole; LA, lateral amygdala; MGB, medial geniculate body; MGD, dorsal division of the MGD; PBS, phosphate buffered saline; PD, posterodorsal; VA, ventral area.

Discussion

Together, our results suggest that the MGD, VA, and LA form a network important for processing the transition from movement-generated sound to silence. Freezing triggered by the cessation of movement sound is likely to rely on cells with sound offset responses, although further experiments are necessary to confirm this hypothesis. Reports demonstrating the presence of sound offset neurons in the MGD and their output regions in the auditory cortex [1720,2628] support our proposed network. Indeed, a growing body of work has identified neurons with sound offset responses throughout the auditory system. Particularly relevant to this study is the presence of offset cells in the marginal zone of the MGV, including its dorsal region, which might have been affected by our inactivation experiments [18]. These reports suggest the existence of an “offset pathway” thought to be of major importance in processing speech, sound localization, and movement detection [29]. Importantly, in most of these studies, the offset responses are transient, lasting a fraction of a second, whereas in our study, freezing lasts for several seconds. One possibility is that cells with offset responses act as a trigger and downstream targets drive the long-lasting defensive responses. Alternatively, prior exposure to shock, a necessary condition for rats to freeze upon silence onset, may lead to plastic changes that mediate a change from transient to sustained sound offset responses in any of the identified brain regions. Although we identified 2 input stations to the LA, there are multiple scenarios by which information may flow in this circuit. One scenario is that of a feed-forward circuit whereby MGD drives VA, which, in turn, activates LA. This activation is probably indirect because evidence for direct projections from the VA to the amygdala is lacking [30] (but see [16]). Alternatively, feedback loops within the identified circuit may contribute to silence-triggered freezing. VA neurons project back to the MGD, allowing for top-down enhancement of relevant stimuli, possibly instructed by the amygdala. Indeed, neurons in LA that project back to primary auditory cortex are required for the expression of freezing to conditioned pure tones [31]. A similar feedback loop has been proposed for the encoding of behavioral relevance or context of visual stimuli, whereby the LA would contribute to the formation of neuronal ensembles in visual cortex that encode stimulus relevance, independent of ensembles that encode visual stimulus identity [32].

The current study brings new insights into the mechanisms by which prey animals use the movement of others to infer danger, advancing our knowledge regarding how defense circuits respond to natural sound cues. By describing a pathway that includes auditory regions that have not been previously implicated in defense responses, it expands the network that processes auditory cues in the context of danger. More broadly, this study sets the stage to further our understanding of how sensory stimuli and their behavioral relevance are encoded by sensory systems and brain regions involved in motivated survival behaviors.

Methods

Ethical statement

The Champalimaud Center for the Unknown follows the National Law (Decreto-Lei 113/2013), European Directive 2010/63, and Federation of European Laboratory Animal Science Associations (FELASA) guidelines and recommendations concerning laboratory animal experimentation and welfare. The experiments in this study were approved under the protocol ID 014304.

Animal care

Naive male Sprague Dawley rats (300–350 g) for c-fos experiments and 200–250 g for optogenetic and muscimol experiments were obtained from a commercial supplier (Harlan and Charles Rivers, respectively). After arrival, animals were pair housed in Plexiglas top filtered cages and maintained on a 12-hour light/dark cycle with ad libitum access to food and water. All behavioral procedures were performed during the light phase of the cycle.

For the c-fos experiments, animals were kept in pairs and acclimated for at least 1 week before experimental manipulation. For optogenetic experiments, animals were separated 4–6 days after arrival and subjected to virus injection and/or optic fiber implantation surgery. After this procedure, animals were kept alone in Plexiglas boxes before experimental manipulation. For muscimol experiments, animals were separated on the fourth day after arrival and subjected to cannula implantation surgery. After the procedure, animals were kept alone for 7 days before experimental manipulations.

Viral vector, neuronal tracer, and muscimol

The viral vector Adeno-associated virus expressing ArchT (AAV2/5 CAG-ArchT-GFP 1.3 × x1012 vg/mL) was produced by and purchased to University of North Carolina (UNC) vector core facility. The viral vector was diluted with sterile phosphate buffered saline (PBS) immediately before injection to obtain a concentration of 4 × 1011 vg/mL.

The transynaptic neuronal tracer Cholera Toxin subunit B Alexa Fluor 555 Conjugate (CTB Alexa 555, 1 mg/mL) was produced by and purchased from Life Technologies. The GABA agonist muscimol (Cat# 2763-96-4) was bought from Sigma-Aldrich.

Stereotactic surgery

Animals were anaesthetized with isoflurane (Vetflurane 1,000 mg/g, Virbac) and placed in a stereotaxic apparatus (David Kopf Instruments). Small craniotomies were made using standard aseptic techniques.

For the LA inhibition experiments, animals assigned to the ArchT + light and Ct ArchT groups received injections targeted bilaterally to the LA (stereotaxic coordinates from Bregma, anterior–posterior: −3.3 mm, dorsal–ventral: −8.1, medial–lateral: 5.2 mm; derived from Paxinos and Watson 2007 [33]) using stainless steel guide cannula (24 gauge; Plastics One). Following cannula guide placement, 0.3–0.4 μL injections of rAAV2/5-CAG-ArchT-GFP were made through a stainless steel injection cannula (31 gauge; Plastics One), attached to a Hamilton syringe via polyethylene tubing. Injections were controlled through an automatic pump (PHD 2000; Harvard Apparatus). For animals assigned to the ArchT + light group optical fibers (200-μm, 0.37 numerical aperture, Doric Lenses) were implanted in the LA (stereotaxic coordinates from Bregma, anterior–posterior: −3.3 mm, dorsal–ventral: −8.15 or 8.05 mm, medial–lateral: 5.2 mm [33]) and affixed to the skull using stainless steel mounting screws (Plastics One) and dental cement (TAB 2000, Kerr). No fibers were implanted in animals assigned to the Ct ArchT group. Rats were kept for 4 weeks before any behavioral manipulation to allow maximal expression of the virus. Animals of the Ct light group were subjected to the same procedure, including implanting optical fibers, but no virus was injected.

For the dorsal medial geniculate nucleus (MGD) inhibition experiments, the procedures were similar, but injections were targeted bilaterally to the MGD (stereotaxic coordinates from Bregma, anterior–posterior: −5.8 mm, dorsal–ventral: −5.3, medial–lateral: ±3.4 mm [33]) and 0.2 μL of rAAV2/5-CAG-ArchT-GFP was injected.

For the VA and PD inhibition experiments, animals were subjected to a stereotaxic surgery to bilaterally implant stainless steel guide cannulas (24 gauge, Plastics One) in either the PD (stereotaxic coordinates from Bregma, anterior–posterior: −5.7 mm, dorsal–ventral: −4.3 mm, and medial–lateral: +6.5 mm [33]) or VA (stereotaxic coordinates from Bregma, anterior–posterior: −3.5 mm, dorsal–ventral: −6.5 mm, medial–lateral: +6.5 mm [33]) areas of the auditory cortex. Following cannula implantation, a dummy cannula (31 gauge, Plastics One) was kept until test day.

For the retrograde tracing of auditory thalamus projections to these regions (used to confirm that the area that was inactivated indeed received input from the MGD), we took advantage of the already implanted guide cannulas, and posterior to the test day, CTB Alexa 555 (0.2 μL) was bilaterally injected in half of the experimental animals following the same protocol as the one used to inject the viral vector. Animals were humanely killed days after this procedure to allow sufficient transport of the tracer.

All injection sites and fiber placements were verified histologically, and rats were excluded if either were not on the correct place.

Behavioral apparatus

Four distinct environments were used in this study: conditioning box, test box–silence test, tone conditioning box, and test box–tone test.

Both conditioning boxes (model H10-11R-TC, Coulbourn Instruments) have a shock floor of metal bars (model H10-11R-TC-SF, Coulbourn Instruments) and are placed inside a sound isolation chamber (Action Automation and Controls). In these boxes, a precision programmable shocker (model H13-16, Coulbourn Instruments) delivered the footshocks, and tones were produced by a sound generator (RM1, Tucker-Davis Technologies) and delivered through a horn tweeter (model TL16H8OHM, VISATON).

The test box–silence test consisted of a 2-partition chamber made of clear Plexiglas walls (Gravoplot) divided in 2 equal halves, placed inside a sound attenuation chamber. The test box–tone test consisted of a plastic box with a round platform in the bottom. Both test boxes had bedding on the floor. Tones were generated, delivered, and calibrated using the same apparatus as the conditioning boxes. To minimize generalization between environments, conditioning with shock (day 1) took place with light, and silence test (day 2) took place in the dark (Fig 1A). For tone conditioning experiments, tone conditioning took place in the dark, and recall took place under dim yellow light. Rats’ behavior was tracked by a video camera mounted on the ceiling of the attenuating cubicle. A surveillance video acquisition system was used to record and store all videos on hard disk.

Behavioral procedures

All rats were preexposed to the different boxes prior to the beginning of the experiments. To test the role of auditory motion cues, the previously recorded movement-evoked sound [4] was played during exposure to the test box–silence test and during the silence test. To generate this sound, 1 single rat was recorded as it moved around in 1 of the partitions of the social interaction box with bedding on the floor. The sound of this rat moving around was recorded through a microphone placed over the chamber (Avisoft-UltraSoundGate system 416H, microphone model CM16/CMPA). This allowed recording of ultrasounds. Sections of the recording without vocalizations were chosen for the playback.

Optogenetic experiments

For the silence test experiments, rats were trained and tested as previously described [4]. During the test session, a fiber-optic cable terminating in 2 ferrules (Branching Fiber-optic 200 μm, 0.22 numerical aperture, Doric lenses) was connected to the chronically implanted optic fibers. Animals in the ArchT + light and Ct light groups received laser illumination (estimated 30 mW at the tip of the fiber) that started 10 seconds before the silence. Illumination lasted until 5 seconds after the resumption of the playback of the movement-evoked sound. After the test session, animals returned to their home cages.

To test the role of the MGD in recall after tone conditioning, the same animals that were tested to the silence were exposed to the tone conditioning box and test box–tone test during the following 4 days of experiment. On the fifth day, after a 5-minute baseline, animals received 3 tone–shock pairings (5-kHz tone, 70 dB, 15 seconds coterminating with 1-mA shock, 0.5 seconds). Animals were tested to the sound the day after in the test box–tone test, in which animals were exposed to 3-tone presentation, (5-kHz tone, 70 dB, 15 seconds). Laser illumination was identical to the one used in LA experiments.

c-fos experiments

Experiments followed a modified protocol of the silence test experiments (described previously), in which the test session lasted for 9 minutes. In these experiments, animals in the silence group were exposed to 2 periods of silence with the duration of 1 minute each (and with a period of 3 minutes separation), whereas for the control group (continuous sound), the recorded sound of movement was played throughout the entire test session.

Muscimol experiments

Experiments followed the same protocol as the silence test experiments; however, in the test day, animals were injected with 0.2 μl muscimol (0.5 μg/μl concentration at 0.1 μl/min rate) in either the VA or PD. The injections were performed with the help of an injection cannula, attached to a Hamilton syringe controlled by an automatic pump (PHD 2000; Harvard Apparatus). Animals were left undisturbed for a period of 1 hour and 30 minutes after which they were tested.

Histology

Animals were deeply anesthetized with pentobarbital (600 mg/kg, i.p.) and transcardially perfused with PBS (0.01 M), followed by ice-cold 4% paraformaldehyde (Paraformaldehyde Granular; cat#19210; Electron Microscopy Sciences) in 0.1 M phosphate buffer (PB) (PFA-PB). Brains were removed and postfixed in 4% PFA-PB and kept at 4°C.

Coronal sections containing the LA (2.50 mm to 3.80 mm posterior to Bregma), VA (3.20 mm to 4.60 mm posterior to Bregma), PD (5.40 mm to 6.7 mm posterior to Bregma), and/or the MGB of the auditory thalamus (5.60 mm to 6.50 mm posterior to Bregma) were cut and mounted.

Mounted slices were rehydrated for 10 minutes with PBS and 500 μl of a 1:1,000 diluted 4′, 6-diamidino-2-phenylindole (DAPI) solution (D9542, Sigma-Aldrich) was applied to each slide and incubated for 20 minutes in a shaker (10 rpm). Slices were then washed 3 times with PBS and rinsed with distilled water. Slices were coverslipped with Moviol 4–88 (81381, Sigma-Aldrich).

Fluorescent images were taken with the confocal microscope Zeiss LSM 710, Axioimager2, Objective Plan-Apochromat 20x/0.8M27, and software Zen 2010. Pictures were processed in ImageJ for compiling z-stacks, and exposure adjustments (same adjustment for both channels) were made with Photoshop.

Immunohistochemistry experiments examining c-fos expression in the MGB were performed in brain slices from animals that were rapidly and deeply anaesthetized with pentobarbital (600 mg/kg, i.p.) 2 hours after the beginning of the behavioral paradigm. While anesthetized, animals were transcardially perfused with PBS, followed by ice-cold 4% PFA-PB. Brains were removed and postfixed in 4% PFA-PB for 24 hours and subsequently cryoprotected in 20% glycerol (J.T.Baker) in 0.1 M PB for 72 hours at 4°C. Using a sliding microtome, sections of 40 μm containing the auditory thalamus (5.40 to 6.40 posterior to Bregma) were cut and collected in PBS. Next, sections were transferred to a 0.1% sodium azide (Sigma-Aldrich) in PBS solution for storage. The immunohistochemical staining was performed simultaneously for all brain sections analyzed. Staining was performed in free-floating sections. Sections were washed 3× for 10 minutes with PBS, incubated for 10 min with 0.9% H2O2 (Sigma-Aldrich), washed again 3×10 minutes in PBS and blocked in PBS with 1% bovine serum albumin (BSA) (cat#A7906, Sigma-Aldrich) and 0.1% Triton X-100 for 1 hour at RT. Slices were then incubated O.N. RT with anti c-fos 1ry AB (1:500; rabbit; sc-52 Santa Cruz Biotechnology) in PBS with 1% BSA and 0.1% Triton X-100. The next morning, sections were washed 3× for 10 minutes with PBS and incubated with goat anti-rabbit biotinylated 2ry antibody (1:1,000; Cat#405008, Southern Biotec) in PBS with 1% BSA and 0.1% Triton X-100 for 1 hour at RT. Sections were washed 3× for 10 minutes in PBS, incubated with horseradish peroxidase streptavidin (1:300; cat#SA-5004; Vector Laboratories) in PBS with 0.2% Triton X-100 for 1 hour RT, washed 3× for 10 minutes in PBS-B, and developed in diaminobenzidine tablets (DAB) (cat# D4418-50 SET; Sigma) for 3 minutes.

