Highlights
-
•
The study explored young men’s and women’s selfie engagement on social media.
-
•
Pathological narcissism was not a significant predictor of selfie-engagement.
-
•
Body surveillance predicted selfie-engagement, especially among women.
-
•
Positive selfie-expectancies predicted selfie-engagement, especially among women.
-
•
Men’s and women’s body-objectification played a pivotal role on selfie-engagement.
Keywords: Self-objectification, Body image, Body surveillance, Pathological narcissism, Selfie expectancies, Selfie-engagement
Abstract
The current use of social media platforms by active young users/creators of visual content provides an easy medium to achieve narcissistic goals of self-promotion and attention-seeking, and to socialize with self-objectification experiences. One of the most popular activities associated with social media use is selfie-sharing. Consequently, the global focus on online physical appearance approval could reinforce selfie-engagement as a specific body image-related behavior, potentially associated with selfie-marketing strategies for self-improvement, and problematic social media use. The present study evaluated the main direct effect of pathological narcissism, objectified body consciousness, and expectations toward selfies on young women’s and men’s selfie-engagement. A total of 570 young adults (66.8% females; mean age = 24.4 years, SD = 3.6) participated in an online survey study. Variables were assessed using the Pathological Narcissism Inventory (Fossati, Feeney, Pincus, Borroni, & Maffei, 2015), Objectified Body Consciousness Scale (Dakanalis et al., 2015), Selfie-expectancies Scale (Boursier & Manna, 2018), and a measure of selfie-engagement. Hierarchical regression analyses were performed on independent male and female subsamples. Results showed that body surveillance and positive selfie-expectancies are consistent selfie-behavior predictors, among both men (R2 = 0.227; p < .001) and women (R2 = 0.332; p < .001). Furthermore, findings confirm women’s involvement in appearance concerns and body-image related practices, even though men’s engagement in body-objectification deserve attention. The study provides novel findings in the field of self-objectification research as well as contributing to the ongoing debate concerning which psychological factors can be predictive of males’ and females’ selfie-engagement. The implications of these findings are also discussed in light of the debate on social media use and misuse.
1. Introduction
1.1. Selfie sharing on social media
Social media use is increasingly widespread among young people. For this reason, social networking site (SNS) use has been argued as “a way of being” (Kuss & Griffiths, 2017, p.5) even though it has the potential to provide risky opportunities, especially among teenagers and young adults (Livingstone, 2008, Munno et al., 2017). In this regard, much interest has been addressed concerning the problematic use of social media (e.g., Al-Menayes, 2015, Andreassen et al., 2016, Balakrishnan and Griffiths, 2017), thus evidencing the need to distinguish which specific activities individuals are eventually addicted to via social media use (Kuss & Griffiths, 2017). Similarly, as part of the debate on the controversial conceptual and operational definitions of behavioral addictions (Billieux et al., 2015, Griffiths, 2005, Rumpf et al., 2019, Starcevic, 2016) it has also been highlighted there is a need to identify psychological processes underlining behaviors to define them as excessive or dysfunctional (Kardefelt-Winther et al., 2017).
Nowadays, one of the most popular activities associated with social media use is selfie-sharing. Certainly, web-mediated communication platforms represent a perfect environment for socializing with the dominant forms of online content-sharing (i.e. self-images) (Dhir, Pallesen, Torsheim, & Andreassen, 2016). Indeed, it has been stated that photo-sharing positively correlates with SNS use, significantly predicted by people’s duration of SNS usage (Doğan & Adıgüzel, 2017). Additionally, the great opportunity of increasing self-disclosure (obviously also via self-images sharing) and monitoring one’s own popularity through positive feedback might trigger a behavior-reward feedback loop that serves as a basis for social media addiction (Guedes et al., 2016, Hawk et al., 2019).
In recent years, the substantial growth of social media has promoted the spread of user-generated content (i.e., selfies/video/posts/stories), increasing self-published personal information/images, and facilitating opportunities for self-promotion and attention-seeking (Weiser, 2018). According to Nadkarni and Hofmann (2012), social media use fulfils two social needs: self-presentation and the need to belong. In this regard, selfie-sharing appears to be principally associated with one of the aforementioned factors: self-presentation/promotion (Boursier and Manna, 2018, Doğan and Çolak, 2016, Reich et al., 2018, Sorokowska et al., 2016).
Recently, psychological mechanisms underlying selfie-behavior have been explored, including social pressure, attention-seeking, belonging, documenting, archiving, retaining special moments, and being creative (Bruno et al., 2018, de Vaate et al., 2018, Etgar and Amichai-Hamburger, 2017, Sung et al., 2016). Attitudes toward selfie-sharing have been analyzed among adolescents and young adults, in an attempt to estimate the key role of self-presentation and self-disclosure, as well as of self-improvement (self-esteem/self-confidence) via others’ approval (Albury, 2015, Boursier and Manna, 2018, Diefenbach and Christoforakos, 2017, Etgar and Amichai-Hamburger, 2017, Katz and Crocker, 2015, Sung et al., 2016).
Moreover, a core element included in selfie-taking, that should be considered, is personal agency comprising the photographers’ consciousness in creating, modifying, and sharing their own self-images (Lim, 2016). In this regard, increasing recent research focused on selfie-related practices such as cropping, editing, and manipulating photos before posting them on SNSs (Boursier and Manna, 2019, Chang et al., 2019, McLean et al., 2019, McLean et al., 2015). Overall selfie-behavior appears to be a complex phenomenon. From this perspective, selfie-marketing (i.e., photo preparing strategies, selfie-taking, selfie-editing, selfie-posting) and expectancies underlying selfie-posting/selfie-sharing might help to clarify quality, as well as frequency, of selfie usage (Boursier & Manna, 2018). Moreover, it has been recently evidenced that self-management utilizing selfie-posting represents a positive outcome of selfie-behavior among adolescents, despite the risk of manipulating selfies and controlling body image through self-portraits (in order to garner approval from peers) might be considered potentially dangerous (Boursier & Manna, 2019). Additionally, psychopathological factors associated with an obsessive–compulsive desire of selfie-taking have been proposed, addressing the potentially addictive nature of this behavior (Balakrishnan and Griffiths, 2017, Griffiths and Balakrishnan, 2018).
Finally, even though posting selfies has been assumed as a gendered process (Albury, 2015), typically engaging girls and women, gender-related differences associated with selfie behavior have been explored, demonstrating that males and females tend to post different selfies (Boursier and Manna, 2018, Dhir, 2016, Qiu et al., 2015, Sorokowska et al., 2016, Sorokowski et al., 2015). However, the specific use of selfie-sharing as a tool for self-presentation and self-promotion via social media has been confirmed in both males and females, also according to specific selfie-related strategies (Boursier and Manna, 2018, Dhir et al., 2016, Kim and Chock, 2017). More specifically, women’s attitude toward selfie-posting and photo-editing has been assessed (Dhir, 2016). Young women share selfies on social media in order to receive positive feedback (Nelson, 2013), and selfie-editing seems to be related to the typical young woman’s attempts to cultivate an ideal form of online self-presentation (Chae, 2017). Overall, a “selfie policy” that emphasizes selecting the ideal photo appears popular mainly among young women (Senft and Baym, 2015, Warfield, 2014). Simultaneously, young male’s involvement in photo-tagging gratifications (Dhir, 2016) and selfie-posting strategies to improve self-confidence, popularity, and specifically, sexual self-attractiveness have recently been stated (Boursier & Manna, 2018).
The widespread common habits of online self-disclosure via self-images sharing and self-improvement via online selfie-marketing strategies, in order to garner others’ approval, make selfie-engagement a matter of debate on social media use and misuse. Indeed, especially comparison-oriented people (e.g. adolescents, narcissists) appear frequently involved in selfie-editing - because of the desire for more ideal online self-presentation - and are consequently engaged in more frequent selfie-taking behavior and social media use (Chae, 2017).
1.2. Narcissism and selfies
Due to the opportunity of displaying individual grandiosity on SNSs, recent scholarly literature has increasingly focused upon narcissism and its association with social media use. Indeed, SNSs represent ideal environments to achieve narcissistic goals given the opportunity of controlling self-presentation on such platforms (Casale, Fioravanti, & Rugai, 2016a).
Narcissists particularly tend to be ‘active’ SNS users (i.e., content-creators, more engaged in posts and like/comment production, photo posting, and uploading) (Brailovskaia and Bierhoff, 2016, Davenport et al., 2014). In fact, many studies have reported a positive association between narcissism and specific SNS use including status updates or picture postings (e.g., Marshall et al., 2015, Ryan and Xenos, 2011, Buffardi and Campbell, 2008, Carpenter, 2012, DeWall et al., 2011), uploading attractive photos and promoting one’s own visual content (Mehdizadeh, 2010), photo “liking” and commenting (Panek, Nardis, & Konrath, 2013), making efforts to attract admiring friends (Davenport et al., 2014), and number of online friends and followers (Bergman et al., 2011, Buffardi and Campbell, 2008, Carpenter, 2012, Davenport et al., 2014, Panek et al., 2013).
Consequently, many studies have identified narcissism as an important predictor of selfie practices on SNSs (see Weiser, 2018 for a review; Sanecka, 2017, Sung et al., 2016, Wang et al., 2018).
Indeed, compared with low narcissistic individuals, people with high narcissism are more likely to use selfie-marketing for self-presentation and self-promotion on SNSs (Fox et al., 2018, Sanecka, 2017), to edit and post selected attractive selfies, in order to elicit positive response, and grow popularity utilizing visual-content sharing apps. Similarly, narcissists appear to perceive their selfies as more attractive than individuals with a lower level of narcissism (Moon, Lee, Lee, Choi, & Sung, 2016). Halpern, Valenzuela, and Katz (2016) suggested that selfies might have a self-reinforcement effect whereby narcissists frequently take selfies in order to maintain positive views of themselves, which in turn increases their narcissism levels. Indeed, frequent selfie-takers and heavy social media users are likely to be extravert and narcissist (Chae, 2017). It has also been shown that narcissism predicts selfie-liking among adolescents (Charoensukmongkol, 2016). Moreover, gender-related studies state that narcissism appears to significantly predict selfie-posting frequency, especially among females (Barry et al., 2017, Fox and Rooney, 2015, Lee and Sung, 2016, McCain et al., 2016, Sorokowski et al., 2015, Weiser, 2015, Weiser, 2018). More specifically, admiration demand and vanity promote increasing selfie-posting among females (Sorokowski et al., 2015).
Furthermore, unlike previous studies in this area, Etgar and Amichai-Hamburger (2017) and, more recently, Wu, Song, and Ma (2019) did not find an association between selfies and narcissism, while Arpaci (2018) observed that attitudes, intentions, narcissism, and selfie-posting behavior demonstrated mutual correlations only among young men, and surprisingly not among women. Finally, very recently Giordano et al. (2019) pointed out that high levels of narcissism are associated to more frequent selfie-related behaviors, which mediate the relationship between narcissism and problematic smartphone use, both in young men and women.
As previous literature has stated, two subtypes of narcissism appear to co-exist, characterizing distinct and separate, or fluctuating and co-occurring personality traits (Miller et al., 2018). Grandiose narcissism (or ‘overt’ narcissism) reflects grandiosity traits and it is typical of individuals who search for admiration, show high self-esteem, exhibitionism, dominance and arrogance (Miller and Campbell, 2008, Wink, 1996). Vulnerable narcissism (or ‘covert’ narcissism) characterizes individuals with low self-esteem, insecure sense of grandiosity, shame, and being hypersensitive evaluation by others (Dickinson and Pincus, 2003, Pincus and Roche, 2011).
To date, only a few studies have explored the association between different subtypes of narcissism and selfie-posting among adolescents and young adults. One recent study evidenced that higher levels of grandiose-exhibitionist narcissism and lower levels of self-esteem were associated with posting more selfies especially among females (March & McBean, 2018). Another study reported that grandiose narcissism was associated with posting more selfies and experiencing more positive affects when taking selfies (McCain et al., 2016). Conversely, the same study found that vulnerable narcissism was associated with negative affect when taking selfies.
It seems that previous studies differ in their methodologies and measures. However, even though an association between grandiose/exhibitionist tendencies and selfie-posting behaviors appears to be consistent with many findings across multiple samples (Singh, Farley, & Donahue, 2018), these results demonstrate that selfie-posting behavior is a multidimensional phenomenon, and not uniquely associated with narcissistic personality traits.
For instance, recently Barry et al. (2017) reported a significant association between some particular dimensions of narcissism and specific categories of selfies (e.g., vulnerable narcissism and physical appearance selfies), confirming the relationship between narcissism and variables concerning societal attitudes about appearance, expressed by carrying out social media-related practices (Barry et al., 2017). Finally, a cross-sectional study by Wang et al. (2018) among Chinese young adults showed the mediating role of body satisfaction between narcissism and selfie-posting, and the moderating effect of attitudes toward selfies on the relationship between body satisfaction and selfie-posting.
1.3. Body objectification on SNSs
As a result of predominantly image-based SNSs, the endorsement of photos as a medium to express one’s own identity and obtaining social approval has promoted the interest of a new research field, in which appearance evaluation and comparison, body concerns, and objectification potentially occur.
According to the objectification theory (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997), as a result of an internalization process – known as self-objectification – an outside observer’s perspective on physical selves might be assumed and internalized, together with socio-cultural body standards, that individuals could feel forced – more or less – to accomplish. McKinley and Hyde (1996) associated this experience with three specific components of objectified body consciousness (OBC): body surveillance (the individual’s constant body monitoring, due to the assumption of an outside observer’s perspective), body shame (the perceived failure in achieving ideal standards of beauty), and appearance control beliefs (personal belief of controlling one’s own bodily appearance).
Traditionally, body-objectification has been considered a gendered-process, valid and true exclusively for women in Western societies. Media exposure to cultural standards of beauty promoted objectified body images (for a review, see Grabe, Ward, & Hyde, 2008), encouraging women’s self-body objectification, in terms of body surveillance and shame (Aubrey, 2006, Fredrickson and Roberts, 1997, Meier and Gray, 2014). According to this perspective, high exposure to pictures and appearance-related conversations and comparisons on SNSs (e.g., Facebook and Instagram) are strictly related to appearance concerns and they promote self-objectification (Arroyo and Brunner, 2016, Bell et al., 2018, Cohen et al., 2018, Fardouly and Vartanian, 2015, Fardouly et al., 2015, Fardouly et al., 2018, Feltman and Szymanski, 2018, Manago et al., 2015, Meier and Gray, 2014, Trekels et al., 2018). However, the current widespread use of social media platforms for peer interactions by active users/creators of visual content (no more view-only users) provides a new and easy medium to socialize with self-objectification experiences and increase objectified body consciousness (Boursier et al., 2020, Caso et al., 2019, de Vries and Peter, 2013, Manago et al., 2015, Ramsey and Horan, 2018), particularly relying upon women’s body dissatisfaction (Casale, Gemelli, Calosi, Giangrasso, & Fioravanti, 2019).
Nevertheless, on SNS profiles, individuals habitually appear to look at themselves from an observer’s perspective (Fardouly et al., 2015). Consequently, body-objectification is now becoming prevalent among male as well as female active social media users (e.g., Dakanalis et al., 2015, Holland and Tiggemann, 2016, Karsay et al., 2018, Manago et al., 2015, Moradi, 2010, Vandenbosch and Eggermont, 2012).
Furthermore, social media use could be a pivotal catalyst for appearance concerns, likely leading to appearance-related activities, such as selfie-sharing and selfie-editing (Brown and Tiggemann, 2016, Cohen et al., 2017, Doğan and Çolak, 2016, Holland and Tiggemann, 2016, Mills et al., 2018).
From this perspective, on the one hand, many empirical studies have confirmed the problematic close relationship between body image management and SNS use (Manago et al., 2015, Moya-Garofano and Moya, 2019, Slater and Tiggemann, 2015, Tiggemann & Slater, 2013, Tiggemann and Slater, 2015, Kuss and Griffiths, 2017; Salomon & Brown, 2019). On the other hand, many scholars have investigated selfie-behavior on SNSs in relation to body image and appearance preoccupations (Boursier and Manna, 2019, Gilliland et al., 2018, Mills et al., 2018, Seyfi and Arpacı, 2016, Shin et al., 2017, Veldhuis et al., 2018).
Recently it has been reported that higher frequency of posting objectified self-images might be associated with trait self-objectification and receiving more likes in young adult women (Bell et al., 2018). Furthermore, surveillance has been evidenced as a moderator of the relationship between photo investment and body dissatisfaction in young women (Cohen et al., 2018). Chang et al. (2019) pointed out that selfie-posting has a direct and positive association with body esteem among adolescents. Moreover, it has been evidenced that positive feedback and body satisfaction mediate the relationship between selfie-posting and self-esteem among females (Wang et al., 2018). A few studies have shown that greater selfie-posting behavior is associated with greater body satisfaction especially among females (Cohen et al., 2018, Ridgway and Clayton, 2016). However, it has also been reported that selfie-investment and manipulation are related to body dissatisfaction among both males and females (Lonergan et al., 2019). Previously, McLean et al. (2015) also found high selfie-investment and manipulation, especially among adolescent girls dissatisfied with their own body appearance.
In summary, the empirical evidence suggests there is a clear association between selfie-posting and body-esteem/satisfaction, often influenced by others’ approval and comparisons via social networking sites. However, only a few studies have analyzed the specific impact of OBC on active SNS users (i.e., Boursier et al., 2020, Veldhuis et al., 2018, Lamp et al., 2019, Zheng et al., 2019). More specifically, Zheng et al. (2019) observed that self-objectification predicted selfie-posting especially among girls with higher levels of imaginary audience ideation, highlighting the pivotal role of an internalized observer’s view. Veldhuis et al. (2018) noted the predictive role of body surveillance on greater engagement in selfie-related activities on SNSs, especially for young women. Lamp et al. (2019) reported that body surveillance highly affected selfie-frequency and photo manipulation among women. Finally, Boursier et al. (2020) evidenced the mediating effect of body image control in photos on the relationship between body appearance control beliefs and SNS problematic use in girls.
1.4. The present study
Previous literature has demonstrated an association between narcissism and body image concerns, body-objectification and SNS use, narcissism and selfie-posting behavior, and more recently between body surveillance and selfie-posting. Moreover, different findings suggested addressing attention on gender differences when focusing on body objectification and selfie-behavior. However, no previous studies have explored the combined effect of narcissism, objectified body consciousness, and expectancies toward selfies upon individual’s selfie-behavior, comparing the influence of these three factors. In light of this, the present study evaluated the predictive role of these components on young women’s and men’s selfie-engagement, hypothesizing that higher selfie-engagement could be predicted by higher (grandiose/vulnerable) narcissism, objectified body consciousness and positive expectations toward selfies. Moreover, considering men’s and women’s different engagement in selfie-sharing and body objectification, the role of these components was explored among different male and female samples, expecting different patterns. Indeed, consistent with female’s typical involvement in body appearance concerns and related activities, it has been expected that narcissistic traits (particularly vulnerable narcissism), combined with higher body surveillance and positive expectancies of self-improvement through selfie-sharing could predict selfie-engagement, especially in women. Additionally, due to the interest recently addressed concerning male body objectification, the predictive role of these components was also explored specifically on men’s selfie-engagement.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants and procedure
Data were collected via an online survey. Participant recruitment was carried out by advertisements placed on Italian university web communities visited by many undergraduate students. The call for participation in the online study contained a website link that participants had to click on to complete the questionnaire. A total of 570 participants (mean age = 24.4 years, SD = 3.60), comprising 189 males (33.2%) and 381 females (66.8%) took part in an online survey study. Before filling out the online questionnaire, all participants were informed about the nature of the research and the measures to be used in generating the data. General information about the aim of the study was also declared before starting the survey. Participation was voluntary, confidentiality and anonymity were assured, and all participants were informed that they could withdraw from the study at any time. No course credits or remunerative rewards were given. The study was approved by the research team’s University Research Ethics Committees and was conducted according to the ethical guidelines for psychological research laid down by the Italian Psychological Association (AIP).
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI)
The Italian version of the PNI (Fossati, Feeney, Pincus, Borroni, & Maffei, 2015; original English version by Pincus, 2013, Pincus et al., 2009) was used to assess overt and covert characteristics of grandiose and vulnerable narcissism. The PNI is a 52-item scale rated on a six-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all like me) to 6 (very much like me). The PNI consists of seven first-order scales labeled: contingent self-esteem (e.g., “It’s hard to feel good about myself unless I know other people admire me”), exploitativeness (e.g., “I find it easy to manipulate people”), self-sacrificing self-enhancement (e.g., “I try to show what a good person I am through my sacrifices”), hiding the self (e.g., “I often hide my needs for fear that others will see me as needy and dependent”), grandiose fantasy (e.g., “I often fantasize about performing heroic deeds”), devaluing (e.g., “Sometimes I avoid people because I’m concerned that they’ll disappoint me”), and entitlement rage (e.g., “I typically get very angry when I’m unable to get what I want from others”). Moreover, the PNI yields two second-order scales: narcissistic vulnerability (obtained from the average score of contingent self-esteem, hiding the self, devaluing, and entitlement rage) and narcissistic grandiosity (obtained from the average score of exploitativeness, self-sacrificing self-enhancement, and grandiose fantasy). In the present study, Cronbach’s alpha values of the first-order scales were very good and ranged from 0.76 (exploitativeness) to 0.92 (contingent self-esteem). The Cronbach's alphas for grandiose narcissism and vulnerable narcissism were 0.66 and 0.83 respectively.
2.2.2. Objectified Body Consciousness Scale (OBCS)
The 24-item Italian version of the OBCS (Dakanalis et al., 2015; original English version by McKinley & Hyde, 1996) was used. This scale comprises three eight-item subscales that assess body surveillance (e.g., “I often worry about whether the clothes I am wearing make me look good”), body shame (e.g., “I feel ashamed of myself when I haven’t made the effort to look my best”), and appearance control beliefs (e.g., “I think a person can look pretty much how they want to if they are willing to work at it”). Participants reported their agreement with items on a 7-point scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Appropriate items were reverse-coded. In the present study, Cronbach’s alpha values were 0.76 for body surveillance, 0.85 for body shame, and 0.75 for appearance control beliefs.
2.2.3. Selfie-Expectancies Scale (SES)
The 23-item SES (Boursier & Manna, 2018) assesses positive and negative expectancies concerning selfie-behavior. The scale comprises seven different factors: relational worries (e.g., “How much selfie-taking might damage your reputation?”), internet-related anxieties (e.g., “How much selfie-taking might worry you because your photos/identity could be stolen?”), sexual desire (e.g., “How much selfie-taking improves your sexual fantasies?”), ordinary practice (e.g., “How much selfie-taking is a habit?”), self-confidence (e.g., “How much selfie-taking improves your self-esteem?”), self-presentation (e.g., “How much selfie-taking is a way to show to the others the best part of you?”), and generalized risks (e.g., “How much selfie-taking might cause you problems in the future?”). Each item is answered on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (totally disagree) to 5 (totally agree). In the present study, the Cronbach’s α values for each SES subscale ranged from 0.65 (sexual desire) to 0.91 (internet-related anxieties).
2.2.4. Selfie engagement
According to earlier studies and matters arising from focus groups on selfie-taking and selfie-sharing behaviors, previously conducted in different contexts (Boursier & Manna, 2018), a measure was developed to assess practices of sharing selfies. Participants were asked to respond to five self-report items, directed to assess their selfie-engagement, in terms of concern and time spent for posting and choosing selfies to share on SNSs. More specifically, two items were adapted from the Selfie Frequency Scale (Boursier and Manna, 2018, Manna and Boursier, 2017) and assessed the frequency to which participants share selfies on their SNS profile (“How many selfies do you share on social networking sites?”) or send them via chat (“How many selfies do you share in chats (for example in WhatsApp chat-rooms or Instagram Direct)?”), rated from 1 (less than once a month) to 8 (more than twice a day). Considering the pivotal role of self-presentation and positive feedback (such as “likes”) in selfie practice (Boursier & Manna, 2018), two items explored how often participants used a selfie as SNS profile image (“How often your profile pictures on social networking sites are selfies?”) and how often they used a selfie that gets many “likes” as their SNS profile image (“How often do you use a selfie that received many likes as profile pictures on social networking sites are selfies?”). Finally, considering the pivotal role of selfie-related behaviors before sharing photos on SNSs (Boursier and Manna, 2019, de Vaate et al., 2018, McLean et al., 2015), one item evaluated how often participants took multiple selfies to share the best one on SNSs (“How often do you take more selfies to choose the best one to share on social networking sites?”). These three items were rated from 1 (never) to 5 (always). Standardized measures were used. The measure was observed to have adequate internal consistency in the present study (α = 0.70).
2.3. Statistical analysis
Descriptive statistics and Pearson’s correlations between the study variables were performed. Independent t-tests were used to assess gender differences, and the magnitudes of the differences were evaluated utilizing effect sizes (Cohen’s d). Hierarchical regression analyses were performed to explore the predictive effect of narcissistic vulnerability, narcissistic grandiosity, objectified body consciousness, and selfie-expectancies dimensions on selfie-engagement, for each gender. All statistical analyses were performed using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences SPSS (Version 23 for Windows).
3. Results
Descriptive analyses were performed, and gender differences with related effect sizes were calculated (Table 1). As shown in Table 1, compared to males, females reported statistically significant higher scores in OBC body surveillance, OBC body shame, SES internet-related anxieties, SES self-presentation, and in selfie engagement. Males had higher scores on narcissistic grandiosity. Zero-order correlations of the study variables are shown in Table 2.
Table 1.
Total sample | Males | Females | t | d | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
PNI narcissistic vulnerability | 3.158 (0.878) | 3.149 (0.921) | 3.162 (0.857) | .165n.s. | 0.01 |
PNI narcissistic grandiosity | 3.574 (0.790) | 3.689 (0.852) | 3.518 (0.753) | 2.443* | 0.22 |
OBCS body surveillance | 4.311 (1.037) | 4.085 (1.097) | 4.422 (0.988) | 3.693*** | 0.33 |
OBCS body shame | 3.57 (1.369) | 3.309 (1.270) | 3.699 (1.398) | 3.229** | 0.29 |
OBCS appearance control beliefs | 4.951 (0.967) | 5.044 (0.987) | 4.905 (0.956) | 1.615n.s. | 0.14 |
SES relational worries | 2.282 (1.077) | 2.299 (1.128) | 2.273 (1.052) | .271n.s. | 0.02 |
SES web-related anxieties | 3.308 (1.312 | 3.019 (1.371) | 3.451 (1.259) | 3.743*** | 0.33 |
SES sexual desire | 1.604 (0.638) | 1.661 (0.662) | 1.576 (0.625) | 1.511n.s. | 0.13 |
SES ordinary practice | 3.751 (0.921) | 3.665 (0.967) | 3.794 (0.896) | 1.571n.s. | 0.14 |
SES self-confidence | 2.186 (1.017) | 2.073 (0.997) | 2.242 (1.024) | 1.868n.s. | 0.17 |
SES self-presentation | 2.801 (0.951) | 2.63 (0.964) | 2.885 (0.934) | 3.045** | 0.27 |
SES generalized risks | 2.664 (0.902) | 2.568 (0.903) | 2.711 (0.898) | 1.791n.s. | 0.16 |
Selfie engagement | 0.000 (0.667) | -0.217 (0.624) | 0.107 (0.663) | 5.599*** | 0.50 |
Note. PNI: Pathological Narcissism Inventory; OBCS: Objectified Body Consciousness Scale; SES: Selfie Expectancies Scale.
*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001; n.s. = non-significant.
Table 2.
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | 13 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 PNI narcissistic vulnerability | – | 0.628** | 0.310** | 0.521** | −0.367** | 0.271** | 0.144** | 0.278** | 0.151** | 0.408** | 0.270** | 0.106* | 0.252** | (0.012)a |
2 PNI narcissistic grandiosity | 0.685** | – | 0.195** | 0.331** | −0.224** | 0.169** | 0.146** | 0.243** | 0.085 | 0.369** | 0.275** | 0.120* | 0.246** | (0.057)a |
3 OBCS body surveillance | 0.209** | 0.193** | – | 0.564** | −0.131* | 0.142** | 0.047 | 0.124* | 0.017 | 0.196** | 0.153** | −0.027 | 0.281** | (0.185)a |
4 OBCS body shame | 0.515** | 0.286** | 0.415** | – | −0.383** | 0.185** | 0.066 | 0.164** | 0.065 | 0.270** | 0.202** | 0.041 | 0.198** | (−0.046)a |
5 OBCS appearance control beliefs | −0.346** | −0.107 | 0.006 | −0.354** | – | −0.200** | −0.004 | −0.093 | −0.088 | −0.215** | −0.068 | −0.046 | −0.061 | (0.039)a |
6 SES relational worries | 0.407** | 0.247** | 0.143* | 0.298** | −0.218** | – | 0.346** | 0.196** | 0.161** | 0.218** | 0.282** | 0.467** | 0.107* | (0.041)a |
7 SES web-related anxieties | 0.340** | 0.222** | −0.042 | 0.201** | −0.226** | 0.448** | – | 0.026 | 0.020 | 0.125* | 0.526** | 0.415** | 0.088 | (−0.019)a |
8 SES sexual desire | 0.266** | 0.201** | 0.120 | 0.182* | −0.038 | 0.313** | 0.192** | – | 0.249** | 0.389** | 0.278** | 0.094 | 0.332** | (0.185)a |
9 SES ordinary practice | 0.114 | 0.101 | 0.149* | 0.102 | 0.032 | 0.168* | 0.104 | 0.374** | – | 0.264** | 0.171** | 0.099 | 0.082 | (−0.058)a |
10 SES self-confidence | 0.334** | 0.311** | 0.206** | 0.165* | −0.069 | 0.221** | 0.148* | 0.410** | 0.335** | – | 0.596** | 0.072 | 0.450** | (0.179)a |
11 SES self-presentation | 0.366** | 0.287** | 0.032 | 0.169* | −0.190** | 0.407** | 0.599** | 0.450** | 0.344** | 0.530** | – | 0.225** | 0.407** | (0.195)a |
12 SES generalized risks | 0.106 | −0.025 | −0.091 | 0.033 | −0.155* | 0.525** | 0.489** | 0.096 | −0.001 | −0.032 | 0.207** | – | −0.105* | (−0.197)a |
13 Selfie engagement | 0.221** | 0.252** | 0.303** | 0.215** | 0.013 | 0.213** | 0.129 | 0.331** | 0.238** | 0.389** | 0.357** | −0.007 | – | |
(−0.056)a | (0.094)a | (0.196)a | (0.069)a | (0.079)a | (0.045)a | (−0.041)a | (0.115)a | (0.021)a | (0.138)a | (0.158)a | (−0.020)a |
Note. PNI: Pathological Narcissism Inventory; OBCS: Objectified Body Consciousness Scale; SES: Selfie Expectancies Scale.
** p = .01; * p = .05; a Partial correlation controlling for Selfie engagement.
Before running the hierarchical regressions, multicollinearity was checked. There was no indication of multicollinearity (Table 3), as tolerance statistics were above 0.2 and variance influence factors were well below 10 (Bowerman & O’Connell, 1990). The hierarchical regressions for both men and women are shown in Table 3. In the female sample, both narcissistic vulnerability and narcissistic grandiosity were significant in the first step. After adding OBCS, body surveillance appeared a significant predictor of selfie-engagement, narcissistic vulnerability did not remain a significant predictor, and narcissistic grandiosity was still a significant predictor. In the third step, adding SES, sexual desire, self-confidence, self-presentation, and generalized risks were significant predictors. Body surveillance was still a significant predictor, but narcissistic grandiosity did not remain a significant predictor. The final model accounted for 33.2% of the variance (F(7,368) = 16.447; p < .001). For the male sample, in the first step, narcissistic vulnerability and narcissistic grandiosity were not significant. In the second step, body surveillance was a significant predictor. Finally, adding SES in the third step, only self-presentation was significant, and body surveillance remained a significant predictor. The final model accounted for 27.7% of the variance in males’ selfie engagement (F(7,176) = 4.811; p < .001).
Table 3.
Collinearity Statistics |
||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Females | B | SE | Β | t | Sign. | R2 | AdjR2 | SE | R2 Change | F(dfn,dfd) | p | Tolerance | VIF | |
Step 1 | PNI narcissistic vulnerability | 0.124 | 0.049 | 0.160 | 2.521 | <0.05 | 0.076 | 0.071 | 0.639 | 0.076 | 15.560(2,378) | <0.001 | 0.605 | 1.652 |
PNI narcissistic grandiosity | 0.128 | 0.056 | 0.145 | 2.288 | <0.05 | 0.605 | 1.652 | |||||||
Step 2 | PNI narcissistic vulnerability | 0.089 | 0.054 | 0.115 | 1.651 | 0.100 | 0.124 | 0.112 | 0.625 | 0.047 | 6.773(3,375) | <0.001 | 0.478 | 2.092 |
PNI narcissistic grandiosity | 0.128 | 0.055 | 0.145 | 2.334 | <0.05 | 0.605 | 1.652 | |||||||
OBCS body surveillance | 0.161 | 0.040 | 0.240 | 4.064 | <0.001 | 0.672 | 1.488 | |||||||
OBCS body shame | −0.016 | 0.032 | −0.033 | −0.483 | 0.629 | 0.510 | 1.960 | |||||||
OBCS appearance control beliefs | 0.023 | 0.037 | 0.033 | 0.614 | 0.540 | 0.803 | 1.245 | |||||||
Step 3 | PNI narcissistic vulnerability | 0.011 | 0.049 | 0.014 | 0.221 | 0.825 | 0.332 | 0.311 | 0.55 | 0.209 | 16.447(7,368) | <0.001 | 0.449 | 2.227 |
PNI narcissistic grandiosity | 0.054 | 0.049 | 0.061 | 1.093 | 0.275 | 0.580 | 1.724 | |||||||
OBCS body surveillance | 0.127 | 0.035 | 0.189 | 3.605 | <0.001 | 0.660 | 1.514 | |||||||
OBCS body shame | −0.025 | 0.028 | −0.053 | −0.893 | 0.372 | 0.508 | 1.970 | |||||||
OBCS appearance control beliefs | 0.025 | 0.934 | 0.036 | 0.740 | 0.460 | 0.776 | 1.289 | |||||||
SES relational worries | 0.025 | 0.033 | 0.040 | 0.778 | 0.437 | 0.673 | 1.486 | |||||||
SES web-related anxieties | −0.011 | 0.030 | −0.021 | −0.368 | 0.713 | 0.560 | 1.785 | |||||||
SES sexual desire | 0.184 | 0.051 | 0.174 | 3.618 | <0.001 | 0.789 | 1.268 | |||||||
SES ordinary practice | −0.037 | 0.033 | −0.050 | −1.106 | 0.270 | 0.887 | 1.127 | |||||||
SES self-confidence | 0.138 | 0.040 | 0.213 | 3.483 | <0.01 | 0.486 | 2.057 | |||||||
SES self-presentation | 0.176 | 0.046 | 0.248 | 3.813 | <0.001 | 0.428 | 2.339 | |||||||
SES generalized risks | −0.146 | 0.038 | −0.197 | −3.859 | <0.001 | 0.693 | 1.443 |
Collinearity Statistics |
||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Males | B | SE | β | t | Sign. | R2 | AdjR2 | SE | R2 Change | F(dfn,dfd) | p | Tolerance | VIF | |
Step 1 | PNI narcissistic vulnerability | 0.063 | 0.066 | 0.092 | 0.951 | 0.343 | 0.068 | 0.058 | 0.606 | 0.068 | 6.777(2,186) | <0.01 | 0.531 | 1.884 |
PNI narcissistic grandiosity | 0.138 | 0.071 | 0.189 | 1.940 | 0.054 | 0.531 | 1.884 | |||||||
Step2 | PNI narcissistic vulnerability | 0.035 | 0.075 | 0.052 | 0.475 | 0.636 | 0.138 | 0.115 | 0.587 | 0.070 | 4.965(3,183) | <0.01 | 0.390 | 2.565 |
PNI narcissistic grandiosity | 0.116 | 0.071 | 0.159 | 1.642 | 0.102 | 0.505 | 1.979 | |||||||
OBCS body surveillance | 0.131 | 0.044 | 0.231 | 3.000 | <0.01 | 0.794 | 1.259 | |||||||
OBCS body shame | 0.036 | 0.044 | 0.073 | 0.810 | 0.419 | 0.584 | 1.713 | |||||||
OBCS appearance control beliefs | 0.046 | 0.049 | 0.072 | 0.936 | 0.351 | 0.791 | 1.264 | |||||||
Step 3 | PNI narcissistic vulnerability | −0.054 | 0.073 | −0.080 | −0.742 | 0.459 | 0.277 | 0.227 | 0.549 | 0.138 | 4.811(7,176) | <0.001 | 0.356 | 2.810 |
PNI narcissistic grandiosity | 0.084 | 0.067 | 0.115 | 1.259 | 0.210 | 0.491 | 2.035 | |||||||
OBCS body surveillance | 0.112 | 0.042 | 0.197 | 2.652 | <0.01 | 0.747 | 1.338 | |||||||
OBCS body shame | 0.038 | 0.042 | 0.078 | 0.918 | 0.360 | 0.567 | 1.763 | |||||||
OBCS appearance control beliefs | 0.048 | 0.046 | 0.077 | 1.047 | 0.297 | 0.767 | 1.304 | |||||||
SES relational worries | 0.029 | 0.049 | 0.053 | 0.599 | 0.550 | 0.531 | 1.884 | |||||||
SES web-related anxieties | −0.024 | 0.044 | −0.052 | −0.549 | 0.584 | 0.449 | 2.225 | |||||||
SES sexual desire | 0.113 | 0.073 | 0.119 | 1.540 | 0.125 | 0.684 | 1.461 | |||||||
SES ordinary practice | 0.013 | 0.047 | 0.020 | 0.281 | 0.779 | 0.783 | 1.277 | |||||||
SES self-confidence | 0.097 | 0.052 | 0.154 | 1.848 | 0.066 | 0.588 | 1.701 | |||||||
SES self-presentation | 0.143 | 0.067 | 0.220 | 2.127 | <0.05 | 0.383 | 2.614 | |||||||
SES generalized risks | −0.015 | 0.059 | −0.022 | −0.262 | 0.794 | 0.563 | 1.775 |
Note. PNI: Pathological Narcissism Inventory; OBCS: Objectified Body Consciousness Scale; SES: Selfie Expectancies Scale.
4. Discussion
The present study surveyed a specific sample of Italian young women and men and tested a hierarchical regression model to explore the predictive role of vulnerable/grandiose narcissism, objectified body consciousness, and expectancies toward selfies on males’ and females’ selfie-engagement. Consistent with literature, results showed that women are more involved in selfie-posting behavior (Albury, 2015, Dhir et al., 2016). Moreover, results aligned with the female’s involvement in experiences of body-objectification, even though the small difference between males’ and females’ body surveillance and body shame scores highlighted increasing self-objectification processes among males (Vandenbosch & Eggermont, 2013). Finally, partially in line with previous studies on Italian samples, the present findings showed higher male overt narcissism and inclination for grandiosity, but not a higher female covert predisposition (Casale et al., 2016a, Casale et al., 2016b). This study’s findings showed a high correlation among the variables considered. In particular, the experience of body shame and body surveillance, due to the interiorization of an observer’s point of view, and appeared to be related to narcissistic personality traits, especially in hypersensitive women. This result is clearly in line with the description of individuals with vulnerable narcissistic traits, characterized by low self-esteem, shame, and hypersensitivity to evaluation by others (Dickinson and Pincus, 2003, Pincus and Roche, 2011). Furthermore, a strong correlation appeared between narcissism and “positive” expectancies toward selfies (self-confidence, self-presentation, sexual desire) in both males and females. In other words, men and women with vulnerable/grandiose narcissistic traits seem to share their body images through selfie-posting and expect an improvement in their self-confidence. Finally, in the present study, males’ and females’ selfie-engagement results particularly related to positive expectations (an increase of self-confidence and self-presentation via selfie-posting), thus confirming the role performed by the expectancies in this practice (Boursier & Manna, 2018).
Concerning the regression model, findings partially confirmed the hypothesis. Body surveillance and positive selfie-expectancies have been evidenced as clear and consistent selfie predictors. On the contrary, pathological narcissism had no predictive effect on selfie-engagement, when compared to expectations underlying selfie activities, and objectified body consciousness. Moreover, in terms of gender, no great differences were found. Men’s and women’s selfie-engagement was similarly predicted by body surveillance and positive selfie-expectancies. However, as hypothesized, higher selfie-engagement was predicted by higher body surveillance and positive expectations toward self-improvement via selfie-sharing, particularly among women. On the contrary, no influence was evidenced for pathological vulnerable narcissistic traits as it was expected in the female sample. Furthermore, paying attention to gender peculiarities, specific women’s expectancies that predicted selfie-engagement have been evidenced and discussed.
These results contribute to the ongoing controversial debate on whether and how personality traits influence selfie-posting (Etgar and Amichai-Hamburger, 2017, Wu et al., 2019), also supporting the analysis of the interconnection among different aspects (Arpaci, 2018, Wang et al., 2018). Among the explored factors, the role of body image appeared to be extremely significant, together with people’s expectations, highlighting the implication of the photographer’s personal agency in selfie-related behaviors.
According to recent findings (Veldhuis et al., 2018, Lamp et al., 2019, Zheng et al., 2019), the internalization of an observer’s view on body appearance (i.e., body surveillance) might play a pivotal and arguable role especially in women’s selfie-engagement, but not exclusively on them. This finding confirms the expected greater women’s involvement in appearance concerns and body-image related practices, even though men’s results deserve attention. Indeed, in the present study, men also appeared to feel pressure on their body appearance. Interestingly, males’ body surveillance alone accounted for 13.8% of the variance in predicting selfie-engagement. According to recent findings (Daniel and Bridges, 2010, Karsay et al., 2018, Manago et al., 2015, Moradi, 2010, Vandenbosch and Eggermont, 2013), social and cultural stereotypes concerning beautiful and performing bodies, globally shared and reinforced through social media content creation, influenced heavily both females’ and males’ online behavior.
Finally, the role of positive outcome expectancies in addressing and reinforcing individuals’ behavior (Patrick & Maggs, 2009) appears to be in line with literature on internet-related practices (Brand, Laier, & Young, 2014), and with previous studies concerning expectancies underlying selfie-behavior (Boursier & Manna, 2018). Believing that selfie-posting could be a useful tool for self-presentation likely leads people to online, more or less authentic, self-disclosure (Christoforakos and Diefenbach, 2016, Nguyen and Barbour, 2017, Warfield, 2014, Williamson et al., 2017). Similarly, a potentially large audience can be ready to approve (or dislike) individual’s images shared online, and this belief could reinforce the expectation of self-confidence promotion via selfie-engagement (Taylor, Hinck, & Lim, 2017).
According to Boursier and Manna (2018), self-presentation and self-confidence are viewed as basic expectations that frequently lead boys and girls to selfie-posting. Selfies are used as self-presentation tools, despite the potentially generalized risks related to online photo-sharing, which exclusively characterized girl’s worries. This contradictory behavior remains a controversial issue, and previously discussed in relation to the “privacy paradox” (Barnes, 2006) specifically observed in females, whereby despite declared privacy concerns, women do not decrease their selfie-sharing activities (Dhir, Torsheim, Pallesen, & Andreassen, 2017). The present study findings seemed to confirm this paradox, because women’s selfie-engagement was predicted by positive expectations, notwithstanding the perceived potential risk due to self-images sharing. Moreover, differently from previous results on adolescents (Boursier & Manna, 2018), in the present study, the expectation of increasing self-confidence and sexual desire characterized only women’s selfie-engagement. This result seems to entail and reinforce females’ predisposition to body-objectification (i.e., women’s body as an object of desire), need for appearance reassurance, and searching for “likes” (Bell et al., 2018). However, selfie-sharing activities seem to promote women’s expectation of increasing personal excitement and sexual fantasies, also showing the women’s desire dimension (i.e. subject, not only object, of sexual desire) which deserves attention and further exploration.
These findings, reinforced by the coexistent role of body surveillance, appear to strengthen the pervasive influence of body objectification on this internet-related practice. Nevertheless, it remains a critical issue relating to self-image sharing on SNSs, whether individuals can share perfect body images, potentially manipulated in order to achieve approval and popularity, promoting their need for narcissistic admiration (Casale et al., 2016b, Casale et al., 2018, Chae, 2017).
In conclusion, the present study’s findings provided some novel observations. Overall, they highlighted the pivotal influence of self-objectification on women’s and men’s social media use, and specifically on self-image sharing via selfie-posting behavior. This result supports the need to take into account the widespread (global) diffusion and internalization of a body image web-culture among young women and men. However, a specific interest should be addressed in the male population, whose behavior has been traditionally less studied on this topic. Further research could examine male populations to delineate whether and which risky factors are displayed and associated with body surveillance and social media use.
The present study’s findings demonstrated new insights into individuals’ selfie-behavior contributing to the ongoing debate concerning the psychological and psychopathological facets of internet-related practices. Therefore, the specific key role played by individuals’ body appearance and body images-sharing via social media – a main role compared to personality traits, as this study evidenced – deserves further empirical attention. Indeed, as stated previously, visual content on SNS platforms could potentially have stronger effects on body image concerns due to their central focus on image sharing (Fardouly & Vartanian, 2016). Moreover, the great visual attention directed towards body appearance might trigger behaviors such as body image control and selfie-marketing, potentially related to self-objectification (McLean et al., 2019) and maladaptive use of digital tools (Giordano et al., 2019). In this regard, selfie-behavior might be considered dysfunctional when related to an objectified use of body images via social media, which could reinforce individuals’ body concerns and lead to a problematic social media use (Boursier et al., 2020). For instance, people’s expectancies underlying selfie behavior and people’s higher engagement in selfie-taking, selfie-editing (i.e., photo-manipulation), and selfie-sharing might involve women and men in a dysfunctional use of social platforms (Wang, Xie, Fardouly, Vartanian, & Lei, 2019), reinforcing an (appearance-related) behavior-reward feedback loop (Hawk et al., 2019). Preliminary results on this issue have been previously discussed (Boursier and Gioia, 2019a, Boursier and Gioia, 2019b). However, further research on this interesting topic is needed. Therefore, in this regard, these findings might contribute to the debate on which specific psychological processes underlining people’s activities allow to differentiate between common and dysfunctional (eventually excessive) behaviors (Billieux et al., 2015, Kardefelt-Winther et al., 2017).
Finally, the present study partially contributes to the need for discussing self-presentational concerns in models of narcissistic personality (Casale et al., 2016b).
Some limitations of the present study also need to be addressed when interpreting the findings. Firstly, the study used a self-report survey and its potential biases are well-known. Secondly, the cross-sectional nature of the study and specific geographic area of the sample limit the ability to formally test causality of the data. Furthermore, the participants were not gender-balanced (with significantly more females participating). Finally, other aspects could have been explored alongside the variables investigated here. For example, additional investigations are needed to evaluate the specific role of photo-manipulation practices in body objectification, body satisfaction, and selfie-engagement, in male as well as in female samples. Additionally, personal agency entailing the photographer’s consciousness in selfie-marketing for self-promotion (Chang et al., 2019, Lim, 2016) deserves great attention because it leads to the potential risk of self-falsification. Moreover, within the whole complexity of selfie-behavior, it would be interesting to more deeply explore psychological and psychopathological factors associated with specific typologies of selfies (Barry et al., 2017). Furthermore, different selfie usages should be identified, in order to distinguish between common internet-related practices and problematic/addictive behaviors. Finally, the findings have clinical implications because they clearly show the need for a broader focus on body concerns, since the use of body images appear to be pivotal in social media-related practices and content, among women as well among men The aforementioned dangerous opportunity of self-falsification, by manipulating personal images in order to achieve others’ approval, shows potential risks for males’ and females’ identity construction in young adulthood.
CRediT authorship contribution statement
Valentina Boursier: Conceptualization, Methodology, Investigation, Writing - original draft preparation, Project administration. Francesca Gioia: Investigation, Formal analysis. Mark D. Griffiths: Validation, Writing - review & editing.
Declaration of Competing Interest
In the present study there were no funding sources neither conflict of interest.
Contributor Information
Valentina Boursier, Email: valentina.boursier@unina.it.
Francesca Gioia, Email: francesca.gioia@unina.it.
Mark D. Griffiths, Email: mark.griffiths@ntu.ac.uk.
References
- Albury K. Selfies | selfies, sexts and sneaky hats: Young people's understandings of gendered practices of self-representation. International Journal of Communication. 2015;9(12):1734–1745. [Google Scholar]
- Al-Menayes J.J. Dimensions of social media addiction among university students in Kuwait. Psychology and Behavioral Sciences. 2015;4(1):23–28. [Google Scholar]
- Andreassen C.S., Billieux J., Griffiths M.D., Kuss D.J., Demetrovics Z., Mazzoni E., Pallesen S. The relationship between addictive use of social media and video games and symptoms of psychiatric disorders: A large-scale cross-sectional study. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2016;30(2):252–262. doi: 10.1037/adb0000160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arpaci I. The moderating effect of gender in the relationship between narcissism and selfie-posting behavior. Personality and Individual Differences. 2018;134:71–74. doi: 10.1016/j.pad.2018.06.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Arroyo A., Brunner S.R. Negative body talk as an outcome of friends’ fitness posts on social networking sites: Body surveillance and social comparison as potential moderators. Journal of Applied Communication Research. 2016;44(3):216–235. [Google Scholar]
- Aubrey J.S. Exposure to sexually objectifying media and body self-perceptions among college women: An examination of the selective exposure hypothesis and the role of moderating variables. Sex Roles. 2006;55(3–4):159–172. [Google Scholar]
- Balakrishnan J., Griffiths M.D. Social media addiction: What is the role of content in YouTube? Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2017;6:364–377. doi: 10.1556/2006.6.2017.058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes, S. B. (2006). A privacy paradox: Social networking in the United States. First Monday, 11(9). Retrieved November 25, 2019, from: https://firstmonday.org/ojs/index.php/fm/article/view/1394.
- Barry C.T., Doucette H., Loflin D.C., Rivera-Hudson N., Herrington L.L. Let me take a selfie: Associations between self-photography, narcissism, and self-esteem. Psychology of Popular Media Culture. 2017;6:48–60. [Google Scholar]
- Bell B.T., Cassarly J.A., Dunbar L. Selfie-objectification: Self-objectification and positive feedback (“likes”) are associated with frequency of posting sexually objectifying self-images on social media. Body Image. 2018;26:83–89. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2018.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bergman S.M., Fearrington M.E., Davenport S.W., Bergman J.Z. Millennials, narcissism, and social networking: What narcissists do on social networking sites and why. Personality and Individual Differences. 2011;50(5):706–711. [Google Scholar]
- Billieux J., Schimmenti A., Khazaal Y., Maurage P., Heeren A. Are we overpathologizing everyday life? A tenable blueprint for behavioral addiction research. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2015;4(3):119–123. doi: 10.1556/2006.4.2015.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boursier, V., & Gioia, F. (2019a) Controllo dell'immagine e del corpo nei ragazzi. Fattori predittivi e protettivi dell'uso problematico dei social network - Boys' body image control. Predictive and protective factors of problematic Social Network sites use. Oral communication at XIII Congresso Nazionale Associazione SIPSA Società Italiana di Psicologia della Salute, Florence, Italy.
- Boursier, V., & Gioia, F. (2019b) The predicting role of body shame and body control beliefs on teens’ problematic social network sites use. Oral communication at XXI Congresso Nazionale della Sezione di Psicologia Clinica e Dinamica, Milan, Italy.
- Boursier V., Manna V. Selfie expectancies among adolescents. Construction and validation of an instrument to assess expectancies toward selfies among boys and girls. Frontiers in Psychology. 2018;9:839. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00839. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boursier V., Manna V. Intimacy and developing personal relationships in the virtual world. Hershey, PA; IGI Global: 2019. Relational body identities: Body image control through self-portraits –A revision of the Body Image Control in Photos Questionnaire; pp. 40–63. [Google Scholar]
- Boursier V., Gioia F., Griffiths M.D. Objectified body consciousness, Body image control in photos, and problematic social networking: The role of appearance control beliefs. Frontiers in Psychology. 2020;10 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowerman B.L., O'connell R.T. Brooks/Cole; 1990. Linear statistical models: An applied approach. [Google Scholar]
- Brand M., Laier C., Young K.S. Internet addiction: Coping styles, expectancies, and treatment implications. Frontiers in Psychology. 2014;5(1256):1–14. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brailovskaia J., Bierhoff H.W. Cross-cultural narcissism on Facebook: Relationship between self-presentation, social interaction and the open and covert narcissism on a social networking site in Germany and Russia. Computers in Human Behavior. 2016;55:251–257. [Google Scholar]
- Brown Z., Tiggemann M. Attractive celebrity and peer images on Instagram: Effect on women's mood and body image. Body Image. 2016;19:37–43. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruno N., Pisanski K., Sorokowska A., Sorokowski P. Understanding selfies. Frontiers in Psychology. 2018;9:44. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buffardi L.E., Campbell W.K. Narcissism and social networking web sites. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2008;34(10):1303–1314. doi: 10.1177/0146167208320061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carpenter C.J. Narcissism on Facebook: Self-promotional and anti-social behaviour. Personality and Individual Differences. 2012;52:482–486. [Google Scholar]
- Casale S., Fioravanti G., Rugai L. Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism: Who is at higher risk for social networking addiction? Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking. 2016;19:510–515. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2016.0189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casale S., Fioravanti G., Rugai L., Flett G.L., Hewitt P.L. The interpersonal expression of perfectionism among grandiose and vulnerable narcissists: Perfectionistic self-presentation, effortless perfectionism, and the ability to seem perfect. Personality and Individual Differences. 2016;99:320–324. [Google Scholar]
- Casale S., Rugai L., Fioravanti G., Puccetti C. Narcissism and authentic self: An unfeasible marriage? Personality and Individual Differences. 2018;135(2018):131–136. [Google Scholar]
- Casale, S., Gemelli, G., Calosi, C., Giangrasso, B., & Fioravanti, G. (2019). Multiple exposure to appearance-focused real accounts on Instagram: Effects on body image among both genders. Current Psychology, 1–10. Epub ahead of print. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00229-6.
- Caso D., Fabbricatore R., Muti F., Starace C. Sessualizzazione e oggettivazione femminile su Instagram: Il ruolo delle influencer. Psicologia Sociale. 2019;3:441–463. [Google Scholar]
- Chae J. Virtual makeover: Selfie-taking and social media use increase selfie-editing frequency through social comparison. Computers in Human Behavior. 2017;66:370–376. [Google Scholar]
- Chang L., Li P., Loh R.S.M., Chua T.H.H. A study of Singapore adolescent girls’ selfie practices, peer appearance comparisons, and body esteem on Instagram. Body Image. 2019;29:90–99. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charoensukmongkol, P. (2016). Exploring personal characteristics associated with selfie-liking. Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyberspace, 10(2). Retrieved November 25, 2019, from: https://doi.org/10.5817/CP2016-2-7.
- Christoforakos, L., Diefenbach, S. (2016). Das Selfie-Paradoxon – Kaum einer mag sie, alle tun es, Poster presentation at the German Psychological Society (DGPs) Congress, Leipzig.
- Cohen R., Newton-John T., Slater A. The relationship between Facebook and Instagram appearance-focused activities and body image concerns in young women. Body Image. 2017;23:183–187. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2017.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen R., Newton-John T., Slater A. ‘Selfie’-objectification: The role of selfies in self-objectification and disordered eating in young women. Computers in Human Behavior. 2018;79:68–74. [Google Scholar]
- Dakanalis A., Carrà G., Calogero R., Fida R., Clerici M., Zanetti M.A., Riva G. The developmental effects of media-ideal internalization and self-objectification processes on adolescents’ negative body-feelings, dietary restraint, and binge eating. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2015;24(8):997–1010. doi: 10.1007/s00787-014-0649-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daniel S., Bridges S.K. The drive for muscularity in men: Media influences and objectification theory. Body Image. 2010;7(1):32–38. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davenport S.W., Bergman S.M., Bergman J.Z., Fearrington M.E. Twitter versus Facebook: Exploring the role of narcissism in the motives and usage of different social media platforms. Computers in Human Behavior. 2014;32:212–220. [Google Scholar]
- de Vaate A.J.N.B., Veldhuis J., Alleva J.M., Konijn E.A., van Hugten C.H. Show your best self(ie): An exploratory study on selfie-related motivations and behavior in emerging adulthood. Telematics and Informatics. 2018;35(5):1392–1407. [Google Scholar]
- de Vries D.A., Peter J. Women on display: The effect of portraying the self online on women’s self-objectification. Computers in Human Behavior. 2013;29(4):1483–1489. [Google Scholar]
- DeWall C.N., Buffardi L.E., Bonser I., Campbell W.K. Narcissism and implicit attention seeking: Evidence from linguistic analyses of social networking and online presentation. Personality and Individual Differences. 2011;51(1):57–62. [Google Scholar]
- Dhir A. Why do young people avoid photo tagging? A new service avoidance scale. Social Science Computer Review. 2016;35(4):480–497. doi: 10.1177/0894439316653636. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dhir A., Pallesen S., Torsheim T., Andreassen C.S. Do age and gender differences exist in selfie-related behaviours? Computers in Human Behavior. 2016;63:549–555. [Google Scholar]
- Dhir A., Torsheim T., Pallesen S., Andreassen C.S. Do online privacy concerns predict selfie behavior among adolescents, young adults and adults? Frontiers in Psychology. 2017;8(815):1–12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diefenbach S., Christoforakos L. The selfie paradox: Nobody seems to like them yet everyone has reasons to take them. An exploration of psychological functions of selfies in self-presentation. Frontiers in Psychology. 2017;8:7. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickinson K.A., Pincus A.L. Interpersonal analysis of grandiose and vulnerable narcissism. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2003;17:188–207. doi: 10.1521/pedi.17.3.188.22146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doğan U., Çolak T.S. Self-concealment, social network sites usage, social appearance anxiety, loneliness of high school students: A model testing. Journal of Education and Training Studies. 2016;4(6):176–183. [Google Scholar]
- Doğan U., Adıgüzel A. Effect of selfie, social network sites usage, number of photos shared on social network sites on happiness among University Students: A Model Testing. Journal of Education and Practice. 2017;8(27) ISSN 2222-1735. [Google Scholar]
- Etgar S., Amichai-Hamburger Y. Not all selfies took alike: Distinct selfie motivations are related to different personality characteristics. Frontiers in Psychology. 2017;8:842. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00842. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fardouly J., Diedrichs P.C., Vartanian L.R., Halliwell E. The mediating role of appearance comparisons in the relationship between media usage and self-objectification in young women. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2015;39(4):447–457. [Google Scholar]
- Fardouly J., Vartanian L.R. Negative comparisons about one's appearance mediate the relationship between Facebook usage and body image concerns. Body Image. 2015;12:82–88. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fardouly J., Vartanian L.R. Social media and body image concerns: Current research and future directions. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2016;9:1–5. [Google Scholar]
- Fardouly J., Willburger B.K., Vartanian L.R. Instagram use and young women’s body image concerns and self-objectification: Testing mediational pathways. New Media & Society. 2018;20(4):1380–1395. [Google Scholar]
- Feltman C.E., Szymanski D.M. Instagram use and self-objectification: The roles of internalization, comparison, appearance commentary, and feminism. Sex Roles. 2018;78(5–6):311–324. [Google Scholar]
- Fossati A., Feeney J., Pincus A., Borroni S., Maffei C. The structure of pathological narcissism and its relationships with adult attachment styles: A study of italian nonclinical and clinical adult participants. Psychoanalytic Psychology. 2015;32(3):403–431. [Google Scholar]
- Fox J., Rooney M.C. The Dark Triad and trait self-objectification as predictors of men’s use and self-presentation behaviors on social networking sites. Personality and Individual Differences. 2015;76:161–165. [Google Scholar]
- Fox A.K., Bacile T.J., Nakhata C., Weible A. Selfie-marketing: Exploring narcissism and self-concept in visual user-generated content on social media. Journal of Consumer Marketing. 2018;35(1):11–21. [Google Scholar]
- Fredrickson B.L., Roberts T.A. Objectification theory: Toward understanding women's lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 1997;21(2):173–206. [Google Scholar]
- Gilliland E., Kiss M.J., Morrison M.A., Morrison T.G. Characterological correlates of selfie taking behavior. Psychology. 2018;9(6):1530–1545. [Google Scholar]
- Giordano C., Salerno L., Pavia L., Cavani P., Coco G.L., Tosto C., Di Blasi M. Magic mirror on the wall: Selfie-related behavior as mediator of the relationship between narcissism and problematic smartphone use. Neuropsychiatry. 2019;16(5–6):197–205. doi: 10.36131/clinicalnpsych2019050602. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grabe S., Ward L.M., Hyde J.S. The role of the media in body image concerns among women: A meta-analysis of experimental and correlational studies. Psychological Bulletin. 2008;134(3):460–476. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths M.D. A ‘components’ model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework. Journal of Substance Use. 2005;10:191–197. [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths M.D., Balakrishnan J. The psychosocial impact of excessive selfie-taking in youth: A brief overview. Education and Health. 2018;36(1):3–5. [Google Scholar]
- Guedes E., Sancassiani F., Carta M.G., Campos C., Machado S., King A.L.S., Nardi A.E. Internet addiction and excessive social networks use: What about facebook? Clinical Practice and Epidemiology in Mental Health. 2016;12:43–48. doi: 10.5935/MedicalExpress.2016.01.01. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halpern D., Valenzuela S., Katz J.E. “Selfie-ists” or “Narci-selfiers”?: A cross-lagged panel analysis of selfie taking and narcissism. Personality and Individual Differences. 2016;97:98–101. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.019. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hawk S.T., van den Eijnden R.J.J.M., van Lissac C.J., ter Bogt T.F.M. Narcissistic adolescents' attention-seeking following social rejection: Links with social media disclosure, problematic social media use, and smartphone stress. Computers in Human Behavior. 2019;92:65–75. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.10.032. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Holland G., Tiggemann M. A systematic review of the impact of the use of social networking sites on body image and disordered eating outcomes. Body Image. 2016;17:100–110. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kardefelt-Winther D., Heeren A., Schimmenti A., VanRooij A., Maurage P., Carras M.…Billieux J. How can we conceptualize behavioural addiction without pathologizing common behaviours? Addiction. 2017;112(10):1709–1715. doi: 10.1111/add.13763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karsay K., Knoll J., Matthes J. Sexualizing media use and self-objectification: A meta-analysis. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2018;42(1):9–28. doi: 10.1177/0361684317743019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katz J.E., Crocker E.T. Selfies| selfies and photo messaging as visual conversation: Reports from the United States, United Kingdom and China. International Journal of Communication. 2015;9(12):1861–1872. [Google Scholar]
- Kim J.W., Chock T.M. Personality traits and psychological motivations predicting selfie posting behaviors on social networking sites. Telematics and Informatics. 2017;34(5):560–571. [Google Scholar]
- Kuss D.J., Griffiths M.D. Social networking sites and addiction: Ten lessons learned. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2017;14(3):311. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14030311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lamp S.J., Cugle A., Silverman A.L., Thomas M.T., Liss M., Erchull M.J. Picture perfect: The relationship between selfie behaviors, self-objectification, and depressive symptoms. Sex Roles. 2019:1–9. doi: 10.1007/s11199-019-01025-z. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lee J.A., Sung Y. Hide-and-seek: Narcissism and “selfie”-related behavior. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2016;19(5):347–351. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2015.0486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lim W.M. Understanding the selfie phenomenon: Current insights and future research directions. European Journal of Marketing. 2016;50(9/10):1773–1788. [Google Scholar]
- Livingstone S. Taking risky opportunities in youthful content creation: Teenagers' use of social networking sites for intimacy, privacy and self-expression. New Media & Society. 2008;10(3):393–411. [Google Scholar]
- Lonergan A.R., Bussey K., Mond J., Brown O., Giffiths S., Muray S.B., Mitchison D. Me, my selfie, and I: The relationship between editing and posting selfies and body dissatisfaction in men and women. Body Image. 2019;28:39–43. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2018.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manago A.M., Ward L.M., Lemm K.M., Reed L., Seabrook R. Facebook involvement, objectified body consciousness, body shame, and sexual assertiveness in college women and men. Sex Roles. 2015;72(1–2):1–14. [Google Scholar]
- Manna, V., & Boursier, V. (2017). “How often do you…? A study of sexting and selfie prevalence among adolescents,” In Paper Presented at the 15th European Congress of Psychology, Amsterdam. July 11-14 (Amsterdam).
- March E., McBean T. New evidence shows self-esteem moderates the relationship between narcissism and selfies. Personality and individual differences. 2018;130:107–111. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.03.053. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Marshall T.C., Lefringhausen K., Ferenczi N. The Big Five, self-esteem, and narcissism as predictors of the topics people write about in Facebook status updates. Personality and Individual Differences. 2015;85:35–40. [Google Scholar]
- McCain J.L., Borg Z.G., Rothenberg A.H., Churillo K.M., Weiler P., Campbell W.K. Personality and selfies: Narcissism and the Dark Triad. Computers in Human Behavior. 2016;64:126–133. [Google Scholar]
- McKinley N.M., Hyde J.S. The objectified body consciousness scale: Development and validation. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 1996;20(2):181–215. [Google Scholar]
- McLean S.A., Jarman H.K., Rodgers R.F. How do “selfies” impact adolescents’ well-being and body confidence? A narrative review. Psychology Research and Behavior Management. 2019;12:513–521. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S177834. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLean S.A., Paxton S.J., Wertheim E.H., Masters J. Photoshopping the selfie: Self photo editing and photo investment are associated with body dissatisfaction in adolescent girls. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2015;48(8):1132–1140. doi: 10.1002/eat.22449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehdizadeh S. Self-presentation 2.0: Narcissism and self-esteem on Facebook. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2010;13(4):357–364. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2009.0257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meier E.P., Gray J. Facebook photo activity associated with body image disturbance in adolescent girls. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2014;17(4):199–206. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2013.0305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller J.D., Campbell W.K. Comparing clinical and social-personality conceptualizations of narcissism. Journal of Personality. 2008;76:449–476. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00492.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller J.D., Lynam D.R., Vize C., Crowe M., Sleepe C., Maples-Keller J.L., Campbell W.K. Vulnerable narcissism is (mostly) a disorder of neuroticism. Journal of Personality. 2018;86:186–199. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mills J.S., Musto S., Williams L., Tiggemann M. “Selfie” harm: Effects on mood and body image in young women. Body Image. 2018;27:86–92. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2018.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moon J.H., Lee E., Lee J.A., Choi T.R., Sung Y. The role of narcissism in self-promotion on Instagram. Personality and Individual Differences. 2016;101:22–25. [Google Scholar]
- Moradi B. Addressing gender and cultural diversity in body image: Objectification theory as a framework for integrating theories and grounding research. Sex Roles. 2010;63(1–2):138–148. [Google Scholar]
- Moya-Garofano A., Moya M. Focusing on one’s own appearance leads to body shame in women but not men: The mediating role of body surveillance and appearance-contingent self-worth. Body image. 2019;29:58–64. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Munno D., Cappellin F., Saroldi M., Bechon E., Guglielmucci F., Passera R., Zullo G. Internet addiction disorder: Personality characteristics and risk of pathological overuse in adolescents. Psychiatry Research. 2017;248:1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nadkarni A., Hofmann S.G. Why do people use Facebook? Personality and Individual Differences. 2012;52(3):243–249. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.11.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson O. Dark undercurrents of teenage girls’ selfies. The Age. 2013;11(07):2013. [Google Scholar]
- Nguyen, A. J., & Barbour, K. (2017). Selfies as expressively authentic identity performance. First Monday, 22, 1–11. Retrieved November 27, 2019, from: https://digital.library.adelaide.edu.au/dspace/bitstream/2440/116728/2/hdl_116728.pdf.
- Panek E.T., Nardis Y., Konrath S. Mirror or megaphone? How relationships between narcissism and social networking site use differ on Facebook and Twitter. Computers in Human Behavior. 2013;29:2004–2012. [Google Scholar]
- Patrick M.E., Maggs J.L. Does drinking lead to sex? Daily alcohol–sex behaviors and expectancies among college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2009;23(3):472–481. doi: 10.1037/a0016097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pincus A.L. The pathological narcissism inventory. In: Ogrudniczuk J., editor. Understanding and treating pathological narcissism. American Psychological Association; Washington, DC: 2013. pp. 93–110. [Google Scholar]
- Pincus A.L., Ansell E.B., Pimentel C.A., Cain N.M., Wright A.G.C., Levy K.N. Initial construction and validation of the pathological narcissism inventory. Psychological Reports. 2009;61:499–510. doi: 10.1037/a0016530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pincus A.L., Roche M.J. Narcissistic grandiosity and narcissistic vulnerability. In: Campbell W.K., Miller J.D., editors. Handbook of narcissism and narcissistic personality disorder. Wiley; New York: 2011. pp. 31–40. [Google Scholar]
- Qiu L., Lu J., Yang S., Qu W., Zhu T. What does your selfie say about you? Computers in Human Behavior. 2015;52:443–449. [Google Scholar]
- Ramsey L.R., Horan A.L. Picture this: Women’s self-sexualization in photos on social media. Personality and Individual Differences. 2018;133:85–90. [Google Scholar]
- Reich S., Schneider F.M., Heling L. Zero Likes-Symbolic interactions and need satisfaction online. Computers in Human Behavior. 2018;80:97–102. [Google Scholar]
- Ridgway J.L., Clayton R.B. Instagram unfiltered: Exploring associations of body image satisfaction, Instagram# selfie posting, and negative romantic relationship outcomes. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2016;19(1):2–7. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2015.0433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rumpf H.J., Brandt D., Demetrovics Z., Billieux J., Carragher N., Brand M.…Borges G. Epidemiological challenges in the study of behavioral addictions: A call for high standard methodologies. Current Addiction Reports. 2019 doi: 10.1007/s40429-019-00262-2. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ryan T., Xenos S. Who uses Facebook? An investigation into the relationship between the Big Five, shyness, narcissism, loneliness, and Facebook usage. Computers in Human Behavior. 2011;27(5):1658–1664. [Google Scholar]
- Salomon I., Brown C.S. The selfie generation: Examining the relationship between social media use and early adolescent body image. Journal of Early Adolescence. 2019;39(4):539–560. [Google Scholar]
- Sanecka E. The dark side of social media: Associations between the dark triad of personality, self-disclosure online and selfie-related behaviours. Journal of Education Culture and Society. 2017;2:71–88. [Google Scholar]
- Senft T.M., Baym N.K. What does the selfie say? Investigating a global phenomenon. International Journal of Communication. 2015;9(Feature):1588–1606. [Google Scholar]
- Seyfi M., Arpacı İ. The relationship between appearance concerns and selfie sharing on social media. İstanbul Üniversitesi İletişim Fakültesi Dergisi| Istanbul University Faculty of Communication Journal. 2016;51:143–154. [Google Scholar]
- Shin Y., Kim M., Im C., Chong S.C. Selfie and self: The effect of selfies on self-esteem and social sensitivity. Personality and Individual Differences. 2017;111:139–145. [Google Scholar]
- Singh S., Farley S.D., Donahue J.J. Grandiosity on display: Social media behaviors and dimensions of narcissism. Personality and Individual Differences. 2018;134:308–313. [Google Scholar]
- Slater A., Tiggemann M. Media exposure, extracurricular activities, and appearance-related comments as predictors of female adolescents’ self-objectification. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2015;39(3):375–389. [Google Scholar]
- Sorokowska A., Oleszkiewicz A., Frackowiak T., Pisanski K., Chmiel A., Sorokowski P. Selfies and personality: Who posts self-portrait photographs? Personality and Individual Differences. 2016;90:119–123. [Google Scholar]
- Sorokowski P., Sorokowska A., Oleszkiewicz A., Frackowiak T., Huk A., Pisanski K. Selfie posting behaviors are associated with narcissism among men. Personality and Individual Differences. 2015;85:123–127. [Google Scholar]
- Starcevic V. Behavioural addictions: A challenge for psychopathology and psychiatric nosology. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 2016;50(8):721–725. doi: 10.1177/0004867416654009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sung Y., Lee J.A., Kim E., Choi S.M. Why we post selfies: Understanding motivations for posting pictures of oneself. Personality and Individual Differences. 2016;97:260–265. [Google Scholar]
- Taylor S.H., Hinck A.S., Lim H. An experimental test of how selfies change social judgments on facebook. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2017;20(10):610–614. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2016.0759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiggemann M., Slater A. NetGirls: The Internet, Facebook, and body image concern in adolescent girls. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2013;46(6):630–633. doi: 10.1002/eat.22141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiggemann M., Slater A. The role of self-objectification in the mental health of early adolescent girls: Predictors and consequences. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2015;40(7):704–711. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsv021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trekels J., Ward L.M., Eggermont S. I “like” the way you look: How appearance-focused and overall Facebook use contribute to adolescents' self-sexualization. Computers in Human Behavior. 2018;81:198–208. [Google Scholar]
- Vandenbosch L., Eggermont S. Understanding sexual objectification: A comprehensive approach toward media exposure and girls' internalization of beauty ideals, self-objectification, and body surveillance. Journal of Communication. 2012;62(5):869–887. [Google Scholar]
- Vandenbosch L., Eggermont S. Sexualization of adolescent boys: Media exposure and boys’ internalization of appearance ideals, self-objectification, and body surveillance. Men and Masculinities. 2013;16(3):283–306. [Google Scholar]
- Veldhuis J., Alleva J.M., Bij de Vaate A.J., Keijer M., Konijn E.A. Me, my selfie, and I: The relations between selfie behaviors, body image, self-objectification, and self-esteem in young women. Psychology of Popular Media Culture. 2018 doi: 10.1037/ppm0000206. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Y., Wang X., Liu H., Xie X., Wang P., Lei L. Selfie posting and self-esteem among young adult women: A mediation model of positive feedback and body satisfaction. Journal of Health Psychology. 2018 doi: 10.1177/1359105318787624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Y., Xie X., Fardouly J., Vartanian L.R., Lei L. The longitudinal and reciprocal relationships between selfie-related behaviors and self-objectification and appearance concerns among adolescents. New media and Society. 2019;1–22 doi: 10.1177/1461444819894346. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Warfield, K. (2014). Making selfies/making self: Digital subjectivities in the selfie. Paper presented at the Fifth International Conference on the Image and the Image Knowledge Community, Freie Universität, Berlin, Germany.
- Weiser E.B. # Me: Narcissism and its facets as predictors of selfie-posting frequency. Personality and Individual Differences. 2015;86:477–481. [Google Scholar]
- Weiser E.B. Shameless selfie promotion: Narcissism and its association with selfie-posting behavior. In: Hai-Jew S., editor. Selfies as a mode of social media and work space research. IGI Global; Hershey, PA: 2018. pp. 1–27. [Google Scholar]
- Williamson P., Stohlman T., Polinsky H. Me, my selfie and I: A survey of self-disclosure motivations on social media. IAFOR Journal of Cultural Studies. 2017;2(2):71–85. [Google Scholar]
- Wink P. Two faces of narcissism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1996;61:74–94. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.61.4.590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu M.S., Song C., Ma Y. Selfie taking may be nonharmful: Evidence from adaptive and maladaptive narcissism among Chinese young adults. Human Behavior and Emerging Technologies. 2019;1(3):240–244. [Google Scholar]
- Zheng D., Ni X.-L., Luo Y.-J. Selfie posting on social networking sites and female adolescents’ self-objectification: The moderating role of imaginary audience ideation. Sex Roles. 2019;80:325–331. [Google Scholar]