Abstract
The ability to switch between glycolysis and ketosis promotes survival by enabling metabolism through fat oxidation during periods of fasting. Carbohydrate restriction or stress can also elicit metabolic switching. Keto-adapting from glycolysis is delayed in aged rats, but factors mediating this age-related impairment have not been identified. We measured metabolic switching between glycolysis and ketosis, as well as glycogen dynamics, in young and aged rats undergoing time-restricted feeding (TRF) with a standard diet or a low carbohydrate ketogenic diet (KD). TRF alone reversed markers of insulin-related metabolic deficits and accelerated metabolic switching in aged animals. A KD+TRF, however, provided additive benefits on these variables. Remarkably, the ability to keto-adapt was not related to glycogen levels and KD-fed rats showed an enhanced elevation in glucose following epinephrine administration. This study provides new insights into the mechanisms of keto-adaptation demonstrating the utility of dietary interventions to treat metabolic impairments across the lifespan.
Keywords: glucose, diet, ketogenic diet, keto-adaptation, intermittent fasting
INTRODUCTION
Patterns of energy consumption are a powerful regulator of mortality and longevity. In addition to caloric restriction, which has been extensively shown to extend maximum lifespan across species [1–7], time restricted feeding (TRF; also referred to as meal feeding or intermittent fasting) may also improve morbidity and mortality rates [8]. Intermittent periods of eating inevitably lead to episodes of ketogenesis, in which fat is oxidized into ketone bodies, reducing the need for glycolysis [8–10].
Without fasting, ketosis can also be achieved with high fat, low carbohydrate ketogenic diets (KD). Importantly, long-term ketosis has also been shown to prevent cognitive and physical declines associated with aging [11–18]. It remains to be determined, however, whether TRF alone can produce sufficient levels of ketosis to elicit similar health benefits and the extent to which TRF with a KD may have additive benefits on the healthspan of older adults and other animals.
The process of switching from glycolysis to a reliance on fat-derived ketone bodies as a primary energy source is termed keto-adaptation [19, 20]. While nutritional ketosis has been proposed as an intervention for a number of age-related metabolic and neurological diseases [21–23], aged animals take longer than young to produce high levels of circulating ketone bodies [14, 15]. Understanding the factors responsible for delayed keto-adaptation in old animals, and how to overcome this, are critical as brain utilization of ketone bodies directly correlates with circulating levels in the periphery [24–26]. Thus, in order to design precision ketogenic diet therapy [27] that can improve cognitive functioning and physical health in older adults, it is imperative to understand ketogenesis across the lifespan and the metabolic variables that can delay or promote metabolic switching.
The prevailing theory regarding the initiation of ketogenesis is that ketone body production is triggered once the body depletes its glycogen stores. This glycogen depletion hypothesis likely stems from the body’s natural metabolic response to vigorous exercise or longer fasts [28, 29]. Following 1 hour of exercise, there is an inverse relationship between glycogen content and ketone body levels [30]. Given the presumed relationship between keto-adaptation and glycogen levels, it is conceivable that aged rats take longer to achieve stable nutritional ketosis due to higher levels of glycogen or altered glycogen dynamics. There are differences in body mass with age [15], as well as evidence that aged rats have reduced glycogenolytic capacity [31]. Specifically, the hepatic release of glycogen [32], which increases blood glucose in response to epinephrine [33], is decreased in aged compared to young rats [34]. Thus, old animals may be deficient at converting glycogen to glucose on demand, which would decrease the rate of glycogen depletion. An alternative to the glycogen depletion hypothesis is that old animals have impaired metabolic switching, herein defined as the ability to utilize either carbohydrates or fat as a fuel source when appropriate. In line with this idea, insulin insensitivity occurs in advanced age [35–37], and fasting insulin levels are often elevated. Since insulin inhibits fat oxidation, higher insulin levels could also delay keto-adaptation. The current study tested the extent to which altered keto-adaptation in old age was related to impaired glycogen dynamics or metabolic dysfunction in rats that were placed on TRF (fed once per day) with a standard diet (SD) or TRF in combination with an isocaloric ketogenic diet (KD). For some comparisons, a third group of rats fed ad libitum (also referred to as free feeding) standard rodent chow was included.
RESULTS
Age-related delays in keto-adaptation are not due to altered feeding patterns
Keto-adaptation is the coordinated set of metabolic adaptations that ensures proper interorgan fuel supply in the face of low carbohydrate availability [38]. This process is enabled by metabolic switching from glycolysis to ketosis in which glucose oxidation is replaced by lipid oxidation and the synthesis of ketone bodies (primarily beta-hydroxybutyrate and acetoacetate) in the liver. A proxy for keto-adaptation is elevated, asymptotic levels of blood beta-hydroxybutyrate that exceed 2.5 mmol/L in response to consumption of a high-fat, low carbohydrate ketogenic diet (KD).
A potential explanation for delayed keto-adaptation, as previously observed in aged compared to young rats [14, 15], could be altered food consumption patterns. To rule out differences in food consumption, and to evaluate the amount of time that rats were fasted during TRF, caloric intake was measured for three hours postprandially and at 24 hours. Young (4 mo) and aged (20 mo) male Fischer 344 x Brown Norway F1 Hybrid rats fed 51 kcal of the SD or KD once per day, as well as young and aged rats free-fed regular chow ad libitum, were evaluated for this comparison. The ab libitum fed groups allowed for a comparison of food consumption patterns during TRF, as used here, and ad libitum feeding that is common in aging studies.
ANOVA-RM with the between subjects factors of age and diet group indicated a significant effect of time point (F[8,224] = 329.96; p < 0.001; Figure 1C) on the amount of food consumed for all groups, as well as a significant effect of diet group (F[2,28] = 35.86; p < 0.001), but no effect of age (F[1,28] = 0.58; p = 0.45). Critically, the consumption patterns of young and aged rats on the SD or KD did not vary, indicating that altered caloric intake over time cannot account for the age-related delay in keto-adaptation. Diet group, however, significantly interacted with time point (F[16,224] = 67.59; p < 0.001). Specifically, the ad libitum-fed rats did not consume the same density of calories in the 3-hour postprandial period, and consumed significantly more calories 24-hr after feeding (F[3,30] = 113.326; p < 0.001). Furthermore, after 24 hours, the aged ad libitum-fed rats consumed significantly more kcal than the young rats of the same feeding group.
Because the caloric intake in the SD and KD groups was significantly less in a 24-hr period than animals that were fed ad libitum, we compared the body weights of these animals over the course of 6 months (Figure 1B). Prior to diet implementation, there were no differences in weight across eventual diet groups (F[2,32] = 0.33; p = 0.72), though aged rats weighed significantly more than young (F[1,32] = 588.96; p < 0.001). ANOVA-RM indicated a significant main effect of week (F[23,644] = 12.89; p < 0.001), age (F[1,28] = 250.67; p < 0.001) and diet (F[2,28] = 27.16; p < 0.001) across 24 weeks. Furthermore, both age (F[23,644] = 17.72; p < 0.001) and diet (F[23,644] = 6.38; p < 0.001) significantly interacted with week number. However, there was no significant interaction between age and diet (F[2,28] = 1.45; p = 0.25). This pattern in the data is due, at least in part, to the ad libitum-fed animals gaining significantly more weight than TRF rats. In fact, ad libitum-fed rats had significant weight gain across the 6 months for both young (F[1,18] = 29.35; p < 0.001) and aged rats (F[1,18] = 8.71; p < 0.02). It should be noted, however, that even on TRF of 51 kcal/day, both SD (t[6] = 8.60; p < 0.001) and KD (t[6] = 4.49; p < 0.01) fed young mature adult rats weighed significantly more at the end of the 6 months. These data demonstrate that pathological overconsumption was prevented in the current animals without caloric restriction.
After establishing that food consumption patterns did not vary between age groups, postprandial blood glucose (Figure 2A) and ketone body levels (BHB; Figure 2B) during the first day of initiation of a medium chain triglyceride (MCT) oil-based KD or a calorically-matched and micronutrient equivalent standard diet (SD). This procedure was done to examine if age-related delays in keto-adaptation could be replicated in these animals. Blood measurements were conducted every 3 days for 28 days in rats that underwent TRF with access to 51 kcal given once daily. In both young and aged rats, the KD decreased blood glucose levels and increased serum BHB. Note, levels of BHB in the KD-fed rats that significantly differed from the SD-fed rats were achieved before those levels stabilized across weeks. Thus, keto-adaptation was defined as the point at which BHB in the blood reached stable levels that exceeded 2.5 mmol/L.
The reduction in glucose and concurrent increase in BHB can be combined to quantify the overall level of ketosis using the glucose ketone index (GKI; Figure 2C). Importantly, the GKI tracks better with therapeutic efficacy of KDs in cancer than either glucose or BHB levels alone [39]. Briefly, the GKI is the ratio of the molecular weight normalized serum glucose level (expressed as mol/g x dL), to the serum BHB levels (mM). Thus, lower GKI values are indicative of higher levels of ketosis. Therefore, the time required for GKI to be asymptotically lowered can be used to extrapolate the time it takes an animal to keto-adapt.
During 1 month of TRF with either the isocaloric KD or SD, the GKI was significantly lower in KD-fed animals (F[1,14] = 1459.55; p < 0.001). Moreover, the GKI was significantly lower in young compared to aged rats (F[1,14] = 20.23; p = 0.001), indicating lower levels of ketosis in older animals. When examined longitudinally, the GKI significantly changed across the 28 days (F[9,126] = 4.21; p < 0.001). The interaction between days on the diet and diet group was also significant (F[9,126] = 6.12; p < 0.001), such that the GKI in KD-fed rats significantly decreased while the GKI of SD-fed rats did not. Importantly, GKI levels across days on the diet significantly interacted with age (F[9,126] = 2.78; p < 0.01), with young rats having significantly lower GKI values compared to aged rats on days 4 (t[7] = 7.11; p = 0.001) and 10 (t[7] = 3.14; p = 0.02). No difference across age groups was detected from day 13 onward (p > 0.1). These data replicate a previous observation that aged rats require longer than young to keto-adapt, but can maintain stable levels of ketogenesis thereafter [15].
Age-related metabolic impairments delay keto-adaptation rate
To assess the contribution of age-related metabolic differences on keto-adaptation, young and aged rats fed ad libitum underwent an insulin tolerance test (ITT). Insulin powerfully inhibits ketogenesis [40], therefore insulin receptor insensitivity and hyperinsulinemia could delay keto-adaptation. Notably, all animals were fed ad libitum prior to initiating the KD or SD, so these rats represent the basal metabolic state prior to experimental diets being implemented. ANOVA-RM with the between subjects factor of age indicated a significant effect of time point (F[6,54] = 83.33; p < 0.001), but glucose levels at baseline were not significantly different between age groups (t[9] = 1.70; p = 0.12; Figure 3A). The area under the curve for glucose (Figure 3B, 3C) was significantly greater for aged rats relative to young (t[9] = 3.55; p = 0.006), demonstrating an impaired glucose response to the ITT. BHB levels also did not significantly differ at baseline across age groups (t[9] = 1.48; p = 0.17; Figure 3D), but BHB declined in response to the insulin injection (Figure 3E). However, the area under the curve did not differ across age groups (t[9] = 0.38; p = 0.71; Figure 3E, 3F).
To further characterize metabolic impairments between young and aged ad libitum-fed rats, levels of several markers of peripheral metabolism were obtained (Figure 4). When fasted, aged rats had significantly higher insulin (t[29] = 3.14; p = 0.004), leptin (t[49] = 3.54; p < 0.001) and C-peptide (t[48] = 3.41; p < 0.01). In a subset of these rats, fasted glucose was also obtained and a homeostatic model assessment (HOMA) value could be calculated as (insulin (mU/I) x glucose (mg/dL)) / 405. Aged rats had a significant elevation in HOMA relative to young rats (t[20] = 3.36; p = 0.003; Figure 4D). Together these data show that aged rats fed ad libitum have metabolic impairments and it is notable that elevated HOMA values are a risk factor for diabetes type II [41] as well as cardiovascular disease [42]. Because insulin inhibits ketosis, the hyperinsulinemia in aged rats could be responsible for delayed keto-adaptation in aging.
Because aged rats that were allowed to eat ad libitum had significant impairments in insulin regulation, the potential of both TRF with the SD and TRF with the KD to reverse insulin impairments was investigated during a 4-week period of keto-adaptation. Fasted insulin, leptin, and C-peptide levels, as well as glucose levels for the generation of HOMA values, were measured in young and aged rats undergoing TRF with either the SD or KD for 1, 4, 7 or 28 days.
Fasted insulin levels differed significantly across diet groups (F[1,64] = 17.74; p < 0.001), but not age groups (F[1,64] = 1.64; p = 0.21). However, age and diet did significantly interact (F[1,64] = 7.75; p = < 0.01). While there was no overall main effect of length of time on the diet (F[3,64] = 0.40; p = 0.76), length of time on the diet significantly interacted with age (F[3,64] = 4.69; p < 0.01) but not diet group (F[3,64] = 0.45; p = 0.72). In animals on the KD, fasted insulin levels were significantly greater in aged compared to young animals (F[1,32] = 13.35; p = 0.001; Figure 5A). Interestingly, this age difference was not evident on day 28 of the KD (t[8] = 0.28; p = 0.79). There was no difference across age (F[1,32] = 0.82; p = 0.37) in SD-fed rats over 28 days of TRF, however aged rats had significantly lower fasted insulin than young at day 28 (t[8] = 2.49; p = 0.04; Figure 5B).
Leptin was significantly elevated in aged rats relative to young (F[1,64] = 64.21; p < 0.001), but was not altered by diet or time groups (p > 0.1 for both; Figure 5C-D). However, time group significantly interacted with both age (F[3,64] = 4.76; p < 0.01) and diet group (F[3,64] = 3.19; p = 0.03). When diet groups were compared individually, leptin was significantly higher in aged rats fed both the KD (F[1,32] = 42.99; p < 0.001) and SD (F[1,32] = 24.15; p < 0.001). While time point did not reach significance for either diet group (KD p = 0.06; SD p = 0.07), there was a significant interaction between age and time point for KD fed rats only (F[3,32] = 3.57; p = 0.03).
C-peptide levels were also significantly elevated in aged relative to young rats (F[1,64] = 13.37; p < 0.01; Figure 5E). While both trended towards significance, neither diet (F[1,64] = 3.45; p = 0.07) nor time point (F[3,64] = 2.64; p = 0.06) significantly affected C-peptide levels. However, the significant interaction between age and diet (F[1,64] = 10.34; p < 0.01) indicates that age was a significant factor for the KD-fed rats but not SD-fed rats (Figure 5E-F). Therefore, the 2 diet groups were considered individually. C-peptide did not change significantly over time for either SD (F[3,32] = 1.32; p = 0.29) nor KD (F[3,32] = 1.95; p = 0.14). However, C-peptide levels were significantly lower in young rats relative to aged rats on the KD (F[1,32] = 20.96; p < 0.001) but not SD (F[1,32] = 0.11; p = 0.74), though this age difference was not present at 28 days.
Lastly, glucose levels at the time of blood collection were combined with insulin levels to derive a HOMA value (see above). While age did not significantly affect HOMA values overall (F[1,64] = 0.76; p = 0.39), age significantly interacted with diet group (F[1,64] = 9.24; p < 0.01) and time group (F[1,64] = 3.09; p = 0.03; Figure 5G-H). Furthermore, the KD significantly lowered HOMA values (F[1,64] = 30.33; p < 0.001), so diet groups were analyzed separately. Aged, KD-fed rats had significantly higher HOMA values than young (F[1,32] = 16.54; p < 0.001), and this significantly changed over the 28-day keto-adaptation period (F[1,32] = 4.00; p = 0.02) such that by day 28, aged rats did not differ from young (t[8] = 0.11; p = 0.91). Conversely, aged, SD-fed rats did not exhibit higher HOMA values than young (F[1,32] = 1.53; p = 0.23), and HOMA values were not affected by length of time across the 28 days of TRF (F[1,32] = 0.54; p = 0.66).
To investigate the potential of TRF and the KD to reverse impaired insulin tolerance in aged subjects, rats were given an insulin tolerance test (ITT) before and after 8 weeks of dietary intervention. At baseline, while all rats were consuming chow ad libitum, the insulin injection significantly affected peripheral glucose levels over time for all groups (F[6,150] = 49.81; p < 0.001; data not shown). There were no differences in glucose response to insulin across assigned diet groups (F[2,25] = 0.41; p = 0.67). Interestingly, ketone body levels were also significantly changed over time following an injection of insulin (F[6,66] = 23.26; p < 0.001) such that they appeared to increase as the glucose level decreased. There were no differences across designated diet groups prior to diet implementation (F[1,31] = 1.26; p = 0.30) in BHB response to insulin.
Following 8 weeks on their respective diets, the significant main effect of time point on glucose response to insulin remained significant across all groups (F[6,156] = 199.23; p < 0.001; Figure 6). After the 8 weeks of experimental diets, however, there was a significant diet group effect (F[2,26] = 10.12; p = 0.001), but no significant effect of age (F[1,26] = 2.04; p = 0.17). Furthermore, time point significantly interacted with diet group (F[12,156] = 3.13; p = 0.001) but not age (F[12,156] = 0.43; p = 0.86). Because of the significant effects of diet group, glucose and BHB results are plotted separately for each diet group in Figures 6A–6F. Notably, the AUC only significantly differed across age groups in the ad libitum-fed rats (t[9] = 3.55; p = 0.006) but not the SD (t[12] = 1.06; p = 0.31) or KD groups (t[11] = 0.32; p = 0.75). These data suggest that age-related impairments in insulin sensitivity were ameliorated in both TRF groups.
A glucose tolerance test (GTT) was also administered following 7 weeks of the diets. While the glucose injection did result in increased circulating glucose over time (F[6,186] = 34.09; p < 0.001; Supplementary Figure 1A), there were no differences across age (F[1,31] = 0.14; p = 0.71) or diet groups (F[2,31] = 1,15; p = 0.33), and none of these variables significantly interacted (p ≥ 0.10 for all comparisons). See Supplementary material and Supplementary Figure 1 for more information.
Previous ketogenic diet experience enhances keto-adaptation beyond time-restricted feeding alone
The TRF implemented in the current experiments was associated with a ~21 hour fast, as both young and aged rats consumed close to 100% of the available calories within 3 hours (Figure 1C). Because intermittent fasting [43] and the KD [44] have both been reported to improve insulin receptor sensitivity, as well as other markers of metabolic function, it is possible that either manipulation could enhance keto-adaptation in young and aged rats. To test this, young and aged rats were divided equally into groups A and B, with both groups receiving once daily TRF of 51 kcal as described previously with either the KD or SD for 6 weeks. Group A was placed on the KD for 4 consecutive weeks and then cycled onto the SD for 1 week, followed by a final week of the KD. Group B was placed on the SD for 4 weeks, followed by 1 week of the KD and a final week of the SD (Figure 7A). Blood glucose and BHB levels were collected every 3 days to calculate the GKI across the different diet cycles. Figure 7B shows the GKI values for groups A and B during KD initiation across the different cycles. During the cycles on the SD, all GKI values rapidly returned to high baseline levels on the first day and this did not vary between age groups (F[1,14] = 1.35; p = 0.26; Figure 7C).
As in the previous cohorts of rats, when dietary ketosis with TRF was initiated following ad libitum feeding, aged rats showed significant delays in keto-adaptation (F[1,21] = 21.05; p < 0.001). Keto-adaptation was compared between rats that were naïve to dietary ketosis prior to Day 1 that cycled into ketosis following ad libitum feeding (Group A cycle 1) to rats that were naïve to dietary ketosis prior to Day following 4 weeks of TRF (Group B cycle 2). Overall, across days 1-7 on the KD, the GKI significantly decreased as a function of days on the diet (F[2,28] = 22.89; p < 0.001). The GKI, however, was significantly lower in Group B during cycle 2, compared to Group A during cycle 1 (F[1,14] = 16.74; p = 0.001), indicating that 4 weeks of fasting through TRF facilitated the ability to flexibly switch from glycolysis to ketosis compared to consuming food ad libitum, presumably by decreasing insulin levels. Across both cycles of ketosis initiation, the significant main effect of age persisted (F[1,14] = 10.44; p = 0.001), with aged rats having higher GKI values relative to young animals. Interestingly, there was a significant interaction effect between age and cycle (F[1,14] = 8.20; p = 0.01), indicating that the fasting through TRF expedited keto-adaptation in aged rats to a greater extent than young. This interaction is likely due to metabolic impairments and hyperinsulinemia of aged rats while being fed ad libitum, which was at least partially being overcome by TRF.
To examine the impact of previous experience on the KD to the rate of keto-adaptation in young and aged animals, we then compared GKI values from rats that were naïve to dietary ketosis prior to Day 1 (Group A cycle 1) to their GKI values during their second cycle of ketosis (Group A cycle 3). GKI levels were significantly lower during the second keto-adaptation cycle than the first (F[1,14] = 52.12; p < 0.001). Moreover, cycle significantly interacted with age (F[1,14] = 6.66; p = 0.02), indicating that the age-associated delay in keto-adaptation was mitigated by previous experience on the KD.
Because TRF with the SD was sufficient to influence keto-adaptation, the degree to which previous experience in ketosis influenced successive cycles of keto-adaptation was also examined. Specifically, Group A cycle 3 (TRF with prior KD experience) was compared to Group B cycle 2 (TRF and KD naïve prior to Day 1). Critically, both groups had 1 month of TRF prior to transitioning to the KD, with these groups differing only in terms of previous experience on the KD. Importantly, the GKI was significantly lower in Group A cycle 3 compared to Group B cycle 2 (F[1,14] = 15.89; p = 0.001). This observation indicates that a previous cycle of keto-adaptation enhanced the rate of metabolic switching to promote higher rates of ketogenesis in a shorter amount of time following carbohydrate restriction. In other words, the increased rate of keto-adaptation induced by 4 weeks of TRF was further augmented by previous experience with keto-adaptation. In contrast to Cycle 1 in Group A (first cycle of keto-adaptation following ad libitum feeding), there were no significant effects of age (F[1,14] = 2.43; p = 0.14) across the two later cycles.
Keto-adaptation in young and aged rats is decoupled from glycogen content
One hypothesis regarding the initiation of ketosis is that fat oxidation is promoted when glycogen content in the liver and muscles is depleted. The age-related delay in keto-adaptation observed in aged animals could therefore be due to increased glycogen levels in liver or muscle. Thus, to determine how glycogen dynamics interact with ketosis, we measured the glycogen content in the liver and muscle of young and aged rats on the SD or KD for at least 12 weeks prior to sacrifice. Most rats were fasted for at least 15 hours prior to sacrifice, however a small subset of rats was fed 1 hour prior to sacrifice. While there were no differences in liver glycogen content between age groups (F[1,50] = 2.50; p = 0.12; Figure 8A), there was a significant decrease in liver glycogen in KD-fed rats (F[1,50] = 6.60; p = 0.01). Additionally, for the SD-fed rats only, fasted rats had significantly less glycogen in their liver than fed rats (F[1,50] = 4.88; p = 0.03). This difference between fasted and unfasted animals was particularly evident in the aged rats and was not observed in the KD group, indicating that ketosis prevents fasting-induced glycogen depletion. These data suggest that long-term ketosis reduced liver glycogen content, but these levels were not affected while fasting. In contrast, aged rats on a SD that were fasted quickly lost liver glycogen, which was not observed in young rats. There were no differences in glycogen content within the muscle across age (F[1,50] = 0.47; p = 0.50), diet (F[1,50] = 0.04; p = 0.85) or fasted versus fed groups (F[1,50] = 1.31; p = 0.26; Figure 8B). Furthermore, there were no significant interactions between any of these variables (p > 0.14 for all).
To determine if the glycogen content within the liver changed during the process of keto-adaptation, 40 young and 40 aged rats were split into groups 1-4, which were fed 51 kcal once daily of the KD or SD for 1, 3, 7 or 28 days and then sacrificed. Because age differences in liver glycogen were only detected in fasted animals, all rats were sacrificed 18-22 hours after eating for quantification of liver glycogen content. Consistent with our previous data, following 1 and 3 days of consuming the SD, liver glycogen was significantly lower in aged rats relative to young (t[8] = 3.00; p = 0.02 and t[8] = 3.23; p = 0.01, respectively; Figure 8C). In the KD group at days 1 and 3, there was no age difference in liver glycogen (t[8] = 0.51; p = 0.62 and t[8] = 0.16; p = 0.88, respectively). By days 7 and 28, there was no longer a significant difference in liver glycogen content across age or diet groups nor did these two factors significantly interact (p > 0.26 for all comparisons). When time point was considered as a factor, there was a significant change in glycogen levels by day (F[3,54] = 3.18; p = 0.03), as well by diet (F[1,54] = 6.01; p = 0.02). While there was no significant effect of age group (F[1,54] = 1.8; p = 0.17), this factor did significantly interact with length of time on the diet (F[1,54] = 5.26; p = 0.03). This could be due to the tendency for glycogen levels to increase after 3 days on the diet in KD-fed rats.
Surprisingly, in young rats, liver glycogen content did not correlate with the GKI during the initiation of TRF in either KD-fed rats (R2 = 0.08; p = 0.24) or SD-fed rats (R2 = 0.04; p = 0.42; Figure 8D). Furthermore, there was no correlation between liver glycogen and GKI in aged KD-fed rats during the initiation of ketosis (R2 = 0.12; p = 0.14; Figure 8E). However, in the SD-fed rats that switched from ad libitum feeding to TRF, liver glycogen in aged SD-fed rats significantly negatively correlated with GKI (R2 = 0.25; p = 0.03). These data demonstrate that it is not critical for liver glycogen to be depleted to initiate dietary ketosis in either aged or young animals. Moreover, in aged rats that are transitioning from ad libitum feeding to TRF of a SD, more glycogen is associated with lower GKI values. This suggests that the aged rats that adapt to being fasted by decreasing blood glucose and increasing BHB level spare liver glycogen content.
Metabolic responses to a physiological stressor are enhanced by ketosis, but not time-restricted feeding
To investigate whether liver glycogen could still be utilized in ketotic animals, the metabolic response to stress, which involves a rapid hepatic conversion of glycogen to glucose [45], was pharmacologically induced. Importantly, this evolutionarily conserved metabolic response to a stressor can be essential for properly adapting to activities of daily living, especially during strenuous circumstances. To investigate this, serum levels of glucose and BHB following epinephrine injections were quantified in rats fed ad libitum with traditional rodent chow (Envigo, Teklad 2918). These same animals were then placed on the KD or SD for 12 weeks. At both time points, rats were given an intraperitoneal injection of either saline or 0.1mg/kg epinephrine 30 minutes postprandially, and blood glucose and BHB levels were quantified 30 minutes post-injection. Injections were separated by a 48-hour washout period, with the order counterbalanced across animals. The post-injection values of blood glucose, BHB and the GKI for saline and epinephrine were then used to calculate the percent change between saline and epinephrine injections for blood levels of glucose (Figure 9A), BHB (Figure 9B) and the GKI (Figure 9C).
All rats, including those on the KD, showed a marked increase in blood glucose in response to epinephrine (Figure 9). In fact, there was a significant effect of diet on epinephrine-induced increases in blood glucose, with KD-fed rats having a larger change relative to the baseline saline condition (F[1,23] = 5.21; p = 0.032). This observation indicates a conservation of the stress-induced glucose response while in ketosis. The enhanced ability to mobilize glucose in response to a stressor was not due to the TRF alone, as the SD-fed rats did not show this same enhancement between ad libitum feeding and 12 weeks of the SD (F[1,13] = 0.29; p = 0.599).
In line with these data, blood levels of the pancreatic hormone glucagon, which is required for the breakdown of glycogen to glucose, was not altered in KD-fed rats (F[1,13] = 0.77; p = 0.394; Figure 10A). While we did not observe an age difference in our SD-fed rats, when older groups of animals were included there was a significant difference between young and old animals (Figure 10B, 10C), replicating previously published work [46, 47].
Although BHB levels were unaffected by epinephrine during ad libitum feeding with chow or during TRF with the SD, epinephrine decreased blood BHB levels in KD-fed rats. Moreover, in rats that went from ad libitum feeding to TRF with the KD, there was a significant change in the percent decrease in BHB induced by epinephrine (F[1,23] = 8.90; p = 0.007). In KD-fed rats, the glucose increase and concurrent decrease in BHB, was associated with a significant epinephrine-induced increase in GKI that did not occur during ad libitum feeding (F[1,23] = 15.39; p = 0.001). Despite the isocaloric 51 kcal per day across both diet groups, the abrupt increase in the post-epinephrine GKI experienced by the KD-fed rats was not observed in the SD-fed group, indicating that TRF alone did not enhance the metabolic response to stress in either young or aged rats. The present observations suggest that long-term nutritional ketosis does not deplete glycogen stores. Moreover, hepatic glycogen can be mobilized in response to a physiological stressor to a greater extent when in ketosis relative to glycolysis alone.
DISCUSSION
Here we show that age-related metabolic dysfunction is mitigated by changing an ad libitum energy consumption pattern to time-restricted feeding (TRF; once daily feeding) of 51 kcal. These benefits in metabolic health were further bolstered by a high fat, low carbohydrate ketogenic diet (KD). We replicated previous data [14, 15] demonstrating an age-related impairment in keto-adaptation, and extended these observations to show that keto-adaptation induced by diet does not correlate with liver glycogen depletion. While a lack of correlation between liver glycogen and level of ketosis does not in itself mandate a decoupling of these two processes, taken together with the rest of the data presented here, it is clear that ketosis can be decoupled from glycogen depletion. Surprisingly, when fasted, rats in long-term ketosis had levels of liver and muscle glycogen that were comparable to young rats on the standard diet (SD) and considerably higher than aged SD-fed rats. Moreover, the KD appeared to enhance glycogen dynamics, with KD-fed rats showing an enhanced increase in blood glucose in response to epinephrine. Thus, age-related declines in metabolic switching do not appear to result from differences in glycogen storage and dynamics, but rather emerge from metabolic dysfunction associated with hyperinsulinemia.
Aged rats had elevated fasting and postprandial insulin levels while ad libitum feeding with standard rodent chow, as well as during the initial stages of keto-adaptation. In young rats, fasting insulin levels decreased significantly following the first 24 hours of a KD. This decrease in fasting insulin was not observed in aged rats during the first week of a KD. In addition to insulin deficits, aged animals fed ad libitum with chow had significantly elevated leptin, c-peptide, and HOMA. While some metabolic factors were significantly improved following TRF with the SD, such as the insulin tolerance test, combining TRF with a KD was most efficacious at restoring metabolic markers in aged rats to levels comparable to young animals, as well as enhancing the epinephrine-induced increase in peripheral glucose.
If metabolic switching is defined as being able to switch bi-directionally between periods of carbohydrate/glucose usage and fatty acid/ketone body usage [48], then the KD enhanced metabolic switching beyond what was observed with TRF alone. While TRF did increase the rate at which aged rats could transition from glycolysis to ketosis, enhanced keto-adaptation was further augmented by TRF+KD. These data concur with previous studies demonstrating the additive benefits of food restriction with a KD at increasing seizure threshold [49]. Surprisingly, young and aged rats in our study consuming the KD still showed an enhanced glucose response to an acute increase in epinephrine, beyond what was observed in rats fed TRF+SD. This observation further supports the notion that TRF+KD may confer the greatest improvement in metabolic switching. Enhancing metabolic switching may be particularly important in aged populations. While epinephrine is able to improve memory performance in young rats through hepatic glucogenesis, aged rats show an attenuated elevation in blood glucose in response to epinephrine, and no cognitive benefits. Thus, old animals do not cognitively benefit from this physiological stress response that is an evolutionary conserved mechanism to promote survival [50].
In the short term, consumption of a large quantity of fatty acids, and relatively little glucose, initiates an inhibition of glycogen production via the glucose fatty acid cycle [51]. These effects are due to changes in enzymatic activity in response to dietary composition [52]. However, the glucose fatty acid cycle is dysfunctional in metabolically impaired individuals, such as those with type 2 diabetes. Thus, it is conceivable that the aged rats in the current study, which had elevated fasting insulin and impaired insulin sensitivity, may have metabolic deficits that produce impairments in the glucose fatty cycle. How this specifically interacts with long-term versus cyclical dietary ketosis is an avenue of investigation worth pursuit. Additionally, weight differences across age groups may also be an influencing factor on keto-adaptation, as obesity increases the incidence of insulin resistance and other metabolic issues, and could be addressed in future studies through the implementation of weight control measures prior to KD intervention.
Although TRF alone can enhance metabolic switching, and improve cognitive health across the lifespan [8, 9, 48, 53–55], the current data suggest that additional benefits can be obtained through nutritional ketosis. It is also notable that the benefits of TRF for cognitive function might be due to comparisons with ad libitum fed rodents that are metabolically morbid [56]. In fact, it is well documented that excessive energy consumption can lead to cognitive impairments [57–59]. A strong indicator that TRF alone is not sufficient for extending the cognitive healthspan is the wealth of studies demonstrating age-related cognitive decline in rodents while utilizing tasks requiring appetitive motivation. Many tests of cognitive function require animals to be restricted to <85% of their body weight to motivate participation by feeding once daily, making a large portion of investigations on the neurobiology of cognitive aging into incidental experiments of caloric restriction with TRF. While incentive motivation for food rewards does not change with age [60], robust age-related deficits are observed on working memory [61–63], other executive functions [64–66], as well as behaviors that require the prefrontal cortex and medial temporal lobe [14, 67–69].
Importantly, there may be pertinent differences in metabolism, brain aging, and responses to dietary ketosis between males and females [70]. Unfortunately, the current lack of availability of female Fischer 344 x Brown Norway hybrid rats from maintained rodent colonies (https://www.nia.nih.gov/research/dab/aged-rodent-colonies-handbook) did not permit the consideration of sex as a variable in the current experiments. Potential interactions of sex and dietary ketosis will need to be examined in future studies.
We’ve shown that combining TRF with a KD provides a more robust restoration of metabolic health than TRF alone. Furthermore, as the number of individuals living with metabolic disease (e.g. diabetes, metabolic syndrome, insulin resistance, etc.) continues to increase, these data demonstrate the translational potential of TRF+KD for alleviating many of the health burdens facing these patients. In addition to the metabolic factors measured here, a previous study has shown that this feeding pattern can enhance cognitive function and decrease anxiety-like behaviors [14]. Moreover, TRF+KD alters transporter protein expression within the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus [14, 15], increases BDNF [71, 72], decreases pain and inflammation [73], modulates KATP channels [72, 74], modifies neurotransmitter levels [75, 76] and lessens amyloid and tau pathologies [77]. Each of these biochemical alterations induced by ketosis are likely to confer resilience to cognitive decline.
In addition to peripheral health, memory deficits and other types of cognitive impairment have a profound contribution to decreasing quality of life in individuals and their caregivers. Although the increased mean life expectancy has had a commensurate increase in the number of aged people living without physical limitations [78], the same cannot be said for cognitive deficits. In fact, the proportion of individuals experiencing mild or severe memory impairments will reach 1 in 3 by the 8th decade of life [79], and disrupted brain bioenergetics is a common feature of aging [80, 81]. Improving metabolic function in the aged population can have profound impacts on cognitive outcomes, as the brain disproportionately uses metabolic resources [80] Furthermore, cerebral glucose metabolism decreases with age, most notably within the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex [82], both of which are critical for higher cognitive function [83–85]. Importantly, this decline correlates with worse spatial memory performance [82]. Thus, developing metabolic-based interventions for improving peripheral metabolic health will likely have profound therapeutic potential for increasing the cognitive healthspan.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Subjects and handling
Young (4 months; n = 112; mean weight 337.59g) and aged (20 months: n = 112; mean weight 526.04) male Fischer 344 x Brown Norway F1 Hybrid rats from the NIA colony at Charles River were used in this study (see Table 1 for sample sizes per experiment). Rats were housed individually and maintained on a reverse 12-hr light/dark cycle, and all feeding, metabolic and biochemical assays were performed in the dark phase. Rats were given one week to acclimate to the facility prior to any intervention. All experimental procedures were performed in accordance with National Institutes of Health guidelines and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of Florida.
Table 1. Sample size per dietary group per experiment.
Figure | Ad libitum chow-fed rats | TRF standard diet-fed rats | TRF ketogenic diet-fed rats |
Figure 1: Feeding pattern detection | Young n = 6 Aged n = 5 | Young n = 7 Aged n = 5 | Young n = 7 Aged n = 4 |
Figure 2: Keto-adaptation rate | Young n = 5 Aged n = 4 | Young n = 5 Aged n = 4 | |
Figure 3-4: Age-related metabolic impairment | Young n = 6-27 Aged n = 5-24 | ||
Figure 5: Insulin during keto-adaptation | Young n = 20 Aged n = 20 | Young n = 20 Aged n = 20 | |
Figure 6: Insulin tolerance testing | Young n = 6 Aged n = 5 | Young n = 7 Aged n = 7 | Young n = 7 Aged n = 6 |
Figure 7: Naïve vs experienced keto-adaptation | Young n = 10; Aged n = 8 All rats alternated dietary regiments, see text and Figure 7 for details. | ||
Figure 8: Glycogen content in liver and muscle during keto-adaptation | Young n = 20 Aged n = 20 | Young n = 20 Aged n = 20 | |
Figure 9: Epinephrine-induced stress response | Young n = 6 Aged n = 8 | Young n = 6 Aged n = 7 | |
Figure 10A: Systemic glucagon | Young n = 4 Aged n = 4 | Young n = 5 Aged n = 4 | |
Figure 10B, 10C: Epinephrine-induced stress response in advanced age | Young n = 6 Aged n = 10 Advanced Age n = 5 | ||
Supplementary Figure 1: Glucose tolerance testing | Young n = 6 Aged n = 5 | Young n = 7 Aged n = 7 | Young n = 7 Aged n = 6 |
Dietary interventions
For all experiments, the same ketogenic diet (KD) and standard diet (SD) were used as published previously [14, 15, 68]. An additional group of rats were fed ad libitum with standard laboratory chow (Envigo, Teklad 2918). The KD was a high fat/low carbohydrate diet (Lab Supply; 5722, Fort Worth, Texas) mixed with MCT oil (Neobee 895, Stephan, Northfield, Illinois) with a macronutrient profile of 76% fat, 4% carbohydrates, and 20% protein (see Figure 1A). The micronutrient-matched SD (Lab Supply; 1810727, Fort Worth, Texas) had a macronutrient profile of 16% fat, 65% carbohydrates, and 19% protein (see Figure 1A). The KD and SD were made fresh weekly and kept at 4°C until use each day. For additional micronutrient details for both diets, see [15]. The Teklad standard laboratory chow had a macronutrient composition of 5% fat, 75% carbohydrates and 20% protein. The SD and KD were micronutrient matched, and rats were fed the same number of calories each day (~51kcal) during the dark phase between 14:00 and 17:00 hours (lights on at 20:00) while on TRF, regardless of diet. Access to water was ad libitum for all rats on all diets, and rats were weighed daily when fed. These different diets are reported to not alter water consumption patterns or induce a prolonged stress response [15].
Blood collection, testing, and metabolic data
All blood samples collected prior to sacrifice were collected from a small nick in the tail. Rats were briefly restrained, tails were cleaned with an alcohol swab, and then a clean scalpel blade was used to make a superficial cut approximately 1 inch from the end of the tail. Following blood collection, wounds were wiped clean and Wonder Dust (Farnam Companies, Phoenix, AZ) was applied to promote wound healing. Blood collected on the day of sacrifice was collected directly from the trunk immediately following decapitation.
For blood glucose and ketone body (β-hydroxybutyrate; BHB) determination, one drop per test was collected directly onto the appropriate test strip (Abbott Diabetes Care, Inc, Alameda, CA; glucose SKU#: 9972865 and ketone SKU#: 7074565) in a Precision Xtra blood monitoring system (Abbott Diabetes Care, Inc, Alameda, CA; SKU#: 9881465).
For the insulin tolerance test (ITT) and glucose tolerance test (GTT), rats were fasted overnight and injected intraperitoneally with either 0.75 U/kg of human insulin (Hanna Pharmaceutical Supply Co #NC0769896) or 1 g/kg glucose (Fisher Scientific #D16-500). Blood glucose and ketone body levels were measured as described above at 0, 15, 30, 45, 60, and 120 minutes post injection.
For metabolic biomarker quantification, approximately 750 μL of blood was collected from the tail nick at the time of glucose testing. Collected blood was left at room temperature for at least 25 minutes prior to centrifugation at 13,000g for 10 minutes at 4°C. 0.35 μL of supernatant was collected into a tube containing 0.4 μL DPP-IV inhibitor (EMD Millipore, NC9010908), 0.4 μL protease inhibitor cocktail (Millipore Sigma, S8820-2TAB) and 0.32 μL 0.9% PBS. Samples were kept frozen at -80°C prior to use in the Rat Metabolic Array (MRDMET; EVE Technologies, Calgary, AB). This multiplex immunoassay (BioPlex 200) quantified levels of insulin, leptin, C-peptide 2 and glucagon. Each sample was analyzed in duplicate. All values outside of the liner range were excluded.
Epinephrine administration
Prior to starting any dietary intervention, half of the rats were given 0.1mg/kg epinephrine (diluted in 0.9% sterile saline solution) and half were given the equivalent volume of saline delivered via intraperitoneal injection. After 30 minutes, glucose and BHB were determined as described above. Groups were counterbalanced for drug across two days of testing with one day off in between. That is, following a 48-hour washout period, the groups were reversed and the process was repeated. This entire 2-day process was repeated after 12 weeks on the TRF+SD and TRF+KD fed rats, with epinephrine injections occurring 30 minutes postprandially.
Tissue collection and glycogen quantification
On the day of sacrifice, a subset of rats was fed their respective diets one hour prior to being anesthetized. All other rats were sacrificed without feeding since the prior day (approximately 15-22 hours fasted). All rats were rapidly sedated in a jar containing concentrated isoflurane and then decapitated once the righting reflex had ceased. The liver and tibialis anterior muscle were isolated, rinsed in cold PBS and immediately frozen on dry ice. Tissue was stored at -80°C until use.
Glycogen content in the liver and muscle tissue were determined per mg wet tissue weight, modeled after previously published protocols [86–88]. Approximately 50 mg frozen tissue was placed in a pre-weighed tube containing 660 μL NaOH at 100°C and tissue weight was determined. Tubes were boiled at 100°C for 15 minutes, vortexed and then brought to neutral pH using 320 μL 2M HCl. Samples were vortexed again and 150 μL of muscle, or 20 μL of liver with 130 μL of ddH2O, were placed into two tubes each. A sodium acetate solution was added to each tube, one with and one without the addition of amyloglucosidase, and samples were incubated at 37°C for 2 hours. Following a 20-minute centrifuge at 14,000 RPM, 50 μL of supernatant was plated in triplicate into a 96 well plate. 100 μL Tris buffer was added to each well, which was then read on a Synergy HT Microplate Spectrophotometer (BioTek) at 340 nm. 50 μL hexokinase solution was added to each well, and the plate was incubated at room temperature on a shaker, away from light before reading again at 340 nm. The optical density reading before the addition of hexokinase was subtracted from the final optical density reading to correct for free glucose values. Final glycogen content was extrapolated using a standard glycogen curve run on each plate and normalized to tissue weight.
Quantification and statistical analysis
All data are expressed as group means ± standard error of the mean (SEM) unless otherwise reported. The glucose ketone index (GKI) was calculated as described in a previous publication [39]. Briefly, the glucose (mg/dL) was divided by the molecular weight of glucose (expressed as (mol/(g x dL))), and this value was then divided by the BHB value (mM). Differences across age and diet groups were analyzed using a two-factor ANOVA with the between subjects factors of age (2 levels: young and aged) and diet (2 levels: SD and KD or 3 levels: SD, KD and ad libitum). When time was a factor, an ANOVA-RM was used with the same factors. Post hoc analyses were done utilizing independent-samples t-tests where appropriate. Null hypotheses were rejected at the level of p > 0.05. All analyses were performed with the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences v25 (IBM, Armonk, NY) or GraphPad Prism version 7.03 for Windows (GraphPad Software, La Jolla, California USA).
Supplementary Material
Footnotes
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS: ARH designed the study, contributed to data acquisition, analysis and interpretation, and prepared the manuscript. LMT, QPF, KTC, BMM, NF & MH contributed to data acquisition and analysis. DD contributed to interpretation and prepared the manuscript. SNB designed the study, contributed to data analysis, interpretation and prepared the manuscript.
CONFLICTS OF INTEREST: No authors have a conflicts of interest to report.
FUNDING: This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health, National Institute on Aging (RF1AG060977; 1F31AG058455).
REFERENCES
- 1.Masoro EJ. Overview of caloric restriction and ageing. Mech Ageing Dev. 2005; 126:913–22. 10.1016/j.mad.2005.03.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Fontana L, Klein S. Aging, adiposity, and calorie restriction. JAMA. 2007; 297:986–94. 10.1001/jama.297.9.986 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Fontana L, Partridge L, Longo VD. Extending healthy life span—from yeast to humans. Science. 2010; 328:321–26. 10.1126/science.1172539 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.de Cabo R, Carmona-Gutierrez D, Bernier M, Hall MN, Madeo F. The search for antiaging interventions: from elixirs to fasting regimens. Cell. 2014; 157:1515–26. 10.1016/j.cell.2014.05.031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Longo VD, Mattson MP. Fasting: molecular mechanisms and clinical applications. Cell Metab. 2014; 19:181–92. 10.1016/j.cmet.2013.12.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Mattson MP, Allison DB, Fontana L, Harvie M, Longo VD, Malaisse WJ, Mosley M, Notterpek L, Ravussin E, Scheer FA, Seyfried TN, Varady KA, Panda S. Meal frequency and timing in health and disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014; 111:16647–53. 10.1073/pnas.1413965111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Longo VD, Panda S. Fasting, Circadian Rhythms, and Time-Restricted Feeding in Healthy Lifespan. Cell Metab. 2016; 23:1048–59. 10.1016/j.cmet.2016.06.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Mitchell SJ, Bernier M, Mattison JA, Aon MA, Kaiser TA, Anson RM, Ikeno Y, Anderson RM, Ingram DK, de Cabo R. Daily Fasting Improves Health and Survival in Male Mice Independent of Diet Composition and Calories. Cell Metab. 2019; 29:221–228.e3. 10.1016/j.cmet.2018.08.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Anson RM, Guo Z, de Cabo R, Iyun T, Rios M, Hagepanos A, Ingram DK, Lane MA, Mattson MP. Intermittent fasting dissociates beneficial effects of dietary restriction on glucose metabolism and neuronal resistance to injury from calorie intake. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003; 100:6216–20. 10.1073/pnas.1035720100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Mitchell SJ, Madrigal-Matute J, Scheibye-Knudsen M, Fang E, Aon M, González-Reyes JA, Cortassa S, Kaushik S, Gonzalez-Freire M, Patel B, Wahl D, Ali A, Calvo-Rubio M, et al. Effects of Sex, Strain, and Energy Intake on Hallmarks of Aging in Mice. Cell Metab. 2016; 23:1093–112. 10.1016/j.cmet.2016.05.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Bredesen DE. Reversal of cognitive decline: a novel therapeutic program. Aging (Albany NY). 2014; 6:707–17. 10.18632/aging.100690 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Choi HR, Kim J, Lim H, Park YK. Two-Week Exclusive Supplementation of Modified Ketogenic Nutrition Drink Reserves Lean Body Mass and Improves Blood Lipid Profile in Obese Adults: A Randomized Clinical Trial. Nutrients. 2018; 10:10. 10.3390/nu10121895 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Greco T, Glenn TC, Hovda DA, Prins ML. Ketogenic diet decreases oxidative stress and improves mitochondrial respiratory complex activity. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2016; 36:1603–13. 10.1177/0271678X15610584 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hernandez AR, Hernandez CM, Campos K, Truckenbrod L, Federico Q, Moon B, McQuail JA, Maurer AP, Bizon JL, Burke SN. A Ketogenic Diet Improves Cognition and Has Biochemical Effects in Prefrontal Cortex That Are Dissociable From Hippocampus. Front Aging Neurosci. 2018; 10:391. 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00391 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Hernandez AR, Hernandez CM, Campos KT, Truckenbrod LM, Sakarya Y, McQuail JA, Carter CS, Bizon JL, Maurer AP, Burke SN. The Antiepileptic Ketogenic Diet Alters Hippocampal Transporter Levels and Reduces Adiposity in Aged Rats. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2018; 73:450–58. 10.1093/gerona/glx193 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hussain TA, Mathew TC, Dashti AA, Asfar S, Al-Zaid N, Dashti HM. Effect of low-calorie versus low-carbohydrate ketogenic diet in type 2 diabetes. Nutrition. 2012; 28:1016–21. 10.1016/j.nut.2012.01.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kephart WC, Mumford PW, Mao X, Romero MA, Hyatt HW, Zhang Y, Mobley CB, Quindry JC, Young KC, Beck DT, Martin JS, McCullough DJ, D’Agostino DP, et al. The 1-Week and 8-Month Effects of a Ketogenic Diet or Ketone Salt Supplementation on Multi-Organ Markers of Oxidative Stress and Mitochondrial Function in Rats. Nutrients. 2017; 9:1019. 10.3390/nu9091019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Westman EC, Yancy WS Jr, Mavropoulos JC, Marquart M, McDuffie JR. The effect of a low-carbohydrate, ketogenic diet versus a low-glycemic index diet on glycemic control in type 2 diabetes mellitus. Nutr Metab (Lond). 2008; 5:36. 10.1186/1743-7075-5-36 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Phinney SD. Ketogenic diets and physical performance. Nutr Metab (Lond). 2004; 1:2. 10.1186/1743-7075-1-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Volek JS, Freidenreich DJ, Saenz C, Kunces LJ, Creighton BC, Bartley JM, Davitt PM, Munoz CX, Anderson JM, Maresh CM, Lee EC, Schuenke MD, Aerni G, et al. Metabolic characteristics of keto-adapted ultra-endurance runners. Metabolism. 2016; 65:100–10. 10.1016/j.metabol.2015.10.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gasior M, Rogawski MA, Hartman AL. Neuroprotective and disease-modifying effects of the ketogenic diet. Behav Pharmacol. 2006; 17:431–39. 10.1097/00008877-200609000-00009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hartman AL. Neuroprotection in metabolism-based therapy. Epilepsy Res. 2012; 100:286–94. 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2011.04.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Paoli A, Rubini A, Volek JS, Grimaldi KA. Beyond weight loss: a review of the therapeutic uses of very-low-carbohydrate (ketogenic) diets. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2013; 67:789–96. 10.1038/ejcn.2013.116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Bentourkia M, Tremblay S, Pifferi F, Rousseau J, Lecomte R, Cunnane S. PET study of 11C-acetoacetate kinetics in rat brain during dietary treatments affecting ketosis. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab. 2009; 296:E796–801. 10.1152/ajpendo.90644.2008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Daniel PM, Love ER, Moorehouse SR, Pratt OE, Wilson P. Factors influencing utilisation of ketone-bodies by brain in normal rats and rats with ketoacidosis. Lancet. 1971; 2:637–38. 10.1016/S0140-6736(71)80073-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Hawkins RA, Mans AM, Davis DW. Regional ketone body utilization by rat brain in starvation and diabetes. Am J Physiol. 1986; 250:E169–78. 10.1152/ajpendo.1986.250.2.E169 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Lukosaityte J, Allen HJ, Lennon DJ, Borum PR. BMI Z-Score Improves after 24 Months of Precision Ketogenic Therapy in Patients with Abnormal Baseline BMI Z-Score. FASEB J. 2017; 31:963.3–963.3. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Dohm GL, Tapscott EB, Barakat HA, Kasperek GJ. Influence of fasting on glycogen depletion in rats during exercise. J Appl Physiol Respir Environ Exerc Physiol. 1983; 55:830–33. 10.1152/jappl.1983.55.3.830 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Ivy JL. Muscle glycogen synthesis before and after exercise. Sports Med. 1991; 11:6–19. 10.2165/00007256-199111010-00002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Adams JH, Koeslag JH. Carbohydrate homeostasis and post-exercise ketosis in trained and untrained rats. J Physiol. 1988; 407:453–61. 10.1113/jphysiol.1988.sp017425 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Morris KA, Chang Q, Mohler EG, Gold PE. Age-related memory impairments due to reduced blood glucose responses to epinephrine. Neurobiol Aging. 2010; 31:2136–45. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.12.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Talley CE, Kahn S, Alexander LJ, Gold PE. Epinephrine fails to enhance performance of food-deprived rats on a delayed spontaneous alternation task. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2000; 73:79–86. 10.1006/nlme.1999.3920 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.McNay EC, Gold PE. Age-related differences in hippocampal extracellular fluid glucose concentration during behavioral testing and following systemic glucose administration. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2001; 56:B66–71. 10.1093/gerona/56.2.B66 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Morris KA, Gold PE. Epinephrine and glucose modulate training-related CREB phosphorylation in old rats: relationships to age-related memory impairments. Exp Gerontol. 2013; 48:115–27. 10.1016/j.exger.2012.11.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Fink RI, Kolterman OG, Griffin J, Olefsky JM. Mechanisms of insulin resistance in aging. J Clin Invest. 1983; 71:1523–35. 10.1172/JCI110908 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Ryan AS. Insulin resistance with aging: effects of diet and exercise. Sports Med. 2000; 30:327–46. 10.2165/00007256-200030050-00002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Refaie MR, Sayed-Ahmed NA, Bakr AM, Abdel Aziz MY, El Kannishi MH, Abdel-Gawad SS. Aging is an Inevitable Risk Factor for Insulin Resistance. J Taibah Univ Med Sci. 2006; 1:30–41. 10.1016/S1658-3612(06)70005-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Volek JS, Noakes T, Phinney SD. Rethinking fat as a fuel for endurance exercise. Eur J Sport Sci. 2015; 15:13–20. 10.1080/17461391.2014.959564 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Meidenbauer JJ, Mukherjee P, Seyfried TN. The glucose ketone index calculator: a simple tool to monitor therapeutic efficacy for metabolic management of brain cancer. Nutr Metab (Lond). 2015; 12:12. 10.1186/s12986-015-0009-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Fukao T, Lopaschuk GD, Mitchell GA. Pathways and control of ketone body metabolism: on the fringe of lipid biochemistry. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids. 2004; 70:243–51. 10.1016/j.plefa.2003.11.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Meigs JB, Rutter MK, Sullivan LM, Fox CS, D’Agostino RB Sr, Wilson PW. Impact of insulin resistance on risk of type 2 diabetes and cardiovascular disease in people with metabolic syndrome. Diabetes Care. 2007; 30:1219–25. 10.2337/dc06-2484 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Bonora E, Formentini G, Calcaterra F, Lombardi S, Marini F, Zenari L, Saggiani F, Poli M, Perbellini S, Raffaelli A, Cacciatori V, Santi L, Targher G, et al. HOMA-estimated insulin resistance is an independent predictor of cardiovascular disease in type 2 diabetic subjects: prospective data from the Verona Diabetes Complications Study. Diabetes Care. 2002; 25:1135–41. 10.2337/diacare.25.7.1135 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Mattson MP, Wan R. Beneficial effects of intermittent fasting and caloric restriction on the cardiovascular and cerebrovascular systems. J Nutr Biochem. 2005; 16:129–37. 10.1016/j.jnutbio.2004.12.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Ballard KD, Quann EE, Kupchak BR, Volk BM, Kawiecki DM, Fernandez ML, Seip RL, Maresh CM, Kraemer WJ, Volek JS. Dietary carbohydrate restriction improves insulin sensitivity, blood pressure, microvascular function, and cellular adhesion markers in individuals taking statins. Nutr Res. 2013; 33:905–12. 10.1016/j.nutres.2013.07.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Sherwin RS, Saccà L. Effect of epinephrine on glucose metabolism in humans: contribution of the liver. Am J Physiol. 1984; 247:E157–65. 10.1152/ajpendo.1984.247.2.E157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Gold PE. Glucose and age-related changes in memory. Neurobiol Aging. 2005. (Suppl 1); 26:60–64. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Korol DL. Enhancing cognitive function across the life span. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2002; 959:167–79. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2002.tb02091.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Mattson MP, Moehl K, Ghena N, Schmaedick M, Cheng A. Intermittent metabolic switching, neuroplasticity and brain health. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2018; 19:63–80. 10.1038/nrn.2017.156 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bough KJ, Valiyil R, Han FT, Eagles DA. Seizure resistance is dependent upon age and calorie restriction in rats fed a ketogenic diet. Epilepsy Res. 1999; 35:21–28. 10.1016/S0920-1211(98)00125-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Gold PE, Korol DL. Forgetfulness during aging: an integrated biology. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014; 112:130–38. 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.03.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Randle PJ, Garland PB, Hales CN, Newsholme EA. The glucose fatty-acid cycle. Its role in insulin sensitivity and the metabolic disturbances of diabetes mellitus. Lancet. 1963; 1:785–89. 10.1016/S0140-6736(63)91500-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Hue L, Taegtmeyer H. The Randle cycle revisited: a new head for an old hat. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab. 2009; 297:E578–91. 10.1152/ajpendo.00093.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Harvie M, Wright C, Pegington M, McMullan D, Mitchell E, Martin B, Cutler RG, Evans G, Whiteside S, Maudsley S, Camandola S, Wang R, Carlson OD, et al. The effect of intermittent energy and carbohydrate restriction v. daily energy restriction on weight loss and metabolic disease risk markers in overweight women. Br J Nutr. 2013; 110:1534–47. 10.1017/S0007114513000792 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Harvie MN, Pegington M, Mattson MP, Frystyk J, Dillon B, Evans G, Cuzick J, Jebb SA, Martin B, Cutler RG, Son TG, Maudsley S, Carlson OD, et al. The effects of intermittent or continuous energy restriction on weight loss and metabolic disease risk markers: a randomized trial in young overweight women. Int J Obes. 2011; 35:714–27. 10.1038/ijo.2010.171 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Hatori M, Vollmers C, Zarrinpar A, DiTacchio L, Bushong EA, Gill S, Leblanc M, Chaix A, Joens M, Fitzpatrick JA, Ellisman MH, Panda S. Time-restricted feeding without reducing caloric intake prevents metabolic diseases in mice fed a high-fat diet. Cell Metab. 2012; 15:848–60. 10.1016/j.cmet.2012.04.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Martin B, Ji S, Maudsley S, Mattson MP. “Control” laboratory rodents are metabolically morbid: why it matters. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010; 107:6127–33. 10.1073/pnas.0912955107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Bocarsly ME, Fasolino M, Kane GA, LaMarca EA, Kirschen GW, Karatsoreos IN, McEwen BS, Gould E. Obesity diminishes synaptic markers, alters microglial morphology, and impairs cognitive function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2015; 112:15731–36. 10.1073/pnas.1511593112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Kanoski SE, Davidson TL. Western diet consumption and cognitive impairment: links to hippocampal dysfunction and obesity. Physiol Behav. 2011; 103:59–68. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.12.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Stranahan AM, Norman ED, Lee K, Cutler RG, Telljohann RS, Egan JM, Mattson MP. Diet-induced insulin resistance impairs hippocampal synaptic plasticity and cognition in middle-aged rats. Hippocampus. 2008; 18:1085–88. 10.1002/hipo.20470 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Simon NW, LaSarge CL, Montgomery KS, Williams MT, Mendez IA, Setlow B, Bizon JL. Good things come to those who wait: attenuated discounting of delayed rewards in aged Fischer 344 rats. Neurobiol Aging. 2010; 31:853–62. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.06.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Bañuelos C, Beas BS, McQuail JA, Gilbert RJ, Frazier CJ, Setlow B, Bizon JL. Prefrontal cortical GABAergic dysfunction contributes to age-related working memory impairment. J Neurosci. 2014; 34:3457–66. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5192-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Beas BS, McQuail JA, Banuelos C, Setlow B, Bizon JL. Prefrontal cortical GABAergic signaling and impaired behavioral flexibility in aged F344 rats. Neuroscience. 2017; 345:274–86. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.02.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Hernandez CM, McQuail JA, Schwabe MR, Burke SN, Setlow B, Bizon JL. Age-Related Declines in Prefrontal Cortical Expression of Metabotropic Glutamate Receptors that Support Working Memory. eNeuro. 2018; 5:5. 10.1523/ENEURO.0164-18.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Barense MD, Fox MT, Baxter MG. Aged rats are impaired on an attentional set-shifting task sensitive to medial frontal cortex damage in young rats. Learn Mem. 2002; 9:191–201. 10.1101/lm.48602 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Hernandez CM, Vetere LM, Orsini CA, McQuail JA, Maurer AP, Burke SN, Setlow B, Bizon JL. Decline of prefrontal cortical-mediated executive functions but attenuated delay discounting in aged Fischer 344 × brown Norway hybrid rats. Neurobiol Aging. 2017; 60:141–52. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.08.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Samson RD, Venkatesh A, Patel DH, Lipa P, Barnes CA. Enhanced performance of aged rats in contingency degradation and instrumental extinction tasks. Behav Neurosci. 2014; 128:122–33. 10.1037/a0035986 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Hernandez AR, Maurer AP, Reasor JE, Turner SM, Barthle SE, Johnson SA, Burke SN. Age-related impairments in object-place associations are not due to hippocampal dysfunction. Behav Neurosci. 2015; 129:599–610. 10.1037/bne0000093 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Hernandez AR, Reasor JE, Truckenbrod LM, Campos KT, Federico QP, Fertal KE, Lubke KN, Johnson SA, Clark BJ, Maurer AP, Burke SN. Dissociable effects of advanced age on prefrontal cortical and medial temporal lobe ensemble activity. Neurobiol Aging. 2018; 70:217–32. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.06.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Johnson SA, Turner SM, Santacroce LA, Carty KN, Shafiq L, Bizon JL, Maurer AP, Burke SN. Rodent age-related impairments in discriminating perceptually similar objects parallel those observed in humans. Hippocampus. 2017; 27:759–76. 10.1002/hipo.22729 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Zhao L, Mao Z, Woody SK, Brinton RD. Sex differences in metabolic aging of the brain: insights into female susceptibility to Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2016; 42:69–79. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.02.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Garriga-Canut M, Schoenike B, Qazi R, Bergendahl K, Daley TJ, Pfender RM, Morrison JF, Ockuly J, Stafstrom C, Sutula T, Roopra A. 2-Deoxy-D-glucose reduces epilepsy progression by NRSF-CtBP-dependent metabolic regulation of chromatin structure. Nat Neurosci. 2006; 9:1382–87. 10.1038/nn1791 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Masino SA, Rho JM. Mechanisms of Ketogenic Diet Action. In: Noebels JL, Avoli M, Rogawski MA, Olsen RW, Delgado-Escueta AV, editors. Jasper’s Basic Mechanisms of the Epilepsies. 4th ed. Bethesda (MD): National Center for Biotechnology Information; 2012. 10.1093/med/9780199746545.003.0078 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK98219/ [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Ruskin DN, Kawamura M, Masino SA. Reduced pain and inflammation in juvenile and adult rats fed a ketogenic diet. PLoS One. 2009; 4:e8349. 10.1371/journal.pone.0008349 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Ma W, Berg J, Yellen G. Ketogenic diet metabolites reduce firing in central neurons by opening K(ATP) channels. J Neurosci. 2007; 27:3618–25. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0132-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Yudkoff M, Daikhin Y, Nissim I, Horyn O, Lazarow A, Luhovyy B, Wehrli S, Nissim I. Response of brain amino acid metabolism to ketosis. Neurochem Int. 2005; 47:119–28. 10.1016/j.neuint.2005.04.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Yudkoff M, Daikhin Y, Horyn O, Nissim I, Nissim I. Ketosis and brain handling of glutamate, glutamine, and GABA. Epilepsia. 2008. (Suppl 8); 49:73–75. 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2008.01841.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Kashiwaya Y, Bergman C, Lee JH, Wan R, King MT, Mughal MR, Okun E, Clarke K, Mattson MP, Veech RL. A ketone ester diet exhibits anxiolytic and cognition-sparing properties, and lessens amyloid and tau pathologies in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2013; 34:1530–39. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.11.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Christensen K, Doblhammer G, Rau R, Vaupel JW. Ageing populations: the challenges ahead. Lancet. 2009; 374:1196–208. 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61460-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Blazer D, Yaffe K, Liverman C. Committee on the Public Health Dimensions of Cognitive Aging; Board on Health Sciences Policy; Institute of Medicine. Cognitive Aging: Progress in Understanding and Opportunities for Action. Washington (DC): National Academies Press; 2015. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK316202/ [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Cunnane S, Nugent S, Roy M, Courchesne-Loyer A, Croteau E, Tremblay S, Castellano A, Pifferi F, Bocti C, Paquet N, Begdouri H, Bentourkia M, Turcotte E, et al. Brain fuel metabolism, aging, and Alzheimer’s disease. Nutrition. 2011; 27:3–20. 10.1016/j.nut.2010.07.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Mattson MP, Arumugam TV. Hallmarks of Brain Aging: Adaptive and Pathological Modification by Metabolic States. Cell Metab. 2018; 27:1176–99. 10.1016/j.cmet.2018.05.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Gage FH, Kelly PA, Björklund A. Regional changes in brain glucose metabolism reflect cognitive impairments in aged rats. J Neurosci. 1984; 4:2856–65. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-11-02856.1984 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Frith C, Dolan R. The role of the prefrontal cortex in higher cognitive functions. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 1996; 5:175–81. 10.1016/S0926-6410(96)00054-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Morris RG, Garrud P, Rawlins JN, O’Keefe J. Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature. 1982; 297:681–83. 10.1038/297681a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Sloan HL, Good M, Dunnett SB. Double dissociation between hippocampal and prefrontal lesions on an operant delayed matching task and a water maze reference memory task. Behav Brain Res. 2006; 171:116–26. 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.03.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Passonneau JV, Gatfield PD, Schulz DW, Lowry OH. An enzymic method for measurement of glycogen. Anal Biochem. 1967; 19:315–26. 10.1016/0003-2697(67)90167-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Passonneau JV, Lauderdale VR. A comparison of three methods of glycogen measurement in tissues. Anal Biochem. 1974; 60:405–12. 10.1016/0003-2697(74)90248-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Shetty PK, Sadgrove MP, Galeffi F, Turner DA. Pyruvate incubation enhances glycogen stores and sustains neuronal function during subsequent glucose deprivation. Neurobiol Dis. 2012; 45:177–87. 10.1016/j.nbd.2011.08.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.