Sections were mounted on electrostatic slides, air dried, dehydrated in ethanol and xylenes, and coverslipped with DPX. Bright-field images were taken in Zeiss Axioimager M1 microscope equipped with a CCD camera (Hamamatsu C8484), with objective 20×/0.80. Sections from comparable anterior-posterior levels were selected for scoring, and images from each subnucleus were taken systematically from the same area based on distance from reference points specific for each anterior-posterior level. Cell counts were scored using NIH Image J. For the initial analysis, cell counts for each subnucleus of the thalamus were averaged into a single score for each rat. For the analysis along the anterior-posterior axis of the MGD, we divided the sections in anterior (sections including and posterior to 5.64 until 5.76 [including] relative to Bregma), medial (sections posterior to 5.76 until 6.00 [including] posterior to Bregma), and posterior (sections posterior to 6.00 until 6.24 [including] relative to Bregma) and averaged the c-fos—labeling cells in those slices.

Analysis of behavioral data

Animals’ freezing behavior during the silence test was automatically scored using FreezeScan from Clever Sys. Baseline freezing levels were calculated using the median percentage of freezing during the 60 seconds preceding the onset of silence. To avoid confounding factors, such as freezing, triggered by other cues that were not the onset of silence, animals were considered outliers and thus excluded if their baseline freezing was higher than the third quartile + 1.5 (third quartile–first quartile) baseline freezing of the population used in each experiment (LA, MGD, VA, and PD experiments were considered separately). Because of the noise generated by the shutter used in the optogenetics experiments, animals that were freezing more than 50% in the 10 seconds period between the opening of the shutter and silence onset were also excluded.

Statistical analysis

For data analysis of the silence test behavioral experiments, we focused on the percentage of freezing during the silence gaps (that lasted 1 minute) and used as baseline the minute immediately preceding the silence interval. For data analysis of the tone test behavioral experiments, we analyzed data in a similar way but focused on the period of tone presentation (that lasted 15 seconds) and used as baseline the 15 seconds immediately preceding it. In this manner, we ensure that both measures have the same sampling time.

A Shapiro–Wilk test was used to access the normality of our data. The behavioral data were, in general, not normally distributed, and sample sizes were small, so we used nonparametric tests only.

For comparisons between groups (when comparing the change in freezing between baseline and silence periods), we used a Wilcoxon–Mann–Whitney test. For comparisons within group (comparing percentages of freezing during baseline and silence), we conducted a Wilcoxon signed-ranked test. For the c-fos behavioral data, we averaged the percentage of freezing during the 2 periods of 1 minute preceding the silence inserts and the 2 periods of silence. For comparisons within the group (silence or continuous sound), we conducted a Wilcoxon signed-ranked test. For comparisons between groups of the c-fos-labeled cells, our data were not normally distributed, and therefore, we used a Mann–Whitney test. For analyzing the effect of position along the anterior-posterior axis and exposure to silence in the expression of c-fos, we conducted a 2-way ANOVA for unbalanced design given that our data were normally distributed. Statistical analysis was performed using MATLab.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. The LA is necessary for the expression of freezing driven by the cessation of movement-evoked sound.

(A) Coronal slices representing fiber placements and spread of virus expression for ArchT + light group. (B) Fiber placement and/or injection site for the Ct light (light blue) and Ct ArchT (dark blue) groups. (C) Same as Fig 1D, but for animals of the 2 control groups. Horizontal bar represents the median value of the group (Ct light = 66.13%, Ct ArchT = 73.67%, Wilcoxon rank sum test, rank sum = 72) (S1 Data, Sheet S1 Fig). (D–F) Line graph showing average time spent freezing during the minute immediately preceding the cessation of the movement-evoked sound (baseline) and the minute of silence for each rat of the ArchT + light (n = 7), Ct light (n = 9), and Ct ArchT (n = 6) groups. Wilcoxon signed rank test. Baseline versus silence ArchT + light signedrank = 1; Ct light signedrank = 1; Ct ArchT signedrank = 0. (S1 Data, Sheet S1 Fig). ArchT, archaerhodopsin from Halorubrum strain TP009; Ct light, control light; Ct ArchT, control ArchT; LA, lateral amygdala.

(TIF)

S2 Fig. Representative pictures of c-fos-labeled cells in MGB.

Representative images of coronal slices from rats exposed to silence gaps showing c-fos labeling in different subnuclei of the MGB. In particular, we observe c-fos-positive cells in the shell of the MGV, an area particularly responsive to the offset of sounds. c-fos-positive cells in this region were not quantified because it does not project directly to the LA. LA, lateral amygdala; MGB, medial geniculate body; MGV, ventral division of the MGB.

(TIF)

S3 Fig. The MGD is necessary for the expression of freezing driven by the cessation of the movement-evoked sound but not by a discrete auditory cue.

(A) Coronal slices representing fiber placements and spread of virus expression for ArchT + light group. (B) Fiber placement and/or injection site for the Ct light (light blue) and Ct ArchT (dark blue) groups. (C) Representative image of cells in MGD expressing ArchT-GFP. (D) Same as Fig 3D but for animals of the 2 control groups. Horizontal bar represents the median value (Ct light = 49.47%, Ct ArchT = 68.93%, Wilcoxon rank sum test, rank sum = 48) (S1 Data, Sheet S3 Fig). (E–G) Line graph showing average time spent freezing during the minute immediately preceding the cessation of the movement-evoked sound (baseline) and the minute of silence for each rat of the ArchT + light (n = 7), Ct light (n = 8), and Ct ArchT (n = 5) groups with surgeries targeting the MGD. Wilcoxon signed rank test, baseline versus silence ArchT + light signedrank = 0; Ct light signedrank = 1; Ct ArchT signedrank = 0. (S1 Data, Sheet S3 Fig). ArchT, archaerhodopsin from Halorubrum strain TP009; Ct light, control light; Ct ArchT, control Arch T; GFP, green fluorescent protein; LA, lateral amygdala; MGB, medial geniculate body; MGD, dorsal division of the MGB.

(TIF)

S4 Fig. Effect of MGD inactivation during the first 15 seconds of the silence test in comparison with tone test.

(A) Line graph showing average time spent freezing during the 15 seconds immediately preceding the cessation of the movement-evoked sound or tone (baseline) and the 15 seconds of stimulus (silence or tone) for each rat of the ArchT + light (n = 7) and Control (n = 13) groups. Wilcoxon signed rank test, baseline versus silence ArchT + light signedrank = 0; Control signedrank = 1; baseline versus tone ArchT + light signedrank = 0; Control signedrank = 0 (S1 Data, Sheet S4 Fig). (B) Individual dots represent the change in the percentage of time each animal spends freezing during the 15 seconds immediately preceding the cessation of the movement-evoked sound or tone (baseline) and the 15 seconds of stimulus (silence or tone) for each rat of the ArchT + light (n = 7) and Control (n = 13) groups. Horizontal bar represents the median value of the group (Silence test ArchT + light = 0.00%, Control = 37.07%; Tone test ArchT + light = 73.33%, Control = 40%). Kruskal–Wallis test chi-squared = 8 (S1 Data, Sheet S4 Fig) (S1 Data, Sheet S4 Fig). ArchT, archaerhodopsin from Halorubrum strain TP009; MGB, medial geniculate body; MGD, dorsal division of the MGB.

(TIF)

S5 Fig

Coronal slices representing injection site for PBS (light purple) and muscimol (dark purple) in (A) PD and (B) VA. Areas shaded in gray correspond to PD and VA (respectively). PBS, phosphate buffered saline; PD, posterodorsal; VA, ventral area.

(TIF)

S1 Data. Raw data file.

(XLSX)

S1 Movie. Neurons in MGD expressing ArchT-GFP.

ArchT, archaerhodopsin from Halorubrum strain TP009; GFP, green fluorescent protein; MGD, dorsal division of the medial geniculate body.

(MP4)

Acknowledgments

We thank Alexandra Medeiros and Andreia Cruz for their input to the project and Alexandra in particular for her effort in the histological characterization of the MGD. We also want to thank all the members of the Behavioral Neuroscience lab, as well as Samuel Walker and Tiago Marques, for comments on the manuscript. We thank Glenn Schafe and Stephanie Maddox for the training in immunohistochemical techniques to detect the expression of immediate early genes. This work was developed with the support from the Champalimaud Histopathology platform and the research infrastructure Congento LISBOA-01-0145-FEDER-022170.

Abbreviations

ArchT

archaerhodopsin from Halorubrum strain TP009

Ct light

control light

Ct ArchT

control ArchT

CTB

cholera toxin B

DAPI

4', 6-diamidino-2-phenylindole

GFP

green fluorescent protein

LA

lateral amygdala

MGB

medial geniculate body

MGD

dorsal division of the MGB

MGV

ventral division of the MGB

MGM

medial division of the MGB

PBS

phosphate buffered saline

PD

posterodorsal area

PIL

posterior intralaminar thalamic nucleus

PP

peripeduncular nucleus

SG

suprageniculate nucleus

VA

ventral area

Data Availability

All relevant data will be within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

This work was developed with the financial support of the Champalimaud Foundation (https://m.fchampalimaud.org/en/champalimaud-research/); support to the Champalimaud Vivarium as part of the research infrastructure Congento, co-financed by Lisboa Regional Operational Programme (Lisboa2020), under the PORTUGAL 2020 Partnership Agreement, through the European Regional Development Fund (ERDF) and Fundação para a Ciência e Tecnologia (Portugal) under the project LISBOA-01-0145-FEDER-022170; MAM was supported by the European Research Council (ERC-2013-StG-337747 "C.o.C.O."); AP was supported by Fundação para a Ciência e Tecnologia (grant SFRH/BD/33943/2009). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Holle LI, Radford AN. The development of alarm call behaviour in mammals and birds. Anim Behav. 2009;78(4):791–800. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.07.021 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Murray TG, Zeil J, Magrath RD. Sounds of Modified Flight Feathers Reliably Signal Danger in a Pigeon. Curr Biol. 2017;27(22):3520–5. 10.1016/j.cub.2017.09.068 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Randall JA, Rogovin KA, Shier DM. Antipredator behavior of a social desert rodent: footdrumming and alarm calling in the great gerbil, Rhombomys opiums. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2000;48(2):110–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Pereira AG, Cruz A, Lima SQ, Moita MA. Silence resulting from the cessation of movement signals danger. Curr Biol. 2012;22(16):627–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Catania KC, Hare JF, Campbell KL. Water shrews detect movement, shape, and smell to find prey underwater. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2008;105(2):571–6. 10.1073/pnas.0709534104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Carr CE, Christensen-Dalsgaard J. Sound localization strategies in three predators. Brain Behav Evol. 2015;86:17–27. 10.1159/000435946 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Friedel P, Young BA, Van Hemmen JL. Auditory localization of ground-borne vibrations in snakes. Phys Rev Lett. 2008;100(4):2–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Zhao Z, Chen Z, Xiang X, Hu M, Xie H, Jia X, et al. Zona incerta GABAergic neurons integrate prey-related sensory signals and induce an appetitive drive to promote hunting. Nat Neurosci. 2019;22(6):921–32. Available from: 10.1038/s41593-019-0404-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Mortimer B, Rees WL, Koelemeijer P, Nissen-meyer T. Classifying elephant behaviour through seismic vibrations. Curr Biol. 2018;28(9):R547–8. Available from: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.03.062 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Herry C, Johansen JP. Encoding of fear learning and memory in distributed neuronal circuits. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17(12):1644–54. 10.1038/nn.3869 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Theunissen FE, Elie JE. Neural processing of natural sounds. Nat Rev. 2014;15(June):355–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Moczulska KE, Tinter-Thiede J, Peter M, Ushakova L, Wernle T, Bathellier B, et al. Dynamics of dendritic spines in the mouse auditory cortex during memory formation and memory recall. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2013;110(45):18315–20. 10.1073/pnas.1312508110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Atsak P, Orre M, Bakker P, Cerliani L, Roozendaal B, Gazzola V, et al. Experience modulates vicarious freezing in rats: A model for empathy. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(7). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Wiegert JS, Mahn M, Prigge M, Printz Y, Yizhar O. Silencing Neurons: Tools, Applications, and Experimental Constraints. Neuron. 2017;95(3):504–29. Available from: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Doron NN, Ledoux JE. Cells in the posterior thalamus project to both amygdala and temporal cortex: A quantitative retrograde double-labeling study in the rat. J Comp Neurol. 2000;425(2):257–74. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Romanski L, LeDoux J. Information Cascade from Primary Auditory Cortex to the Amygdala: Corticocortical and Corticoamygdaloid Projections of the Temporal Cortex in the Rat. Cereb Cortex. 2006;515–32. Available from: http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/abstract/3/6/515 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Bordi F, LeDoux JE. Response properties of single units in areas of rat auditory thalamus that project to the amygdala. I. Acoustic discharge patterns and frequency receptive fields. Exp brain Res. 1994;98(2):261–274. 10.1007/bf00228414 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.He J. ON and OFF Pathways Segregated at the Auditory Thalamus of the Guinea Pig. J Neurosci. 2001;21(21):8672–9. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-21-08672.2001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.He J. OFF responses in the auditory thalamus of the guinea pig. J Neurophysiol. 2002;88(5):2377–86. 10.1152/jn.00083.2002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Anderson LA, Linden JF. Mind the gap: Two dissociable mechanisms of temporal processing in the auditory system. J Neurosci. 2016;36(6):1977–95. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1652-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Smith PH, Uhlrich DJ, Manning KA, Banks MI. Thalamocortical projections to rat auditory cortex from the ventral and dorsal divisions of the medial geniculate nucleus. J Comp Neurol. 2012;520(1):34–51. 10.1002/cne.22682 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Zhang Z, Yu YQ, Liu CH, Chan YS, He J. Frequency tuning and firing pattern properties of auditory thalamic neurons: An in vivo intracellular recording from the guinea pig. Neuroscience. 2008;151(1):293–302. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.09.082 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Anderson LA, Linden JF. Physiological differences between histologically defined subdivisions in the mouse auditory thalamus. Hear Res. 2011;274(1–2):48–60. Available from: 10.1016/j.heares.2010.12.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Orsini CA, Maren S. Glutamate receptors in the medial geniculate nucleus are necessary for expression and extinction of conditioned fear in rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2009;92(4):581–9. Available from: 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.07.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Donishi T, Kimura A, Okamoto K, Tamai Y. “Ventral” area in the rat auditory cortex: A major auditory field connected with the dorsal division of the medial geniculate body. Neuroscience. 2006;141(3):1553–67. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.04.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Liu J, Whiteway MR, Sheikhattar A, Butts DA, Babadi B, Kanold PO. Parallel Processing of Sound Dynamics across Mouse Auditory Cortex via Spatially Patterned Thalamic Inputs and Distinct Areal Intracortical Circuits. Cell Rep. 2019;27(3):872–885.e7. Available from: 10.1016/j.celrep.2019.03.069 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Llano DA, Sherman SM. Evidence for Nonreciprocal Organization of the Mouse Auditory Thalamocortical-Corticothalamic Projection Systems. J Comp Neurol. 2008;507:1209–27. 10.1002/cne.21602 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Doron NN, Ledoux JE, Semple MN. Redefining the tonotopic core of rat auditory cortex: Physiological evidence for a posterior field. J Comp Neurol. 2002;453(4):345–60. 10.1002/cne.10412 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Kopp-Scheinpflug C, Sinclair JL, Linden JF. When Sound Stops: Offset Responses in the Auditory System. Trends Neurosci. 2018;41(10):712–28. 10.1016/j.tins.2018.08.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Kimura A, Donishi T, Okamoto K, Imbe H, Tamai Y. Efferent connections of the ventral auditory area in the rat cortex: Implications for auditory processing related to emotion. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25(9):2819–34. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05519.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Yang Y, Liu DQ, Huang W, Deng J, Sun Y, Zuo Y, et al. Selective synaptic remodeling of amygdalocortical connections associated with fear memory. Nat Neurosci. 2016;19(10):1348–55. 10.1038/nn.4370 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ramesh RN, Burgess CR, Sugden AU, Gyetvan M, Andermann ML. Intermingled Ensembles in Visual Association Cortex Encode Stimulus Identity or Predicted Outcome. Neuron. 2018;100(4):900–915.e9. Available from: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Paxinos G, Watson C. The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. Sixth Edition. Elsevier Academic Press; 2007 [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Gabriel Gasque

9 Sep 2019

Dear Dr Moita,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript entitled "A newly identified auditory circuit is involved in processing a natural sound cue of danger" for consideration as a Short Report by PLOS Biology.

Your manuscript has now been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editorial staff, as well as by an Academic Editor with relevant expertise, and I am writing to let you know that we would like to send your submission out for external peer review.

However, before we can send your manuscript to reviewers, we need you to complete your submission by providing the metadata that is required for full assessment. To this end, please login to Editorial Manager where you will find the paper in the 'Submissions Needing Revisions' folder on your homepage. Please click 'Revise Submission' from the Action Links and complete all additional questions in the submission questionnaire.

Please re-submit your manuscript within two working days, i.e. by Sep 11 2019 11:59PM.

Login to Editorial Manager here: https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology

During resubmission, you will be invited to opt-in to posting your pre-review manuscript as a bioRxiv preprint. Visit http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/preprints for full details. If you consent to posting your current manuscript as a preprint, please upload a single Preprint PDF when you re-submit.

Once your full submission is complete, your paper will undergo a series of checks in preparation for peer review. Once your manuscript has passed all checks it will be sent out for review.

Feel free to email us at plosbiology@plos.org if you have any queries relating to your submission.

Kind regards,

Gabriel Gasque, Ph.D.,

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

Decision Letter 1

Gabriel Gasque

14 Oct 2019

Dear Dr Moita,

Thank you very much for submitting your manuscript "A newly identified auditory circuit is involved in processing a natural sound cue of danger" for consideration as a Short Report at PLOS Biology. Your manuscript has been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editors, by an Academic Editor with relevant expertise, and by three independent reviewers. You will note that reviewer 3, Jennifer Linden, has signed her comments.

In light of the reviews (below), we will not be able to accept the current version of the manuscript, but we would welcome resubmission of a much-revised version that takes into account the reviewers' comments. We cannot make any decision about publication until we have seen the revised manuscript and your response to the reviewers' comments. Your revised manuscript is also likely to be sent for further evaluation by the reviewers.

Your revisions should address the specific points made by each reviewer. As you will see, reviewer 3 could be potentially satisfied with additional analyses and clarifications. However, reviewers 1 and 2 think that additional experiments are needed. Having discussed these comments with the Academic Editor, we think that for your revision to be successful at PLOS Biology, you need to include the additional data requested, but paying special attention to the concerns raised by reviewers 1 and 3.

Please note also note that Short Reports should be four main figures or less. Therefore, please either add the new data to existing figures or to supporting information.

Please submit a file detailing your responses to the editorial requests and a point-by-point response to all of the reviewers' comments that indicates the changes you have made to the manuscript. In addition to a clean copy of the manuscript, please upload a 'track-changes' version of your manuscript that specifies the edits made. This should be uploaded as a "Related" file type. You should also cite any additional relevant literature that has been published since the original submission and mention any additional citations in your response.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

Before you revise your manuscript, please review the following PLOS policy and formatting requirements checklist PDF: http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/file?id=9411/plos-biology-formatting-checklist.pdf. It is helpful if you format your revision according to our requirements - should your paper subsequently be accepted, this will save time at the acceptance stage.

Please note that as a condition of publication PLOS' data policy (http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/data-availability) requires that you make available all data used to draw the conclusions arrived at in your manuscript. If you have not already done so, you must include any data used in your manuscript either in appropriate repositories, within the body of the manuscript, or as supporting information (N.B. this includes any numerical values that were used to generate graphs, histograms etc.). For an example see here: http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1001908#s5.

For manuscripts submitted on or after 1st July 2019, we require the original, uncropped and minimally adjusted images supporting all blot and gel results reported in an article's figures or Supporting Information files. We will require these files before a manuscript can be accepted so please prepare them now, if you have not already uploaded them. Please carefully read our guidelines for how to prepare and upload this data: https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/figures#loc-blot-and-gel-reporting-requirements.

Upon resubmission, the editors will assess your revision and if the editors and Academic Editor feel that the revised manuscript remains appropriate for the journal, we will send the manuscript for re-review. We aim to consult the same Academic Editor and reviewers for revised manuscripts but may consult others if needed.

We expect to receive your revised manuscript within two months. Please email us (plosbiology@plos.org) to discuss this if you have any questions or concerns, or would like to request an extension. At this stage, your manuscript remains formally under active consideration at our journal; please notify us by email if you do not wish to submit a revision and instead wish to pursue publication elsewhere, so that we may end consideration of the manuscript at PLOS Biology.

When you are ready to submit a revised version of your manuscript, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' where you will find your submission record.

Thank you again for your submission to our journal. We hope that our editorial process has been constructive thus far, and we welcome your feedback at any time. Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or comments.

Sincerely,

Gabriel Gasque, Ph.D.,

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

*****************************************************

Reviewer remarks:

Reviewer #1: Pereira and colleagues describe a circuit of three nuclei involved in the freezing behaviour after hearing the cessation of sound made by conspecifics. They used a combination of pharmacological and optogenetic silencing together with behavioural experiments. C-fos and tracing data were used to corroborate the results. Overall, the manuscript is short, straight forward and interesting to a wide range of scientists. I have three major concerns that I would ask the authors to address and a number of minor comments that can be easily fixed listed below.

Major concerns:

1. The ArchT expression was not driven by a MGD specific cre-line. So the specificity of the optogenetic silencing was entirely based on the correct (and spatially restricted) injection of the viral vector into the MGD. As the MGD is small and very close to other potential contributors to this circuit like the other parts of the MGB, which can also generate offset responses (Anderson & Linden, 2016), the injection site and specificity needs to be validated. In fact, the results of the silencing look almost too good to be true, which made me wonder if the ArchT actually silenced larger parts of the MGB than just the MGD. This would still be a good, new and interesting result – but would not make claims that might not be justified. Please either show the injection sites of all tested animals to make sure ArchT is expressed only in MGD. Or change the wording to have the whole MGB be part of that circuit, rather than just MGD. Or repeat a couple of the experiments with a cre-dependent ArchT expression in a cre-line that is explicitly expressed in MGD.

2. What is the spectrum/level of the movement evoked sound? Is it recorded from one or multiple rats? Are you sure there is no vocalization emitted in addition to plain movement sound – need to be tested in ultrasonic range. Please include this information in the methods.

3. How can you be sure that the increase in c-fos expression is really due to exposure to silent gaps? It could just be increased neural activity to any change in stimulus, while the ongoing sound is perceived as “boring” background. Has this been tested with another sound paradigm: e.g. ongoing noise as control (as you have done before), and then some other stimulus where you may have modulated noise instead of silence?

Minor comments:

There are groups which question the validity of the muscimol inactivation of the cortex too, but I would let this one go here.

Line 75: there is a question mark sign – should be an arrow (maybe just say: “White arrow shows…”)

Fig. 2C: Please provide higher magnification images so that the reader can see neurons rather than what looks like speckles of dirt. A counterstain for e.g. VGLUT or MAP2 would be also very helpful.

Line 166, 175, 359, 361: typo: kHz

Lines 173/177: ranksum or rank sum – be consistent!

Fig. 4: please indicate in the figure where the PBS or muscimol was injected (e.g. muscimol (into PD)), otherwise the reader needs to scramble though the legend first to get the message of the figure…

Why were only male mice used? Many journals now insist that both sexes were used, if not a scientific reason prevents the use on both.

Line 259, 349, 351: typo: fiber

Lines 373/374: use dots as decimals

Reviewer #2: Ana et al., reported that an auditory cue, the cessation of movement-evoked sound, induced defensive freezing, and further identified the underlying neural circuits comprising of MGd, ACx and LA. The authors thought this is a unique offset pathway for “cessation of movement-evoked sound” induced defense behavior. The study is mostly based on the behavioral analysis. However, there is major problem in their behavioral design. The authors’ interpretation of the behavior results and the conclusion could be completely wrong. The proposed new circuit is the classical circuit for fear conditioning.

1 The defense behavior induced by “cessation of movement-evoked sound” examined in this study was first reported by this research group in 2012 in current biology. This is not an innate behavior (as suggested for impending danger), but learned defense as it required conditioning training with foot shock (Fig. 1a).

2 This is more problematic for the interpretation of the whole study. This training can directly establish an association between “cessation of movement-evoked sound” (e.g. caused by the trainee rat) and footshock, i.e. some fear conditioning. That means the nature of the later defense behavior could be simply a learned defense under a context-cue of “cessation of movement-evoked sound”. This possibility also explains a similar behavior (in a different context) previously reported by authors in 2012. Unfortunately, this was not examined for these years.

3 This alternative interpretation explains why the proposed neural circuits for this behavior is in fact the same as that for fear conditioning, including VA, LA, MGB etc. It is not a new circuit.

4 The authors should at least establish some correlation between neuronal activity in LA, VA, MGd, with the behavior. Is this really an offset responding pathway? How neurons in LA,VA, MGd respond during the cessation of movement-evoked sound and the following 1 minute silence.

5 Experiments with specific silencing of projections from VA and MGD to LA would be helpful. Is LA activation sufficient for inducing freezing in those trainee rats?

Reviewer #3, Jennifer Linden: "A newly identified auditory circuit is involved in processing a natural sound cue of danger"

Pereira AG, Farias M and Moita MA

This article documents the discovery of an auditory circuit that drives freezing following cessation (offset) of movement-evoked rustling sounds, such as would be produced by the freezing of another animal. The circuit involves the dorsal subdivision of the medial geniculate body of the auditory thalamus (MGD), the ventral area of the auditory cortex (VA), and the lateral amygdala (LA). The authors use combined optogenetic and behavioural studies to demonstrate that this circuit is necessary to produce offset-evoked freezing behaviour.

This article is likely to be of wide scientific interest, because it is the first demonstration (to my knowledge) of a neural circuit driving behavioural responses to sound offset (rather than sound onset). Neural responses to sound offsets have been reported throughout the central auditory system, but the perceptual significance of these responses is not yet well understood. This paper provides clear evidence that higher central auditory brain areas contribute to behavioural responses to sound offsets. Moreover, this paper is extremely well-written: systematic and thorough yet succinct. Well done!!

I have a few major comments and several minor suggestions.

MAJOR COMMENTS:

(1) Lines 104-106: "Interestingly, prior electrophysiology studies in anaesthetised rodents showed a higher prevalence of sound offset responses in the MGD relative to other sub-nuclei of the auditory thalamus and higher number of offset cells in the caudal part of MGD." This is potentially misleading. It is true that previous studies have found evidence for offset responses in the MGD, but strong evidence for offset responses has also been reported in the MGV, especially in the "lateral shell" of the MGV. See for example:

He (2001) J Neurosci 21:8672–8679

He (2002) J Neurophysiol 88:2377–2386

Anderson and Linden (2016) J Neurosci 36:1977–1995

The authors should clarify that strong evidence for offset responses has also been reported in MGV, and also explain why this subdivision was not studied here (presumably because it does not project both directly and indirectly to the amygdala?).

(2) Figure 2b: Please comment in the text on the high variance in the MGD data. Is it possible that outliers in the "MGD" cases are at an extreme edge of this subdivision, on the border with other subregions, such as the MGV? See first major comment above.

(3) Figure 2 and associated text: It is quite confusing to use the term "silence" to mean an otherwise continuous sound interrupted by two silent gaps. The stimulus was definitely not "silence". Perhaps other terminology could be used here, e.g. "sound with silent gaps".

(4) Figure 3: Comparison between positive results in Fig. 3d ("silence test") and negative results in Fig. 3e ("tone test") is potentially flawed. The "silence test" results show the change in the percentage of time the animals spent freezing during the 1min immediately preceding the cessation of the movement sound and the 1min immediately following cessation. The tone test results show the change in percentage of time the animals spent freezing during the 15sec immediately before the tone and the 15sec of tone presentation. The tone test is likely to be underpowered relative to the silence test, because the time periods used for measuring freezing behaviour were much shorter in the tone test. To address this issue, results for the silence test could be re-computed using 15sec intervals, and either shown or at least mentioned in the text. Also: presumably the laser illumination *timing* must have been different for the silence test and the tone test, with a much longer period of optogenetic activation in the silence test than the tone test? This should also be mentioned as a possible reason for caution about interpretation of the negative result for the tone test. It is possible that the experiment and the analysis were simply less sensitive for the tone test, hence the negative result.

(5) In all of the figures shown, the Control data is pooled from Control-light and Control-ArchT conditions. While it is admirable that the authors tried two different control conditions, the Control-light condition is the far more critical one. Do all results hold when comparisons (e.g. of percent change in freezing) are made between ArchT-light and only Control-light conditions? Supplementary Figures seem to indicate that this is the case, but it would be worth stating in the text for every analysis shown in the main figures. It is especially important to mention whether results held for comparisons to the Control-light condition alone because of the point mentioned in the Methods section, lines 439-442: "Due to the noise generated by the shutter used in the optogenetics experiments, animals that were freezing more than 50% in the 10sec period between the opening of the shutter and silence onset were... excluded". In the tone test (Fig. 3e), the 10sec period between the opening of the shutter and tone cue onset would be 66% of the total pre-tone period (compared to 17% of the pre-silence period in the silence test); was the same exclusion criterion used for the tone test as for the silence test?

MINOR CORRECTIONS:

line 64: "rats freezing behaviour" -- should be "rats' freezing behaviour"

line 73 (Fig. 1 legend): More information should be added here to aid interpretation of the mmunohistochemical images: e.g., "Pink, cell bodies labelled with DAPI; green, diffuse LA terminals labelled with ArchT."

line 75 (Fig. 1 legend): missing symbol for indicating tip of injector --- arrow?

line 77 (Fig. 1 legend): Please clarify here that Control condition includes both Control-light and Control-ArchT conditions. This is mentioned in the text but should also be mentioned in the legend to avoid confusion. See also major comment about mentioning results for Control-light comparison alone

line 147: figure reference should be to Figure 3c and 3d

line 148: figure reference should be to Figure 2c, 2d and 2e

lines 148-149: Anatomical and physiological properties of the MGD in rodents have also been addressed in:

He (2002) J Neurophysiol 88:2377-2386

Zhang, Yu, Liu, Chan and He (2008) Neuroscience 151:293-302

Anderson and Linden (2011) Hear Res 274:48-60

lines 205-208 and lines 210-211 (Fig. 4 legend): More information should be provided here to aid interpretation of the images, e.g. regarding subdivisions of amygdala indicated with dashed lines and localization of anterogradely labelled neurons from MGD or VA.

line 259 and elsewhere in methods: "optic fibber" should be "optic fiber"

line 262: "forth day" should be "fourth day"

line 271: "Phosphate Basal Solution" should be "Phosphate Buffered Saline", presumably??

line 275: "bought to Sigma-Aldrich" should be "bought from Sigma-Aldrich"

line 282: "groups were targeted bilaterally" should read "groups received injections targeted bilaterally"

line 312: "to this regions" should be "to these regions"

line 415: "byotinilated" should be "biotinylated"

Decision Letter 2

Gabriel Gasque

3 Feb 2020

Dear Dr Moita,

Thank you for submitting your revised Short Report entitled "Thalamic, cortical, and amydgala involvement in the processing of a natural sound cue of danger" for publication in PLOS Biology. I have now obtained advice from original reviewers 1 and 3, and have discussed their comments with the Academic Editor. You will note that reviewer 1, Conny Kopp-Scheinpflug, has signed her comments.

We're delighted to let you know that we're editorially satisfied with your manuscript. However before we can formally accept your paper and consider it "in press", we also need to ensure that your article conforms to our guidelines. A member of our team will be in touch shortly with a set of requests. As we can't proceed until these requirements are met, your swift response will help prevent delays to publication. Please also make sure to address the data and other policy-related requests noted at the end of this email.

*Copyediting*

Upon acceptance of your article, your final files will be copyedited and typeset into the final PDF. While you will have an opportunity to review these files as proofs, PLOS will only permit corrections to spelling or significant scientific errors. Therefore, please take this final revision time to assess and make any remaining major changes to your manuscript.

NOTE: If Supporting Information files are included with your article, note that these are not copyedited and will be published as they are submitted. Please ensure that these files are legible and of high quality (at least 300 dpi) in an easily accessible file format. For this reason, please be aware that any references listed in an SI file will not be indexed. For more information, see our Supporting Information guidelines:

https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/supporting-information

*Published Peer Review History*

Please note that you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*Early Version*

Please note that an uncorrected proof of your manuscript will be published online ahead of the final version, unless you opted out when submitting your manuscript. If, for any reason, you do not want an earlier version of your manuscript published online, uncheck the box. Should you, your institution's press office or the journal office choose to press release your paper, you will automatically be opted out of early publication. We ask that you notify us as soon as possible if you or your institution is planning to press release the article.

*Protocols deposition*

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/submission-guidelines#loc-materials-and-methods

*Submitting Your Revision*

To submit your revision, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' to find your submission record. Your revised submission must include a cover letter, a Response to Reviewers file that provides a detailed response to the reviewers' comments (if applicable), and a track-changes file indicating any changes that you have made to the manuscript.

Please do not hesitate to contact me should you have any questions.

Sincerely,

Gabriel Gasque, Ph.D.,

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

------------------------------------------------------------------------

ETHICS STATEMENT:

-- Please include the ID number of your protocol approved by the Champalimaud Center for the Unknown.

------------------------------------------------------------------------

DATA POLICY:

--Please double check your entries in the S1 Data file, as some of these seem to be mislabeled; for example, in Fig 2, data for panel d is not included, in Fig 3, there are two entries for panel d but none for panel e.

--Please ensure that the figure legends in your manuscript include information on where the underlying data can be found and ensure your supplemental data file/s has a legend.

------------------------------------------------------------------------

BLOT AND GEL REPORTING REQUIREMENTS:

For manuscripts submitted on or after 1st July 2019, we require the original, uncropped and minimally adjusted images supporting all blot and gel results reported in an article's figures or Supporting Information files. We will require these files before a manuscript can be accepted so please prepare and upload them now. Please carefully read our guidelines for how to prepare and upload this data: https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/figures#loc-blot-and-gel-reporting-requirements

------------------------------------------------------------------------

Reviewer remarks:

Reviewer #1, Conny Kopp-Scheinpflug: I would like to commend the authors to their revisions and a fine paper. They have properly addressed my initial concerns. This is an interesting follow-up of their 2012 study and will be of interest to a broad readership.

Reviewer #3: The authors have done a very good job with addressing the reviewers' comments, including with some useful additional supplementary figures. I have no additional queries.

Decision Letter 3

Gabriel Gasque

9 Apr 2020

Dear Dr Moita,

On behalf of my colleagues and the Academic Editor, Jennifer K Bizley, I am pleased to inform you that we will be delighted to publish your Short Reports in PLOS Biology.

The files will now enter our production system. You will receive a copyedited version of the manuscript, along with your figures for a final review. You will be given two business days to review and approve the copyedit. Then, within a week, you will receive a PDF proof of your typeset article. You will have two days to review the PDF and make any final corrections. If there is a chance that you'll be unavailable during the copy editing/proof review period, please provide us with contact details of one of the other authors whom you nominate to handle these stages on your behalf. This will ensure that any requested corrections reach the production department in time for publication.

Early Version

The version of your manuscript submitted at the copyedit stage will be posted online ahead of the final proof version, unless you have already opted out of the process. The date of the early version will be your article's publication date. The final article will be published to the same URL, and all versions of the paper will be accessible to readers.

PRESS

We frequently collaborate with press offices. If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximise its impact. If the press office is planning to promote your findings, we would be grateful if they could coordinate with biologypress@plos.org. If you have not yet opted out of the early version process, we ask that you notify us immediately of any press plans so that we may do so on your behalf.

We also ask that you take this opportunity to read our Embargo Policy regarding the discussion, promotion and media coverage of work that is yet to be published by PLOS. As your manuscript is not yet published, it is bound by the conditions of our Embargo Policy. Please be aware that this policy is in place both to ensure that any press coverage of your article is fully substantiated and to provide a direct link between such coverage and the published work. For full details of our Embargo Policy, please visit http://www.plos.org/about/media-inquiries/embargo-policy/.

Thank you again for submitting your manuscript to PLOS Biology and for your support of Open Access publishing. Please do not hesitate to contact me if I can provide any assistance during the production process.

Kind regards,

Alice Musson

Publishing Editor,

PLOS Biology

on behalf of

Gabriel Gasque,

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Fig. The LA is necessary for the expression of freezing driven by the cessation of movement-evoked sound.

    (A) Coronal slices representing fiber placements and spread of virus expression for ArchT + light group. (B) Fiber placement and/or injection site for the Ct light (light blue) and Ct ArchT (dark blue) groups. (C) Same as Fig 1D, but for animals of the 2 control groups. Horizontal bar represents the median value of the group (Ct light = 66.13%, Ct ArchT = 73.67%, Wilcoxon rank sum test, rank sum = 72) (S1 Data, Sheet S1 Fig). (D–F) Line graph showing average time spent freezing during the minute immediately preceding the cessation of the movement-evoked sound (baseline) and the minute of silence for each rat of the ArchT + light (n = 7), Ct light (n = 9), and Ct ArchT (n = 6) groups. Wilcoxon signed rank test. Baseline versus silence ArchT + light signedrank = 1; Ct light signedrank = 1; Ct ArchT signedrank = 0. (S1 Data, Sheet S1 Fig). ArchT, archaerhodopsin from Halorubrum strain TP009; Ct light, control light; Ct ArchT, control ArchT; LA, lateral amygdala.

    (TIF)

    S2 Fig. Representative pictures of c-fos-labeled cells in MGB.

    Representative images of coronal slices from rats exposed to silence gaps showing c-fos labeling in different subnuclei of the MGB. In particular, we observe c-fos-positive cells in the shell of the MGV, an area particularly responsive to the offset of sounds. c-fos-positive cells in this region were not quantified because it does not project directly to the LA. LA, lateral amygdala; MGB, medial geniculate body; MGV, ventral division of the MGB.

    (TIF)

    S3 Fig. The MGD is necessary for the expression of freezing driven by the cessation of the movement-evoked sound but not by a discrete auditory cue.

    (A) Coronal slices representing fiber placements and spread of virus expression for ArchT + light group. (B) Fiber placement and/or injection site for the Ct light (light blue) and Ct ArchT (dark blue) groups. (C) Representative image of cells in MGD expressing ArchT-GFP. (D) Same as Fig 3D but for animals of the 2 control groups. Horizontal bar represents the median value (Ct light = 49.47%, Ct ArchT = 68.93%, Wilcoxon rank sum test, rank sum = 48) (S1 Data, Sheet S3 Fig). (E–G) Line graph showing average time spent freezing during the minute immediately preceding the cessation of the movement-evoked sound (baseline) and the minute of silence for each rat of the ArchT + light (n = 7), Ct light (n = 8), and Ct ArchT (n = 5) groups with surgeries targeting the MGD. Wilcoxon signed rank test, baseline versus silence ArchT + light signedrank = 0; Ct light signedrank = 1; Ct ArchT signedrank = 0. (S1 Data, Sheet S3 Fig). ArchT, archaerhodopsin from Halorubrum strain TP009; Ct light, control light; Ct ArchT, control Arch T; GFP, green fluorescent protein; LA, lateral amygdala; MGB, medial geniculate body; MGD, dorsal division of the MGB.

    (TIF)

    S4 Fig. Effect of MGD inactivation during the first 15 seconds of the silence test in comparison with tone test.

    (A) Line graph showing average time spent freezing during the 15 seconds immediately preceding the cessation of the movement-evoked sound or tone (baseline) and the 15 seconds of stimulus (silence or tone) for each rat of the ArchT + light (n = 7) and Control (n = 13) groups. Wilcoxon signed rank test, baseline versus silence ArchT + light signedrank = 0; Control signedrank = 1; baseline versus tone ArchT + light signedrank = 0; Control signedrank = 0 (S1 Data, Sheet S4 Fig). (B) Individual dots represent the change in the percentage of time each animal spends freezing during the 15 seconds immediately preceding the cessation of the movement-evoked sound or tone (baseline) and the 15 seconds of stimulus (silence or tone) for each rat of the ArchT + light (n = 7) and Control (n = 13) groups. Horizontal bar represents the median value of the group (Silence test ArchT + light = 0.00%, Control = 37.07%; Tone test ArchT + light = 73.33%, Control = 40%). Kruskal–Wallis test chi-squared = 8 (S1 Data, Sheet S4 Fig) (S1 Data, Sheet S4 Fig). ArchT, archaerhodopsin from Halorubrum strain TP009; MGB, medial geniculate body; MGD, dorsal division of the MGB.

    (TIF)

    S5 Fig

    Coronal slices representing injection site for PBS (light purple) and muscimol (dark purple) in (A) PD and (B) VA. Areas shaded in gray correspond to PD and VA (respectively). PBS, phosphate buffered saline; PD, posterodorsal; VA, ventral area.

    (TIF)

    S1 Data. Raw data file.

    (XLSX)

    S1 Movie. Neurons in MGD expressing ArchT-GFP.

    ArchT, archaerhodopsin from Halorubrum strain TP009; GFP, green fluorescent protein; MGD, dorsal division of the medial geniculate body.

    (MP4)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Pereira Response to Reviewer .pdf

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data will be within the paper and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLoS Biology are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES