Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Schizophr Res. 2019 Nov 30;216:357–366. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.11.013

Relationship of Prolonged Acoustic Startle Latency to Diagnosis and Biotype in the Bipolar-Schizophrenia Network on Intermediate Phenotypes (B-SNIP) Cohort

Nicholas Massa 1,2, Andrew V Owens 3, Wesley Harmon 4, Arpita Bhattacharya 5, Elena I Ivleva 6, Sarah Keedy 7, John A Sweeney 8, Godfrey D Pearlson 9, Matcheri S Keshavan 10, Carol A Tamminga 6, Brett A Clementz 11,*, Erica Duncan 1,3,*
PMCID: PMC7239737  NIHMSID: NIHMS1545225  PMID: 31796306

Abstract

Background

Latency of the acoustic startle reflex is the time from presentation of the startling stimulus until the response and provides an index of neural processing speed. Latency is prolonged in schizophrenia, is 90% heritable, and predicts conversion to schizophrenia in a high-risk population. The Bipolar-Schizophrenia Network for Intermediate Phenotypes (B-SNIP) consortium investigates neurobiological features found in psychotic disorders spanning diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia (SCZ), schizoaffective disorder (SAD), and psychotic bipolar disorder (BP). We investigated whether differences in startle latency and prepulse inhibition (PPI) occur in probands, their first-degree relatives, and neurobiologically defined subgroups of the probands (Biotypes).

Methods

1143 subjects were included from the B-SNIP cohort: 143 with SCZ, 178 SCZ relatives (SCZ-Fam), 123 with SAD, 152 SAD relatives (SAD-Fam), 138 BP, 183 BP relatives (BP-Fam), and 226 controls (CON). A Biopac system recorded the eyeblink component of the startle reflex during startle testing.

Results

Latency differed by diagnosis (F(3,620)=5.10, p=0.002): SCZ, SAD, and BP probands had slower latency than CON, with relatives intermediate. Biotypes 1 and 2 had slower latency than CON (p<0.031) but Biotype 3 did not differ from CON. PPI did not separate CON from other subjects when analyzed by diagnoses nor when analyzed by biotype. Biotype 1 relatives had slower latency (F(3,663)=3.49, p=0.016) and more impaired PPI than Biotype 2 and 3 relatives (F(3,663)=2.77, p=0.041).

Conclusion

Startle latency is prolonged in psychotic disorders that cross traditional diagnostic categories. These data suggest a genetic difference between biotypes that span across clinically defined diagnoses.

Keywords: Latency, Acoustic startle, Prepulse inhibition, Schizophrenia, Bipolar, Biotype

1.1. Introduction

Schizophrenia (SCZ), schizoaffective disorder (SAD) and bipolar disorder (BP) are disabling mental illnesses that each have a prevalence of approximately 1% (McGrath et al., 2008; Merikangas et al., 2011) and account for over $150 billion of total healthcare costs to the US healthcare system (Cloutier et al., 2016; Cloutier et al., 2018). These psychiatric disorders have significant overlap in clinical features, brain structure, synaptic dysfunction, pharmacological treatment, and genetic determinants (Narayanan et al., 2015).

The Bipolar-Schizophrenia Network for Intermediate Phenotypes (B-SNIP) Consortium was created to investigate biological differences and overlap in individuals with SCZ, SAD, or psychotic BP and their first-degree relatives to help guide the development of endophenotypes either within or across DSM diagnoses (Tamminga et al., 2014). Recent research conducted by this consortium has identified three experimental psychosis constructs—biotypes—derived from a battery of cognitive and neurophysiologic measures (Clementz et al., 2016; Pearlson et al., 2016). DSM categories do not map onto the neurobiologically defined biotypes. Biotype 1 was characterized by poor cognition and impaired sensorimotor function; Biotype 2 was characterized by moderately impaired cognition and hyper-responsivity to sensorimotor events; Biotype 3 subjects, while still psychotic, were most similar to healthy controls (CON) (Clementz et al., 2016).

A potential means of further investigating the neurological function of these individuals across biotypes is through the acoustic startle reflex (ASR), which has been extensively investigated in psychiatric disorders and animal models. It is a reflexive response to sudden intense acoustic stimuli seen ubiquitously in mammals that serves as a pre-attentive reaction to ready the organism to respond to potential threats in the environment. The output reflex is mediated by a pontine-based, three-synapse subcortical neural circuit (Koch, 1999).

A very well researched modulation of the ASR is prepulse inhibition (PPI), a reduction of the ASR by a non-startling stimulus presented shortly before the more intense startling stimulus. PPI has been investigated as an operational measure of sensorimotor gating (Graham, 1975; Hoffman and Searle, 1968; Wynn et al., 2004). Multiple publications have reported impaired PPI in subjects with SCZ (Braff et al., 2001), and PPI is generally accepted as an endophenotype for SCZ used for the discovery of genes associated with SCZ (Greenwood et al., 2016; Greenwood et al., 2011). However, PPI has limitations as it is at least partially normalized by treatment with second generation antipsychotics (Fargotstein et al., 2018; Swerdlow et al., 2008; Turetsky et al., 2007), and has somewhat modest heritability (38–58%; Anokhin et al., 2003; Greenwood et al., 2007; Hasenkamp et al., 2010). PPI was not included in the biological and cognitive battery used to discern biotypes by the B-SNIP consortium (Ivleva et al., 2014).

Startle latency is the time in milliseconds (ms) required for the startle reflex to occur after a startling stimulus. Subjects with SCZ have a slower latency compared to healthy controls (CON) (Braff et al., 1978; Braff et al., 1999; Geyer and Braff, 1982; Hasenkamp et al., 2010; Ludewig et al., 2002; Swerdlow et al., 2006; Swerdlow et al., 2018 ; Weike et al., 2000), although a smaller number of studies did not detect prolonged latency in SCZ (Braff et al., 1992; Mackeprang et al., 2002; Parwani et al., 2000). The slowing of latency persists in those SCZ subjects who are treated with antipsychotic medications (Fargotstein et al., 2018). Startle latency is up to 90% heritable in a sample of SCZ and CON subjects and their first-degree relatives (Hasenkamp et al., 2010). Additionally, slower latency predicts conversion to psychosis during a two-year follow-up period in young subjects at high-risk for development of SCZ (Cadenhead et al., 2013). Latency is thus able to provide a putative index of neural processing speed (Hasenkamp et al., 2010) and can serve as an endophenotype of SCZ separate from PPI (Fargotstein et al., 2018).

The principal aims of this study were to determine whether prolonged acoustic startle latency and impaired PPI were associated with membership in the specific DSM diagnoses (either in probands or in their relatives) and B-SNIP psychosis biotypes since startle latency had not been previously assessed in the B-SNIP cohort. Further, we hypothesized that prolonged latency could be associated with cognitive impairment through impaired functioning of neural circuits during cognitive processing. Therefore, a secondary aim was to investigate whether slowing of latency associated with greater cognitive impairments in these data across diagnoses. These analyses present an opportunity to refine our understanding of the neurobiology underlying psychotic disorders spanning traditional DSM diagnoses and to further characterize biotypes through independent measures.

1.2. Methods

1.2.1. Cohort Description

1143 individuals from the B-SNIP cohort who had completed the acoustic startle session were analyzed. Within this cohort there were 143 with schizophrenia (SCZ), 178 SCZ first-degree relatives (SCZ-Fam), 123 with schizoaffective disorder (SAD), 152 SAD first-degree relatives (SAD-Fam), 138 who had bipolar disorder (BP), 183 who were relatives of those with BP (BP-Fam), and 226 controls (CON). All proband subjects were clinically stable, medicated outpatients. Relatives of probands with a psychosis diagnosis remained in their respective relative cohorts. CON subjects had no personal history of psychiatric diagnosis, and no family history of SCZ/BP spectrum disorders in either first or second-degree relatives. Axis I diagnoses were based on the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR (First et al., 2001) and Axis II diagnoses in relatives were captured using the Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality disorders (Pfohl et al., 1997). Subjects were excluded for: a history of seizures or head injury with a loss of consciousness >30min, a diagnosis of substance abuse in the preceding 30 days or substance dependence in the previous 3 months, or a history of systemic medical or neurological disorder known to affect brain function. Subjects were also required to have a negative urine drug screen for drugs of abuse on the day of testing and an age-corrected Wide Range Achievement Test-IV (WRAT) Reading subtest standard score (SS) ≥ 65 (Hochberger et al., 2018; Wilkinson and Robertson, 2006). All probands underwent symptom ratings for psychosis and affective symptom domains as well as assessments for cognition (Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS))(Keefe et al., 2004).

1.2.2. Biotype Determination

The subjects in the biotype analyses were SCZ, SAD, and BP probands grouped by previously constructed biotypes in prior work from the B-SNIP consortium. Biotype classification was based upon cognitive and neurobiological differences in those with psychosis such that only probands were included in its creation. These biotypes were created with composite biomarker variables of cognitive control (BACS composite score, stop signal task, and antisaccade errors) and sensorimotor reactivity (EEG intrinsic activity, N100 ERP, paired S2 ERP, and P300 ERP) through k-means clustering of the gap statistic and the preclustering step of the TwoStep cluster analysis to statistically differentiate biotypes from one another (Clementz et al., 2016). Acoustic startle data were not utilized in the creation of biotypes due to a prior analysis that found no significant difference in PPI amongst diagnostic groups (Ivleva et al., 2014) and, additionally that too fewer subjects had startle data than data for the other measure used for the cluster analysis (Clementz et al., 2016). In the present cohort, 53 probands and 64 of the relatives were missing sufficient biomarker data to generate a biotype classification. Of the remaining subjects, there were 66 in Biotype 1, 140 in Biotype 2, and 147 in Biotype 3. Other analyses included members of each biotype as well as their relatives. There were 83 family members associated with Biotype 1 (Biotype 1-Fam), 170 family members associated with Biotype 2 (Biotype 2-Fam), and 195 family members associated with Biotype 3 (Biotype 3-Fam).

1.2.3. Acoustic Startle Acquisition

Electromyographic activity (EMG) was captured from the right orbicularis oculi for quantification of acoustic startle magnitude, latency and PPI, using methods described in Ivleva et al. (2014). A session began with a 3-minute acclimation period with 70 dB white noise. Startle pulse alone trials contained 116 dB white noise lasting 40ms. The first 3 startle responses were discarded. The prepulse+pulse trials contained a 20ms, 80 dB white noise prepulse followed by the same 116dB startling pulse. Following the first 3 trials, the paradigm included 18 pulse alone trials; 12 prepulse+pulse trials with a 120ms interstimulus interval (ISI) between prepulse and startling pulse for PPI. Intertrial intervals varied from 12s to 20s. Twelve prepulse+pulse trials with a 4500ms ISI were included to assess prepulse facilitation. Results of those trials will be reported elsewhere. These trials were evenly divided into 2 blocks and were randomized within each block. The acquired EMG signal was filtered with low and high-frequency cutoffs at 28 and 500 Hz, respectively, and then rectified and smoothed using MindWare software (MindWare Technologies, Inc., Gahanna, OH). Startle non-responders were excluded from both the latency and PPI analyses if they had a measurable startle response to less than 50% of the first pulse alone trials as per a prior report on this cohort (Ivleva et al., 2014). Startle magnitude was assessed as the peak magnitude of the EMG contraction, and the latency was assessed as the time of the peak magnitude following the acoustic stimulus (Massa et al., 2017). PPI was calculated as the percent inhibition of magnitude in prepulse+pulse trials compared to the magnitude of pulse alone trials in the 120ms trial types following the formula:

PPI=100(pulse alone magnitude)(120ms prepulse+pulse magnitude)(pulse alone magnitude)

1.2.4. Statistical Methods

Statistical differences in age, race, sex, handedness, medication status, or cognitive function of the cohort was determined by use of one-way Chi square (for categorical variables) or analysis of variance (ANOVA, for continuous variables) using either DSM diagnosis or biotype as the between subject variable. Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) ratings were assessed by use of a one-way ANOVA.

Startle magnitude was not normally distributed, and as such was log transformed to achieve acceptable skewness and kurtosis for use in parametric testing. PPI was calculated using the log-transformed values. To eliminate the effect of the block of the session, the average startle latency and PPI of the two blocks were used in this analysis. Furthermore, in all models assessing latency, the log transformed startle magnitude to pulse alone trials was included as a covariate.

Repeated measures analyses of variance with covariates (ANCOVAs) were used to compare startle outcome measures separately for DSM diagnoses and biotypes. For models that included probands and relatives of these probands, the error terms and corresponding significance values were adjusted by subtracting the number of relatives from the error term and calculating the corresponding significance using F tables to account for the non-independence of related subjects. Linear regression was used to quantify the association of acoustic startle latency, acoustic startle magnitude, and prepulse inhibition with cognitive variables. Chlorpromazine equivalents for antipsychotic medication were not significant in preliminary models and so were not included in the final models. The covariates included in each ANCOVA model for latency were age, race, sex, diagnosis or biotype, and log transformed startle magnitude to pulse alone trials since the latter significantly affected latency. Magnitude and PPI ANCOVAs included the covariates age, race, sex, diagnosis or biotype, and also site (due to some significant effects on magnitude and PPI). Linear regressions were used to examine the relation of cognition to startle variables in probands. The BACS variable utilized a calculated Z-score adjusted for age and sex so for regressions on BACS the only additional terms in these regressions were race and diagnosis (and log of startle magnitude for regressions examining latency). Terms in the regressions on the WRAT were similar except that age and sex were added since the WRAT was not an adjusted Z-score. All statistics were completed using either SPSS version 23 (Armonk, NY) or SAS 9.4 software (Cary, North Carolina).

1.3. Results

1.3.1. Diagnosis Results

Demographics and descriptive statistics of the cohort (N=1143) based upon diagnosis group (CON, BP, BP-Fam, SCZ, SCZ-Fam, SAD, and SAD-Fam) can be seen in Table 1. As expected, there were several significant differences across subject groups, especially regarding sex, race, age, medications, and PANSS domain and total scores (p<0.0001). Table 2 contains the descriptive statistics of the cognition and startle variables used in this publication. Throughout all the cognition variables there was a significant difference in scores across group, with SCZ performing significantly worse as reported previously in the B-SNIP sample (Hill et al., 2013). When assessing latency in this cohort all repeated measures ANCOVAs included age, race, sex, and log transformed startle magnitude to pulse alone trials. All repeated measures ANCOVAs for magnitude and PPI included age, race, sex, and site.

Table 1:

Demographic and clinical information by diagnostic group

Controls Bipolar Bipolar Familes Schizophrenia Schizophrenia
Families
Schizoaffective Schizoaffective
Families
Chi Sq/ F
Value
p Value
Demographics, n 226 138 183 143 178 123 152
Sex, n(%)b
 Male 106(47.11) 49(35.51) 69(37.70) 94(65.73) 54(30.34) 46(37.40) 46(30.26) 57.85 <.0001
 Female 119(52.89) 89(64.49) 114(62.30) 49(34.27) 124(69.66) 77(62.60) 105(69.08)
 Missing 1(0.44) - - - - - 1(0.66)
Race, n(%)c
 White 148(65.49) 108(78.26) 154(84.15) 74(51.75) 114(64.04) 68(55.28) 93(61.18) 57.09 <.0001
 Black 58(25.66) 20(14.49) 23(12.57) 53(37.06) 50(28.09) 49(39.84) 49(32.24) 50.69 <.0001
 Other 20(8.85) 10(7.25) 6(3.28) 16(11.19) 14(7.87) 6(4.88) 10(6.58)
Ethnicity, n(%)
 Hispanic 26(11.50) 15(10.87) 16(8.74) 18(12.59) 19(10.67) 17(13.82) 19(12.5) 2.46 0.873
Handedness, n(%)d
 Right 201(88.94) 110(79.71) 155(84.70) 119(83.22) 152(85.39) 107(86.99) 129(84.87) 16.54 0.168
 Left 17(7.52) 20(14.49) 24(13.11) 13(9.09) 16(8.99) 10(8.13) 15(9.87)
 Ambidextrous or Missing 8(3.54) 8(5.80) 4(2.18) 11(7.69) 10(5.62) 6(4.87) 8(5.26)
Medication, n(%)
 Typical Antipsychotic 0(0.0) 7(5.26) 0(0.00) 12(8.39) 2(1.12) 14(11.38) 1(0.66) 60.25 <.0001
 Atypical Antipsychotic 0(0.0) 93(69.92) 11(6.25) 103(72.03) 18(10.11) 92(74.80) 13(8.55) 566.44 <.0001
 Mood Stabilizer 0(0.0) 101(75.94) 22(12.50) 30(20.98) 9(5.06) 67(54.47) 14(9.21) 411.7 <.0001
 Missing 4 5 7 11(7.69) 9(5.06) 1(0.81) 8(5.26)
Age, Years, Mean(SD)a 37.87(12.77) 35.49(13.48) 40.17(15.89) 34.13(11.93) 43.47(15.87) 38.28(12.17) 39.43(16.28) 7.5 <.0001e,f,g,h
Chlorpromazine Equivalents, n - 82 6 79 12 84 11
 Mean (SD) - 342.62(359.05) 226.85(191.20) 606.98(502.95) 460.28(585.18) 503.55(418.30) 423.03(528.08) 3.42 0.0052j
No Antipsychotic Medication, n - 33 164 17 149 16 130
PANSS, n 128 17 128 17 122 19
PANSS, Mean(SD)
 Positive Symptoms - 13.33(4.25) 13.76(4.92) 17.69(5.6) 17.82(5.93) 18.88(4.86) 15.37(4.55) 19 <.0001i,j,k
 Negative Symptoms - 12.66(4.27) 11.71(4.44) 17.31(6.52) 13.94(4.56) 16.27(4.79) 12.11(3.18) 14.6 <.0001i,j,k
 General Symptoms - 30.20(7.54) 29.76(9.31) 34.55(8.17) 33.12(9.99) 36.96(8.05) 30.05(7.79) 10.65 <.0001i,j,k
 Total 56.19(13.29) 55.24(17.24) 69.55(17.38) 64.88(17.58) 72.11(14.91) 57.53(13.41) 18.1 <.0001i,j,k
a

Significant difference between Controls and Bipolar (p<0.05)

b

Significant difference between Controls and Bipolar Family (p<0.05)

c

Significant difference between Controls and Schizophrenia (p<0.05)

d

Significant difference between Controls and Schizoaffective (p<0.05)

e

Significant difference between Controls and Schizophrenia Family (p<0.05)

f

Significant difference between Controls and Schizoaffective Family (p<0.05)

g

Significant difference between Bipolar and Bipolar Family (p<0.05)

h

Significant difference between Schizophrenia and Schizophrenia Family (p<0.05)

i

Significant difference between Schizoaffective and Schizoaffective Family (p<0.05)

j

Significant difference between Bipolar and Schizophrenia (p<0.05)

k

Significant difference between Bipolar and Schizoaffective (p<0.05)

l

Significant difference between Schizophrenia and Schizoaffective (p<0.05)

Table 2.

Cognitive scores and startle variables by diagnostic group

Controls Bipolar Bipolar
Familes
Schizophrenia Schizophrenia
Families
Schizoaffective Schizoaffective
Families
F Value p Value
Cognitive Testing, n 222 135 172 136 166 118 145
WRATSB, Mean(SD) 101.81(14.09) 100.85(13.87) 103.36(15.91) 93.97(17.36) 96.27(16.56) 95.18(13.62) 97.72(16.24) 8.55 <.0001c,d,e,j
BACS, Z Scores, Mean(SD)
  Composite −0.03(1.15) −0.89(1.33) −0.05(1.16) −1.75(1.32) −0.47(1.21) −1.51(1.29) −0.51(1.35) 42.77 <.0001a,c,d,e,f,g,h,i,j,k
  Verbal Memory −0.03(1.14) −0.41(1.31) −0.12(1.11) −0.94(1.34) −0.3(1.33) −0.93(1.46) −0.46(1.27) 11.99 <.0001c,d,f,h,j,k
  Digit sequencing −0.14(1.16) −0.56(1.09) −0.02(0.98) −1.22(1.19) −0.3(1.13) −0.95(1.11) −0.43(1.33) 20.33 <.0001a,c,d,g,h,i
  Token Motor 0.01(1.08) −0.93(1.26) −0.30(1.09) −1.38(1.17) −0.47(1.07) −1.49(1.08) −0.36(0.99) 40.23 <.0001a,c,d,e,f,g,h,i,j,k
  Verbal Fluency 0.13(1.07) −0.25(1.20) 0.13(1.18) −0.78(1.18) −0.02(1.11) −0.5(1.14) 0.01(1.16) 12.72 <.0001c,d,g,h,i,j
  Symbol Coding −0.04(1.01) −0.88(1.09) −0.10(1.07) −1.41(1) −0.51(1.1) −1.36(1.1) −0.43(1.18) 39.55 <.0001a,c,d,e,f,g,h,i,j,k
  Tower of London −0.06(1.08) −0.29(1.26) 0.19(0.96) −0.88(1.54) −0.24(1.1) −0.55(1.19) −0.23(1.13) 12.32 <.0001c,d,j
Startle testing, n 225 138 182 143 178 123 152
Latency, ms, Mean(SD)
  Pulse Alone 132.39(19.25) 139.13(19.48) 137.70(17.12) 138.21(21.31) 138.71(19.47) 138.26(18.98) 136.28(18.28) 2.96 0.0071a,e
  120ms Prepulse+Pulse 135.98(18.15) 138.94(20.05) 136.24(16.90) 140.35(17.59) 137.46(18.55) 140.29(17.79) 137.84(19.94) 1.51 0.170
Magnitude, μV, Mean(SD)
  Pulse Alone 11.96(0.99) 12.47(1.03) 12.34(0.93) 12.39(1.01) 12.20(1.03) 12.06(0.97) 12.17(1.02) 5.57 <.0001a,b,c,k
  120ms Prepulse+Pulse 11.52(0.89) 11.94(0.86) 11.79(0.93) 11.89(0.88) 11.72(0.90) 11.60(0.86) 11.69(0.92) 4.86 <.0001a,b,c,k
Percent PPI, Mean(SD)
  120ms 22.57(43.66) 25.94(44.31) 29.42(41.65) 25.21(44.86) 26.91(37.01) 23.18(40.80) 23.74(42.87) 0.58 0.746
a

Significant difference between Controls and Bipolar (p<0.05)

b

Significant difference between Controls and Bipolar Family (p<0.05)

c

Significant difference between Controls and Schizophrenia (p<0.05)

d

Significant difference between Controls and Schizoaffective (p<0.05)

e

Significant difference between Controls and Schizophrenia Family (p<0.05)

f

Significant difference between Controls and Schizoaffective Family (p<0.05)

g

Significant difference between Bipolar and Bipolar Family (p<0.05)

h

Significant difference between Schizophrenia and Schizophrenia Family (p<0.05)

i

Significant difference between Schizoaffective and Schizoaffective Family (p<0.05)

j

Significant difference between Bipolar and Schizophrenia (p<0.05)

k

Significant difference between Bipolar and Schizoaffective (p<0.05)

l

Significant difference between Schizophrenia and Schizoaffective (p<0.05)

In a repeated measures ANCOVA comparing latency in CON, SCZ and SCZ-Fam subjects, SCZ subjects were slower than CON, with the SCZ-Fam group intermediate between SCZ and CON (F(2,371)=4.86, p=0.008, η2=0.018; Figure 1). Post hoc tests indicated that SCZ subjects were significantly slower than CON in pulse alone trials (p=0.004, Hedges g=−0.29; Figure 1a) and in 120ms prepulse trials (p=0.009, Hedges g=−0.24; Figure 1b). SCZ subjects also had slower latency than SCZ-Fam in the 120ms prepulse trials (p=0.04, Hedges g=0.16; Figure 1b). In a model comparing pulse alone startle magnitude, both SCZ (p<0.001, Hedges g=0.43) and SCZ-Fam (p=0.013, Hedges g=0.24) had higher pulse alone magnitudes compared to CON (F(2,372)=7.354, p=0.001, η2=0.027).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Difference in startle latency between controls and probands or families of probands by diagnostic group. * indicates difference was significant at p < 0.05 compared to controls. Brackets with * indicates significant differences between bracketed groups in post hoc tests (p < 0.05). (a) Pulse alone trials. (b) 120 ms prepulse+pulse trials. SCZ = schizophrenia probands; SCZ-Fam = family members of schizophrenia probands; SAD = schizoaffective probands; SAD-Fam = family members of schizoaffective probands; BP = bipolar probands; BP-Fam = family members of bipolar probands;

In a similar model comparing latency in CON, SAD, and SAD-Fam, SAD subjects were slower than CON with the SAD-Fam group intermediate between SAD and CON (F(2,342)=3.78, p=0.024, η2=0.015; Figure 1). Post hoc tests indicated that SAD subjects had significantly longer latency than CON in both pulse alone (p=0.008, Hedges g= −0.31; Figure 1a) and 120ms trials (p=0.03, Hedges g= −0.24; Figure 1b). In a one-way ANOVA comparing pulse alone startle magnitude between SAD, SAD-Fam, and CON there were no significant differences between subject groups (F(2,343)=1.82, p=0.16, η2=0.007).

A repeated measures ANCOVA comparing latency in BP, BP-Fam and CON found significant differences based upon group, with BP the slowest and BP-Fam intermediate (F(2,364)=4.05, p=0.018, η2=0.015; Figure 1). In post-hoc tests, BP and BP-Fam had significantly longer latency than CON in pulse alone trials (p=0.004, Hedges g= −0.35; p=0.05, Hedges g= −0.29; Figure 1a). In the 120ms trials, BP were also significantly longer than CON (p=0.039, Hedges g= −0.16; Figure 1b). In an ANOVA model comparing pulse alone startle magnitude, both BP (p=0.001, Hedges g=0.51) and BP-Fam (p=0.016, Hedges g=0.39) had higher pulse alone magnitudes compared to CON (F(2,365)=5.90, p=0.003, η2=0.021).

A model comparing latency in BP, SAD, SCZ, and CON was significant for diagnosis (F(3,620)=5.10, p=0.002, η2=0.024; Figure 1). In a series of post hoc tests all probands (BP, SAD, and SCZ) were slower than CON in pulse alone trials (p=0.003, Hedges g=−0.35; p=0.012, Hedges g=−0.31; p=0.003, Hedges g=−0.29). Similarly, all probands (BP, SAD, and SCZ) were significantly slower than CON at 120ms trials (p=0.043, Hedges g= −0.15; p=0.036, Hedges g= −0.24; p=0.004, Hedges g= −0.24). A model comparing pulse alone magnitude in BP, SAD, SCZ and CON was significant for diagnosis, with both BP and SCZ having significantly larger magnitudes compared to CON (F(3,620)=6.295, p<0.001, η2=0.03; p=0.002, Hedges g=0.51; p<0.001, Hedges g=0.43). The SCZ group had a significantly larger pulse alone magnitude when compared to SAD (p=0.016, Hedges g=−0.32).

We constructed ANCOVAs to assess PPI in 120ms trials in each diagnostic subset (SCZ, SCZ-fam or SAD, SAD-Fam or BP, and BP-Fam) compared to CON and in all the probands (SCZ, SAD, BP) compared to CON. PPI was not significant in any of these models (all p-values were >0.4).

1.3.2. Biotype Results

Demographics and descriptive statistics of the cohort (n=801) separated into biotypes and their respective family members can be seen in Table 3. There were many significant differences across subject groups, especially regarding race, atypical antipsychotic medication, and PANSS subscale and total scores (p<0.05). Table 4 contains the descriptive statistics of the cognitive and startle variables used throughout this publication for subjects divided by biotype. Given that the biotype determination factored in cognition, there was a significant difference in cognitive scores across group with Biotype 1 performing significantly worse (see Table 4).

Table 3:

Demographic and clinical information by Biotype

Biotype 1 Biotype 1
Families
Biotype 2 Biotype 2
Families
Biotype 3 Biotype 3
Families
Chi-Sq/
F Value*
p Value
Demographics, n 66 83 140 170 147 195
Sex, n(%)b
 Male 32(48.48) 30(36.14) 60(42.86) 54(31.76) 73(49.66) 69(35.38) 19.09 0.004
 Female 34(51.52) 53(63.86) 80(57.14) 116(68.24) 74(50.34) 126(64.62)
Race, n(%)
 White 30(45.45) 40(48.19) 95(67.86) 127(74.71) 98(66.67) 150(76.92) 39.68 <0.0001
 Black 28(42.42) 35(42.17) 35(25.00) 37(21.76) 37(25.17) 31(15.90) 31.95 <0.0001
 Other 8(12.12) 8(9.64) 10(7.14) 6(3.53) 12(8.16) 14(7.18)
Ethnicity, n(%)
 Hispanic 14(21.21) 15(18.07) 18(12.86) 20(11.76) 14(9.52) 14(7.18) 13.84 0.032
Handedness, n(%)
 Right 56(86.15) 66(79.52) 123(87.86) 147(86.47) 124(84.93) 175(89.74) 14.83 0.251
 Left 6(9.23) 14(16.87) 16(11.43) 17(10) 19(13.01) 16(8.21)
 Ambidextrous or Missing 4(4.62) 3(3.61) 1(0.71) 6(3.53) 4(2.05) 4(2.05)
Medication, n(%)
 Typical Antipsychotic 8(12.31) 1(1.20) 9(6.43) 2(1.18) 12(8.28) 0 51.68 <0.0001
 Atypical Antipsychotic 54(83.08) 14(16.87) 112(80.00) 15(8.82) 100(68.97) 10(5.13) 543.53 <0.0001
 Mood Stabilizer 33(50.77) 13(15.66) 70(50.00) 16(9.41) 77(53.10) 11(5.64) 288.2 <0.0001
 Missing 1 3(3.61) 0 2(1.18) 2 7(3.59)
Age, Years, Mean(SD) 36.65(13.34) 40.94(14.84) 35.04(11.89) 40.26(16.37) 35.16(12.79) 41.81(16.38) 5.61 <0.0001b,c
Chlorpromazine Equivalents, n 46 8 92 13 86 7
 Mean(SD) 519.50(404.83) 325.70(609.86) 557.57(528.88) 594.38(522.16) 403.32(361.45) 150.11(99.62) 2.23 0.052
No Antipsychotic Medication, n 3 65 19 151 33 178
PANSS, n 64 13 134 19 146 16
PANSS, Mean(SD)
 Positive Symptoms 17.13(5.68) 15.69(5.09) 17.06(5.63) 17.00(5.28) 15.34(5.12) 13.81(5.68) 2.52 0.029
 Negative Symptoms 17.28(6.95) 11.85(5.10) 15.46(4.97) 13.16(3.98) 14.39(5.65) 12.50(3.90) 4.89 0.0002a,e
 General Symptoms 33.95(8.98) 30.77(8.20) 34.77(8.48) 32.74(9.00) 32.11(7.94) 28.94(9.59) 2.58 0.026
 Total 68.36(18.88) 58.31(15.57) 66.30(16.39) 62.89(16.30) 61.84(16.06) 55.25(17.36) 3.61 0.003
*

All tests of association include the controls where appropriate

a

Significant association between Biotype 1 and Biotype 1 Families (p<0.05)

b

Significant association between Biotype 2 and Biotype 2 Families (p<0.05)

c

Significant association between Biotype 3 and Biotype 3 Families (p<0.05)

d

Significant association between Biotype 1 and Biotype 2 (p<0.05)

e

Significant association between Biotype 1 and Biotype 3 (p<0.05)

f

Significant association between Biotype 2 and Biotype 3 (p<0.05)

Table 4.

Cognitive scores and startle variables by Biotype

Biotype 1 Biotype 1
Families
Biotype 2 Biotype 2
Families
Biotype 3 Biotype 3
Families
F Value* p Value
Cognitive Testing, n 64 81 136 161 145 190
WRATSB, Mean(SD) 88.59(15.15) 91.33(16.34) 94.49(13.37) 98.51(17.01) 103.61(14.45) 103.33(15.17) 16.75 <0.0001e,f
BACS, Z Scores, Mean(SD)
 Composite −2.74(0.88) −1.20(1.18) −1.93(0.94) −0.47(1.25) −0.22(0.83) 0.15(1.08) 108.99 <0.0001a,b,c,d,e,f
 Verbal Memory −1.82(1.2) −0.90(1.34) −1.14(1.23) −0.29(1.27) 0.09(1.05) 0.00(1.10) 38.12 <0.0001a,b,d,e,f
 Digit sequencing −1.75(1.05) −0.81(1.09) −1.25(0.93) −0.31(1.23) −0.15(0.91) 0.10(1.05) 42.70 <0.0001a,b,d,e,f
 Token Motor −1.88(0.94) −0.88(1.06) −1.54(1.02) −0.52(1.07) −0.74(1.2) −0.04(0.99) 54.96 <0.0001a,b,c,e,f
 Verbal Fluency −1.45(1.01) −0.51(1.10) −0.88(0.93) −0.07(1.12) 0.27(1.07) 0.36(1.12) 40.59 <0.0001a,b,d,e,f
 Symbol Coding −2.1(0.9) −0.84(1.09) −1.5(0.89) −0.38(1.15) −0.51(0.91) −0.11(1.08) 60.83 <0.0001a,b,c,d,e,f
 Tower of London −1.53(1.38) −0.58(1.12) −0.95(1.26) −0.22(1.07) 0.22(0.92) 0.21(0.98) 35.22 <0.0001a,b,d,e,f
Startle testing, n 65 83 139 170 147 195
Latency, ms, Mean(SD)
 Pulse Alone 138.39(21.29) 140.16(17.55) 137.82(19.84) 137.73(18.87) 136.99(19.32) 136.41(17.65) 2.62 0.016
 120ms Prepulse+Pulse 141.29(19) 142.71(18.19) 138.92(19.22) 136.77(18.59) 139(18.89) 135.26(17.82) 2.61 0.016
Magnitude, μV, Mean(SD)
 Pulse Alone 12.39(1.03) 12.06(0.99) 12.34(1.06) 12.29(1.03) 12.43(0.96) 12.28(0.95) 4.76 <0.0001
 120ms Prepulse+Pulse 11.9(0.9) 11.70(0.92) 11.82(0.91) 11.74(0.93) 11.83(0.87) 11.79(0.90) 3.23 0.004
Percent PPI, Mean(SD)
 120ms 24.33(46.69) 15.49(51.11) 25.94(41.57) 30.22(36.78) 32.47(34.12) 26.02(41.12) 2.06 0.056
*

All F Values and p Values utilize One-way ANOVAs including the controls

a

Significant association between Biotype 1 and Biotype 1 Families (p<0.05)

b

Significant association between Biotype 2 and Biotype 2 Families (p<0.05)

c

Significant association between Biotype 3 and Biotype 3 Families (p<0.05)

d

Significant association between Biotype 1 and Biotype 2 (p<0.05)

e

Significant association between Biotype 1 and Biotype 3 (p<0.05)

f

Significant association between Biotype 2 and Biotype 3 (p<0.05)

ANCOVA models were used to assess differences in startle latency, magnitude, and PPI between biotype probands and controls or between family members and controls. In the ANCOVA that included only probands and controls, there was a strong trend level association between startle latency and biotype membership (F(3,567)=2.32, p=0.075, η2=0.012; Figure 2). Because we hypothesized individual differences in the biotypes, we examined post hocs that indicated that Biotypes 1 and 2 had significantly slower startle latency when compared to CON in pulse alone trials (p=0.03, Hedges g=−0.28; p=0.001, Hedges g=−0.24; Figure 2a), whereas Biotype 3 did not differ from CON. For the 120ms trials, none of the biotypes were significantly slower than CON (p>0.1 Figure 2b).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Difference in startle latency between controls and probands or families of probands by biotype group. * indicates difference was significant at p < 0.05 compared to controls. Brackets with * indicates significant differences between bracketed groups in post hoc tests (p < 0.05). (a) Pulse alone trials. (b) 120 ms prepulse+pulse trials. Biotype 1-Fam = family members of Biotype 1 probands; Biotype 2-Fam = family members of Biotype 2 probands; Biotype 3-Fam = family members of Biotype 3 probands.

In a model assessing startle magnitude in biotype probands versus controls, all biotypes had significantly larger pulse alone magnitude compared to CON (F(3,567)=4.327, p=0.005, η2=0.022). In similar ANOVAs assessing PPI, probands analyzed by biotype did not differ from CON in PPI.

We hypothesized that the underlying genetics of the three biotypes might differ and manifest in startle differences between family members and CON when a family member was classified according to the biotype of their proband relatives. An ANCOVA on biotype that compared latency in family members to CON was significant (F(3,663)=3.49, p=0.016, η2=0.016). In post hoc tests only Biotype 1-Fam was significantly slower than CON in the pulse alone trials (p=0.011, Hedges g=−0.41; Figure 2a). Additionally, Biotype 1-Fam was slower than Biotype 3-Fam (p=0.039, Hedges g=−0.21). In 120ms trials, Biotype 1-Fam had significantly slower latency than Biotype 2-Fam and Biotype 3-Fam (p=0.027, Hedges g=0.37; p=0.002, Hedges g=0.32; p=0.004, Hedges g=0.42; Figure 2b).

A model comparing startle magnitude between biotype family members and CON indicated significant differences in startle magnitude (F(3,663)=4.51, p=0.004, η2=0.02). In post hoc tests both Biotype 2-Fam (p=0.002, Hedges g=0.33) and Biotype 3-Fam (p=0.014, Hedges g=0.34) had startle magnitudes significantly larger than CON. Biotype 2-Fam also had a greater startle magnitude when compared to Biotype 1-Fam (p=0.015, Hedges g= −0.23).

In a similar ANOVA comparing PPI in relatives of biotype probands to CON, there was a significant difference in PPI (F(3,663)=2.77, p=0.041, η2=0.012; Figure 3). Post hoc tests found that PPI in Biotype 1-Fam was significantly reduced compared to both Biotype 2-Fam and Biotype 3-Fam (p=0.006, Hedges g=−0.35; p=0.044, Hedges g=−0.25; Figure 3).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Percent prepulse inhibition in control subjects and family members of biotype probands. Brackets with * indicate significant differences between bracketed groups in post hoc tests (p < 0.05). Biotype 1-Fam = family members of Biotype 1 probands; Biotype 2-Fam = family members of Biotype 2 probands; Biotype 3-Fam = family members of Biotype 3 probands.

1.3.3. Cognition and Startle Variables

We used multivariate linear regressions to analyze the association between cognition and acoustic startle variables. Models assessing the association between cognition and latency found that slower latency in pulse alone and 120ms trial types was associated with worse composite BACS scores (β=−0.18, p=0.038; β=−0.098, p=0.018). A subsequent series of models examined the association between startle magnitude and cognition. We found that pulse alone magnitude was positively associated with higher WRAT scores (β=0.095, p=0.027). In models examining the association between PPI and cognition, 120ms PPI was positively associated with a higher WRAT score (β=0.092, p=0.015). Other associations between PPI and cognition were not significant.

1.4. Discussion

The main objective of this study was to determine whether startle variables, chiefly latency and PPI, differed from CON in DSM psychotic disorders, and whether a transdiagnostic categorization of subjects based on biotype would reveal differences in startle measures between the biotypes.

This study provides further evidence that startle latency is slower (i.e. longer) in SCZ and SAD subjects when compared to CON subjects. This finding supports previous literature that reported a significant slowing of latency in SCZ compared to CON (Braff et al., 1978; Braff et al., 1999; Fargotstein et al., 2018; Geyer and Braff, 1982; Hasenkamp et al., 2010; Swerdlow et al., 2006). However, it should be acknowledged that several studies that did not detect a difference in startle latency based upon diagnostic group (Braff et al., 1992; Cadenhead et al., 2000; Geyer and Braff, 1982; Parwani et al., 2000). The current study also provides evidence that startle latency is slower in those with BP than CON, and that there is no significant difference in PPI between BP and CON. This is in accord with a prior study by Carroll et al. (Carroll et al., 2007) that reported a significantly longer startle latency in those with mixed-type bipolar disorder when compared to psychiatric controls. Similarly, the study conducted by Sánchez-Morla et al. (2016) reported increased startle latency in those with euthymic BP as compared to CON.

The current study did not find a significant difference in PPI attributed to diagnosis when comparing SCZ, SAD, or BP to CON, recapitulating a similar finding in a smaller set of B-SNIP subjects previously published (Ivleva et al., 2014). This could be attributed to antipsychotic medication status as highlighted and discussed in Fargotstein et al. (2018), given the high proportion of probands subjects with SCZ, SAD, and BP who were on second-generation antipsychotics in the B-SNIP cohort. It should be noted that preliminary regression models did not find a significant effect of medication status (antipsychotic treatment vs. no antipsychotic treatment), but the number of probands not on antipsychotic medication was quite small (BP: 24%, SAD: 13%, SCZ: 12%). Levels of PPI in 120 ms trials are typically higher in both SCZ and CON groups than what we report herein (see review in Hamm et al., 2001). In particular, the low levels of PPI in our CON group may also contribute to the lack of significant differences between our CON and patient groups in addition to the medicated status of our patient groups. Furthermore, baseline startle magnitude was lower in CON than in other subjects groups in this study, for reasons that are unclear. Typically, when pulse alone startle magnitude is low, there is greater PPI given that PPI is calculated as a percent decrease in magnitude to prepulse trials compared to pulse alone trials. Thus, the lower magnitude in CON subjects renders the low PPI in this group noteworthy.

The above points notwithstanding, our negative results for diagnostic group are consistent with the study by Carroll et al. (2007) that reported no significant difference in PPI between those with mixed-type BP compared to controls. However, our results contrast with the study conducted by Sánchez-Morla et al. (2016) that found impaired PPI in euthymic BP when compared to CON. While these studies (Carroll et al., 2007; Sanchez-Morla et al., 2016) both implicate slowing of startle latency in BP spectrum disorder, the association between BP and PPI is likely confounded by the different disease states of BP studied as well as the presence of psychosis. The overarching purpose of the work being conducted by the B-SNIP consortium is to utilize an intermediate phenotype, or endophenotype approach to the identification of both biomarkers and genes associated with psychosis that span across traditional DSM diagnostic categories. Through this endophenotype approach the B-SNIP consortium has defined three different biotypes that were based upon unique neurobiological characteristics (Clementz et al., 2016). In the current analyses, as shown in Figure 2, the Biotype 1 and Biotype 2 probands were slower in latency than CON whereas Biotype 3 was not. Furthermore, latency was significantly slower in relatives of Biotype 1 probands than in relatives of Biotypes 2 and 3, supporting the possibility of genetic differences between biotypes.

The PPI analysis by biotype found no significant difference between biotype probands and CON, nor amongst the three biotype probands. However, the analysis of relatives of biotype probands indicated that PPI was significantly impaired in Biotype 1 relatives compared to Biotypes 2 and 3 relatives.

In comparison to the other biotypes, Biotype 1 is associated with impaired cognitive function and sensorimotor reactivity, more severe negative symptoms, greater reduction in gray matter density, greater psychosocial impairment, having the highest proportion of affected relatives, higher polygenic risk scores, and lowest cannabis use (Clementz et al., 2016; Hochberger et al., 2018; Ivleva et al., 2017; Reilly et al., 2017; Tamminga et al., 2017). Biotype 3 in prior work was the least impaired in other measures (Clementz et al., 2016). In the current study Biotype 1 relatives have longer latency than Biotype 2 and 3 relatives. In consonance with our new data, decreased gray matter density associates with impaired PPI (Kumari et al., 2005), and SCZ risk genes have been found to associate with slowing of latency and impaired PPI (Greenwood et al., 2011; Hong et al., 2008; Quednow et al., 2009; Roussos et al., 2008a; Roussos et al., 2008b; Roussos et al., 2016; Smith et al., 2017).

A secondary objective of this study was to investigate the relationship between cognition and slowing of startle latency in the current cohort spanning DSM diagnoses. This objective is based on an underlying hypothesis that slowing of neural circuits subserving cognition (as indexed by slowing of startle latency) could in part underlie cognitive impairment. Indeed, we found that slower startle latency associated with worse cognition in our cohort of probands (BP, SCZ, and SAD) and CON. However, the latency versus BACS results herein are in contrast to a study by Hasenkamp et al. (2011) in a cohort of CON and SCZ that did not detect a significant association between decreased cognitive scores and slower acoustic startle latency or increased magnitude. While the results of the paper indicate a potential additional association between startle latency and the Conner’s Continuous Performance Test, it was not significant after Bonferroni correction. There was also a weak correlation between lower startle magnitude and an increase in preservative errors on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task, however this too did not survive correction for multiple tests. In light of the current results, further study of the interaction of latency and cognition may be warranted.

The findings herein regarding diagnostic and biotype differences in baseline startle magnitude, although not a main focus of this study, are worthy of note. In particular, baseline magnitude was notably lower in our CON group, for unclear reasons. Generally larger startle magnitudes in schizophrenia spectrum subjects has not been reported (Braff et al., 1978; Braff et al., 1992; Swerdlow et al., 2006) and has not been nearly as intensively studied as PPI and, more recently, latency. To put our magnitude findings in context, magnitude is highly heritable in human subjects (Anokhin et al., 2003; Hasenkamp et al., 2010). Decreased magnitude has been reported in bipolar subjects in one study (Carroll et al., 2007) although no difference in magnitude was detected in others (Perry et al., 2001; Barrett et al., 2005). Of note, startle magnitude is modulated by dopaminergic signaling (Davis, 1980), which in turn is highly relevant to the pathophysiology of schizophrenia (Davis et al., 1991).

This study has several limitations. The study consists largely of chronically ill and medicated proband with psychosis. As a result, there are many potential illness and medication confounds that need to be considered such as lifetime disease burden, past substance use disorders, and the chronic effects of medication (Tamminga et al., 2013). Furthermore, the cross-sectional design of the study limits the scope of the association between biotype and acoustic startle response as we are unable to discern the longitudinal effects of biotype membership on this physiological measure. Another limitation is that the startle paradigm used herein did not include prepulse+pulse trials with 30ms or 60ms between prepulse and pulse as has been commonly used in the field. These additional trial types could have conferred additional detail on the between group differences in latency and PPI.

In summary, startle latency is prolonged in psychotic disorders across traditional diagnostic categories. Biotype 1 and 2 probands had slower latency than CON, but Biotype 3 probands did not. These data suggest a genetic difference between biotypes that span across clinically defined psychotic diagnoses. PPI findings were less robust since PPI did not distinguish DSM diagnostic groups from CON nor did PPI differ between biotype probands. However, PPI in relatives of Biotype 1 probands had impaired PPI compared to Biotypes 2 and 3 relatives, suggesting a genetic difference in PPI relevant-genes amongst the biotypes. We hypothesize that acoustic startle latency is a putative index of neuronal processing speed, with the potential to serve as an endophenotype of SCZ and more broadly, psychosis. As a future direction we plan to investigate the genetics underlying the biotypes to determine how their genetics and underlying molecular signature confers risk of psychosis (Lencer et al., 2017). This approach may lead to the discovery of individualized treatments for psychotic patients based on their specific underlying genetics and neurobiology.

Acknowledgments

Funding body agreements and policies

Supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (MH-077851, MH-078113, MH-077945, MH-077852, MH-077862), a NARSAD/Brain & Behavior Research Foundation John Kennedy Harrison Young Investigator Award (17801), and a Veterans Affairs Merit Review grant (I01CX000974). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not represent the official views of the National Institute of Mental Health or the Department of Veterans Affairs.

Footnotes

Disclosures and conflict of interest

Infrastructure support was provided by the Research and Development, the Mental Health Service Lines, and the Center of Visual and Neurocognitive Rehabilitation at the Atlanta Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Decatur, GA. Additional infrastructure support was provided by the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences of the Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA. ED has received research support for work unrelated to this project from Auspex Pharmaceuticals, Inc. and Teva Pharmaceuticals, Inc. Other authors have nothing to disclose. ED is a full time attending psychiatrist in the Mental Health Service Line at the Atlanta Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Decatur, GA. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the Department of Veterans Affairs.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Anokhin AP, Heath AC, Myers E, Ralano A, Wood S, 2003. Genetic influences on prepulse inhibition of startle reflex in humans. Neuroscience Letters 353(1), 45–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Barrett SL, Kelly C, Watson DR, Bell R, King DJ, 2005. Normal levels of prepulse inhibition in the euthymic phase of bipolar disorder. Psychol Med 35, 1737–1746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Braff D, Stone C, Callaway E, Geyer M, Glick I, Bali L, 1978. Prestimulus effects on human startle reflex in normals and schizophrenics. Psychophysiology 14, 339–343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Braff D, Swerdlow N, Geyer M, 1999. Symptom correlates of prepulse inhibition deficits in male schizophrenic patients. Am J Psychiatry 156(4), 596–602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Braff DL, Geyer MA, Swerdlow NR, 2001. Human studies of prepulse inhibition of startle: normal subjects, patient groups, and pharmacological studies. Psychopharmacology 156, 234–258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Braff DL, Grillon C, Geyer MA, 1992. Gating and habituation of the startle reflex in schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 49(3), 206–215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cadenhead K, Addington J, Bearden C, Cannon T, Cornblatt BA, Mathalon DH, McGlashan T, Perkins D, Seidman LJ, Tsuang M, Walker E, Woods S, 2013. Startle latency and magnitude predict clinical outcome in the psychosis prodrome: Findings from the North American Prodromal Longitudinal Study (NAPLS). Neuropsychopharmacol 38(S2), S448–S449. [Google Scholar]
  8. Cadenhead KS, Swerdlow NR, Shafer KM, Diaz M, Braff DL, 2000. Modulation of the startle response and startle laterality in relatives of schizophrenic patients and in subjects with schizotypal personality disorder: evidence of inhibitory deficits. Am J Psychiatry. 157(10), 1660–1668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Carroll CA, Vohs JL, O'Donnell BF, Shekhar A, Hetrick WP, 2007. Sensorimotor gating in manic and mixed episode bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord 9(3), 221–229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Clementz BA, Sweeney JA, Hamm JP, Ivleva EI, Ethridge LE, Pearlson GD, Keshavan MS, Tamminga CA, 2016. Identification of distinct psychosis biotypes using brain-based biomarkers. Am J Psychiatry 173(4), 373–384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cloutier M, Aigbogun MS, Guerin A, Nitulescu R, Ramanakumar AV, Kamat SA, DeLucia M, Duffy R, Legacy SN, Henderson C, Francois C, Wu E, 2016. The economic burden of schizophrenia in the United States in 2013. J Clin Psychiatry 77(6), 764–771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cloutier M, Greene M, Guerin A, Touya M, Wu E, 2018. The economic burden of bipolar I disorder in the United States in 2015. J Affect Disord 226, 45–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Davis KL, Kahn RS, Ko G, Davidson M, 1991. Dopamine in schizophrenia: a review and reconceptualization. Am J Psychiatry 148, 1474–1486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Davis M, 1980. Neurochemical modulation of sensory-motor reactivity: acoustic and tactile startle reflexes. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 4, 241–263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fargotstein M, Hasenkamp W, Gross R, Cuthbert B, Green A, Swails L, Lewison B, Boshoven W, Keyes M, Duncan E, 2018. The effect of antipsychotic medications on acoustic startle latency in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 198, 28–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. First M, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW, 2001. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders. Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute. [Google Scholar]
  17. Geyer MA, Braff DL, 1982. Habituation of the blink reflex in normals and schizophrenic patients. Psychophysiology 19(1), 1–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Graham F, 1975. The more or less startling effects of weak prestimulus. Psychophysiology 12, 238–248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Greenwood TA, Braff DL, Light GA, Cadenhead KS, Calkins ME, Dobie DJ, Freedman R, Green MF, Gur RE, Gur RC, Mintz J, Nuechterlein KH, Olincy A, Radant AD, Seidman LJ, Siever LJ, Silverman JM, Stone WS, Swerdlow NR, Tsuang DW, Tsuang MT, Turetsky BI, Schork NJ, 2007. Initial heritability analyses of endophenotypic measures for schizophrenia: The Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 64(11), 1242–1250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Greenwood TA, Lazzeroni LC, Calkins ME, Freedman R, Green MF, Gur RE, Gur RC, Light GA, Nuechterlein KH, Olincy A, Radant AD, Seidman LJ, Siever LJ, Silverman JM, Stone WS, Sugar CA, Swerdlow NR, Tsuang DW, Tsuang MT, Turetsky BI, Braff DL, 2016. Genetic assessment of additional endophenotypes from the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia Family Study. Schizophr Res 170(1), 30–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Greenwood TA, Lazzeroni LC, Murray SS, Cadenhead KS, Calkins ME, Dobie DJ, Green MF, Gur RE, Gur RC, Hardiman G, Kelsoe JR, Leonard S, Light GA, Nuechterlein KH, Olincy A, Radant AD, Schork NJ, Seidman LJ, Siever LJ, Silverman JM, Stone WS, Swerdlow NR, Tsuang DW, Tsuang MT, Turetsky BI, Freedman R, Braff DL, 2011. Analysis of 94 candidate genes and 12 endophenotypes for schizophrenia from the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 168(9), 930–946. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hamm AO, Weike AI, Schupp HT, 2001. The effect of neuroleptic medication on prepulse inhibition in schizophrenia patients: current status and future issues. Psychopharmacol 156, 259–265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hasenkamp W, Epstein MP, Green A, Wilcox L, Boshoven W, Lewison B, Duncan E, 2010. Heritability of acoustic startle magnitude, prepulse inhibition, and startle latency in schizophrenia and control families. Psychiatry Res 178(2), 236–243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hasenkamp W, Kelley M, Egan G, Green A, Wilcox L, Boshoven W, Lewison B, Duncan E, 2011. Lack of relationship between acoustic startle and cognitive variables in schizophrenia and control subjects. Psychiatry Res 187(3), 324–328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hill SK, Reilly JL, Keefe RS, Gold JM, Bishop JR, Gershon ES, Tamminga CA, Pearlson GD, Keshavan MS, Sweeney JA, 2013. Neuropsychological impairments in schizophrenia and psychotic bipolar disorder: findings from the Bipolar-Schizophrenia Network on Intermediate Phenotypes (B-SNIP) study. Am J Psychiatry 170(11), 1275–1284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hochberger WC, Combs T, Reilly JL, Bishop JR, Keefe RSE, Clementz BA, Keshavan MS, Pearlson GD, Tamminga CA, Hill SK, Sweeney JA, 2018. Deviation from expected cognitive ability across psychotic disorders. Schizophr Res 192, 300–307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hoffman HS, Searle JL, 1968. Acoustic and temporal factors in the evocation of startle. J Acoust Soc Am 43(2), 269–282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hong LE, Wonodi I, Stine OC, Mitchell BD, Thaker GK, 2008. Evidence of missense mutations on the neuregulin 1 gene affecting function of prepulse inhibition. Biol Psychiatry 63(1), 17–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Ivleva EI, Clementz BA, Dutcher AM, Arnold SJM, Jeon-Slaughter H, Aslan S, Witte B, Poudyal G, Lu H, Meda SA, Pearlson GD, Sweeney JA, Keshavan MS, Tamminga CA, 2017. Brain structure biomarkers in the psychosis biotypes: Findings from the Bipolar-Schizophrenia Network for Intermediate Phenotypes. Biol Psychiatry 82(1), 26–39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Ivleva EI, Moates AF, Hamm JP, Bernstein IH, O'Neill HB, Cole D, Clementz BA, Thaker GK, Tamminga CA, 2014. Smooth pursuit eye movement, prepulse inhibition, and auditory paired stimuli processing endophenotypes across the schizophrenia-bipolar disorder psychosis dimension. Schizophr Bull 40(3), 642–652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Keefe RS, Goldberg TE, Harvey PD, Gold JM, Poe MP, Coughenour L, 2004. The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery. Schizophr Res 68(2–3), 283–297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Koch M, 1999. The neurobiology of startle. Prog Neurobiol 59, 107–128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kumari V, Antonova E, Zachariah E, Galea A, Aasen I, Ettinger U, Mitterschiffthaler MT, Sharma T, 2005. Structural brain correlates of prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response in healthy humans. Neuroimage 26(4), 1052–1058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Lencer R, Mills LJ, Alliey-Rodriguez N, Shafee R, Lee AM, Reilly JL, Sprenger A, McDowell JE, McCarroll SA, Keshavan MS, Pearlson GD, Tamminga CA, Clementz BA, Gershon ES, Sweeney JA, Bishop JR, 2017. Genome-wide association studies of smooth pursuit and antisaccade eye movements in psychotic disorders: findings from the B-SNIP study. Transl Psychiatry 7(10), e1249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Ludewig K, Geyer MA, Etzensberger M, Vollenweider FX, 2002. Stability of the acoustic startle reflex, prepulse inhibition, and habituation in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 55(1-2), 129–137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Mackeprang T, Kristiansen KT, Glenthoj BY, 2002. Effects of antipsychotics on prepulse inhibition of the startle response in drug-naive schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychiatry 52(9), 863–873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Massa NM, Duncan E, Jovanovic T, Kerley K, Weng L, Gensler L, Lee SS, Norrholm S, Powers A, Almli LM, Gillespie CF, Ressler K, Pearce BD, 2017. Relationship between Toxoplasma gondii seropositivity and acoustic startle response in an inner-city population. Brain Behav Immun 61, 176–183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. McGrath J, Saha S, Chant D, Welham J, 2008. Schizophrenia: a concise overview of incidence, prevalence, and mortality. Epidemiol Rev 30, 67–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Merikangas KR, Jin R, He JP, Kessler RC, Lee S, Sampson NA, Viana MC, Andrade LH, Hu C, Karam EG, Ladea M, Medina-Mora ME, Ono Y, Posada-Villa J, Sagar R, Wells JE, Zarkov Z, 2011. Prevalence and correlates of bipolar spectrum disorder in the world mental health survey initiative. Arch Gen Psychiatry 68(3), 241–251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Narayanan B, Ethridge LE, O'Neil K, Dunn S, Mathew I, Tandon N, Calhoun VD, Ruano G, Kocherla M, Windemuth A, Clementz BA, Tamminga CA, Sweeney JA, Keshavan MS, Pearlson GD, 2015. Genetic Sources of Subcomponents of Event-Related Potential in the Dimension of Psychosis Analyzed From the B-SNIP Study. Am J Psychiatry 172(5), 466–478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Parwani A, Duncan E, Bartlett E, Madonick S, Efferen T, Rajan R, Sanfilipo M, Chappell P, Chakravorty S, Gonzenbach S, Ko G, Rotrosen J, 2000. Impaired prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in schizophrenics. Biol Psychiatry 47(7), 662–669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Pearlson GD, Clementz BA, Sweeney JA, Keshavan MS, Tamminga CA, 2016. Does Biology Transcend the Symptom-based Boundaries of Psychosis? Psychiatr Clin North Am 39(2), 165–174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Perry W, Minassian A, Feifel D, Braff DL, 2001. Sensorimotor gating deficits in bipolar disorder patients with acute psychotic mania. Biol Psychiatry 50, 418–424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Pfohl B, Blum N, Zimmerman M, 1997. Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality (SIDP). Department of Psychiatry, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA. [Google Scholar]
  45. Quednow BB, Schmechtig A, Ettinger U, Petrovsky N, Collier DA, Vollenweider FX, Wagner M, Kumari V, 2009. Sensorimotor gating depends on polymorphisms of the serotonin-2A receptor and catechol-O-methyltransferase, but not on neuregulin-1 Arg38Gln genotype: a replication study. Biol Psychiatry 66(6), 614–620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Reilly JL, Hill SK, Gold JM, Keefe RS, Clementz BA, Gershon E, Keshavan MS, Pearlson G, Tamminga CA, Sweeney JA, 2017. Impaired context processing is attributable to global neuropsychological impairment in schizophrenia and psychotic bipolar disorder. Schizophr Bull 43(2), 397–406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Roussos P, Giakoumaki SG, Bitsios P, 2008a. The dopamine D(3) receptor Ser9Gly polymorphism modulates prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex. Biol Psychiatry 64(3), 235–240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Roussos P, Giakoumaki SG, Rogdaki M, Pavlakis S, Frangou S, Bitsios P, 2008b. Prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex depends on the catechol O-methyltransferase Val158Met gene polymorphism. Psychological Medicine 38(11), 1651–1658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Roussos P, Giakoumaki SG, Zouraraki C, Fullard JF, Karagiorga VE, Tsapakis EM, Petraki Z, Siever LJ, Lencz T, Malhotra A, Spanaki C, Bitsios P, 2016. The relationship of common risk variants and polygenic risk for schizophrenia to sensorimotor gating. Biol Psychiatry 79(12), 988–996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Sanchez-Morla EM, Mateo J, Aparicio A, Garcia-Jimenez MA, Jimenez E, Santos JL, 2016. Prepulse inhibition in euthymic bipolar disorder patients in comparison with control subjects. Acta Psychiatr Scand 134(4), 350–359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Smith AK, Jovanovic T, Kilaru V, Lori A, Gensler L, Lee SS, Norrholm SD, Massa N, Cuthbert B, Bradley B, Ressler KJ, E. D, 2017. A gene-based analysis of acoustic startle latency. Frontiers Psychiatry 8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Swerdlow NR, Light GA, Cadenhead KS, Sprock J, Hsieh MH, Braff DL, 2006. Startle gating deficits in a large cohort of patients with schizophrenia: relationship to medications, symptoms, neurocognition, and level of function. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63(12), 1325–1335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Swerdlow NR, Light GA, Thomas ML, Sprock J, Calkins ME, Green MF, Greenwood TA, Gur RE, Gur RC, Lazzeroni LC, Nuechterlein KH, Radant AD, Seidman LJ, Siever LJ, Silverman JM, Stone WS, Sugar CA, Tsuang DW, Tsuang MT, Turetsky BI, Braff DL, 2018. Deficient prepulse inhibition in schizophrenia in a multi-site cohort: Internal replication and extension. Schizophr Res 198, 6–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Swerdlow NR, Weber M, Qu Y, Light GA, Braff DL, 2008. Realistic expectations of prepulse inhibition in translational models for schizophrenia research. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 199(3), 331–388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Tamminga CA, Ivleva EI, Keshavan MS, Pearlson GD, Clementz BA, Witte B, Morris DW, Bishop J, Thaker GK, Sweeney JA, 2013. Clinical phenotypes of psychosis in the Bipolar-Schizophrenia Network on Intermediate Phenotypes (B-SNIP). Am J Psychiatry 170(11), 1263–1274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Tamminga CA, Pearlson G, Keshavan M, Sweeney J, Clementz B, Thaker G, 2014. Bipolar and schizophrenia network for intermediate phenotypes: outcomes across the psychosis continuum. Schizophr Bull 40 Suppl 2, S131–137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Tamminga CA, Pearlson GD, Stan AD, Gibbons RD, Padmanabhan J, Keshavan M, Clementz BA, 2017. Strategies for advancing disease definition using biomarkers and genetics: The Bipolar and Schizophrenia Network for Intermediate Phenotypes. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 2(1), 20–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Turetsky BI, Calkins ME, Light GA, Olincy A, Radant AD, Swerdlow NR, 2007. Neurophysiological endophenotypes of schizophrenia: the viability of selected candidate measures. Schizophr Bull 33(1), 69–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Weike AI, Bauer U, Hamm AO, 2000. Effective neuroleptic medication removes prepulse inhibition deficits in schizophrenia patients. Biol Psychiatry 47(1), 61–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Wilkinson GS, Robertson GJ, 2006. WRAT 4: Wide range achievement test. Psychological Assessment Resources, Lutz, Fl. [Google Scholar]
  61. Wynn JK, Dawson ME, Schell AM, McGee M, Salveson D, Green MF, 2004. Prepulse facilitation and prepulse inhibition in schizophrenia patients and their unaffected siblings. Biol Psychiatry 55(5), 518–523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES