Skip to main content
Personality Neuroscience logoLink to Personality Neuroscience
. 2018 Aug 10;1:e12. doi: 10.1017/pen.2018.10

Affective Neuroscience Theory and Personality: An Update

Christian Montag 1,2,*, Kenneth L Davis 3
PMCID: PMC7219919  PMID: 32435731

Abstract

The present work gives a short overview of central aspects of Jaak Panksepp’s Affective Neuroscience Theory (AN theory) and its relevance for modern personality neuroscience. In contrast to the widely used Big Five approach to studying and understanding human personality, AN theory provides researchers with a distinct roadmap to the biological basis of personality, including molecular and neuroanatomical candidates, to understand individual differences in human behavior. Such molecular and neuroanatomical brain candidates have been derived by means of electrical brain stimulation and pharmacological challenges, while investigating primary emotional systems anchored in the subcortical mammalian brain. Research results derived from the study of emotions in mammals are also of relevance for humans because ancient layers of our minds—those layers where primary emotions originate—have been homologously conserved across species. From an evolutionary perspective, this makes sense because primal emotions represent “built-in tools for survival” for all mammals. In this context, Montag and Panksepp recently illustrated a potential ancient neurobiological effect by carving out robust associations between individual differences in primary emotions (assessed via self-report) and the Big Five in a cross-cultural study with data from the United States, Germany, and China. These associations together with some ideas derived from MacLean’s Triune Brain concept highlighted (a) that primary emotions likely represent the phylogenetically oldest parts of human personality and (b) that primary emotions influence human personality in a bottom-up fashion given their localization in ancient subcortical brain regions. A comment on the work by Montag and Panksepp asked for insights on putative links between primary emotions and facets of the Big Five. Therefore, we provide some first insights into such associations from recent Germany data. In addition, the present work provides a new short version of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales to assess individual differences in primary emotions.

Key words: Affective Neuroscience Theory, Jaak Panksepp, Big Five, five-factor model of personality, primary emotional systems, personality neuroscience

Keywords: Depression, Anxiety/fear, Hormones, Emotion, Systematic review

1. In memory of Jaak Panksepp (1943–2017)

1.1. What is Affective Neuroscience (AN) Theory?

The term Affective Neuroscience (AN) was first coined by Jaak Panksepp (1992), and his full theory (Panksepp, 1998) encompasses seven primary emotional systems that undergird psychological well-being and (affective) brain disorders (Panksepp, 2006). Moreover, his theory deals with questions about the affective lives of animals and consciousness (Panksepp, 2005). Panksepp’s main research methods consisted of electrical stimulation of avian and mammalian brains as well as pharmacological challenges.

Basic features of primary emotional systems were outlined by Panksepp (1998, 2010) as follows. First, activation of each neural network underlying a primary emotion should elicit a characteristic emotional–behavioral action pattern (such as emotional PLAY behavior usually accompanied by 50-kHz chirps in rats; Panksepp & Burgdorf, 2003). Second, activity in such an emotional neural network is initially only triggered by unconditional stimuli (such as the inborn tendency to react with FEAR to the sight of a snake). Third, these basic emotional neural networks are connected to secondary and tertiary brain levels to facilitate learning such that after learning these primary neural networks also can be activated by previously neutral stimuli. Thus, a child learns to FEAR a hot stove. Fourth, with maturity mammals exhibit stronger cortical control over the ancient neural networks underlying primary emotions. For example, most children gradually learn to better regulate their affective emotions (see also a study on the development of frontal lobe functions by Romine & Reynolds, 2005). Fifth, the emotional arousal elicited by these primary emotional systems influences sensory gating processes of the brain. This means that activation of emotional neural networks can modify what information ultimately will be processed from the brain (and also in what manner). In addition, the salience of a stimulus is modified by the activity of neural networks underlying primary emotional systems: When strong FEAR is triggered a person could have difficulty shifting attention to other stimuli aside from the FEAR eliciting source. For example, when a person has a weapon pointed at him/her, they will not remember anything except the weapon (“weapon effect;“ e.g., Tooley, Brigham, Maass, & Bothwell, 1987). Sixth, the arousal generated by activation of the neural network underlying a primary emotion persists beyond the initially triggering event. Please note our usage of the term affect generally conforms with Panksepp’s rule of thumb: “Affects are the subjectively experienced aspects of emotions, commonly called feelings” (Panksepp, 2010, p. 534).

Altogether, Panksepp mapped seven primary emotions across the mammalian brain. These seven emotional systems include the SEEKING, LUST, CARE and PLAY systems (positive emotions) and FEAR, RAGE/ANGER, and PANIC/SADNESS 1 (negative emotions). Panksepp did not include disgust in his group of primary emotions because his evidence strongly suggested that disgust was a sensory affect, which did not satisfy the criteria to be designated a primary emotional action system (Panksepp, 1998). All Panksepp’s primary emotions represent evolved “tools for survival,” which have been largely conserved across mammalian species including homo sapiens. Activation of the SEEKING system energizes the mammalian organism and provides energy for obtaining resources such as the search for food (the homeostatic hunger system), or a mating partner (LUST). The SEEKING system is theoretically being utilized by other primary emotions such as the case of FEAR and SEEKING safety. The SEEKING system is also very likely involved in the initiation of PLAY (for further explanations on the relationship between emotion and motivation see Montag & Panksepp, 2017, p. 4).

Both the LUST and CARE circuitry are closely entwined. The LUST system must represent the evolutionary older emotion because programs for reproduction naturally must preceed the evolutionary development of an emotional CARE system prototypically for nurturing the offspring, but also (perhaps not only in humans) for caring for family, close relatives, and friends. Finally, the PLAY system has been observed in nearly all mammals (perhaps bred out of lab mice) given its relevance for learning social competencies (Pellegrini, 1988) and shaping motoric skills (Pellegrini & Smith, 1998). In particular, rough and tumble play could exert a critical effect on shaping young minds as in learning adaptive limits for interacting socially (Panksepp, 2007; Pellis & Pellis, 2007).

On the side of negative emotions, the FEAR system is of relevance to help the organism get out of a danger zone, for example, via a flight response. The RAGE/ANGER system is triggered by protecting life resources as well as escaping bodily restraint such as being held by a predator. The PANIC/SADNESS system is most strongly triggered by separation distress, such as a child losing contact with a parent or someone being apart from a loved one. This can also be the case when couples break up, and in situations of homesickness (see Montag, Widenhorn-Müller, Panksepp, & Kiefer, 2017).

1.2. AN theory and personality

Many applications of Panksepp’s AN theory have been put forward in the last years, in particular, those to better understand brain disorders with a focus on affective components. Among others Montag et al. (2017b) demonstrated that low SEEKING, high FEAR, and high SADNESS might be at the heart of depression, something also already noted earlier (e.g., Panksepp & Watt, 2011; Panksepp, Wright, Döbrössy, Schlaepfer, & Coenen, 2014).

A relatively new scientific area also profiting from AN theory represents the area of personality neuroscience, which tries to carve out individual differences in brain structure/functionality including molecular foundations to get insights into the neuroscientific/biological underpinnings of human personality. In this context, Davis, Panksepp, and Normansell (2003) published a self-report questionnaire called Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS) assessing individual differences in primary emotions constructed as defined in Panksepp’s AN theory. We believe this self-report inventory contributes to the field, because (i) there is a long tradition of understanding personality by investigating individual differences in strongly genetically determined temperaments (one can study individual differences in temperaments from early in life of infants; e.g., Rothbart, 1986) and (ii) temperament is a concept closely linked to individual differences in emotionality and therefore also of relevance to understand primary affects. The importance of this inventory will be highlighted in more detail below when contrasting the AN theory’s approach to study human personality with the prominent Big Five Model of Personality. However, it is anticipated that beyond its comparisons with Big Five scales, additional biological validation of the ANPS primary emotion scales will be forthcoming to better link these scales to the massive amount of preclinical evidence Panksepp’s group has provided. In the realm of personality psychology, it has been hypothesized that individual differences in primary emotions represent ancient evolutionary foundations of human personality with primary emotions being anchored in the subcortical mammalian brain. These primary emotions drive our behavior in a bottom-up fashion (see also similarities to MacLean‘s (1990) Triune Brain Concept 2 ).

We are aware of current discussions in the scientific scene (between the affective neuroscientists and the constructivists/cognitive neuroscientists) even arguing against the idea that primary emotions exist or that primary emotions (more or less uniquely) arise from subcortical areas (see the detailed constructivist’s view in Barrett, 2017; for a discussion with the cognitive neuroscientist’s view see Panksepp, Lane, Solms, & Smith, 2017). In our opinion abundant evidence speaks for basic emotions arising from subcortical regions of the mammalian brain, perhaps with best arguments arising from decorticated animals showing emotional responses after deep brain stimulation (Panksepp, Normansell, Cox, & Siviy, 1994), but also see human evidence (Damasio, Damasio, & Tranel, 2013; Merker, 2007). However, Panksepp never argued against the learned cortical regulation of emotions (see the Three-Level Nested Brain Hierarchy, Panksepp, 2011), and wisely stated the current discussion might be resolved, if we accept that “such debates may simply reflect investigators working at different levels of control” (Panksepp, 2010, p. 536).

From this bottom-up neuroscience perspective, AN theory uniquely offers a detailed guide for understanding the ancient origins of human personality and offers a reinterpretation of the widely used Big Five Model of Personality. The Big Five Model has been constructed on a lexical background. This means that by applying factorial analysis, psychologists started in the thirties/forties of the last century to identify five broad personality dimensions derived from human language, sometimes summarized with the acronym OCEAN (for the beginning of this kind of research see e.g., Allport & Odbert, 1936; Fiske, 1949): Openness to Experience describes humans who are rather intellectual, creative and open to try new things. Conscientiousness describes diligent and punctual humans who prefer planning and an orderly approach to life. Extraversion relates to humans being described as socially outgoing, talkative, and assertive. Agreeable persons are empathic and good team-players, whereas Neuroticism (or low Emotional Stability) is linked to negative emotionality, being depressed and anxious. Although this model has represented a major step for personality psychology (see McCrae & John, 1992), the statistical methods used to arrive at the five dimensions make it clear that the Big Five represents a descriptive rather than explanatory model of personality. Even with heritability studies demonstrating a solid genetic basis, the Big Five largely lack a theoretical basis for hypothesizing on potentially involved brain neuroanatomy/molecules underlying individual differences in human personality, a significant shortcoming in the world of neuroscience.

How is AN personality-based theory different? As earlier mentioned, Panksepp mapped out his AN theory in detail including the underlying neuroanatomies and neurotransmitter/neuropeptide systems of the earlier mentioned seven primary emotions. If researchers now assess individual differences in primary emotions (e.g., with the ANPS), they indirectly can assume (based on findings from AN theory) which brain anatomy and linked neurotransmitter systems might be involved in certain emotional personality dimensions such as being a high PLAY or high PANIC/SADNESS endophenotype—helping, for example, researchers disentangle the molecular foundations of the emotional parts of personality. To further illustrate this with an example: The SADNESS circuitry is strongly innervated by the opioid and oxytocin brain transmitter systems (Panksepp & Watt, 2011). Thus, physical hugs by a close friend or family member elicit the release of these transmitters and thereby downregulate the activity of the SADNESS system (theoretically explaining why social support in sad times feels so good; see also Holt-Lunstad, Birmingham, & Light, 2008; Løseth, Leknes, & Ellingsen, 2016).

The ANPS (Davis, Panksepp, & Normansell, 2003) was published to facilitate assessing individual differences in primary emotions in accordance with Panksepp’s AN theory (see below for more detail). Using the ANPS, earlier work by Davis, Panksepp, and Normansell (2003), but also evidence from cross-cultural findings by Montag and Panksepp (2017) demonstrated that higher trait SADNESS is robustly linked to higher Big Five Neuroticism. Therefore, screening the genome for genes impacting oxytocinergic and opioid transmission might reveal genetic variants being linked specifically to individual differences in trait PANIC/SADNESS rather than focusing on the higher-order personality trait of Neuroticism. As such, AN theory can be used as a roadmap to study the biological basis of human personality with a clear focus on subcortical emotional regions of the mammalian brain (for a detailed overview on the brain and molecular candidates see the Appendix).

Of note, also other important biologically oriented personality theories exist to guide researchers in the study of personality neuroscience. Among these are the works by Eysenck (1967) with his prominent dimensions Neuroticism and Extraversion, and the works by his former student Jeffrey Gray and his Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS), Behavioral Approach System (BAS), and Fight-Flight-Freeze System (e.g., Gray, 1972; Gray & McNaughton, 2000) and also Cloninger, Svrakic, and Przybeck (1993). Eysenck’s theory on the biological basis of personality proposed first interesting hypotheses on the underlying neural foundations of personality. Whereas the Intro-/Extraversion dimensions and individual differences in arousability should be linked to a massive structure called the ascending reticular activation system (see Yeo, Chang, & Jang, 2013, for a detailed anatomical description), an overactivity/lower threshold of arousability of the limbic system in reaction to stress should underlie Neuroticism (vs. Emotional Stability). Cloninger’s biosocial theory of personality became prominent for providing testable hypotheses on which neurotransmitters should underlie his prominent temperament traits called Novelty Seeking (low dopamine), Harm Avoidance (high serotonin), and Reward Dependence (low norepinephrine; Cloninger, 1986). Panksepp provided both detailed ideas with respect to the neuroanatomy (such as Eysenck) and the transmitter/neuropeptide systems (such as Cloninger) underlying primary emotional systems.

On a questionnaire level, there is a clear overlap between the scales assessing the different personality theories. As provided in Montag, Reuter, Jurkiewicz, Markett, and Panksepp (2013) one can see that, for example, (Eysenck’s) Neuroticism and (ANPS) FEAR correlate with r=.72 and Cloninger’s Harm Avoidance and (ANPS) FEAR with r=.57. With respect to the proposed biological systems underlying the different personality theories an overlap can also be observed in parts; for example, Panksepp suggests an influence of dopamine on the SEEKING system, whereas SEEKING likely is associated with Novelty Seeking (and this in turn according to Cloninger with dopamine). Although overlaps between theories can be observed on the self-report and biological level, as mentioned, Panksepp’s approach to studying the mammalian brain by means of electrical stimulation and pharmacological challenges led to a much more fine-grained picture of the primary emotional systems driving human personality in a bottom-up fashion. Originally, we refrained from providing a detailed list of all brain candidates underlying primary emotional systems, because it is redundant information available in numerous publications (Montag & Panksepp, 2017, in press; Panksepp, 2011). Nevertheless, it became apparent in the review process, that it would be a help for readers to not need to switch to these older publications. Therefore, we provide the readers with this information in the Appendix of this work, again.

Finally, we want to mention that Gray and McNaughton’s revised reinforcement sensitivity theory also represents a much elaborated biologically framework, which in its revised version, in particular, made large progress in disentangling the emotions of FEAR and anxiety (see also McNaughton & Corr, 2004). In Gray and McNaughton’s well-respected work called “The Neuropsychology of Anxiety” they summed up: “… we identity fear (elicited by exposure to aversive stimuli without conflict) with activity in the amygdala, and anxiety (fear to which an approach-based conflict is added) with concurrent activity in the amygdala and septo-hippocampal system” (2000, pp. 122–123). Disentangling anxiety and fear is something that AN theory researchers have not attempted to do because AN theory assumes that all primary emotions arise subcortically and that cortically measured expressions of emotions are likely re-representing subcortical emotional foundations. Much additional preclinical research will be required to adequately resolve such issues. Here, we refer to recent approaches in personality neuroscience, using also self-reports attempting to disentangle both emotions (Corr & Cooper, 2016; Reuter, Cooper, Smillie, Markett, & Montag, 2015).

1.3. The ANPS as a measure of individual differences in primary emotional systems

The ANPS was first published by Davis, Panksepp, and Normansell (2003) and revised by Davis and Panksepp (2011). The ANPS consists of six scales item, with 14 items each assessing Panksepp’s six primary emotions and 12 additional items assessing spirituality. The latter dimension clearly does not represent a primary emotion and was included due to Jaak’s interest in working with recovering alcoholics in Alcoholics Anonymous programs as well its relevance in the treatment of psychiatric patients (e.g., Angres, 2010; see also the self-transcendence dimension in Cloninger’s Temperament and Character Inventory shortly called TCI). The ANPS only covers six instead of seven primal emotions, because items on LUST have not been included given the large possibility to trigger social desirably biased answers carrying over on the answers given on the remaining scales (see Schmitt, Allik, McCrae, & Benet-Martinez, 2007, p. 182). A 2011 revision of the ANPS changed only a small number of items from its earlier version to achieve better psychometric properties. Moreover, it is possible to construct a short lie-scale consisting of five items. The remaining items of the ANPS represent filler items. Davis, Panksepp, and Normansell (2003) stated that all items included on a primary emotion scale were written to reflect the experience of that emotion based on AN theory. That is, “Items for all scales were written with the goal of accessing personal feelings and behavior rather than more cognitive social judgments” (Davis, Panksepp, & Normansell, 2003, p. 56). Thus, the ANPS represents “only” an indirect assessment of one’s emotional nature in the context of personality. In detail, Davis and Panksepp stated “we interpret the ANPS scales as tertiary (thought-mediated) approximations of the influence of the various primary emotional systems in people’s lives. However, it is our working hypothesis that the subcortical primary-processes neural systems, where the foundations of emotions reside, can generate individual differences in normal personality as well as the affective imbalances characterizing mental disorders” (2011, p. 1952). Meanwhile, the ANPS has been translated into many languages. Among these languages are French (Pahlavan, Mouchiroud, Zenasni, & Panksepp, 2008), Spanish (Abella, Panksepp, Manga, Bárcena, & Iglesias, 2011), Turkish (Özkarar-Gradwohl et al., 2014), Italian (Pascazio et al. 2015), and German (Reuter, Panksepp, Davis, & Montag, 2017). A Chinese version has also been translated and published recently (Sindermann et al., 2018) and a Serbian version is following soon (please contact author Christian Montag for further information).

1.4. ANPS validation studies

The ANPS has seen a number of validation studies in the last years confirming clear relationships with the Big Five scales as well as clarifying where the Big Five fails to address key primary emotions. Among these are studies using the ANPS to better understand personality disorders (Geir, Selsbakk, Theresa, & Sigmund, 2014; Karterud et al., 2016), depression (Montag et al., 2017), multiple sclerosis (Sindermann et al., 2017), stroke (Farinelli et al., 2013), creativity (Reuter et al., 2005), bipolar disorder (Savitz, van der Merwe, & Ramesar, 2008a, 2008b), and chill experiences (Laeng, Eidet, Sulutvedt, & Panksepp, 2016). A new study investigated how growing up in rural versus urban areas shape primary emotional systems (Sindermann et al., 2017). The ANPS has also been investigated in the context of vengefulness (Sindermann et al., 2018) and recently religious/spiritual well-being (Hiebler-Ragger et al., 2018). Aside from this, several studies used (molecular) genetic and brain imaging approaches to search for associations with the ANPS (Deris, Montag, Reuter, Weber, & Markett, 2017; Felten, Montag, Markett, Walter, & Reuter, 2011; Montag, Fiebach, Kirsch, & Reuter, 2011; Montag, Sindermann, Becker, & Panksepp, 2016; Reuter, Weber, Fiebach, Elger, & Montag, 2009). Moreover, also endocrinological approaches have been applied (Sindermann et al., 2016; van der Westhuizen & Solms, 2015). In sum, these first studies show that it is feasible to search for associations between biological markers and individual differences with the ANPS.

As mentioned earlier, individual differences in primary emotions could represent the phylogenetically oldest part of human personality driving our behavior in a bottom-up fashion. In a recent paper of Montag and Panksepp (2017) both researchers provide evidence for robust associations between the Big Five/Five-Factor Model of Personality and primary emotions from a cross-cultural study including the original US-data from Davis, Panksepp, and Normansell (2003) together with new data from China and Germany. In all studies, robust correlation patterns could be observed with FEAR, SADNESS, and ANGER being the primary emotions driving Neuroticism, high CARE and low ANGER being the driving forces of high Agreeableness, high SEEKING being the bottom-up force of Openness to Experience and high PLAY being the primary emotional foundation of Extraversion. Note that no link was robustly observed between Conscientiousness and primary emotions across cultures, which fits with the observation that Conscientiousness has only been reliably measured in chimpanzees and homo sapiens (Gosling & John, 1999; King & Figueredo, 1997) and brown capuchin monkeys (Morton et al., 2013). The model relating primary emotions and the Big Five of Personality has been slightly modified in Montag and Panksepp (in press) with the hypothesis that high SEEKING could also be a driving force for high Extraversion, because both constructs are linked to reward processing (for extraversion and reward processing see Smillie, 2013; the medial forebrain bundle is part of “the intrinsic reward SEEKING system of the brain”; Coenen, Schlaepfer, Maedler, & Panksepp, 2011, p. 1972). Nevertheless, this idea was only partially supported in the data presented by Montag and Panksepp (2017): In this work, only two out of three correlation patterns pointed toward such an association. In addition, please see that also in the new data set below the association between Extraversion and SEEKING is lower compared with the other robustly observed associations as described above (see Table 3). The present section dealt foremost with data stemming from self-report. It is anticipated that beyond the comparison of ANPS with Big Five scales, additional biological validation of the ANPS primary emotion scales will be forthcoming to better link these scales to the massive amount of preclinical evidence Panksepp’s group has provided. Nevertheless it needs to be mentioned that currently the ANPS, although theoretically grounded in Panksepp’s AN theory, is probably no more directly linked to the mentioned primary emotions than Carver and White’s scales (1994) to the BIS/BAS or Reuter and Montag’s revised RST-Q (Reinforcement Sensitivtiy Theory-Questionnaire; Reuter et al., 2015) to the BIS/BAS/Fight-Flight-Freeze System and the neural underpinnings of Gray’s systems. Again, future studies are much needed to demonstrate which scales are most closely linked to the neural circuitry of interest.

Table 3.

Correlation patterns between the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales and the Big Five as assessed with the 42-item Big Five short-scale

Big Five SEEKING FEAR CARE ANGER PLAY SADNESS
O .42** .19* .21** −.19* −.07 .20**
C .29** .09 .15* .00 .06 −.04
E .24** −.50** .05 .00 .54** −.36**
A .28** −.18** .38** −.45** .50** −.24**
N −.21** .72** .13 .44** −.40** .63**

Note. Bold printed correlation patterns indicate similarities with the works by Davis, Panksepp, and Normansell (2003) and Montag and Panksepp (2017); italic printed correlation patterns could be 42-item Big Five short-scale specific.

*p<.05; **p<.01.

1.5. Primary emotions as assessed with the ANPS and 42-item Big Five short-scale personality facets

In a recent commentary by Di Domenico and Ryan (2017) on Montag and Panksepp’s work (2017), the authors pointed among others to the importance of exploring associations between facets of the Big Five and individual differences in primary emotions. Among others they hypothesized that PLAY would be associated with the subscale Enthusiasm of Extraversion, but less with the subscale of Assertiveness: “Indeed, like PLAY, Enthusiasm has been linked to both dopamine and endogenous opioids, whereas Assertiveness appears to be more strongly associated with dopamine“ (Di Domenico & Ryan, 2017, p. 2).

To provide first insights into associations between facets of the Big Five Personality Model and primary emotions, we conducted a small study, where participants (N=182; 50 males and 132 females; mean-age: 23.47 [SD=4.57]) were asked to fill in the German version of the ANPS (Reuter et al., 2017) and the German version of the 42-item Big Five short-scale assessing with 42 items the broad Big Five scales as well as several facets (for detailed information on the German version of the 42-item Big Five short-scale see Olaru et al., 2015; for the theoretical background of the 42-item Big Five short-scale, see Tupes & Christal, 1961, as well as Christal, 1994). Answer options of the ANPS were 1= “strongly disagree” to 4= “strongly agree.” Answer options of the 42-item Big Five short-scale were 1= “strongly disagree” to 7= “strongly agree.” Means and standard deviations of the ANPS/42-item Big Five short-scale questionnaire data are presented in Table 1. Internal consistencies of both measures were mostly satisfactory (the lowest being the Neuroticism subscale, Irritated, with a Cronbach’s α of .50, please see Tables 2). The study was approved by the local ethics committee at Ulm University, Ulm, Germany.

Table 1.

Means and standard deviations of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS) and 42-item Big Five short-scale measures

Personality dimension M SD
ANPS
SEEKING 2.81 0.29
FEAR 2.65 0.45
CARE 2.97 0.41
ANGER 2.55 0.43
PLAY 2.99 0.38
SADNESS 2.47 0.37
42-item Big Five short-scale
Extraversion 4.34 0.93
Neuroticism 3.93 1.05
Agreeableness 5.48 0.72
Openness to Experience 4.61 0.94
Conscientiousness 5.46 0.80

Note. The range of answer options was 1–4 with respect to the ANPS and 1–7 with respect to the 42-item Big Five short-scale; 1= “strongly disagree,” and 4 respective 7= “strongly agree”.

Table 2.

Internal consistencies of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS) and the 42-item Big Five short-scale both on scale and subscale levels

Personality dimension Cronbach’s α
ANPS
SEEKING .63
FEAR .84
CARE .77
ANGER .82
PLAY .76
SADNESS .73
42-item Big Five short-scale
Extraversion—complete scale (9) .78
Extraversion—assertiveness (3) .58
Extraversion—social (3) .74
Extraversion—(low) shy (3) .70
Neuroticism—complete scale (9) .83
Neuroticism—irritated (3) .50
Neuroticism—stressed (3) .73
Neuroticism—depressed (3) .79
Agreeableness—complete scale (9) .84
Agreeableness—helpful (3) .73
Agreeableness—friendly (3) .67
Agreeableness—considerate (3) .78
Openness to Experience—complete (9) .78
Openness to Experience—intellectual (3) .61
Openness to Experience—reflective (3) .55
Openness to Experience—scientific (3) .75
Conscientiousness—complete (6) .77
Conscientiousness—hard working (3) .64
Conscientiousness—organized (3) .69

Note. Numbers in brackets give information on the item number of each scale.

We also provide the correlation patterns between primary emotions as assessed with the ANPS and the Big Five as assessed with the 42-item Big Five short-scale. As the 42-item Big Five short-scale has not been applied before in the context of ANPS research (for an exception see Sindermann et al., 2018), the correlation patterns from the present work can provide additional support for the already observed robust cross-cultural correlation patterns between the Big Five and primary emotions as described earlier in this text (taken from Montag & Panksepp, 2017). Indeed, the same robust associations could be observed such as higher FEAR, SADNESS, and ANGER being linked to higher Neuroticism; higher PLAY being associated with higher Extraversion; SEEKING was linked to higher Openness to Experience; and lower ANGER plus higher CARE were associated with higher Agreeableness (see Table 3). Thus, these basic findings seem to generalize across independent Big Five measures. Note that in the original work by Davis, Panksepp, and Normansell (2003) Goldberg’s Big Five adjectives (Goldberg, 1992) were applied, whereas in Montag and Panksepp’s work (2017) data came from the NEO-FFI (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Of importance, the 42-item Big Five short-scale provided basically the same associations with the ANPS as the other Big Five measures applied earlier. However, some unexpected findings such as (1) FEAR and SADNESS being moderately/strongly linked to lower Extraversion, (2) lower PLAY to higher Neuroticism, and (3) higher PLAY related to higher Agreeableness could be 42-item Big Five short-scale specific (see Table 3).

Given the commentary by Di Domenico and Ryan (2017), we also provide in Tables 4–7 the associations between individual differences in primary emotions and the facets of the Big Five Personality Model. We only present the facets of those Big Five dimensions that are most robustly associated with primary emotional systems (again we refer to the work by Montag & Panksepp, 2017).

Table 8.

Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales-adjective ratings (ANPS-AR) items with valence and scale

Adjective Valence ANPS
1. Purposeful (+) SEEKING
2. Anxious (+) FEAR
3. Caring (+) CARE
4. Hot-headed (+) ANGER
5. Funny (+) PLAY
6. Often sad (+) SADNESS
7. Unimaginative (−) SEEKING
8. Nervous (+) FEAR
9. Unsympathetic (−) CARE
10. Aggressive (+) ANGER
11. Not playflul (−) PLAY
12. Socially insecure (+) SADNESS
13. Dynamic (+) SEEKING
14. Relaxed (−) FEAR
15. Nurturing (+) CARE
16. Not argumentative (−) ANGER
17. Jokes around (+) PLAY
18. Socially confident (−) SADNESS
19. Curious (+) SEEKING
20. A worrier (+) FEAR
21. Warm (+) CARE
22. Temperamental (+) ANGER
23. Humorous (+) PLAY
24. Sensitive to rejection (+) SADNESS

Note. + = positive loadings; − = needs to be reversed before a score can be computed.

A new adjective-based short measure to assess individual differences in primary emotional systems (all items are answered with a 7-point Likert scaling ranging from 1= “Very Inaccurate,” 2= “Inaccurate,” 3= “Slightly Inaccurate,” 4= “Neither,” 5= “Slightly Accurate,” 6= “Accurate,” to 7= “Very Accurate”): The ANPS-adjective ratings (ANPS-AR) data reported here were collected on a U.S. student sample (total n=424, 254 females) Cronbach’s αs for the adjective-based ANPS scales ranged from .75 for PLAY to .69 for ANGER except for SEEKING at .51. The ANPS-AR can be used to collect observer ratings of other persons (e.g., therapy clients, family members, or colleagues) in addition to self-ratings. Table 9 shows the intercorrelations of the six ANPS-AR scales.

Selected associations between 42-item Big Five short-scale subscales and the ANPS are presented in Tables 47. In short, SEEKING is most strongly linked to the Intellectual subscale of Openness to Experience (although the remaining subscales are also significantly linked to SEEKING). PLAY associations are strongest for the Extraversion subscales of Social and (low) Shy and Bashful. This is line with the idea of Di Domenico and Ryan’s comment (2017), but see that also the Assertiveness subscale shows a significant association with PLAY. Whereas the FEAR and SADNESS dimensions are most strongly linked to the Neuroticism subscales Depressed and Stressed, ANGER is most strongly linked to being Irritated. Finally, focusing on 42-item Big Five short-scale Agreeableness subscales, CARE is most strongly linked to being Helpful and high ANGER most strongly to being less Friendly and Considerate. Please note that we consider the results from the Correlation Tables 4–7 as first insights into putative associations between individual differences in primary emotions and subscales of the Big Five. Some of the internal consistencies of the respective subscales are in the lower area of acceptability and the associations presented here need to be replicated with the 42-item Big Five short-scale and also other inventories (and subscales) across cultures. Moreover, correction procedures for multiple testing need to be considered. Without proper hypothesis (perhaps aside from the ideas mentioned in Di Domenico and Ryan’s work, one might have needed to adjust the α of .05 to .004 (Bonferroni adjustment with dividing .05 by 14 subscales of the 42-item Big Five short-scale). We refrain from doing this here because we understand the present data as a starting point for further research endeavors. The here presented associations concerning relations between facets of the Big Five and primary emotions in every case need to be replicated. Please see Figure 1 for further illustrations.

Table 6.

Correlation patterns between Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS) FEAR, SADNESS, ANGER, and subscales of Neuroticism

Neuroticism
ANPS Depressed Irritated Stressed
FEAR .66** .48** .64**
SADNESS .58** .42** .56**
ANGER .32** .53** .27**

Note. **p<.01, two tailed.

Table 4.

Correlation patterns between Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS) SEEKING and subscales of Openness to Experience

Openness to Experience
ANPS Intellectual Reflective Scientific
SEEKING .47** .27** .29**

Note. **p<.01, two tailed.

Table 7.

Correlation patterns between Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS) CARE, ANGER, and subscales of Agreeableness

Agreeableness
ANPS Helpful Friendly Considerate
CARE .48** .21** .28**
ANGER −.27** −.42** −.41**

Note. **p<.01, two tailed.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Primary emotional systems influence the Big Five/Five-Factor-Model of Personality in a bottom-up fashion. *More information on a potential SEEKING-Extraversion link is provided on page 4 (right column).

1.6. Outlook on future research directions using AN theory in personality neuroscience

The final part of the paper tries to give an outlook on important avenues for future research in the realm of primary emotional systems in the context of personality neuroscience. Some of the points have been already made by Montag and Panksepp (2017). However, here we propose also new directions.

First of all, we believe that experimental work that can provide evidence of causes rather than limited to correlations is of tremendous importance to illuminate the emotional nature of human personality. Self-report questionnaires naturally can only represent one data layer, which needs to be enriched by more “objective“ measures. This could be the assessment of individual differences in primary emotions by means of brain imaging (largely correlational) or other more experimental techniques (e.g., see Markett, Montag, & Reuter, 2014; Montag et al., 2013). If we want to fully understand the biological underpinnings of primary emotions, we need to take a look at direct (emotional) behavior and if possible also pure raw affects. This is not an easy task, because raw affects only rarely can be observed in human adults, because our “cortical thinking cap” usually holds a tight grip on the activity of neural networks, where primary emotions arise. Good examples of research approaches for studying raw affect in humans include the startle reflex to study the FEAR system (e.g., Montag et al., 2008). Mobbs et al. (2007) brought the concept of defensive distance to the magnetic resonance imaging scanner (for the concept of defensive distance among others see Blanchard, Hynd, Minke, Minemoto, & Blanchard, 2001; Blanchard, Blanchard, Rodgers, & Weiss, 1990). Defensive distance describes the distance between predator and prey. The closer the distance the stronger activity can be observed in the FEAR system. Its response such as fight, flight, or freezing also depends to some extent on the defensive distance.

It also needs to be mentioned that in particular triggering negative primary emotions in humans poses stronger ethical problems than triggering positive primal emotions (but see Damasio et al. 2000; Mobbs et al. 2007; 2009). “Indeed, because of such ethical concerns, JP decided to devote practically all his research effort for the past three decades to the study of positive emotions, especially animal play and ‘laughter,’ and specifically focusing on how such work may help identify new psychiatric treatments …” (Panksepp et al., 2017, p. 204). However, we are of the opinion that primary emotions can be well studied in young children, because their prefrontal cortex has developed less control over ancient neural subcortical circuits. Yet, given the ethical problems with studying negative affect, in children, at least the study of PLAY in childhood might provide excellent insights into the biological basis of becoming an extra-/introvert (again see associations between PLAY and extraversion as presented in Tables 3 and 5).

Table 5.

Correlation patterns between Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS) PLAY and subscales of Extraversion

Extraversion
ANPS Assertive Social (Low) shy and bashful
PLAY .33** .45** .45**

Note. **p<.01, two tailed.

Another new interesting avenue to understand the workings of primary emotions in humans will stem from Psychoinformatics (Montag et al., 2016; Yarkoni, 2012). Psychoinformatics describes a new research discipline where computer science methods are used to better understand psychological phenotypes. At present, the study of digital human–machine interaction is also of high interest for the affective neurosciences, because emotional energy finds its way also into our communication (we see the irony that the link toward lexical approaches of the Big Five is strong in this distinct domain of Psychoinformatics; Schwartz et al., 2013). This could identify how many positive or negative words we use in our text messages (analyzed by textmining techniques) and the kind of movement/locomotion patterns we provide via GPS signals or if we show communication behavior at all (e.g., a depressed person withdraws from his/her social network: Canzian & Musolesi, 2015; Markowetz, Błaszkiewicz, Montag, Switala, & Schlaepfer, 2014). The feasibility to combine smartphone-app recorded data with neuroscientific magnetic resonance imaging scans has been demonstrated, recently (Montag et al., 2017).

A final note concerns again the ANPS itself as a self-report measure to get insights into individual differences in primary emotions. Given the manifold possibilities to collect self-report data via smartphones or the Internet in general, it has become easy to collect a large amount of data sets to also study primary emotions in thousands of participants (e.g., see an example for such a large-scale personality/mood assessment in Andone et al., 2016). A problem for large-scale studies often poses the length of the applied questionnaire. This is, in particular, the case if persons fill in items via small devices such as a smartphone. As the ANPS is a rather long questionnaire, shorter measures to assess individual differences in primary emotions are strongly needed. This also reflects in the already existing short versions of the ANPS by Pingault, Pouga, Grèzes, & Berthoz (2012) and the B-ANPS/ANPS-S presented by Geir et al. (2014). As with very brief forms of the Big Five Model of Personality (e.g., Gosling, Rentfrow, & Swann, 2003; Rammstedt & John, 2007), we would like to present interested researchers a very short form of the ANPS with Table 8.

Table 9.

Intercorrelations of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales-adjective ratings (ANPS-AR) dimensions, n=394

Measure 1 2 3 4 5 6
1. AR-PLAY
2. AR-SEEKING .35**
3. AR-CARE .34** .37**
4. AR-FEAR −.04 −.06 .05
5. AR-ANGER −.03 .03 −.13 .31**
6. AR-SADNESS −.17 −.17* −.02 .59** .17*

Note. *p<.05, two-tailed; **p<.01, two-tailed. p values adjusted for Bonferroni Corrections.

The ANPS-AR adjectives were selected from the sample of 156 Big Five adjectives for subjects with no missing data (which reduced the n to 200 subjects) based on high correlations with the six primary ANPS scales as well on discriminate validity, that is, correlating highest with the corresponding ANPS scale (a criterion reached for all adjectives except “socially insecure” and “socially confident”). The 24 ANPS-AR adjectives were also subjected to exploratory factor analysis (SPSS version 20: Maximum likely extraction, oblimin rotation, seven eigen values >1.0, n=394). With six factors rotated all but three adjectives loaded highest on their designated scale, although the SEEKING items loaded negatively. A six-factor solution was arrived at by rotating two through seven factors. With two factors, all factors appropriately loaded on either a total negative affect or positive affect factor with no exceptions. With rotations from three to six, an additional affective neuroscience dimension emerged until rotating six factors revealed all six affective neuroscience scales. With seven rotations, no item loaded most strongly on the seventh factor, which did not have a loading reaching the 0.30 level. Factor loadings >0.15 can be seen in Table 10. A similar exploratory factor analysis including an additional 27 adjectives targeting the six ANPS primary emotions (six factors rotated) resulted in all but one of the ANPS-AR adjectives loading on its intended scale. The correlations of the six ANPS-AR scales with the six corresponding ANPS scales are listed in Table 11, which again shows the close relationship between the FEAR and SADNESS measures.

Table 10.

Oblique-rotated factor loadings from the 24 Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales-adjective rating (ANPS-AR) items, n=394

Items SADNESS PLAY ANGER CARE FEAR SEEKING(reversed)
Socially insecure .84
Socially confident −.73 .17
Sensitive to rejection .32 .23 .24
Jokes around .81
Humorous .79
Funny .77
Hot-headed .72 .16
Aggressive −.20 .69 −.17
Temperamental .54
Not argumentative −.48
Warm .69
Nurturing .69
Caring .67
Purposeful −.20 .16 .40 −.16 −.23
Unsympathetic .17 −.38
Anxious .80
A worrier .62
Nervous .20 .58
Often sad .20 .53
Relaxed −.40
Unimaginative .68
Curious .19 −.42
Dynamic −.28
Not playful −.19 .28

Table 11.

Correlations of Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS) with ANPS-Adjective Rating (ANPS-AR), n=209

ANPS
ANPS-AR PLAY SEEKING CARE FEAR ANGER SADNESS
PLAY .71** .29** .27** .01 .02 .02
SEEKING .34** .60** .33** −.07 .04 −.05
CARE .21 .30** .70** .15 −.08 .22*
FEAR −.25** −.15 .12 .74** .33** .56**
ANGER .01 .05 −.06 .17 .68** .05
SADNESS −.33** −.26** .00 .57** .28** .59**

Note. Central correlations between same dimensions of the ANPS and ANPS-AR. Alternate versions of the same scale correlations are in boldface.

*p<.05, two-tailed; **p<.01, two-tailed. p values adjusted for Bonferroni Corrections.

In addition, some items or even scales of the original (perhaps also of our short scale) might see some further work-over in the future. For example, the SEEKING scale of the German ANPS could have better psychometric properties (see also the lowest internal consistencies of the ANPS scales in the present work, Table 2) and Reuter et al. (2017) proposed how to improve this scale in the German version of the ANPS manual. Finally, as with other inventories (see the Junior-TCI; e.g., Lyoo et al., 2004), an ANPS version adapted for children will be necessary to be developed in the near future.

2. Conclusions

In sum, we are convinced that AN theory has a lot to offer personality neuroscience. We hope that many researchers will use AN theory as a guide in the investigation of the evolutionary foundations of human personality, namely individual differences in emotionality. This said, while AN theory offers important insights into the emotional aspects of human personality, it is based on preclinical research, and much more research is needed to expand our knowledge of the mammalian brain. In the meantime, this perspective can be enriched by more cognitive views to get a fuller picture of human personality. In terms of administering self-report inventories, we recommend using the ANPS along with more established measures of the Big Five (e.g., BFI-2 by Soto & John, 2017) to get a more comprehensive view of human personality.

Acknowledgments

Financial Support

This work was supported by a Heisenberg grant awarded by the German Research Foundation (C.H., grant number MO 2363/3-2).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors have nothing to disclose.

Appendix

Table A1.

The primary emotional systems of a cross-species affective neuroscience and their underlying neuroanatomical structures and neurotransmitter/neuropeptides (information abstracted from Montag & Panksepp, 2016; Panksepp, 1998, 2005, 2011; this table is exactly taken from Montag & Panksepp, in press)

Panksepp’s primary emotional systems Brain neuroanatomy related to these primary emotional systems Some key neuropeptides/neurotransmitters that arouse the primary emotional systems
FEAR Central and lateral amygdala to medial hypothalamus and dorsal periaqueductal gray (PAG) Glutamate (+), CRF (+), CCK (+), Alpha-MSH (+), Oxytocin (−)
RAGE/ANGER Medial amygdala to bed nucleus of stria terminalis (BNST). Medial and perifornical hypothalamus to PAG Substance P (+), Ach (+), Glutamate (+)
PANIC/SADNESS Anterior cingulate, BNST and preoptic area, dorsomedial thalamus, PAG Opioids (−), Oxytocin (−), Prolactin (−), CRF (+), Glutamate (+)
SEEKING Nucleus accumbens—ventral tegmental area (VTA), mesolimbic and mesocortical outputs, lateral hypothalamus to PAG Dopamine (+), Glutamate (+), Opioids (+), Neurotensin (+), Orexin (+)
CARE Anterior cingulate, BNST, preoptic area, VTA, PAG Oxytocin (+), Prolactin (+), Dopamine (+), Opioids (+/−)
LUST a Cortico-medial amygdala, BNST, preoptic hypothalamus, ventromedial hypothalamus, PAG Gonadal steroids (+), Vasopressin (+ male), Oxytocin (+ female), LH-RH (+)
PLAY Dorso-medial diencephalon, parafascicular area, PAG Opioids (+/−), Glutamate (+), Ach (+), Endocannabinoids (+)

Note. + = excitatory effects; − = inhibiting effects; CRF=corticotropin releasing hormone; CCK=cholecystokinin; alpha-MSH=alpha melanocyte stimulating hormone; Ach=acetylcholine; LH-RH=luteinizing hormone-releasing hormone.

All systems are controlled by glutamate in a facilitatory way and GABA in an inhibitory way. Moreover, the global state control systems, namely brainstem norepinephrine and serotonin systems that ascend throughout higher brain regions, tend to facilitate and inhibit, respectively, all of the primary emotional systems as well as waking/arousal and sleep/relaxation states.

a

LUST is not assessed with the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales, hence potential associations with the Big Five/Five-Factor Model of Personality are not presented in Figure 1.

Inaugural Invited Paper

Footnotes

1

Please note that primary emotions from AN theory are written in all capitals in order not to confuse them with these common language terms written in lower case.

2

Note that MacLean’s concept has often been critized due to its simplicity, nevertheless it serves some heuristic functions (Panksepp, 2002).

References

  1. Abella V., Panksepp J., Manga D., Bárcena C. Iglesias J. A. (2011). Spanish validation of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 14, 926–935. 10.5209/rev_sjop.2011.v14.n2.38 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Allport G. W. Odbert H. S. (1936). Trait-names: A psycho-lexical study. Psychological Monographs, 47, 1–171. 10.1037/h0093360 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andone I., Błaszkiewicz K., Eibes M., Trendafilov B., Montag C. Markowetz A. (2016). Menthal: A framework for mobile data collection and analysis. Proceedings of the 2016 ACM International Joint Conference on Pervasive and Ubiquitous Computing: Adjunct, pp. 624–629. 10.1145/2968219.2971591 [DOI]
  4. Angres D. H. (2010). The Temperament and Character Inventory in addiction treatment. Focus, 8, 187–198. 10.1176/foc.8.2.foc187 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barrett L. F. (2017). How emotions are made: The secret life of the brain. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. [Google Scholar]
  6. Blanchard D. C., Hynd A. L., Minke K. A., Minemoto T. Blanchard R. J. (2001). Human defensive behaviors to threat scenarios show parallels to fear-and anxiety-related defense patterns of non-human mammals. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 761–770. 10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00056-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Blanchard R. J., Blanchard D. C., Rodgers J. Weiss S. M. (1990). The characterization and modelling of antipredator defensive behavior. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 14, 463–472. 10.1016/S0149-7634(05)80069-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Canzian L. Musolesi M. (2015). Trajectories of depression: Unobtrusive monitoring of depressive states by means of smartphone mobility traces analysis. Proceedings of the 2015 ACM International Joint Conference on Pervasive and Ubiquitous Computing, pp. 1293–1304. 10.1145/2750858.2805845 [DOI]
  9. Carver C. S. White T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333. 10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  10. Christal R. E. (1994). The Air Force Self-Description Inventory (Final R&D status report). Brooks AFB, TX: Armstrong Laboratory.
  11. Cloninger C. R. (1986). A unified biosocial theory of personality and its role in the development of anxiety states. Psychiatric Developments, 3, 167–226. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cloninger C. R., Svrakic D. M. Przybeck T. R. (1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975–990. 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820240059008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Coenen V. A., Schlaepfer T. E., Maedler B. Panksepp J. (2011). Cross-species affective functions of the medial forebrain bundle – Implications for the treatment of affective pain and depression in humans. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1971–1981. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Corr P. J. Cooper A. J. (2016). The reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality questionnaire (RST-PQ): Development and validation. Psychological Assessment, 28, 1427–1440. 10.1037/pas0000273 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Costa P. T. Jr. McCrae R. R. (1992). NEO Personality Inventory–Revised (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. [Google Scholar]
  16. Damasio A., Damasio H. Tranel D. (2013). Persistence of feelings and sentience after bilateral damage of the insula. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 833–846. 10.1093/cercor/bhs077 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Damasio A. R., Grabowski T. J., Bechara A., Damasio H., Ponto L. L., Parvizi J., & Hichwa R. D. (2000). Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1049–1056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Davis K. L. Panksepp J. (2011). The brain’s emotional foundations of human personality and the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1946–1958. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Davis K. L. Panksepp J. (2018). The emotional foundations of personality. New York: W. W. Norton. [Google Scholar]
  20. Davis K. L., Panksepp J. Normansell L. (2003). The Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales: Normative data and implications. Neuropsychoanalysis, 5, 57–69. 10.1080/15294145.2003.10773410 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Deris N., Montag C., Reuter M., Weber B. Markett S. (2017). Functional connectivity in the resting brain as biological correlate of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales. NeuroImage, 147, 423–431. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Di Domenico S. Ryan R. M. (2017). Commentary: Primary emotional systems and personality: An evolutionary perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1414 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01414 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Eysenck H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: C. C. Thomas. [Google Scholar]
  24. Farinelli M., Panksepp J., Gestieri L., Leo M. R., Agati R., Maffei M., … Northoff G. (2013). SEEKING and depression in stroke patients: An exploratory study. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 35, 348–358. 10.1080/13803395.2013.776009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Felten A., Montag C., Markett S., Walter N. T. Reuter M. (2011). Genetically determined dopamine availability predicts disposition for depression. Brain and Behavior, 1, 109–118. 10.1002/brb3.20 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fiske D. W. (1949). Consistency of the factorial structures of personality ratings from different sources. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 44, 329–344. 10.1037/h0057198 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Geir P., Selsbakk J. M., Theresa W. Sigmund K. (2014). Testing different versions of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales in a clinical sample. PloS One, 9, e109394 10.1371/journal.pone.0109394 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Goldberg L. R. (1992). The development of markers for the Big-Five factor structure. Psychological Assessment, 4, 26–42. 10.1037/1040-3590.4.1.26 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Gosling S. D. John O. P. (1999). Personality dimensions in nonhuman animals: A cross-species review. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 69–75. 10.1111/1467-8721.00017 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gosling S. D., Rentfrow P. J. Swann W. B. (2003). A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 504–528. 10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gray J. A. (1972). The psychophysiological nature of introversion – extraversion: A modification of Eysenck’s theory In V. D. Nebylitsyn & J. A. Gray (Eds.), Biological bases of individual behavior (pp. 182–205). New York: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  32. Gray J. A. McNaughton N. (2000). A theory of the septo-hippocampal system In J. A. Gray & N. McNaughton (Eds.), The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the function of the septo-hippocampal system (pp. 233–274). New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  33. Hiebler-Ragger M., Fuchshuber J., Dröscher H. B., Vajda C., Fink A. Unterrainer H. F. (2018). Personality influences the relationship between primary emotions and religious/spiritual well-being. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Holt-Lunstad J., Birmingham W. A. Light K. C. (2008). Influence of a “warm touch” support enhancement intervention among married couples on ambulatory blood pressure, oxytocin, alpha amylase, and cortisol. Psychosomatic Medicine, 70, 976–985. 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318187aef7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Karterud S., Pedersen G., Johansen M., Wilberg T., Davis K. Panksepp J. (2016). Primary emotional traits in patients with personality disorders. Personality and Mental Health, 10, 261–273. 10.1002/pmh.1345 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. King J. E. Figueredo A. J. (1997). The Five-factor model plus dominance in chimpanzee personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 257–271. 10.1006/jrpe.1997.2179 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. Laeng B., Eidet L. M., Sulutvedt U. Panksepp J. (2016). Music chills: The eye pupil as a mirror to music’s soul. Consciousness and Cognition, 44, 161–178. 10.1016/j.concog.2016.07.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Løseth G., Leknes S. Ellingsen D. M. (2016). The neurochemical basis of motivation for affiliative touch In H. Olausson, J. Wessberg & F. McGlone (Eds.), Affective touch and the neurophysiology of CT afferents (pp. 239–264). New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  39. Lyoo I. K., Han C. H., Lee S. J., Yune S. K., Ha J. H., Chung S. J.,… Hong K. E. (2004). The reliability and validity of the junior temperament and character inventory. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 45, 121–128. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2003.12.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. MacLean P. D. (1990). The triune brain in evolution: Role in paleocerebral functions. New York: Plenum Press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Markett S., Montag C. Reuter M. (2014). In favor of behavior: On the importance of experimental paradigms in testing predictions from Gray’s revised reinforcement sensitivity theory. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 8, 184 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00184 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Markowetz A., Błaszkiewicz K., Montag C., Switala C. Schlaepfer T. E. (2014). Psycho-Informatics: Big data shaping modern psychometrics. Medical Hypotheses, 82, 405–411. 10.1016/j.mehy.2013.11.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. McCrae R. R. John O. P. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. Journal of Personality, 60, 175–215. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00970.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. McNaughton N. Corr P. J. (2004). A two-dimensional neuropsychology of defense: Fear/anxiety and defensive distance. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 28, 285–305. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00970.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Merker B. (2007). Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: A challenge for neuroscience and medicine. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 63–81. 10.1017/S0140525X07000891 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Mobbs D., Marchant J., Hassabis D., Seymour B., Tan G., Gray M. , … Frith C. (2009). From threat to fear: The neural organization of defensive fear systems in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 12236–12243. 10.1523/jneurosci.2378-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Mobbs D., Petrovic P., Marchant J. L., Hassabis D., Weiskopf N., Seymour B. , … Frith C. D. (2007). When fear is near: Threat imminence elicits prefrontal-periaqueductal gray shifts in humans. Science, 317, 1079–1083. 10.1126/science.1144298 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Montag C., Buckholtz J. W., Hartmann P., Merz M., Burk C., Hennig J.,Reuter M. (2008). COMT genetic variation affects fear processing: Psychophysiological evidence. Behavioral Neuroscience, 122, 901–909. 10.1037/0735-7044.122.4.901 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Montag C., Duke É. Markowetz A. (2016). Toward psychoinformatics: Computer science meets psychology. Computational and Mathematical Methods in Medicine, 2016, 1–10. 10.1155/2016/2983685 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Montag C., Fiebach C. J., Kirsch P. Reuter M. (2011). Interaction of 5-HTTLPR and a variation on the oxytocin receptor gene influences negative emotionality. Biological Psychiatry, 69, 601–603. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.10.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Montag C., Hahn E., Reuter M., Spinath F. M., Davis K. Panksepp J. (2016). The role of nature and nurture for individual differences in primary emotional systems: Evidence from a Twin Study. PloS One, 11, e0151405 10.1371/journal.pone.0151405 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Montag C., Markowetz A., Blaszkiewicz K., Andone I., Lachmann B., Sariyska R., … Markett S. (2017a). Facebook usage on smartphones and gray matter volume of the nucleus accumbens. Behavioural Brain Research, 329, 221–228. 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.04.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Montag C. Panksepp J. (2016). Primal emotional-affective expressive foundations of human facial expression. Motivation and Emotion, 40, 760–766. 10.1007/s11031-016-9570-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Montag C. Panksepp J. (2017). Primary emotional systems and personality: An evolutionary perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 464. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00464 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Montag C. Panksepp J. (in press). Personality neuroscience: Why it is of importance to consider primary emotional systems! In V. Zeigler-Hill & T. K. Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences. Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag. [Google Scholar]
  56. Montag C., Reuter M., Jurkiewicz M., Markett S. Panksepp J. (2013). Imaging the structure of the human anxious brain: A review of findings from neuroscientific personality psychology. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 24, 167–190. 10.1515/revneuro-2012-0085 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Montag C., Sindermann C., Becker B. Panksepp J. (2016). An affective neuroscience framework for the molecular study of Internet addiction. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1906 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01906 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Montag C., Widenhorn-Müller K., Panksepp J. Kiefer M. (2017b). Individual differences in Affective Neuroscience Personality Scale (ANPS) primary emotional traits and depressive tendencies. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 73, 136–142. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.11 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Morton F. B., Lee P. C., Buchanan-Smith H. M., Brosnan S. F., Thierry B., Paukner A.,...Weiss A. (2013). Personality structure in brown capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella): Comparisons with chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), orangutans (Pongo spp.), and rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 127, 282–298. 10.1037/a0031723 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Olaru G., Witthöft M. Wilhelm O. (2015). Methods matter: Testing competing models for designing short-scale Big-Five assessments. Journal of Research in Personality, 59, 56–68. 10.1016/j.jrp.2015.09.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  61. Özkarar-Gradwohl F. G., Panksepp J., İçöz F. J., Çetinkaya H., Köksal F., Davis K. L. & Scherler N. (2014). The influence of culture on basic affective systems: The comparison of Turkish and American norms on the affective neuroscience personality scales. Culture and Brain, 2, 173–192. 10.1007/s40167-014-0021-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  62. Pahlavan F., Mouchiroud C., Zenasni F. Panksepp J. (2008). Validation de l’adaptation française de l’échelle neuro-affective de personnalité/French validation of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS). Revue Européenne de Psychologie Appliquée (European Review of Applied Psychology), 58, 155–163. 10.1016/j.erap.2007.08.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  63. Panksepp J. (1992). A critical role for “affective neuroscience” in resolving what is basic about basic emotions. Psychological Review, 99, 554–560. 10.1037/0033-295X.99.3.554 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Panksepp J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  65. Panksepp J. (2002). Foreword: The MacLean legacy and some modern trends in emotion research In G. A. Cory & R. Gardner (Eds.), The evolutionary neuroethology of Paul MacLean: Convergences and frontiers. (pp. ix–xxvii). Westport, CT: Praeger. [Google Scholar]
  66. Panksepp J. (2005). Affective consciousness: Core emotional feelings in animals and humans. Consciousness and Cognition, 14, 30–80. 10.1016/j.concog.2004.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Panksepp J. (2006). Emotional endophenotypes in evolutionary psychiatry. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 30, 774–784. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.01.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Panksepp J. (2007). Can PLAY diminish ADHD and facilitate the construction of the social brain? Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 16, 57–66. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Panksepp J. (2010). Affective neuroscience of the emotional BrainMind: Evolutionary perspectives and implications for understanding depression. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 12, 533–545. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Panksepp J. (2011). Cross-species affective neuroscience decoding of the primal affective experiences of humans and related animals. PloS One, 6, e21236 10.1371/journal.pone.0021236 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Panksepp J. Burgdorf J. (2003). “Laughing” rats and the evolutionary antecedents of human joy? Physiology & Behavior, 79, 533–547. 10.1016/s0031-9384(03)00159-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Panksepp J., Lane R. D., Solms M. Smith R. (2017). Reconciling cognitive and affective neuroscience perspectives on the brain basis of emotional experience. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 76, 187–215. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Panksepp J., Normansell L., Cox J. F. Siviy S. M. (1994). Effects of neonatal decortication on the social play of juvenile rats. Physiology & Behavior, 56, 429–443. 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90285-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Panksepp J. Watt D. (2011). Why does depression hurt? Ancestral primary-process separation-distress (PANIC/GRIEF) and diminished brain reward (SEEKING) processes in the genesis of depressive affect. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 74, 5–13. 10.1521/psyc.2011.74.1.5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Panksepp J., Wright J. S., Döbrössy M. D., Schlaepfer T. E. Coenen V. A. (2014). Affective neuroscience strategies for understanding and treating depression: From preclinical models to three novel therapeutics. Clinical Psychological Science, 2, 472–494. 10.1177/2167702614535913 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  76. Pascazio L., Bembich S., Nardone I. B., Vecchiet C., Guarino G. Clarici A. (2015). Validation of the Italian translation of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales. Psychological Reports, 116, 97–115. 10.2466/08.09.pr0.116k13w4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Pellegrini A. D. (1988). Elementary-school children’s rough-and-tumble play and social competence. Developmental Psychology, 24, 802–806. 10.1037//0012-1649.24.6.802 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  78. Pellegrini A. D. Smith P. K. (1998). Physical activity play: The nature and function of a neglected aspect of play. Child Development, 69, 577–598. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.00577.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Pellis S. M. Pellis V. C. (2007). Rough-and-tumble play and the development of the social brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 95–98. 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00483.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  80. Pingault J. B., Pouga L., Grèzes J. Berthoz S. (2012). Determination of emotional endophenotypes: A validation of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales and further perspectives. Psychological Assessment, 24, 375–385. 10.1037/a0025692 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Rammstedt B. John O. P. (2007). Measuring personality in one minute or less: A 10-item short version of the Big Five Inventory in English and German. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 203–212. 10.1016/j.jrp.2006.02.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  82. Reuter M., Cooper A. J., Smillie L. D., Markett S. Montag C. (2015). A new measure for the revised reinforcement sensitivity theory: Psychometric criteria and genetic validation. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 9, 38 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Reuter M., Panksepp J., Davis K. Montag C. (2017). Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS) – Deutsche Version. Göttingen: Hogrefe. [Google Scholar]
  84. Reuter M., Panksepp J., Schnabel N., Kellerhoff N., Kempel P. Hennig J. (2005). Personality and biological markers of creativity. European Journal of Personality, 19, 83–95. 10.1002/per.534 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  85. Reuter M., Weber B., Fiebach C. J., Elger C. Montag C. (2009). The biological basis of anger: Associations with the gene coding for DARPP-32 (PPP1R1B) and with amygdala volume. Behavioural Brain Research, 202, 179–183. 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.03.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Romine C. B. Reynolds C. R. (2005). A model of the development of frontal lobe functioning: Findings from a meta-analysis. Applied Neuropsychology, 12, 190–201. 10.1207/s15324826an1204_2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Rothbart M. K. (1986). Longitudinal observation of infant temperament. Developmental Psychology, 22, 356–365. 10.1037//0012-1649.22.3.356 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  88. Savitz J., van der Merwe L. Ramesar R. (2008. a). Dysthymic and anxiety-related personality traits in bipolar spectrum illness. Journal of Affective Disorders, 109, 305–311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Savitz J., van der Merwe L. Ramesar R. (2008. b). Hypomanic, cyclothymic and hostile personality traits in bipolar spectrum illness: A family-based study. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 42, 920–929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Schmitt D., Allik J., McCrae R. Benet-Martinez V. (2007). The geographic distribution of Big Five personality traits. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38, 173–212. [Google Scholar]
  91. Schwartz H. A., Eichstaedt J. C., Kern M. L., Dziurzynski L., Ramones S. M., Agrawal M.,… Ungar L. H. (2013). Personality, gender, and age in the language of social media: The open-vocabulary approach. PloS One, 8, e73791 10.1371/journal.pone.0073791 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Sindermann C., Kendrick K., Becker B., Li M., Li S. Montag C. (2017). Does growing up in urban compared to rural areas shape primary emotional traits? Behavioral Sciences, 7, 60 10.3390/bs7030060 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Sindermann C., Li M., Sariyska R., Lachmann B., Duke É., Cooper A.,… Montag C. (2016). The 2D:4D-ratio and neuroticism revisited: Empirical evidence from Germany and China. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 811 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00811 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Sindermann C., Luo R., Zhao Z., Li Q., Li M., Kendrick K. M. ,... Montag C. (2018). High ANGER and low agreeableness predict vengefulness in German and Chinese participants. Personality and Individual Differences, 121, 184–192. 10.1016/j.paid.2017.09.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  95. Sindermann C., Saliger J., Nielsen J., Markett S., Karbe H. Montag C. (2017). Personality and primary emotional traits: Disentangling multiple sclerosis related fatigue and depression. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology (published online 9 November 2017). 10.1093/arclin/acx104 [DOI] [PubMed]
  96. Smillie L. D. (2013). Extraversion and reward processing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 167–172. 10.1177/0963721412470133 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Soto C. J. John O. P. (2017). The next Big Five Inventory (BFI-2): Developing and assessing a hierarchical model with 15 facets to enhance bandwidth, fidelity, and predictive power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113, 117–143. 10.1037/pspp0000155 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Tooley V., Brigham J. C., Maass A. Bothwell R. K. (1987). Facial recognition: Weapon effect and attentional focus. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 17, 845–859. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1987.tb00294.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  99. Tupes E. C. Christal R. C. (1961). Recurrent personality factors based on trait ratings (USAF ASD Technical Report, 61–97). Bexar County, TX: US Air Force, Lackland Air Force Base. Retrieved from http://www.dtic.mil/dtic/tr/fulltext/u2/267778.pdf [Google Scholar]
  100. van der Westhuizen D. Solms M. (2015). Social dominance and the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales. Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 90–111. 10.1016/j.concog.2014.12.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Yarkoni T. (2012). Psychoinformatics. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 391–397. 10.1177/0963721412457362 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  102. Yeo S. S., Chang P. H. Jang S. H. (2013). The ascending reticular activating system from pontine reticular formation to the thalamus in the human brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 416. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00416 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Personality Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Cambridge University Press

RESOURCES