Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2020 May 13;15(5):e0232855. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0232855

The effects of age and sex on cognitive impairment in schizophrenia: Findings from the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS) study

Junghee Lee 1,2,*, Michael F Green 1,2, Keith H Nuechterlein 1, Neal R Swerdlow 3, Tiffany A Greenwood 3, Gerhard S Hellemann 1,2, Laura C Lazzeroni 4,5, Gregory A Light 3,6, Allen D Radant 7,8, Larry J Seidman 9,10, Larry J Siever 11,12, Jeremy M Silverman 11,12, Joyce Sprock 3,6, William S Stone 9,10, Catherine A Sugar 1,2,13, Debby W Tsuang 7,8, Ming T Tsuang 3,14,15, Bruce I Turetsky 16, Ruben C Gur 16, Raquel E Gur 16, David L Braff 3,6
Editor: Kenji Hashimoto17
PMCID: PMC7219730  PMID: 32401791

Abstract

Recently emerging evidence indicates accelerated age-related changes in the structure and function of the brain in schizophrenia, raising a question about its potential consequences on cognitive function. Using a large sample of schizophrenia patients and controls and a battery of tasks across multiple cognitive domains, we examined whether patients show accelerated age-related decline in cognition and whether an age-related effect differ between females and males. We utilized data of 1,415 schizophrenia patients and 1,062 healthy community collected by the second phase of the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS-2). A battery of cognitive tasks included the Letter-Number Span Task, two forms of the Continuous Performance Test, the California Verbal Learning Test, Second Edition, the Penn Emotion Identification Test and the Penn Facial Memory Test. The effect of age and gender on cognitive performance was examined with a general linear model. We observed age-related changes on most cognitive measures, which was similar between males and females. Compared to controls, patients showed greater deterioration in performance on attention/vigilance and greater slowness of processing social information with increasing age. However, controls showed greater age-related changes in working memory and verbal memory compared to patients. Age-related changes (η2p of 0.001 to .008) were much smaller than between-group differences (η2p of 0.005 to .037). This study found that patients showed continued decline of cognition on some domains but stable impairment or even less decline on other domains with increasing age. These findings indicate that age-related changes in cognition in schizophrenia are subtle and not uniform across multiple cognitive domains.

Introduction

While schizophrenia is considered a neurodevelopmental disorder with fixed early deficits, emerging evidence also supports the idea that schizophrenia patients may show both an early brain dysfunction along with progressive neural and/or behavioral deterioration over the course of the illness. For example, schizophrenia patients showed greater age-related reduction in the brain gray matter volumes than controls after early adulthood [1, 2]. This pattern was disproportionately greater in the clinically important frontal and temporal regions [2]. Schizophrenia patients also showed faster age-related deterioration in white matter integrity, emerging after age 35, and this deterioration was focused in the anterior corpus callosum and the temporal aspects of the superior longitudinal fasciculus, also areas implicated in schizophrenia [24]. Interestingly, age-related deterioration in both gray and white matter appears to be faster in males than females [2], consistent with the idea that schizophrenia pathology may show sexual dimorphism with males showing greater impairment across multiple neurobiological and clinical domains [5, 6]. Patients also showed greater age-related decline in functional connectivity in several neural networks including the frontal-parietal network and cingulo-opercular network compared with healthy control subjects (HCS) [7]. Thus, existing evidence suggests that schizophrenia patients show faster age-related decline in the structure and function of the brain compared to healthy controls.

The evidence for accelerated aging in the structure and function of the brain in schizophrenia raises questions about its potential consequences for cognitive function. Longitudinal studies with recent-onset psychotic patients shows that impaired cognitive performance was largely stable up to 20 years past onset [811]. However, because most studies did not assess healthy controls at follow-up, the stable pattern of cognitive performance in patients does not necessarily rule out that possibility that patients show greater age-related deterioration. Further, most studies on cognitive changes over the course of illness did not assess multiple cognitive domains nor did they examine whether female and male patients show different patterns of performance over time. In addition, most studies have examined only non-social cognition. A recent finding of stable social cognitive performance over 5 years in recent-onset schizophrenia [12] suggests, but did not demonstrate, that age-related changes in cognition in schizophrenia may be similar across several cognitive domains.

To examine these questions regarding stability versus progression of cognitive deficits with age in schizophrenia patients versus healthy controls, this study utilized a large between-subjects data set from a large case-control study of endophenotypes collected by the second phase of the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS-2) [13]. Specifically, this study included a large battery of cognitive tasks tapping into multiple cognitive domains to examine 1) whether schizophrenia patients show age-related changes in cognition that differ from those in healthy controls, and 2) if age-related effects differ between males and females.

Materials and methods

Participants included 1,415 patients diagnosed with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, depressed type, via a Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) and 1,062 healthy community controls from the COGS-2 study. The NIH-funded COGS-2 included data collection from five sites: University of California at Los Angeles (UCLA), University of California at San Diego (UCSD), Mount Sinai School of Medicine (MSSM), University of Pennsylvania (PENN), and University of Washington (UW). The COGS-2 study also included Stanford University as a non-data-collecting site. Details of the recruitment, the selection criteria for participants and clinical assessments are available in the Supplement and elsewhere [13]. We decided to recruit patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, depressed type, for the following reasons. First, findings from genetic studies suggest that schizoaffective disorder, depressed type, is more genetically similar to schizophrenia compared to schizoaffective disorder, bipolar type [14, 15]. Second, schizoaffective disorder, depressed type, showed illness course similar to that of schizophrenia, compared to schizoaffective disorder, bipolar type [16, 17]. The local Institutional Review Boards of each site approved the study, and all participants provided informed consent and were compensated for their participation. For both patients and controls, the Global Assessment of Function Scale (GAF) [18] was administered to characterize samples. Additional clinical assessments for patients included a modified version of the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) [19] and the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS) [20].

Assessments on cognition

The following cognitive tasks from the COGS-2 study were included in this analysis: the Letter-Number Span Task (LNS) [21, 22] for working memory; two forms of the Continuous Performance Test (CPT) [2325] for attention/vigilance; the California Verbal Learning Test, Second Edition (CVLT-II) [26, 27] for verbal memory; and the Penn Emotion Identification Test (PEIT) and PENN Facial Memory Test (PEMT) of the Penn Computerized Neurocognitive Battery (CNB) [28, 29] for facial affect recognition and facial memory, respectively. As details of each paradigm are described in the references cited, brief descriptions are provided below.

The LNS [30] employed a set of intermixed letters and digits that experimenters verbally presented at a rate of one per second and consisted of two conditions, the “Forward” and “Reorder” conditions. In the Forward condition, participants were asked to repeat the letters and numbers in the same order as they were presented. In the Reorder condition, participants were asked to repeat the digits first in ascending order and then letters in alphabetical order. The primary measure for each condition was the total number of correctly recalled sequences (i.e., maximum score for each condition = 21).

The two forms of CPT involved computerized versions of the Degraded Stimulus CPT (DS-CPT) and the CPT-Identical Pairs (CPT-IP) [23]. The DS-CPT employed a quasi-random series of blurred single digits that were presented for 29-ms each at a rate of one digit per second. Participants were asked to detect each target “0”. The CPT-IP employed a series of digits presented for 50 ms in a quasi-random sequence at a rate of one digit per second and had two conditions: a 3-digit condition and a 4-digit condition. Participants were asked to respond whenever the same stimulus occurred twice in a row. For both the DS-CPT and CPT-IP, the primary measure was d prime (d’), a signal/noise discrimination index reflecting attention and vigilance from signal detection theory [31, 32].

The CVLT-II employed a 16-item word list that was presented verbally over five learning trials, followed by a single presentation of a second list of 16-item words [26]. Participants were asked to recall as many words as possible from the first list immediately after each list presentation, after the second list was presented, and then after a 20-minute delay. The primary measure was Trials 1–5 Free Recall Correct, Short-Delay Free Recall, and Long-Delay Free Recall.

The PENN Emotion Recognition Test (PEIT) of the Penn computerized neurocognitive battery (CNB) [28, 29] was employed as a measure of emotion identification. The PEIT employed 40 facial stimuli expressing one of 4 emotions (happy, sad, anger, fear) or neutral facial expressions that were presented one at a time. Participants were asked to identify the emotion displayed. The primary measures were accuracy and reaction time. The PENN Face Memory Test (PEMT) [28, 29] was employed to assess episodic memory for faces. In the first part of the task, the participants were shown a series of 20 faces one at a time and asked to memorize them. During the recognition part, the participants were shown 40 faces, 20 of which were the faces they were shown, and the other 20 were distractors. For each face stimulus, participants were asked to indicate whether they have seen the face before. The primary measures were accuracy and reaction time.

Statistical analysis

For demographic and clinical characteristics, differences by diagnostic group and gender were examined with ANOVA for continuous variables and chi-square tests for categorical variables. The effect of age and gender on cognitive performance in schizophrenia was examined with a general linear model. Specifically, for each dependent measure of the cognitive tasks, a general linear model was conducted, with age, gender and group as fixed factors. Age was included as a continuous variable and gender and group were included as categorical variables. In addition to the linear effect of age, we also examined whether age has a non-linear effect on cognition by adding the quadratic effect of age in the model. The addition of the non-linear effect of age did not improve the model fit, and we report findings from a general linear model with the linear effect of age below. Effect sizes are provided for any significant effects (i.e., partial eta square [η2p]s: η2p of 0.01 represents a small; η2p of 0.06 represents a medium; and η2p of 0.14 represents a large effect size [33].

Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics of the participants

Table 1 presents demographic and clinical characteristics of the participants. For age, we observed a significant effect of group (F(1,2473) = 252.88, p < .001, η2p = .093) and a significant group by sex interaction (F(1,2473) = 14.52, p < .001, η2p = .006). Overall, controls were younger than patients (p < .001). Females were younger than males in the control group (p < .001), but not in the patient group (p = .055). For personal education, we observed a significant effect of group (F(1,2472) = 745.44, p < .001, η2p = .232), a significant effect of sex (F(1,2472) = 13.35, p < .001, η2p = .005) and a significant group by sex interaction (F(1,2472) = 7.79, p < .01, η2p = .003). Females had higher personal education levels than males in the control group (p < .001), but not in the patient group (p = .52). For parental education, we found a significant effect of group (F(1,2269) = 146.62, p < .001, η2p = .061) and a significant group by sex interaction (F(1,2269) = 6.32, p < .05, η2p = .003). Males had higher levels of parental education than females in the patient group (p < .05), but not in the control group (p = .14). For GAF in the past month, we observed a significant effect of group (F(1,2454) = 15438.32, p < .001, η2p = .863) and a significant effect of sex (F(1,2454) = 33.95, p < .001, η2p = .014). Patients had lower GAF than controls (p < .001), and across both groups, females had higher GAF than males (p < .001). Within the patient group, males reported younger onset of psychotic symptoms than females (F(1,1399) = 12.79, p < .001, η2p = .009). Male and female patients did not differ on positive or negative symptoms (p = .15 and p = .46, respectively).

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of participants.

Patients Controls
Female (n = 436) Male (n = 979) Female (n = 540) Male (n = 522)
Age 47.2 (10.5) 45.9 (11.2) 37.3 (13.4) 39.8 (12.7)
Personal Edu. 12.5 (2.2) 12.5 (2.0) 15.2 (2.1) 14.7 (2.2)
Parental Edu. 12.1 (3.1) 12.5 (2.9) 14.1 (2.9) 13.8 (3.1)
% Past Sub Dis
GAF 44.7 (8.7) 43.1 (7.8) 87.5 (7.6) 85.2 (8.5)
Age of Onset 23.2 (3.1) 21.8 (6.2)
Global_SANS 10.8 (6.0) 11.1 (5.5)
Global_SAPS 6.7 (4.1) 7.0 (3.9(

** GAF: GAF of the last month

The effect of age and sex on cognitive performance

For LNS Forward (Fig 1a), we observed a significant effect of age (F(1,2415) = 74.33, p < .001, η2p = .019), a significant effect of group (F(1,2415) = 55.07, p < .001, η2p = .022), and a significant group by age interaction (F(1,2415) = 5.81, p < .05, η2p = .002). No other effect was significant. Patients showed poorer performance than controls. While both groups showed the decline in performance with increasing age, this decline was steeper in the control group. For LNS Reorder (Fig 1b), we observed a significant effect of age (F(1,2410) = 55.89, p < .001, η2p = .022) and a significant effect of group (F(1,2410) = 46.82, p < .001, η2p = .019), but no other effect was significant. Patients showed poorer performance than controls and both groups showed the decline in performance with increasing age.

Fig 1. Cognitive performance in patients (red line) and controls (blue line).

Fig 1

Lines show fitted value of cognitive performance with increasing age, and shaded areas correspond to 95% confidence interval.

For DS-CPT (Fig 1c), we found a significant effect of age (F(1,2184) = 70.73, p < .001, η2p = .031) and a significant effect of group (F(1,2187) = 12.46, p < .001, η2p = .005). No other effect was significant. In both groups, performance declined with increasing age. Patients performed poorer than controls. For the CPT-IP 3-digit condition (Fig 1d), we found a significant effect of age (F(1,2241) = 32.12, p < .001, η2p = .014), a significant effect of group (F(1,2241) = 18.88, p < .001, η2p = .008) and a significant group by age effect (F(1,2241) = 4.46, p < .05, η2p = .001). Patients showed poorer performance than controls. While both groups showed the decline in performance with increasing age, this effect was greater in the patient group than the control group. For CPT-IP with a 4 digit condition (Fig 1e), we found a significant effect of age (F(1,2193) = 38.57, p < .001, η2p = .017) and a significant effect of group (F(1,2193) = 34.77, p < .001, η2p = .015). Both groups showed the decline in performance with increasing age and patients showed poorer performance than controls.

For CVLT-II Trials 1–5 Free Recall Correct (Fig 1f), an effect of age and an effect of group were significant (F(1,2382) = 129.10, p < .001, η2p = .051; and F(1,2382) = 93.62, p < .001, η2p = .037, respectively). In both groups, performance declined with increasing age. Patients showed poorer performance. For CVLT-II short-delay free recall (Fig 1g), we found a significant effect of age (F(1,2382) = 86.43, p < .001, η2p = .035), a significant effect of group (F(1,2382) = 82.64, p < .001, η2p = .033), and a significant group by age interaction (F(1,2382) = 3.97, p < .05, η2p = .001). Patients showed poorer performance than controls. Both groups showed the decline in performance with increasing age, and this aging effect was larger in the control group. Similarly, for CVLT-II long-delay free recall (Fig 1h), we found a significant effect of age (F(1,2382) = 76.75, p < .001, η2p = .031), a significant effect of group (F(1,2382) = 85.44, p < .001, η2p = .034), and a significant group by age interaction (F(1,2382) = 5.34, p < .05, η2p = .002). Patients again showed poorer performance than controls. Both groups showed the decline in performance with increasing age, with the steeper decline in the control group.

For PEIT accuracy (Fig 1i), we found a significant effect of age (F(1,2121) = 53.69, p < .001, η2p = .024) and a significant effect of group (F(1,2121) = 22.67, p < .001, η2p = .010). Both groups showed poorer performance with increasing age, and patients showed poorer performance than controls. For PEIT reaction time (Fig 1j), we found a significant effect of age (F(1,2121) = 262.49, p < .001, η2p = .110) and a significant group by age interaction (F(1,2121) = 12.71, p < .001, η2p = .005). Both groups showed slower reaction time with increasing age, and this effect was larger in the patient group. For accuracy of the PEMT (Fig 1k), we observed a significant effect of age (F(1,2157) = 16.44, p < .001, η2p = .007) and a significant effect of group (F(1,2157) = 17.78, p < .001, η2p = .008). Patients showed poorer performance than controls. In both groups performance declined with increasing age. For reaction time in the PEMT task (Fig 1l), we found a significant effect of age (F(1,2157) = 144.02, p < .001, η2p = .062) and a significant effect age by group interaction (F(1,2157) = 18.91, p < .001, η2p = .008). Both groups showed slower reaction time with increasing age and this effect was greater in the patient group.

Discussion

In this study, using a large sample of chronic, medicated outpatients and controls of COGS2 study, we examined whether schizophrenia patients and healthy controls show differential age-related differences in cognition and whether age-related changes differ between male and female participants. For most cognitive measures, we observed selective age-related differences between controls and patients that did not differ between male and female participants. Specifically, compared to controls, schizophrenia patients showed greater deteriorated performance with increasing age on attention/vigilance and greater slowness for processing social information. Compared to patients, controls showed greater aging effect on working memory and verbal memory. Further, age-related changes in these cognitive measures were subtle as evidenced by small effect sizes (η2p of 0.001 to .008), and much smaller than the prominent between-group differences (i.e., patients showing poorer performance than controls; η2p of 0.005 to .037). This rather subtle effect of accelerated aging on cognition could be detected because of the large sample size.

Previous longitudinal studies with recent-onset psychotic patients showed stable cognitive performance after the onset of illness [811]. However, because most studies had small samples and did not assess healthy controls at follow-up, it remained unclear whether schizophrenia patients showed age-related decline in cognition over time and, if so, whether this decline differs from that of controls. We observed greater age-related cognitive deterioration of patients on some cognitive domains, suggesting a subtle progressive degenerative process. However, on other cognitive domains, we observed smaller age-related effects in schizophrenia compared to controls, but there was still substantial impairment in schizophrenia patients across different ages. This pattern suggests relatively stable levels of cognitive impairment after the onset of illness. Given the critical role of cognitive deficits for functional outcome in schizophrenia [34], our finding of continued decline of cognition along with substantial impairment during adulthood suggests that pharmacological and psychosocial interventions to improve cognitive deficits could benefit schizophrenia patients in all phases of illness.

These findings raise the question of which factors contribute to accelerated cognitive aging in schizophrenia when it is seen. One possibility is that the cognitive decline could be due to accelerated brain aging of schizophrenia patients [1, 2, 7, 35]. Faster brain aging based on neuroimaging in schizophrenia was observed across multiple brain regions, which is consistent with the observation from the current study that patients showed accelerated cognitive aging on attention/vigilance and information processing speed. However, accelerated cognitive aging of patients seen in this study was smaller than one might expect based on the effect sizes of the age-related changes at the neural level. We also did not find greater age-related changes on working memory and verbal memory, even though other studies have found that age-related reduction in the brain gray matter volume in schizophrenia was greater in the frontal and temporal regions [2]. It remains to be determined whether other personal factors (e.g., educational level, cognitive reserve) may play a role of protecting performance from age-related changes in schizophrenia.

The changes for social cognition with age in schizophrenia were observed only in reaction time, not accuracy. In studies with healthy adults, declines with aging appear first in processing speed-based measures, followed by accuracy-based measures [36, 37]. Given that age-related decline was observed with accuracy measures on non-social cognitive tasks in this study, one may wonder whether non-social cognition is more vulnerable to age-related changes than social cognition in schizophrenia. However, we did not measure reaction time for non-social cognitive measures, so we cannot directly test this possibility.

This study had several limitations. It employed a cross-sectional design, and it was not possible to determine whether the same pattern of findings would be seen with a longitudinal study. This study included performance-based cognitive measures, and it remains to be determined whether a similar pattern of accelerated aging would be seen in neural measures of cognition. While this study included individuals over a wide range of ages, we did not include individuals older than 60. Thus, we do not know if a similar level of age-related cognitive decline would be present among patients older than 60 years old.

In summary, with a large sample of schizophrenia patients and controls, this study examined the stability versus progression of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia patients showed small, but significantly greater, age-related cognitive decline on attention/vigilance and social cognition (i.e., reaction time) but less age-related changes on working memory and verbal memory, compared to controls. Further, this age-related change in cognitive performance in schizophrenia was much smaller than the substantial between-group difference and was similar across both male and female patients. The current findings indicate that age-related changes in cognition in schizophrenia are subtle and are not uniform across multiple cognitive domains.

Supporting information

S1 Data

(CSV)

S1 Methods

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

Dr. Larry J. Seidman passed away before the submission of the final version of this manuscript. Dr. Junghee Lee accepts responsibility for the integrity and validity of the data collected and analyzed.

Data Availability

Data are contained within Supporting Information File.

Funding Statement

This study was supported by grants R01-MH065571, R01-MH065588, R01-MH065562, R01-MH065707, R01-MH065554, R01-MH065578, R01-MH065558, R01-MH86135, and K01-MH087889 from the National Institute of Mental Health. Other than providing support, the National Institute of Mental Health does not have any further role in this manuscript.

References

  • 1.Koutsouleris N, Davatzikos C, Borgwardt S, Gaser C, Bottlender R, Frodl T, et al. Accelerated brain aging in schizophrenia and beyond: a neuroanatomical marker of psychiatric disorders. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40(5):1140–53. 10.1093/schbul/sbt142 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Cropley VL, Klauser P, Lenroot RK, Bruggemann J, Sundram S, Bousman C, et al. Accelerated Gray and White Matter Deterioration With Age in Schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2017;174(3):286–95. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16050610 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hawco C, Voineskos AN, Radhu N, Rotenberg D, Ameis S, Backhouse FA, et al. Age and gender interactions in white matter of schizophrenia and obsessive compulsive disorder compared to non-psychiatric controls: commonalities across disorders. Brain Imaging Behav. 2017;11(6):1836–48. 10.1007/s11682-016-9657-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kochunov P, Ganjgahi H, Winkler A, Kelly S, Shukla DK, Du X, et al. Heterochronicity of white matter development and aging explains regional patient control differences in schizophrenia. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37(12):4673–88. 10.1002/hbm.23336 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Bergh S, Hjorthoj C, Sorensen HJ, Fagerlund B, Austin S, Secher RG, et al. Predictors and longitudinal course of cognitive functioning in schizophrenia spectrum disorders, 10years after baseline: The OPUS study. Schizophr Res. 2016;175(1–3):57–63. 10.1016/j.schres.2016.03.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Allen DN, Strauss GP, Barchard KA, Vertinski M, Carpenter WT, Buchanan RW. Differences in developmental changes in academic and social premorbid adjustment between males and females with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2013;146(1–3):132–7. 10.1016/j.schres.2013.01.032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Sheffield JM, Repovs G, Harms MP, Carter CS, Gold JM, MacDonald AW 3rd, et al. Evidence for Accelerated Decline of Functional Brain Network Efficiency in Schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2016;42(3):753–61. 10.1093/schbul/sbv148 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Rund BR, Barder HE, Evensen J, Haahr U, ten Velden Hegelstad W, Joa I, et al. Neurocognition and Duration of Psychosis: A 10-year Follow-up of First-Episode Patients. Schizophr Bull. 2016;42(1):87–95. 10.1093/schbul/sbv083 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bonner-Jackson A, Grossman LS, Harrow M, Rosen C. Neurocognition in schizophrenia: a 20-year multi-follow-up of the course of processing speed and stored knowledge. Compr Psychiatry. 2010;51(5):471–9. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2010.02.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Hoff AL, Sakuma M, Wieneke M, Horon R, Kushner M, DeLisi LE. Longitudinal neuropsychological follow-up study of patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156(9):1336–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Gold S, Arndt S, Nopoulos P, O'Leary DS, Andreasen NC. Longitudinal study of cognitive function in first-episode and recent-onset schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;156(9):1342–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.McCleery A, Lee J, Fiske AP, Ghermezi L, Hayata JN, Hellemann GS, et al. Longitudinal stability of social cognition in schizophrenia: A 5-year follow-up of social perception and emotion processing. Schizophr Res. 2016;176(2–3):467–72. 10.1016/j.schres.2016.07.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Swerdlow NR, Gur RE, Braff DL. Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS) assessment of endophenotypes for schizophrenia: an introduction to this Special Issue of Schizophrenia Research. Schizophr Res. 2015;163(1–3):9–16. 10.1016/j.schres.2014.09.047 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Maj M, Starace F, Pirozzi R. A family study of DSM-III-R schizoaffective disorder, depressive type, compared with schizophrenia and psychotic and nonpsychotic major depression. Am J Psychiatry. 1991;148(5):612–6. 10.1176/ajp.148.5.612 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Cardno AG, Rijsdijk FV, West RM, Gottesman II, Craddock N, Murray RM, et al. A twin study of schizoaffective-mania, schizoaffective-depression, and other psychotic syndromes. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2012;159B(2):172–82. 10.1002/ajmg.b.32011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Andreasen NC, Rice J, Endicott J, Coryell W, Grove WM, Reich T. Familial rates of affective disorder. A report from the National Institute of Mental Health Collaborative Study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44(5):461–9. 10.1001/archpsyc.1987.01800170083011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Keshavan MS, Morris DW, Sweeney JA, Pearlson G, Thaker G, Seidman LJ, et al. A dimensional approach to the psychosis spectrum between bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: the Schizo-Bipolar Scale. Schizophr Res. 2011;133(1–3):250–4. 10.1016/j.schres.2011.09.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hall RC. Global assessment of functioning. A modified scale. Psychosomatics. 1995;36(3):267–75. 10.1016/S0033-3182(95)71666-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Andreasen NC. The scale for the assessment of negative symptoms (SANS). Iowa City, IA: The University of Iowa; 1984. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Andreasen NC. The scale for the assessment of positive symptoms (SAPS). Iowa City, IA: The University of Iowa; 1984. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale. 3rd ed San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Gold JM, Carpenter C, Randolph C, Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR. Auditory working memory and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54(2):159–65. 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830140071013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Nuechterlein KH, Green MF, Calkins ME, Greenwood TA, Gur RE, Gur RC, et al. Attention/vigilance in schizophrenia: performance results from a large multi-site study of the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS). Schizophr Res. 2015;163(1–3):38–46. 10.1016/j.schres.2015.01.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Cornblatt BA, Risch NJ, Faris G, Friedman D, Erlenmeyer-Kimling L. The Continuous Performance Test, identical pairs version (CPT-IP): I. New findings about sustained attention in normal families. Psychiatry Res. 1988;26(2):223–38. 10.1016/0165-1781(88)90076-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Nuechterlein KH, Parasuraman R, Jiang Q. Visual sustained attention: Image degradation produces rapid sensitivity decrement over time. Science. 1983;220:327–29. 10.1126/science.6836276 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Stone WS, Mesholam-Gately RI, Braff DL, Calkins ME, Freedman R, Green MF, et al. California Verbal Learning Test-II performance in schizophrenia as a function of ascertainment strategy: comparing the first and second phases of the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS). Schizophr Res. 2015;163(1–3):32–7. 10.1016/j.schres.2014.10.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Delis DC, Kramer J, Kaplan E, Ober BA. California Verbal Learning Test, 2nd Edition San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Gur RC, Richard J, Hughett P, Calkins ME, Macy L, Bilker WB, et al. A cognitive neuroscience-based computerized battery for efficient measurement of individual differences: Standardization and initial construct validation. J Neurosci Methods. 2010;187(2):254–62. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.11.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Gur RC, Braff DL, Calkins ME, Dobie DJ, Freedman R, Green MF, et al. Neurocognitive performance in family-based and case-control studies of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2015;163(1–3):17–23. 10.1016/j.schres.2014.10.049 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Lee J, Green MF, Calkins ME, Greenwood TA, Gur RE, Gur RC, et al. Verbal working memory in schizophrenia from the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS) study: the moderating role of smoking status and antipsychotic medications. Schizophr Res. 2015;163(1–3):24–31. 10.1016/j.schres.2014.08.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Nuechterlein KH. Vigilance in schizophrenia and related disorders In: Steinhauer SR, Gruzelier JH, Zubin J, editors. Handbook of schizophrenia. Neuropsychology, Psychophysiology and Information Processing. 5 Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1991. p. 397–433. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Green DM, Swets JA. Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley; 1966. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Cohen J, Milles J, Shevlin M. Applying regression and correlations: A guide for students and researchers. London: Sage; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Green MF. Cognitive impairment and functional outcome in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2006;67 Suppl 9:3–8; discussion 36–42. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Shahab S, Mulsant BH, Levesque ML, Calarco N, Nazeri A, Wheeler AL, et al. Brain structure, cognition, and brain age in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and healthy controls. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2019;44(5):898–906. 10.1038/s41386-018-0298-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Starns JJ, Ratcliff R. The effects of aging on the speed-accuracy compromise: Boundary optimality in the diffusion model. Psychol Aging. 2010;25(2):377–90. 10.1037/a0018022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Vaportzis E, Georgiou-Karistianis N, Stout JC. Dual task performance in normal aging: a comparison of choice reaction time tasks. PLoS One. 2013;8(3):e60265 10.1371/journal.pone.0060265 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Kenji Hashimoto

18 Feb 2020

PONE-D-20-00548

The Effects of Age and Sex on Cognitive Impairment in Schizophrenia: Findings from the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS) Study

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Lee,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

The reviewers addressed some minor concerns about your manuscript. Please revise your manuscript carefully.

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by Apr 03 2020 11:59PM. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Kenji Hashimoto, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements:

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at http://www.plosone.org/attachments/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and http://www.plosone.org/attachments/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Thank you for stating the following in the Competing Interests section:

"Dr. Green has been a paid consultant for Biogen, Click Therapeutics, Lundbeck, and Roche. He is a member of the Scientific Advisory Board of Cadent. Dr. Light has been a paid consultant for Astellas Pharma, Inc, Hepatares Therapeutics, NeuroSig, and Taketa Phamaceutical Company, Ltd, and received grants from Boerhinger-Ingelheim. Dr. Nuechterlein received grants from Janssen Pharmaceutica, Posit Science Corporation, and has been a paid consultant for Janssen Pharmaceutica, Astellas Pharma, Inc, Biogen, Inc., Genetech, Inc, MedinCell, Otsuka Pharmaceutical Co, Ltd, Takeda Pharmaceutical Company, Ltd, and Teva Pharmaceutical Industries, Ltd. No other disclosure is reported. "

Please confirm that this does not alter your adherence to all PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials, by including the following statement: "This does not alter our adherence to  PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials.” (as detailed online in our guide for authors http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/competing-interests).  If there are restrictions on sharing of data and/or materials, please state these. Please note that we cannot proceed with consideration of your article until this information has been declared.

Please include your updated Competing Interests statement in your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

Please know it is PLOS ONE policy for corresponding authors to declare, on behalf of all authors, all potential competing interests for the purposes of transparency. PLOS defines a competing interest as anything that interferes with, or could reasonably be perceived as interfering with, the full and objective presentation, peer review, editorial decision-making, or publication of research or non-research articles submitted to one of the journals. Competing interests can be financial or non-financial, professional, or personal. Competing interests can arise in relationship to an organization or another person. Please follow this link to our website for more details on competing interests: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/competing-interests

3. Please ensure that you refer to Figure 1 in your text as, if accepted, production will need this reference to link the reader to the figure.

4. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: In this study, the authors sought to determine whether age-related cognitive delcine is more evident in schizophrenia patinets compared to healthy control people. Data from a sizable number of subjects indicate that this is the case for performace on attention/vigilance. On the other hand, the age-dependent decline for working memory and verbal learning memory was, in fact, more apparent in control subjects.

Although some of these findings may not accord with the hypothesis raised, the persistent deterioration of cognitive function in schizophrenia, reportd here with the large number of participants, would add to the literature on the schizophrenia research.

Reviewer #2: Lee et al. investigated whether patients with schizophrenia show accelerated age-related decline in cognition using 1,415 schizophrenia patients and 1,062 healthy controls from the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS) Study. Multiple cognitive domains, the Letter-Number Span Task, two forms of the Continuous Performance Test, the California Verbal Learning Test, Second Edition, the Penn Emotion Identification Test and the Penn Facial Memory Test, were assessed. They found that patients showed greater age-related deterioration in some cognitive domains, while controls showed greater age-related deterioration in other domains. However, they suggested the age-related changes were much smaller than between case-control differences. This manuscript is well-written and straight-forwarded. The methods and results seem easy to understand. I recommend publishing this study with minor revisions.

There were some concerns as follows;

1. ‘Age-related changes (η2 of 0.001 to .008) were much smaller than between-group differences.’ Please add effect sizes for the group differences to compare effect sizes between the age-related changes and the group differences in abstract and discussion sections.

2. ‘Participants included 1,415 patients diagnosed with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, depressed type’. Why did the authors recruit only depressed type of schizoaffective disorder? How many patients with schizoaffective disorder, depressed type, included in the patients? If the patients were restricted to only patients with schizophrenia, were their outcomes changed?

3. ‘Effect sizes are provided for any significant effects (i.e., partial eta square [η2 p]s.’ Please add scales for large, medium, and small effect sizes (η2).

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes: Kazutaka Ohi, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Gifu University Graduate School of Medicine, Gifu, Japan

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2020 May 13;15(5):e0232855. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0232855.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


16 Apr 2020

We would like to thank the reviewers for their thoughtful comments and the Editor for the opportunity to submit a revised manuscript. As shown below, we address each comment from the Editor and Reviewer 2 and indicate where changes are made in the revised manuscript. The Reviewer 1 did not have any comments. The original comments from the Reviewer 2 are in italics.

Editor:

1. As requested, we ensure that our manuscript meets PLOS One’ style requirement, including those for file naming.

2. As requested, we now clarify that the conflict of interests indicated in the Competing Interests section does not alter our adherence to all PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials (page XX). We also include our updated Competing Interests Statement in the cover letter.

3. As requested, we refer to Fig 1 in the revised manuscript (pages 10-12).

4. As requested, we now include captions for the Supporting Information Files at the end of the revised manuscript (page 17).

Reviewer 2:

1. ‘Age-related changes (n2 of .001 to .008) were much smaller than between-group differences.’ Please add effect sizes for the group differences to compare effect sizes between the age-related changes and the group differences in the abstract and discussion section.

As requested, we now add effect sizes for the between-group differences (�2p of .005 to .037) in the abstract and discussion section (pages 10-12).

2. ‘Participants included 1,415 patients diagnosed with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, depressed type.’ Why did the authors recruit only depressed type of schizophrenia disorder? How many patients with schizoaffective disorder, depressed type, include in the patients? If the patients were restricted to only patients with schizophrenia, were their outcomes changed?

This is an important point and we appreciate the opportunity to clarify the rationale of our sample selection. We decided to recruit patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, depressed type for the following reasons. First, findings from genetic studies suggest that schizoaffective disorder, depressed type, is more genetically similar to schizophrenia compared to schizoaffective disorder, bipolar type (Maj et al. 2001; Cardno et al., 2012). Second, schizoaffective disorder, depressed type, showed illness course similar to that of schizophrenia, compared to schizoaffective disorder, bipolar type (Andreasen et al., 1987; and Keshavan et al., 2011). This is now included in the revised manuscript (page xx).

We also mention that our patient sample includes 59 patients with schizoaffective disorder, depressed type, and 1,315 patients with schizophrenia. Given the very small proportion of patients with schizoaffective disorder (about 4%), we decided to not run additional analyses excluding them.

3. ‘Effect sizes are provided for any significant effects (i.e., partial eta square [n2p]s.’ Please add sales for large, medium, and small effect sizes (n2).

The general rule of thumb regarding the magnitude of effect size for partial eta square, is: 0.01 = small effect, 0.06 = medium effect, and 0.14 = large effect (Cohen, Milles, & Shevlin, 2001). We now mention this in the revised manuscript (page 9).

References:

Andreasen, N. C., Rice, J., Endicott, J., Coryell, W., Grove, W. M., & Reich, T. (1987). Familial rates of affective disorder: A report from the National Institute of Mental Health Collaborative Study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 461-469.

Cardno, A. G., Rijsdijk, F. V., West, R. M., et al. (2012). A twin study of schizoaffective-mania, schizoaffective-depression, and other psychotic syndromes. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet, 159B, 172-182.

Cohen, J., Milles, J., & Shevlin, M. (2001). Applying regression and correlations: A guide for students and researchers. London; Sage.

Maj, M., Starace, F., & Pirozzi, R. (1991). A family study of DSM-III-R schizoaffective disorder, depressed type, compared with schizophrenia and psychotic and nonpsychotic major depression. Am J Psychiatry, 48, 612-616.

Keshavan, M. S., Morris, D. W., Sweeney, J. A., Pearlson, G., Thaker, Gu, Seidman, L. J., Eack, S. M., & Tamminga, C. (2011). A dimensional approach to the psychosis spectrum between bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: The Schizo-Bipolar Scale. Schizophrenia Research, 133, 250-254.

Decision Letter 1

Kenji Hashimoto

23 Apr 2020

The Effects of Age and Sex on Cognitive Impairment in Schizophrenia: Findings from the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS) Study

PONE-D-20-00548R1

Dear Dr. Lee,

We are pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it complies with all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you will receive an e-mail containing information on the amendments required prior to publication. When all required modifications have been addressed, you will receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will proceed to our production department and be scheduled for publication.

Shortly after the formal acceptance letter is sent, an invoice for payment will follow. To ensure an efficient production and billing process, please log into Editorial Manager at https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the "Update My Information" link at the top of the page, and update your user information. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, you must inform our press team as soon as possible and no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

With kind regards,

Kenji Hashimoto, PhD

Section Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #2: Yes: Kazutaka Ohi

Acceptance letter

Kenji Hashimoto

29 Apr 2020

PONE-D-20-00548R1

The Effects of Age and Sex on Cognitive Impairment in Schizophrenia: Findings from the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS) Study

Dear Dr. Lee:

I am pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

For any other questions or concerns, please email plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE.

With kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Prof. Kenji Hashimoto

Section Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Data

    (CSV)

    S1 Methods

    (DOCX)

    Data Availability Statement

    Data are contained within Supporting Information File.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES