Skip to main content
Health Research Policy and Systems logoLink to Health Research Policy and Systems
. 2020 May 18;18:48. doi: 10.1186/s12961-020-00564-2

Using natural experiments to improve public health evidence: a review of context and utility for obesity prevention

Melanie Crane 1,, Erika Bohn-Goldbaum 1, Anne Grunseit 1, Adrian Bauman 1
PMCID: PMC7236508  PMID: 32423438

Abstract

Background

Natural experiments are increasingly valued as a way to assess the health impact of health and non-health interventions when planned controlled experimental research designs may be infeasible or inappropriate to implement. This study sought to investigate the value of natural experiments by exploring how they have been used in practice. The study focused on obesity prevention research as one complex programme area for applying natural experiment studies.

Methods

A literature search sought obesity prevention research from January 1997 to December 2017 and identified 46 population health studies that self-described as a natural experiment.

Results

The majority of studies identified were published in the last 5 years, illustrating a more recent adoption of such opportunities. The majority of studies were evaluations of the impact of policies (n = 19), such as assessing changes to food labelling, food advertising or taxation on diet and obesity outcomes, or were built environment interventions (n = 17), such as the impact of built infrastructure on physical activity or access to healthy food. Research designs included quasi-experimental, pre-experimental and non-experimental methods. Few studies applied rigorous research designs to establish stronger causal inference, such as multiple pre/post measures, time series designs or comparison of change against an unexposed group. In general, researchers employed techniques to enhance the study utility but often were limited in the use of more rigorous study designs by ethical considerations and/or the particular context of the intervention.

Conclusion

Greater recognition of the utility and versatility of natural experiments in generating evidence for complex health issues like obesity prevention is needed. This review suggests that natural experiments may be underutilised as an approach for providing evidence of the effects of interventions, particularly for evaluating health outcomes of interventions when unexpected opportunities to gather evidence arise.

Keywords: Natural experiments, obesity prevention, evaluation methods, study design, physical activity, nutrition, population health interventions, narrative review

Background

Many public health issues are complex, requiring preventive health actions targeted at multiple upstream social and environmental determinates to improve population-level outcomes [1]. Despite this, the published literature is almost entirely focused on short-term individual-level research outcomes and lacking complex, multi-level, population-level intervention evidence [2]. Obesity is now recognised as a complex health issue, driven by multiple interrelated factors, including environmental, social and cultural determinants beyond individual-level determinants of behaviour [35]. Today, over 700 million adults and children are obese [6]. Effective population-wide prevention strategies implemented at-scale are needed to combat obesity; individually targeted strategies, such as health education and behavioural skills, have largely been found to be ineffective and unsustainable [2, 7]. Recommended prevention strategies focus on environmental interventions and policies to promote healthy eating and physical activity, such as taxation and restrictions on advertising of unhealthy food, interventions to increase healthy food availability, and environmental changes to the built environment [8]. However, the effectiveness of these interventions or policies remains limited by a lack of evaluation evidence.

Public Health evidence has been somewhat restricted to individualised prevention and treatment interventions from randomised controlled trial (RCT) studies. The key challenges for assessing complex policy and environmental interventions is that RCT studies are rarely appropriate, or even possible, in most situations [9]. In these complex interventions, the intervention is unlikely to be investigator initiated and the researcher is unlikely to have direct control over the study environment or wider policy influences [10, 11]. Other factors related to the complexity of populations and context make it unrealistic to apply controlled study environments such as the long time over which health behaviours change or outcomes are established [12, 13]. These issues have led to calls for natural experiment studies to improve the evidence base for public health interventions [11, 14].

The Medical Research Council (MRC) in the United Kingdom describes natural experiments as evaluating health or other outcomes where “exposure to the event of intention of interest has not been manipulated by the researcher” [15]. A number of other definitions exists and contributes to widespread confusion that a natural experiment is a type of study design (synonymous with or distinct from a quasi-experiment) rather than what is usually an unplanned ‘opportunity’ for research [15]. There is also some debate as to whether a natural experiment should refer only to studies which are ‘experimental’, i.e. where variation in the outcomes are analysed using methods that attempt to make causal inference. This position is held by the MRC and would include quasi-experimental studies but would exclude observational study designs as insufficient for determining causality [16]. Others contest that even weak study designs may be better than no evidence at all [17, 18]. Hence, in evaluating the contribution of natural experiment studies to the evidence base, it is important to consider both the strength of their designs and their potential value to the existing evidence base.

The purpose of this paper is two-fold: firstly, given the increasing advocacy for using natural experiments, we sought to investigate how natural experiments have been defined and used in practice. Specifically, we describe and assess the characteristics of natural experiments conducted in the area of obesity prevention to reveal the strengths, gaps, and weaknesses and help inform future research practice. Secondly, we explore the value of natural experiments in evaluating real-world interventions by considering the extent to which a planned experiment might be possible or whether this knowledge could only have been generated by a natural experiment.

Methods

Literature search strategy

A literature review was conducted on published peer-reviewed studies that self-described as natural experiments and focused on obesity prevention through improving nutrition or physical activity. The purpose of the review was to explore the use and utility of natural experiments [19], not to determine the outcome effects of interventions, which has been reported elsewhere [20, 21]. A systematic search in Scopus, Web of Science and CINAHL databases identified potential studies by combining two main topics (natural experiment AND population health) with the areas of interest: physical activity, nutrition or obesity (for full search criteria details, see Additional file S1). The search results were then limited by language (English only), article type (original research published/in-press and full-articles only), and dated from 1 January 1997 to 22 December 2017. Articles involving non-human research or not related to obesity prevention or improving nutrition or physical activity were excluded.

Figure 1 depicts the study selection stages. The initial search results were combined (n = 117 articles) and duplicate studies (n = 21 articles) were removed. Titles were scanned and 14 additional articles were removed as non-human studies, resulting in 82 articles. Two authors (MC and EG) read the abstracts to confirm that the articles met the eligibility criteria. A further 37 studies were removed as a commentary or opinion piece (n = 6), a protocol or methods article (n = 7), a review or meta-analysis (n = 8), or unrelated to obesity prevention (n = 12); an additional 4 four studies were removed as they did not self-identify as natural experiments (Additional file S2). The reference lists of the review articles were also searched for natural experiment studies contained within and 1 additional study was identified. The full text of all 46 articles was evaluated to confirm all inclusion criteria were met.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Study selection

Critical review of studies

To investigate use, identified studies were described according to the nature of the intervention (type/setting, theme and scale) and of the evaluation methodology (study design, the exposure, characteristics of the exposed group and inclusion of a control group, and data collection measures and period). We then conducted a qualitative synthesis on the value of the studies to the field (Box 1). This includes consideration of the purpose of the study, and whether it could be answered from a planned, researcher-driven study, following the typologies developed by Dawson and Sim [22]. We discuss whether a planned controlled experiment (specifically an RCT or cluster RCT) would be realistic, considering practical issues such as time and population constraints, the potential for ethical conflict, and other limitations of RCTs [12, 13, 23].

MC and EG independently assessed each of the included studies and met to identify and resolve any coding and data extraction discrepancies. Where the authors disagreed, the other authors were consulted and a consensus reached.

Results

The literature search resulted in 46 studies directly related to the topic of diet, physical activity and obesity prevention, and are described in Table 1. Among the natural experiments conducted in this area over the last 20 years, none were published before 2007, 7 were published prior to 2012 (when the MRC guidance was published) and 4 more were published the same year. The largest annual publication volume occurred in 2017 with 14 published studies, at least double the numbers of any previous year (Additional file S3). The studies were predominantly from North America (22 from the United States of America and 6 from Canada). The remaining studies were from Europe (n = 7), Australia (n = 8) and South America (n = 3).

Table 1.

Studies self-identifying as natural experiments which met inclusion criteria

Author, year Geography Type Exposure Exposed group Unexposed group Intervention outcome Study design Primary data
Allais et al., 2015 [27] France P Food labelling Consumers of supermarket stores No control group (pre exposure)

Sale of healthy

food items

Interrupted time series No
Barlow et al., 2017 [28] Canada P Tariff/trade agreement Residents in country with tax on sugar sweeteners Residents in countries with no tax on sugar sweeteners Sales of sugar foods/beverage Pre/post cohort + control No
Barradas et al., 2017 [29] Colombia C PA programme (park based) Residents near park with PA programme

Residents near parks without programme;

with existing programme

Quality of life Post only Yes
Bernatchez et al., 2015 [30] Canada E Bike share programme City residents near bike-share docks City residents further from bike-share docks Programme awareness Repeated cross-sectional Yes
Brandt et al., 2017 [31] United States P Food regulation Residents in counties with TFA restrictions Residents in counties with no TFA restrictions Hospital admission for cardiovascular events Pre/post cohort + control No
Cawley et al., 2007 [32] United States P PE education Students in high schools with PE requirements Students in high schools without PE requirements PA, weight Post only No
Cawley et al., 2010 [33] United States P Social welfare benefit Elderly receiving inflated social security benefits No control group (variation in exposure) Weight Repeated cross-sectional No
Christian et al., 2013 [34] Australia E

Built environment

(housing relocation)

Residents in a PA-supportive neighbourhood residents in conventional or hybrid neighbourhood Walking Pre/post cohort + control Yes
Christian et al., 2017 [35] Australia E Built environment (housing relocation) Residents in a new neighbourhood No control group Walking Pre/post cohort Yes
Cohen et al., 2012 [36] United States E Green space (outdoor gym equipment) Park users in parks with new fitness equipment Park users in parks without equipment PA METs Pre/post cohort + control Yes
Cornelsen et al., 2017 [37] United Kingdom P Tax (sugar) Customers of restaurant chain No control group (pre exposure) Sugar-sweetened beverage sales Interrupted time series No
Cranney et al., 2016 [38] Australia E

Green space

(outdoor gym equipment)

Park users in parks with new fitness equipment No control group (pre exposure) PA

Repeated cross-sectional;

qualitative interviews

Yes
Datar & Nicosia, 2017 [39] United States P Restricted access unhealthy food Children living in states with school policies on discretionary foods Children in states without policies BMI Post only No
Dill et al., 2014 [40] United States E Built environment (bicycle infrastructure) Residents in streets with bike paths Residents near streets without paths PA/AT Pre/post cohort + control Yes
Dubowitz et al., 2015 [41] United States E Food environment (access to healthy food outlet) Residents in neighbourhoods with no full-service supermarket No control group (baseline data only) Healthy food purchases Pre only Yes
Elbel et al., 2011 [42] United States P Food labelling Consumers (low-income kids/teens) of fast-food outlets in city with mandatory food labelling Consumers (low-income kids/teens) of fast-food outlets in city with no food labelling Food consumption - fast food Pre/post cohort + control Yes
Ferguson et al., 2017 [43] Australia p Food affordability (pricing) Residents in remote Aboriginal communities No control group (variation in exposure) F&V, general food/beverage sales

Interrupted time series; repeat cross-sectional;

Qualitative interviews

Mixed
Frew et al., 2014 [44] United Kingdom C PA programme (access to services) City residents with limited access to free PA services Hypothetical (as if there was no free access) PA-related diseases, QALYS Pre/post cohort Yes
Fu & VanLandingham, 2012 [45] United States O Migration Vietnamese immigrants in America Vietnamese immigrant returnees and native (never-leavers) BMI, waist-hip circumference Post only Yes
Gee et al., 2015 [46] United States O Migration Filipinos in America Native Filipinos BMI, waist-hip circumference Pre/post cohort + control Yes
Giles-Corti et al., 2013 [47] Australia E

Built environment

(housing relocation)

Residents in a PA-supportive neighbourhood residents in conventional or hybrid neighbourhood AT and recreational PA Pre/post cohort + control Yes
Hobin et al., 2017 [48] Canada P Food labelling Consumers of supermarkets with nutrition labelling Consumers of supermarkets without nutrition labelling

Sale of healthy

food items

Pre/post cohort + control;

qualitative interviews

Mixed
Jancey et al., 2016 [49] Australia E Built environment (workplaces) Office workers move to new building No control group (pre exposure) PA Repeated cross-sectional Yes
Jones-Smith et al., 2017 [50] United States O Economic resources (casino developments) Native American new-borns born in area near casinos (greater resources) Native American new-borns near no casino (lower resources) Birth weight (childhood obesity) Repeated cross-sectional No
Jürges et al., 2011 [51] Germany P Education reform (years educated) Country population years of education No control (variation in exposure) Overweight, obesity, smoking Repeated cross-sectional No
Kapinos et al., 2017 [52] United States O Clinical (fetal mal-presentation at birth delivery) Mothers who deliver by caesarean birth Mothers who deliver by vaginal birth Mothers’ postpartum BMI Pre/post cohort + control No
Kesten et al., 2014 [53] United Kingdom E Built environment (AT infrastructure) Commuters in neighbourhoods proximal to AT infrastructure No control group (pre exposure) Awareness of AT infrastructure Qualitative interviews; media Mixed
Kodish et al., 2016 [54] United States O Economic resources (casino ownership) Native American tribal communities with casino ownership profit Tribal communities with little/no cash profit Access to healthy food, PA facilities, etc. Qualitative interviews Yes
Lee et al., 2017 [55] United States E Built environment (transfer to local school) Students transferred to local school No control group (recall pre exposure) AT participation Post only Yes
Madsen, 2011 [56] United States P Clinical (health (BMI) screening optional notification) Students in schools with screening-notified parents Students in schools with screening not notified BMI Repeated cross-sectional; qualitative interviews No
Odoms-Young et al., 2014 [57] United States P Food access (change to nutritional content of welfare food package) Low-income families receiving food security packages No control group (pre exposure)

Dietary intake,

Body weight

Pre/post cohort Yes
Ovrum et al., 2014 [58] Norway P Food access (healthy food) Families of children whose schools give free F&V Families of children who do not have F&V programme or have optional programme F&V consumption Post only Yes
Pollack et al., 2014 [59] United States P Social housing Public housing residents in scattered housing Public housing residents in clustered housing Diet, PA behaviour, BMI, perceived health Post only Yes
Ram et al., 2016 [60] United Kingdom E Social housing City residents from different housing sectors No control group (baseline data only) PA, BMI, self-reported health Pre only Yes
Sadler et al., 2013 [61] United States E Food environment (access to healthy food outlet) Residents in neighbourhoods with new supermarket Residents in neighbourhood with no supermarket F&V intake, food security Pre/post cohort + control Yes
Schultz et al., 2017 [62] United States E Built environment (street redesign) Low-income neighbourhood residents No control group (pre exposure) PA, park usage Repeated cross-sectional Yes
Simões et al., 2017 [63] Brazil C PA programme (community) Cities participating in the programme Non-participatory and not-yet-participatory cities PA Pre/post cohort + control (step wedge) Yes
Stanley et al., 2016 [64] Canada E Public transport strike University students using public transport and other transport modes No control group (pre-exposure) PA, AT participation Pre/post cohort Mixed
Stone et al., 2012 [65] Canada P PA programme (school curriculum) Primary students in school with daily PA policy (mandatory minimum PA minutes) Primary students in school without PA curriculum PA, BMI Post only Yes
Sutherland et al., 2010 [66] United States P Food labelling Consumers of supermarkets with shelf nutrition labelling No control group (variation in exposure) Sale of healthy food items Interrupted time series No
Torres et al., 2017 [67] Colombia C PA programme (park-based) Residents near park with new PA programme

Residents near parks without programme or

with established programme

MVPA Pre/post cohort + control Yes
Tudor-Locke et al., 2008 [68] Australia E Built environment (housing relocation) Residents relocating home within a metropolitan area No control group (pre exposure) PA Pre/post cohort Yes
Veitch et al., 2012 [69] Australia E Green space Users of a refurbished park Users an un-furbished park PA, park usage Pre/post cohort + control Yes
Wagner et al., 2013 [70] United States C Social living arrangements Student at a university No control group (variation in exposure) Various health behaviours (PA; alcohol) Post only No
Watson et al., 2016 [71] Canada P Economic (change in unemployment benefits) Adult workforce population No control group (variation in exposure) Obesity Pre/post cohort No
Zick, 2014 [72] United States P Daylight savings scheme Residents in states with daylight savings Residents in states with no daylight savings MVPA Pre/post cohort + control No

AT active travel, BMI body mass index, C community intervention, E environmental intervention, F&V fruit and vegetable intake, METs metabolic equivalent expenditures, MVPA moderate-to-vigorous physical activity, O other, P policy, PA physical activity, PE physical education, QALYs quality-adjusted life years, TFA trans-fatty acid

Intervention context

Setting and scale

The majority of the natural experiments identified were policy interventions [27, 28, 31, 32, 37, 39, 42, 43, 48, 51, 56, 58, 59, 65, 66, 71, 72], followed by environmental interventions [30, 3436, 38, 40, 41, 47, 49, 53, 55, 6062, 64, 68, 69] and community-based interventions [29, 44, 63, 67, 70]. Fewer studies were of economic interventions [33, 50, 54], individual behaviour interventions [52] and anthropological studies [45, 46] (Table 2). In terms of the scale of implementation, the majority of interventions were conducted at the neighbourhood scale. All but two [31, 42] of the 19 policy interventions were implemented at a national or state level.

Table 2.

Summary of study characteristics

Classification Sub-classification N
Intervention characteristics 46
Intervention setting Policy intervention 19
Environmental intervention 17
Community intervention 5
Individual behaviour intervention 1
Economic intervention 2
Migration 2
Scale of intervention National 8
State/region 12
City/neighbourhood 26
Intervention theme Food labelling 4
Food regulation/taxation 3
Food accessibility 6
Workplace/school/community physical activity programme 7
Built infrastructure development 11
Transport interventions 4
Social environment changes 3
Clinical/screening procedure 2
Economic events 4
Other 2
Evaluation characteristics
Study design Longitudinal pre/post quasi-experiment 15
Longitudinal pre/post cohort (single group) 6
Interrupted time series 4
Repeat cross-sectional surveys 9
Post-test observation only 9
Qualitative only 2
Pre-test observation only 2
Data source Primary 27
Secondary 15
Mixed 4
Data collectiona Short-term (<3 years) 21
Long-term (3+ years) 12

aIncludes time-related study designs only

Intervention theme

The natural experiments related to diet assessed exposure to the introduction of, or changes to, food labelling [27, 42, 48, 66], or food regulation and taxation [28, 31, 37], and access to, or affordability of, healthy food options such as fruit and vegetables [39, 41, 43, 57, 58, 61]. Natural experiments related to physical activity included exposure to workplace physical activity programmes [49], schools [32, 65], park settings [29, 67], community-wide programmes [30, 44, 63], and built environment interventions, including infrastructure for active travel and changes in street design [30, 40, 53, 62], residential/school development [34, 35, 47, 55, 59, 60, 68] and green spaces [36, 38, 69]. Other experiments were conducted to assess physical activity outcomes associated with disruption to transport services [64], daylight saving [72] and social habitat [70]. Obesity-related outcomes more broadly were evaluated in relation to exposure to clinical procedures [52, 56], migration [45, 46], education [51] or economic events [33, 50, 54, 71].

Evaluation characteristics

Study design

A variety of study designs were used, with several quasi-experiments, comparing longitudinal pre/post intervention changes in exposure of a cohort against a control or comparison group [28, 31, 34, 36, 40, 42, 47, 48, 52, 61, 63, 67, 69, 72]. The remaining cohort studies were observational (non-experimental), comparing pre/post exposure without a control group [35, 44, 57, 64, 68, 71]. Four studies conducted interrupted time-series on sales data [27, 37, 43, 66], two without a control comparator (observational only) [43, 66]. Observational studies included repeat cross-sectional surveys [30, 33, 38, 43, 4951, 56, 62], some of which incorporated mixed methods (qualitative interviews or geospatial mapping) [38, 43, 48, 56], or used a cross-sectional single time point only [29, 32, 39, 41, 45, 55, 5860, 65, 70]. One study pooled repeat cross-sectional data as a result of data availability and was therefore unable to assess time-related effects [32]; another [46] was a feasibility study anticipating a larger, longitudinal investigation. Two studies were qualitative only [53, 54].

Data source

Natural experiment studies used a variety of sources. In total, four studies collected both primary and secondary data [43, 48, 53, 64] and 27 studies only collected primary data. Fifteen studies used secondary routinely collected surveillance data from national or state censes and health surveys [3133, 51, 56, 71, 72], hospital administrative data [50, 52], pre-existing cohort studies [39, 70] and food supply [28] or sales data [27, 37, 43, 66]. Three studies used simulated data to establish a hypothetical unexposed group [27, 28, 44].

Exposure

Exposure samples comprised residential populations in a defined neighbourhood or region, consumers of food outlets (e.g. supermarkets or restaurant chain) or physical activity space (e.g. park-setting). A clearly defined unexposed ‘control’ group was used by 15 studies [28, 31, 3436, 40, 42, 4648, 52, 61, 63, 67, 69, 72]. More often, exposure was assessed in the one population group or area without a defined unexposed group (n = 18) [27, 30, 33, 38, 43, 44, 47, 4952, 56, 57, 62, 64, 68, 70, 71] or comparisons made between two groups, one of which was exposed, at a single time point (n = 10) [29, 32, 39, 41, 45, 55, 5860, 65].

Not all studies used a dichotomous definition of exposure. Graded levels of exposure between groups, areas or individuals over time were used in a small number of studies [33, 43, 51, 71]. These studies had no similar intervention characteristics that might suggest a pattern or typology of where and when graded exposure may be necessary.

Evaluation measures

A variety of evaluation measures were used to evaluate obesity prevention interventions, including subjective and objective measures of physical activity (walking, active travel, metabolic equivalents), diet and body mass index, and health behaviour from surveys, physical and manual counters (i.e. accelerometers), as well as secondary data sources listed above. One study used geospatial information systems to identify exposed and unexposed groups [35].

Evaluation methods varied across similar interventions. For example, two studies [36, 38] evaluated the impact of new outdoor gym equipment on physical activity. Only one compared changes in the exposed group (park users/residents) against users of parks without gym equipment. Three studies used different methods to assess sales data to determine the effectiveness of food labelling policies in supermarkets. One of these compared data across three supermarket chains, with one chain acting as the control [55], one compared variation in exposure across a variety of supermarket types [66], and the third study simulated exposure because the labelling policy in question was voluntary and haphazardly implemented [27]. These differences suggest a variety of evaluation measures are applied to natural experiment opportunities.

Evaluation timeframe

Of the studies that included pre and post data collection, studies conducted over a short (less than 3 year) evaluation period, such as community or environmental interventions, predominantly used original sourced data from surveys or observational measures. Studies with a longer evaluation period relied on secondary data collection as could be expected given the time constraints on primary data collection.

Utility

The value of natural experiments for providing real-world population evaluation evidence was appraised in terms of whether the studies could have been conducted as planned researcher-driven experiments. We determined the purpose of the study and found that the majority evaluated the effectiveness of an intervention or impact of a policy (see Additional file 4 Table S2 for full list). In terms of utility, we first found that planned researcher-driven research was not always feasible to answer the research questions raised by the authors and, as such, were opportunities that could only ever be investigated as natural experiments. For example, Jones-Smith et al. [50] sought to establish whether Native American economic resources (from casino ownership) influenced the likelihood of childhood obesity; Zick et al. [72] aimed to assess whether daylight saving time is associated with increased time spent in moderate-to-vigorous physical activity. The purpose of these studies was to examine social or environmental determinants or inequalities in the population, which would not be amenable to/appropriate for researcher manipulation. In some instances, we recognised it would be possible for a researcher to have some involvement in the decisions regarding when and where an intervention occurs in order to obtain baseline data; for example, evaluation of the impact of a new policy taxation [27, 37]. However, if a policy was enacted quickly, baseline data collection would be restricted. Some of the studies involved relocation, such as those for new housing developments in Australia [34, 35, 47, 68], where researcher control over the planning, timing or conditions of the relocation, even if working with the authorities, would likely be unrealistic.

We note that studies aimed at assessing inequalities in the population or determining intervention effectiveness would be unethical as a planned experiment where there was risk of potential harm from intentionally restricting access to medical care [52], economic support [50], educational opportunity [51] or randomising individuals/groups to social benefits [33, 57, 71], freedom to migrate [45, 46] or health services [57, 65].

In other cases, researcher manipulation of study components may have been possible, but the researcher would have been constrained by practical considerations such as time, population sample size or location of exposure [61]. As obesity takes time to develop and tends to reverse, some evaluation questions necessitate a long lag time between exposure and outcome, which negate short-time planning. In one study, historical data was used to investigate the effect of academic schooling on obesity-related health behaviours in adults [51]. A planned study of this nature would be unrealistic, involving long follow-up from schooling to adult years to establish causality; it would also be unethical to control exposure to schooling. Bernatchez et al. [30] evaluates awareness of a new bicycle share programme rather than use; this may be because evaluation occurred too early to measure behaviour change as measured in another similar study [40]. In other cases, a planned experiment would be unpractical because the nature of the intervention necessitates a whole-of-population approach, such as the effect of tariffs applied to certain energy dense foods on unhealthy food consumption [28]. The point of time at which a researcher engages may preclude pre-intervention data collection. For example, in one study, researchers could only collect retrospective survey data on commute mode from parents whose children transferred to a new school [55]; other researchers serendipitously had a public transit strike occur in the midst of measuring spatial behaviour among undergraduate students, allowing a spontaneous pre/post examination on changes to student travel patterns to university [64]. In some situations, a natural experiment may be the only realistic option available despite the absence of a control group because the intervention is so unique that a suitable control group is not feasible. For example, in the study by Cranney et al. [38], the park setting in which equipment was installed may have had unique features specific to its local environment, precluding a suitable control. Similarly, in the study by Barradas et al. [29], some parks were already receiving the intervention programme and intervention settings were pre-determined by another body.

Discussion

This study has characterised the use and value of natural experiments, particularly focusing on the area of obesity prevention and its complex aetiology. Although the number of studies self-described as natural experiments has increased over the past two decades, the body of research, at least in obesity prevention, remains small.

Obesity prevention is a complex issue. Research evidence about interventions implemented in real-world conditions and the impact of policies represent a key gap in the knowledge. Thus, there is a need for greater generation of evidence about the impact and effectiveness of policy strategies, and natural experiments could be better utilised to provide this evidence. In assessing how natural experiment studies have been used, we found that the majority of the studies reviewed were designed to evaluate the impact of a policy or the effectiveness of an environmental intervention. The policy evaluation studies were almost all at the national or state level. Geographically, the studies in this review were located in English-speaking countries, which may reflect the high prevalence rates of obesity in these countries, and the search methods precluded studies in non-English speaking countries; however, it may also be due to the low use of evaluation approaches across other countries and within research groups within these countries. For this reason, WHO Europe recently held a workshop to support and facilitate public health practitioners from participating European countries in the use of natural experiment methods [14].

Strengths and weaknesses of natural experiments

The strengths of natural experiments are in their ability to evaluate the process and outcomes of implementation of policies and interventions within the real-world complex social and political conditions they naturally operate in. The response to the obesity epidemic has required a broad range of policy, environmental and individual behaviour change interventions – necessarily complex interventions able to function within a complex socio-political system [2, 23]. Evaluation research designs need to be flexible and able to measure the interaction between multiple factors [73].

Natural experiments offer opportunistic evidence where a researcher-driven study may be impossible for reasons of intervention timing or exposure. Nevertheless, the ability to make causal inferences from natural experiments depends on optimising the research study design [74]. We identified a variety of designs, including interrupted time-series, cross-sectional and longitudinal cohort designs. Few of these studies were experimental/quasi-experimental, including both pre and post measures for an exposed and a comparator group. However, a surprising number of the studies used a single data collection point to evaluate an intervention and thus could not attribute any observed changes to exposure to the intervention. However, some used mixed methods designs to strengthen study findings. Two studies presented only baseline data (pre-experimental) [41, 60] and may have further intentions for collecting follow-up data; these may be premeditated natural experiments. This evidence represents a weakness in study design for evaluating natural experiment studies rather than a weakness of natural experiments in general, and something the MRC has tried to address through detailed guidance on measurement and statistical methodologies [26].

Another strength of natural experiments is their flexibility. Evaluation periods ranged from a single time point to spanning decades and unsurprisingly the evaluation period determined whether primary or secondary data was used to assess exposure. A variety of outcome measures was used and these sometimes varied across similar interventions. While this flexibility is an advantage of the method, it increases the difficulty for comparisons in evidence reviews [21]. Most studies adapted the research design to fit the existing intervention context. Harnessing resources before policy interventions is a research planning challenge posed by natural experiments. To overcome this, some studies strengthened their findings by the use of multiple data sources. In such situations, both primary and secondary data contribute to the evidence, and largely depend on the level of control the researcher has on the intervention occurring, the group exposed or the timeframe in which the intervention or event occurs.

A further strength of natural experiments is their delivery of scale, allowing for exploration of a wider range of research questions to be investigated at the population level, to enable generalisation [15]. Natural experiments operate under circumstances where randomisation is not possible, there are ethical considerations, identifying suitable controls may problematic, and timing (both researcher timing relative to the intervention and the time length of the intervention) may make the collection of ideal data infeasible. These natural experiments fill a void not otherwise met by traditional designs, and may yield insights into exposure–outcome relationships, which are nonetheless informative for obesity prevention.

Natural experiments are often criticised for their inability to eliminate bias. Benton et al. [75] assessed the risk of bias across a number of natural experiments using an acceptability score based on confounding, participant selection, exposure and outcome measures, and missing data, concluding that evidence on the effects of these interventions was too problematic to be useful. Bias, particularly due to confounding, is a concern for natural experiments [18]. We observed that few studies in this review had a defined control and exposed group – given that these studies are unplanned evaluations, a clearly defined control group may be challenging to establish. Within the studies reviewed, a clearly defined unexposed group was more likely in environmental interventions where the researcher could establish a study design with original data. In contrast, establishing a clearly defined unexposed group becomes more difficult when evaluating policy interventions, as these are generally implemented acutely, and at the population level. Modelling simulations to create control groups was one approach employed [27, 44] to circumnavigate the lack of control group comparator. Others employed a step-wedge design [63] or time-series data to help overcome some limitations of not having a suitable control comparator condition. Simulations may be especially useful when counterfactuals are difficult to establish or create [26]. Leatherdale [18] provides some other suggestions for facilitating greater evaluation evidence of health policies, suggesting the need to improve the ability to evaluate government policy. For this to happen, capacity needs to be built around practitioners either to conduct natural experiments or to work closely with academics so that more robust quasi-experimental methods of evaluation can be employed [76].

Despite these weaknesses, we caution against devaluing natural experiments based on a simple hierarchy of evidence. Applying the same standards of study design quality to natural experiments ignores the contribution they can make to overall evidence generation [17], particularly in regard to the complexity of real-world interventions and policy evidence. In response to the complexity of the obesity epidemic, obtaining policy and intervention impact evidence is critical. Indeed, natural experiments may provide innovative research translation evidence that has been lacking on obesity policy interventions [77, 78]. Therefore, despite the limitations of natural experiments, they provide valuable information on public health efforts to prevent obesity as, otherwise, any effects might remain unknown.

The limitations of this review include our search criteria for identifying studies. It is possible that the confusion around natural experiments and quasi-experiments limits the number of collected studies to those we have discussed. For example, some protocol papers extracted from the initial search identified as a natural experiment, yet subsequent articles by the authors of these papers did not self-identify their intervention as a natural experiment. This suggests that those studies that want to identify as this type of research are not meeting an agreed definition. The synthesis of the utility of studies was limited to the information provided in each article; political, socio-cultural and practical obstacles that affect natural experiments and limit the potential for planned interventions is thus speculative.

Conclusion

This review examined natural experiments in an effort to improve the public health evidence for obesity prevention where a controlled experimental design would be inappropriate. Research using natural experiments has increased over the last few years; however, it remains overlooked in the context of the wider research evidence despite the importance of these interventions taking place in real-world settings. Our findings highlight the strength of natural experiments in improving our understanding of the effectiveness of complex population interventions and providing informed evidence of the impact of policies and novel approaches to understanding health determinants and inequalities. The studies reviewed also reveal that there is need to strengthen research designs to enhance their utility.

Box 1 Determining the utility of a natural experiment.

1. Is there a need to generate population health research evidence about this issue? [24, 25]

Does it provide evidence on the impact or effectiveness of a policy or environmental intervention, address health inequalities in the population or how to change behaviour?

2. Is a planned researcher-controlled study possible? [22]

a. No control – no ability to control where the intervention occurs, whom it affects or when it starts or finishes

b. Partial control – ability to influence exposure timing or location (i.e. able to arrange with the relevant authorities to obtain comparative baseline data)

3. Is a planned researcher-controlled study ethical? [22, 26]

a. Creating the conditions for an experiment to determine this issue would be unethical (e.g. where it may create more harm)

b. Random assignment to the intervention would be unethical (e.g. withholding the only known cure for a disease)

4. Is a planned researcher-controlled study practical? [12, 13, 25]

c. Sufficient population available for randomisation: total number of planned exposed/unexposed clusters required would be unrealistic to achieve

d. Time for follow-up: unlikely for a researcher-driven study could accommodate the prolonged periods necessary for outcomes to establish across entire populations

e. Representativeness of the population: threat to applicability of a study if the population sample is too different from the population it was intended to represent

f. Relative costs: sufficient sample and time lag before health impacts evolve would be costly to implement

g. Programme complexity: the intervention is so complex that a controlled study environment would be infeasible

h. Intervention scale-up: an intervention is known to be efficacious/effective but requires demonstration of its effectiveness on a scale unlikely in a researcher-driven context

i. Real-world conditions: purpose is to document contextual factors constituting ‘real-world’ implementation conditions - experimental approach is antithetical to this purpose

Supplementary information

12961_2020_564_MOESM1_ESM.docx (18.1KB, docx)

Additional file 1: Table S1. Search strategy.

12961_2020_564_MOESM2_ESM.docx (25.1KB, docx)

Additional file 2: Table S2. Describes studies excluded through abstract review.

12961_2020_564_MOESM3_ESM.docx (21.1KB, docx)

Additional file 3: Figure S1. Showing the use of natural experiment studies surrounding nutrition and physical activity interventions on obesity.

12961_2020_564_MOESM4_ESM.docx (24.7KB, docx)

Additional file 4: Table S3. Describes the research aims of included studies.

Acknowledgments

Not applicable.

Abbreviations

MRC

Medical Research Council

RCT

Randomised controlled trial

Authors’ contributions

AB and MC conceived the study. MC led the design, analysis and manuscript writing. EG conducted the literature search and contributed to analysis and manuscript writing. AG contributed to the design and manuscript writing and provided critical feedback. AB contributed to the design and provided critical feedback on the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

The work was funded by the National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia (NHMRC) through its Partnership Centre grant scheme [grant number GNT9100001]. NSW Health, ACT Health, the Australian Government Department of Health, the Hospitals Contribution Fund of Australia and the HCF Research Foundation have contributed funds to support this work as part of the NHMRC Partnership Centre grant scheme. The contents of this paper are solely the responsibility of the individual authors and do not reflect the views of the NHMRC or funding partners.

Availability of data and materials

Data generated or analysed during this study are included in this published article and its additional files. Please contact the corresponding author for any additional queries.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare they have no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Contributor Information

Melanie Crane, Email: melanie.crane@sydney.edu.au.

Erika Bohn-Goldbaum, Email: erika.goldbaum@sydney.edu.au.

Anne Grunseit, Email: anne.grunseit@sydney.edu.au.

Adrian Bauman, Email: adrian.bauman@sydney.edu.au.

Supplementary information

Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1186/s12961-020-00564-2.

References

  • 1.Crane M, Bauman A, Lloyd B, McGill B, Rissel C, Grunseit A. Applying pragmatic approaches to complex program evaluation: A case study of implementation of the New South Wales Get Healthy at Work program. Health Prom J Austrl. 2019;30(3):422. doi: 10.1002/hpja.239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Rutter H, Savona N, Glonti K, Bibby J, Cummins S, Finegood DT, et al. The need for a complex systems model of evidence for public health. Lancet. 2017;390(10112):2602–2604. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31267-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Butland BJS, Kopelman P, McPherson K, Thomas S, Mardell J, Parry V. Tackling Obesities: Future Choices. Project Report. 2nd edition: Government Office for Science. London: Department of Innovation, Universities and Skills; 2007.
  • 4.Roberto CA, Swinburn B, Hawkes C, Huang TTK, Costa SA, Ashe M, et al. Patchy progress on obesity prevention: emerging examples, entrenched barriers, and new thinking. Lancet. 2015;385(9985):2400–2409. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)61744-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Swinburn BA, Sacks G, Hall KD, McPherson K, Finegood DT, Moodie ML, et al. The global obesity pandemic: shaped by global drivers and local environments. Lancet. 2011;378(9793):804–814. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60813-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.GBD 2015 Obesity Collaborators Health Effects of Overweight and Obesity in 195 Countries over 25 Years. New Engl J Med. 2017;377(1):13–27. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1614362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Huang TT-K, Glass TA. Transforming research strategies for understanding and preventing obesity. JAMA. 2008;300(15):1811–1813. doi: 10.1001/jama.300.15.1811. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Gortmaker SL, Swinburn BA, Levy D, Carter R, Mabry PL, Finegood DT, et al. Changing the future of obesity: Science, policy, and action. Lancet. 2011;378(9793):838–847. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60815-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bauman AE, King L, Nutbeam D. Rethinking the evaluation and measurement of health in all policies. Health Promot Int. 2014;29:i143–ii51. doi: 10.1093/heapro/dau049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.McKinnon RA, Orleans CT, Kumanyika SK, Haire-Joshu D, Krebs-Smith SM, Finkelstein EA, et al. Considerations for an obesity policy research agenda. Am J Prev Med. 2009;36(4):351–357. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2008.11.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Sallis JF, Story M, Lou D. Study designs and analytic strategies for environmental and policy research on obesity, physical activity, and diet. Recommendations from a meeting of experts. Am J Prev Med. 2009;36(2 Suppl):S72–SS7. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2008.10.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Sanson-Fisher RW, Bonevski B, Green LW, D’Este C. Limitations of the randomized controlled trial in evaluating population-based health interventions. Am J Prev Med. 2007;33(2):155–161. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2007.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Victora CG, Habicht J-P, Bryce J. Evidence-based public health: moving beyond randomized trials. Am J Public Health. 2004;94(3):400–405. doi: 10.2105/ajph.94.3.400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Snell A, Reeves A, Rieger M, Galea G, Mauer-Stender K, Mikkelsen B, et al. WHO Regional Office for Europe's Natural Experiment Studies Project: an introduction to the series. Eur J Pub Health. 2018;28(2):1–3. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/cky195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Craig P, Cooper C, Gunnell D, Haw S, Lawson K, Macintyre S, et al. Using natural experiments to evaluate population health interventions: new medical research council guidance. J Epidemiol Commun Health. 2012;66(12):1182–1186. doi: 10.1136/jech-2011-200375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Petticrew M, Roberts H. Evidence, hierarchies, and typologies: Horses for courses. J Epidemiol Commun Health. 2003;57(7):527–529. doi: 10.1136/jech.57.7.527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Humphreys DK, Panter J, Sahlqvist S, Goodman A, Ogilvie D. Changing the environment to improve population health: A framework for considering exposure in natural experimental studies. J Epidemiol Commun Health. 2016;70(9):941–946. doi: 10.1136/jech-2015-206381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Leatherdale S. Natural experiment methodology for research: a review of how different methods can support real-world research. Int J Soc Res Methodol. 2019;2(22):19–35. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Gough D, Thomas J, Oliver S. Clarifying differences between review designs and methods. Syst Rev. 2012;1:28. doi: 10.1186/2046-4053-1-28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Kärmeniemi M, Lankila T, Ikäheimo T, Koivumaa-Honkanen H, Korpelainen R. The built environment as a determinant of physical activity: a systematic review of longitudinal studies and natural experiments. Ann Behav Med. 2018;52(3):239–251. doi: 10.1093/abm/kax043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Macmillan F, George ES, Feng X, Merom D, Bennie A, Cook A, et al. Do natural experiments of changes in neighborhood built environment impact physical activity and diet? A systematic review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018;15(2):217. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15020217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Dawson A, Sim J. The nature and ethics of natural experiments. J Med Ethics. 2015;41(10):848–853. doi: 10.1136/medethics-2014-102254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Ogilvie D, Cummins S, Petticrew M, White M, Jones A, Wheeler K. Assessing the evaluability of complex public health interventions: five questions for researchers, funders, and policymakers. Milbank Q. 2011;89(2):206–225. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0009.2011.00626.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Ogilvie D, Adams J, Bauman A, Gregg E, Panter J, Siegel K, et al. Using natural experimental studies to guide public health action: turning the evidence-based medicine paradigm on its head. J Epidemiol Commun Health. 2020;74:203–208. doi: 10.1136/jech-2019-213085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Petticrew M, Cummins S, Ferrell C, Findlay A, Higgins C, Hoy C, et al. Natural experiments: an underused tool for public health? Public Health. 2005;119(9):751–757. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2004.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Craig P, Katikireddi SV, Leyland A, Popham F. Natural experiments: an overview of methods, approaches, and contributions to public health intervention research. Ann Rev Public Health. 2017;38:39–56. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-031816-044327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Allais O, Etilé F, Lecocq S. Mandatory labels, taxes and market forces: an empirical evaluation of fat policies. J Health Econ. 2015;43:27–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2015.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Barlow P, McKee M, Basu S, Stuckler D. Impact of the North American free trade agreement on high-fructose corn syrup supply in Canada: a natural experiment using synthetic control methods. CMAJ. 2017;189(26):E881–E8E7. doi: 10.1503/cmaj.161152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Barradas SC, Finck Barboza C, Sarmiento OL. Differences between leisure-time physical activity, health-related quality of life and life satisfaction: Al Ritmo de las Comunidades, a natural experiment from Colombia. Global Health Promot. 2019;26:5–14. doi: 10.1177/1757975917703303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Bernatchez AC, Gauvin L, Fuller D, Dube AS, Drouin L. Knowing about a public bicycle share program in Montreal, Canada: Are diffusion of innovation and proximity enough for equitable awareness? J Transp Health. 2015;2(3):360–368. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Brandt EJ, Myerson R, Perraillon MC, Polonsky TS. Hospital Admissions for Myocardial Infarction and Stroke Before and After the Trans-Fatty Acid Restrictions in New York. JAMA Cardiol. 2017;2(6):627–634. doi: 10.1001/jamacardio.2017.0491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Cawley J, Meyerhoefer C, Newhouse D. The impact of state physical education requirements on youth physical activity and overweight. Health Econ. 2007;16(12):1287–1301. doi: 10.1002/hec.1218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Cawley J, Moran J, Simon K. The impact of income on the weight of elderly Americans. Health Econ. 2010;19(8):979–993. doi: 10.1002/hec.1541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Christian H, Knuiman M, Bull F, Timperio A, Foster S, Divitini M, et al. A new urban planning code's impact on walking: The Residential Environments Project. Am J Public Health. 2013;103(7):1219–1228. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Christian H, Knuiman M, Divitini M, Foster S, Hooper P, Boruff B, et al. A longitudinal analysis of the influence of the neighborhood environment on recreational walking within the neighborhood: Results from RESIDE. Environ Health Perspect. 2017;125(7):077009. doi: 10.1289/EHP823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Cohen DA, Marsh T, Williamson S, Golinelli D, McKenzie TL, Cohen DA, et al. Impact and cost-effectiveness of family Fitness Zones: a natural experiment in urban public parks. Health Place. 2012;18(1):39–45. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2011.09.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Cornelsen L, Mytton OT, Adams J, Gasparrini A, Iskander D, Knai C, et al. Change in non-alcoholic beverage sales following a 10-pence levy on sugar-sweetened beverages within a national chain of restaurants in the UK: interrupted time series analysis of a natural experiment. J Epidemiol Commun Health. 2017;71(11):1107–1112. doi: 10.1136/jech-2017-209947. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Cranney L, Phongsavan P, Kariuki M, Stride V, Scott A, Hua M, et al. Impact of an outdoor gym on park users' physical activity: A natural experiment. Health Place. 2016;37:26–34. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2015.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Datar A, Nicosia N. The effect of state competitive food and beverage regulations on childhood overweight and obesity. J Adolesc Health. 2017;60(5):520–527. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2016.09.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Dill J, McNeil N, Broach J, Ma L. Bicycle boulevards and changes in physical activity and active transportation: findings from a natural experiment. Prev Med. 2014;69:S74–S78. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2014.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Dubowitz T, Ncube C, Leuschner K, Tharp-Gilliam S. A natural experiment opportunity in two low-income urban food desert communities: research design, community engagement methods, and baseline results. Health Educ Behav. 2015;42:87–96. doi: 10.1177/1090198115570048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Elbel B, Gyamfi J, Kersh R. Child and adolescent fast-food choice and the influence of calorie labeling: a natural experiment. Int J Obes. 2011;35(4):493–500. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2011.4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Ferguson M, O'Dea K, Holden S, Miles E, Brimblecombe J. Food and beverage price discounts to improve health in remote Aboriginal communities: mixed method evaluation of a natural experiment. Aust N Z J Public Health. 2017;41(1):32–37. doi: 10.1111/1753-6405.12616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Frew EJ, Bhatti M, Win K, Sitch A, Lyon A, Pallan M, et al. Cost-effectiveness of a community-based physical activity programme for adults (Be Active) in the UK: an economic analysis within a natural experiment. Br J Sports Med. 2014;48(3):207–212. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2012-091202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Fu H, VanLandingham M. Disentangling the effects of migration, selection and acculturation on weight and body fat distribution: results from a natural experiment involving vietnamese americans, returnees, and never-leavers. J Immigr Minority Health. 2012;14(5):786–796. doi: 10.1007/s10903-012-9595-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Gee GC, de Castro AB, Wang MC, Crespi CM, Morey BN, Kaori F. Feasibility of conducting a longitudinal, transnational study of Filipino migrants to the United States: a dual-cohort design. J Health Care Poor Underserved. 2015;26(2):488–504. doi: 10.1353/hpu.2015.0045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Giles-Corti B, Bull F, Knuiman M, McCormack G, Van Niel K, Timperio A, et al. The influence of urban design on neighbourhood walking following residential relocation: longitudinal results from the RESIDE study. Soc Sci Med. 2013;77:20–30. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.10.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Hobin E, Bollinger B, Sacco J, Liebman E, Vanderlee L, Zuo F, et al. Consumers' response to an on-shelf nutrition labelling system in supermarkets: evidence to inform policy and practice. Milbank Q. 2017;95(3):494–534. doi: 10.1111/1468-0009.12277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Jancey JM, McGann S, Creagh R, Blackford KD, Howat P, Tye M. Workplace building design and office-based workers' activity: a study of a natural experiment. Austr N Z J Public Health. 2016;40(1):78–82. doi: 10.1111/1753-6405.12464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Jones-Smith JC, Dow WH, Oddo VM. Association between Native American-owned casinos and the prevalence of large-for-gestational- age births. Int J Epidemiol. 2017;46(4):1202–1210. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyx054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Jürges H, Reinhold S, Salm M. Does schooling affect health behavior? Evidence from the educational expansion in Western Germany. Econ Educ Rev. 2011;30(5):862–872. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Kapinos KA, Yakusheva O, Weiss M. Cesarean deliveries and maternal weight retention. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2017;17:343. doi: 10.1186/s12884-017-1527-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Kesten JM, Cohn S, Ogilvie D, et al. BMC Public Health. 2014;14:482. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-14-482. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Kodish SR, Gittelsohn J, Oddo VM, Jones-Smith JC. Impacts of casinos on key pathways to health: qualitative findings from American Indian gaming communities in California. BMC Public Health. 2016;16:621. doi: 10.1186/s12889-016-3279-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Lee C, Yoon J, Zhu X. From sedentary to active school commute: multi-level factors associated with travel mode shifts. Prev Med. 2017;95:S28–S36. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Madsen KA. School-based body mass index screening and parent notification: a statewide natural experiment. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2011;165(11):987–992. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Odoms-Young AM, Kong A, Schiffer LA, Porter SJ, Blumstein L, Bess S, et al. Evaluating the initial impact of the revised Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children (WIC) food packages on dietary intake and home food availability in African-American and Hispanic families. Public Health Nutr. 2014;17(1):83–93. doi: 10.1017/S1368980013000761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Ovrum A, Bere E, Øvrum A. Evaluating free school fruit: results from a natural experiment in Norway with representative data. Public Health Nutr. 2014;17(6):1224–1231. doi: 10.1017/S1368980013002504. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Pollack CE, Green HD, Jr, Kennedy DP, Griffin BA, Kennedy-Hendricks A, Burkhauser S, et al. The Impact of public housing on social networks: a natural experiment. Am J Public Health. 2014;104(9):1642–1649. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2014.301949. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Ram B, Nightingale CM, Hudda MT, Kapetanakis VV, Ellaway A, Cooper AR, et al. Cohort profile: Examining Neighbourhood Activities in Built Living Environments in London: the ENABLE London – Olympic Park cohort. BMJ Open. 2016;6(10):e012643. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-012643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Sadler RC, Gillil JA, Arku G. A food retail-based intervention on food security and consumption. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2013;10(8):3325–3346. doi: 10.3390/ijerph10083325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Schultz CL, Wilhelm Stanis SA, Sayers SP, Thombs LA, Thomas IM. A longitudinal examination of improved access on park use and physical activity in a low-income and majority African American neighborhood park. Prev Med. 2017;95:S95–S100. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.08.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Simões EJ, Hallal PC, Siqueira FV, Schmaltz C, Menor D, Malta DC, et al. Effectiveness of a scaled up physical activity intervention in Brazil: a natural experiment. Prev Med. 2017;103:S66–S72. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.09.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Stanley K, Bell S, Kreuger LK, Bhowmik P, Shojaati N, Elliott A, et al. Opportunistic natural experiments using digital telemetry: a transit disruption case study. Int J Geogr Inf Sci. 2016;30(9):1853–1872. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Stone MR, Faulkner GE, Zeglen-Hunt L, Bonne JC. The Daily Physical Activity (DPA) policy in Ontario: is it working? an examination using accelerometry-measured physical activity data. Can J Public Health. 2012;103(3):170–174. doi: 10.1007/BF03403807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Sutherland LA, Kaley LA, Fischer L. Guiding Stars: the effect of a nutrition navigation program on consumer purchases at the supermarket. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010;91(4):1090S–1094S. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2010.28450C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Torres A, Díaz MP, Hayat MJ, Lyn R, Pratt M, Salvo D, et al. Assessing the effect of physical activity classes in public spaces on leisure-time physical activity: “Al Ritmo de las Comunidades” A natural experiment in Bogota, Colombia. Prev Med. 2017;103:S51–SS8. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Tudor-Locke C, Giles-Corti B, Knuiman M, McCormack G. Tracking of pedometer-determined physical activity in adults who relocate: results from RESIDE. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2008;5:39. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-5-39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Veitch J, Ball K, Crawford D, Abbott GR, Salmon J. Park improvements and park activity: a natural experiment. Am J Prev Med. 2012;42(6):616–619. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2012.02.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Wagner B, Jiang L, Hexuan L, Guang G. Gene–environment correlation: difficulties and a natural experiment-based strategy. Am J Public Health. 2013;103(S1):S167–S173. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Watson B, Osberg L, Phipps S. Economic insecurity and the weight gain of Canadian adults: a natural experiment approach. Can Public Policy. 2016;42(2):115–131. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Zick CD. Does daylight savings time encourage physical activity? J Phys Act Health. 2014;11(5):1057–1060. doi: 10.1123/jpah.2012-0300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Moore GF, Evans RE, Hawkins J, Littlecott H, Melendez-Torres GJ, Bonell C, et al. From complex social interventions to interventions in complex social systems: Future directions and unresolved questions for intervention development and evaluation. Evaluation. 2019;25(1):23–45. doi: 10.1177/1356389018803219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Medical Research Council (MRC) Using Natural Experiments to Evaluate Population Health Interventions: Guidance for Producers and Users of Evidence. London: Medical Research Council; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Benton J, Anderson J, Hunter R, French D. The effect of changing the built envoionment on physical activity: a quantitative review of the risk of bias in natural experiments. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2016;13(1):107. doi: 10.1186/s12966-016-0433-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Green J, Roberts H, Petticrew M, Steinbach R, Goodman A, Jones A, et al. Integrating quasi-experimental and inductive designs in evaluation: a case study of the impact of free bus travel on public health. Evaluation. 2015;21(4):391–406. [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Oliver K, Lorenc T, Innvær S. New directions in evidence-based policy research: a critical analysis of the literature. Health Res Policy Syst. 2014;12:34. doi: 10.1186/1478-4505-12-34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Brownson RC, Chriqui JF, Stamatakis KA. Understanding evidence-based public health policy. Am J Public Health. 2009;99(9):1576–1583. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2008.156224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

12961_2020_564_MOESM1_ESM.docx (18.1KB, docx)

Additional file 1: Table S1. Search strategy.

12961_2020_564_MOESM2_ESM.docx (25.1KB, docx)

Additional file 2: Table S2. Describes studies excluded through abstract review.

12961_2020_564_MOESM3_ESM.docx (21.1KB, docx)

Additional file 3: Figure S1. Showing the use of natural experiment studies surrounding nutrition and physical activity interventions on obesity.

12961_2020_564_MOESM4_ESM.docx (24.7KB, docx)

Additional file 4: Table S3. Describes the research aims of included studies.

Data Availability Statement

Data generated or analysed during this study are included in this published article and its additional files. Please contact the corresponding author for any additional queries.


Articles from Health Research Policy and Systems are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES