Abstract
Patterns in resting‐state fMRI (rs‐fMRI) are widely used to characterize the trait effects of brain function. In this aspect, multiple rs‐fMRI scans from single subjects can provide interesting clues about the rs‐fMRI patterns, though scan‐to‐scan variability pose challenges. Therefore, rs‐fMRI's are either concatenated or the functional connectivity is averaged. This leads to loss of information. Here, we use an alternative way to extract the rs‐fMRI features that are common across all the scans by applying common‐and‐orthogonal‐basis‐extraction (COBE) technique. To address this, we employed rs‐fMRI of 788 subjects from the human connectome project and estimated the common‐COBE‐component of each subject from the four rs‐fMRI runs. Since the common‐COBE‐component is specific to a subject, the pattern was used to classify the subjects based on the similarity/dissimilarity of the features. The subset of subjects (n = 107) with maximal‐COBE‐dissimilarity (MCD) was extracted and the remaining subjects (n = 681) formed the COBE‐similarity (CS) group. The distribution of weights of the common‐COBE‐component for the two groups across rs‐fMRI networks and subcortical regions was evaluated. We found the weights in the default mode network to be lower in the MCD compared to the CS. We compared the scores of 69 behavioral measures and found six behaviors related to the use of marijuana, illicit drugs, alcohol, and tobacco; and including a measure of antisocial personality to differentiate the two groups. Gender differences were also significant. Altogether the findings suggested that subtypes exist even in healthy control population, and comparison studies (case vs. control) need to be mindful of it.
Keywords: alcohol; antisocial personality problems; common component; default mode network; drugs; functional MRI; marijuana; tobacco, gender effects
1. INTRODUCTION
The resting state (rs) in which the subjects lie quietly inside the scanner has been widely adopted in neuroimaging research. Research has reliably shown the potential of resting‐state fMRI (rs‐fMRI) data to reveal the intrinsic architecture of the brain (Biswal, Yetkin, Haughton, & Hyde, 1995; Buckner, Krienen, & Yeo, 2013; Fox & Raichle, 2007) and provide important information regarding the brain functions and cognitive abilities (Mennes et al., 2010; Smith et al., 2009; Tavor et al., 2016; Yeo, Tandi, & Chee, 2015). Therefore, researchers are interested to explore how the patterns in rs‐fMRI are manifested across individuals with different traits and lifestyle habits. Consequently, there are attempts to link the rs‐fMRI to variety of traits and behaviors (Bertolero, Yeo, Bassett, & D'Esposito, 2018; Dubois, Galdi, Han, Paul, & Adolphs, 2018; Finn et al., 2015; Hampson, Driesen, Skudlarski, Gore, & Constable, 2006; Kashyap et al., 2019; Kong et al., 2018; Li, Mai, & Liu, 2014; Rosenberg et al., 2016; Smith et al., 2015). In an interesting work by Smith et al. (2015), a positive–negative axis linking various demographic and lifestyle factors to the rs functional connectivity was delineated. They found positive correlations to positive personal qualities (e.g., high performance on memory and cognitive test, life satisfaction) and negative correlation values for the negative traits (like substance use, anger, rule‐breaking behavior).
The way the rs‐fMRI networks are organized in the brain has been associated to our state, psyche, social values as well as lifestyle habits (Buckner, Andrews‐Hanna, & Schacter, 2008; Gusnard & Raichle, 2001; van den Heuvel & Hulshoff Pol, 2010). In this aspect, multiple scans from each subject are helpful and previous research has shown that conclusions from single session could be erroneous (McGonigle et al., 2000) though intersession variabilities too exist (McGonigle et al., 2000; Smith et al., 2005). Such intersession variabilities are not only limited to physiological changes in the subjects inside the scanner but also includes alterations in their cognitive and affective states (Gorgolewski et al., 2015). With the advent of big rs‐fMRI data with multiple scans of each individual, the conventional way either concatenates the rs‐fMRI or averages the functional connectivity over the multiple scans. This might lead to loss of important information from the data. An alternate way would be to extract the features of fMRI that are common across all the rs scans. With this motivation, using the human connectome project (HCP; Smith et al., 2013; Van Essen et al., 2012) data wherein all healthy subjects are scanned four times and performances across numerous behavioral measures are recorded inside and outside the scanner, we aimed to extract the common features shared by the four rs‐fMRI runs of a single subject and explore the pattern (of the features) across all the subjects.
To this aim, we applied the common orthogonal basis extraction (COBE) algorithm (Zhou, Cichocki, Zhang, & Mandic, 2016; Zhou, Zhao, et al., 2016); to the four rs‐fMRI runs acquired from a single HCP subject. We repeated this for all the subjects (n = 788). The COBE algorithm was originally developed to extract the common and individual features from a “multi‐block” data (collection of matrices). The common features are those that are shared by all the blocks whereas the individual features are specific to a block. The number of common components is to be specified by the user. In our case, each run of rs‐fMRI of an individual was taken as a block, so application of COBE to the rs‐fMRI of all the four runs (multi‐block) will decompose the rs‐fMRI signals into a linear sum of a number of common COBE component subspace (shared by all runs) and run‐specific subspaces (specific to a run). The common component represents the spatial distribution of weights across the brain that is shared by the rs‐fMRI's of an individual across all the scans (Refer Section 2.5). If the features are common to all the rs scans of an individual, they could in‐principle be used to classify the healthy HCP subjects based on the similarity/dissimilarity of the pattern. This is because the physiological parameters in healthy individuals are considered to be in a normal range and a similar expectation from the rs‐fMRI features is preconceived. However, this may not be true considering the wide inter‐subject variability in the rs‐fMRI features within the healthy individuals. Hence, one interesting approach would be to identify a subgroup of healthy HCP subjects whose pattern of common COBE component is different from the group, and then find out which behavioral measures account to the differences between the two healthy groups. This will be intriguing given the general trend to classify subjects based on external symptoms (like healthy vs. diseased, control vs. patient, etc.) does not consider the scope for variations within healthy individuals. However, it is undeniable that there can be subtypes within the healthy individuals whose rs‐fMRI features might lie on the edges of the spectrum owing to their lifestyle habits, psychological factors, and physiological traits.
Conceptually, it is worth mentioning our work along with the vast literature that had investigated trait‐level and state‐level aspects of fMRI (Bijsterbosch et al., 2017; Cole, Bassett, Power, Braver, & Petersen, 2014; Gratton et al., 2018; Kong et al., 2018; Krienen, Yeo, & Buckner, 2014; Shirer, Ryali, Rykhlevskaia, Menon, & Greicius, 2012; Wang, Ong, Patanaik, Zhou, & Chee, 2016; Yeo, Krienen, et al., 2015). In our previous work (Kashyap et al., 2019), COBE was applied to the rs‐fMRI of each run in the HCP data. Each run comprised of rs‐fMRI from 803 subjects. Interestingly, the common COBE component of the four runs revealed the state‐effects specific to that run. Subsequently, removing the state‐effects from the rs‐fMRI improved the functional connectivity based behavioral prediction. Here, our goal was to obtain the common COBE component for all the HCP subjects (from their multiple scans) and classify those subjects whose common COBE components were as dissimilar as possible. Subsequently, to understand the behavioral manifestation of the dissimilarity we explored the traits that are different in those subset of subjects.
2. METHODS
2.1. Overview
The rs‐fMRI of 1,094 subjects from the HCP were considered in the study. However, only 788 subjects survived the selection criteria and were preprocessed. Thus, COBE was applied to the four rs‐fMRI scans of these subjects. The first common component of COBE was extracted for every subject. The common components were correlated to form the common‐correlation matrix. The subjects with a dissimilar pattern were then extracted from the matrix and grouped together to form the maximal‐COBE‐dissimilarity (MCD) group. The MCD group and the group of remaining subjects were evaluated for differences in the scores of 62 behavioral measures and 7 physiological parameters.
2.2. Rs‐fMRI data
The HCP S1200 release has a multimodal collection of data across behavioral, structural MRI, rs, and task‐state fMRI, and Magnetoencephalography (MEG) paradigms from healthy adults (Smith et al., 2009; Van Essen et al., 2012). All imaging data were collected on a custom‐made Siemens 3T Skyra scanner that uses a multiband sequence. The rs‐fMRI was acquired in 2 mm isotropic resolution with a TR of 0.72 s for a total of 1,200 frames lasting 14 min and 33 s. The rs‐fMRI and behavioral data were collected on two sessions. Each rs‐fMRI session consisted of two runs. Therefore, in total each subject had four runs of rs scans.
2.3. Preprocessing
The dataset used in the present study was taken from our previous work (Kashyap et al., 2019). The dataset comprised of the MSMAll ICA‐FIX data on fs_LR32K surface space (HCP S1200 manual; Glasser et al., 2013; Griffanti et al., 2014; Salimi‐Khorshidi et al., 2014) from 1,094 subjects. As few studies have pointed out that ICA‐FIX cannot completely remove the head‐motion artifacts, so further nuisance regression was necessary (Burgess et al., 2016; Kong et al., 2018; Li et al., 2019; Siegel et al., 2017). Therefore, an estimate of framewise displacement (FD; Jenkinson, Bannister, Brady, & Smith, 2002) and root‐mean‐square of voxel‐wise differentiated signal (DVARS; Power, Barnes, Snyder, Schlaggar, & Petersen, 2012) was done using fsl_motion_outliers. Consequently, those volumes for which FD >0.2 mm and DVARS >75, as well as uncensored segments of data lasting fewer than five contiguous volumes were flagged as outliers. Nuisance regression was performed with 18 regressors that consisted of a global signal, six motion parameters, averaged ventricular signal, averaged white matter signal, and their temporal derivatives. While computing the regression coefficients in nuisance regression, outlier volumes were ignored. Finally, a bandpass filter (0.009 Hz ≤ f ≤ 0.08 Hz) was applied to the data and the BOLD runs with more than half the volumes flagged as outliers were completely removed. As a result, 82 subjects had all runs removed and were thus eliminated.
Adopting the 400 cortical parcellation by Schaefer et al. (2018), we obtained the preprocessed fMRI time courses from each of the 400 cortical parcels. Additionally, 19 subcortical regions (brain stem, accumbens, amygdala, caudate, cerebellum, diencephalon, hippocampus, pallidum, putamen, and thalamus; Fischl et al., 2002) were included. Thus, there were 419 regions in total, and therefore, the rs‐fMRI of each subject in each run was a 419 x 1200 matrix.
2.4. Behavioral data
We considered a set of 62 behavioral measures and 7 physiological parameters spanning across eight categories including—alertness, cognition, emotion, personality, sensory, health and family history, psychiatric and life function, and substance use (Table S1). These categories provided a considerable range of the participant's traits, lifestyle habits, age, body mass index, and health. Importantly, our analyses were restricted to the subjects who had all four runs surviving the quality control procedure with all 62 behavioral measures and 7 physiological parameters, resulting in a final set of 788 subjects.
2.5. Extraction of common COBE component
For more details about the COBE algorithm, we refer readers to methodological papers (Zhou, Cichocki, et al., 2016; Zhou, Zhao, et al., 2016). Here, we briefly describe how COBE was applied to rs‐fMRI data in this study.
We applied COBE to the rs‐fMRI's of each subject separately. For ease of explanation, let us consider the rs‐fMRI (419 x 1200 matrix) of each run as a block. Here, we would like to recall that our study included only those subjects that had four runs of rs scan. For a subject S, let Rn denote the 419 x 1200 rs‐fMRI matrix for the nth run (where n is a positive integer with n ≤ 4). As illustrated in Figure 1a, COBE seeks to decompose Rn into
where is a 419 x C matrix representing the common component shared across all runs. C is the number of common components, and is defined a priori by the user. In the present study, we evaluated the common COBE component of all the subjects (n = 788) for C = 1. The common COBE component for C = 2 and 3 were also evaluated but not considered further in the analysis (see results). A useful property of COBE is that if COBE was applied twice (sequentially) with C = 1, the two common components will be (in practice) the same as the common components obtained by applying COBE once with C = 2 (Zhou, Cichocki, et al., 2016). This has also been tested in our previous work (Kashyap et al., 2019). We mention this property here since one might assume that a minor difference in the data might affect the decomposition and the components may split up differently; which however is not the case with COBE.
Figure 1.
Illustration of common orthogonal basis extraction (COBE). (a) COBE applied to the four runs of rs‐fMRI of an HCP subject. COBE projects the rs‐fMRI run of a HCP subject onto a common subspace () and run‐specific subspace (. The common component () is shared across all runs. The number of components C spanning the common subspace (i.e., the number of columns of ) is a user‐specified parameter. (b) The spatial map of the weights of the common component (419 x C) for C = 1 across the cortical areas is shown for a random HCP subject
The spatial map of the common COBE component for a subject can be visualized in Figure 1b. The (C x 1200 matrix) is the time course associated with the component. is a 419 x 1200 matrix is representing the common subspace shared by all the runs, while is also a 419 x 1200 matrix representing the projection of the nth run rs‐fMRI onto the run‐specific subspace.
2.6. Common correlation matrix
The common component for every subject (n = 788) was extracted. To classify subjects based on their similarity/dissimilarity of the pattern of the common component, we correlated the component across the subjects. Thus, a 788 x 788 common‐correlation matrix was computed using Pearson's correlation. We seek to select a subset of subjects from the correlation matrix whose common components are as different as possible (i.e., the subject should have a COBE pattern dissimilar to other subjects). To this end, we applied the strategy developed by (Kong et al., 2018). Here, we randomly picked an entry in the common‐correlation matrix (i.e., pair of subjects) whose absolute correlation was less than a threshold of 0.75 (also see Supporting Information for the other thresholds that we tested to ascertain 0.75 as our choice for optimal threshold). We continued adding new random subjects, such that each newly added subject was minimally correlated (absolute r < .75) with the current set of subjects. The procedure terminated when no more subject could be added. The procedure was repeated 1,000 times, resulting in 1,000 sets with varied number of subjects per set. Of these 1,000 sets, we chose the set containing subjects with the smallest maximum absolute correlation. We will refer to this subset of subjects as the MCD group. Subsequently, the remaining subjects formed the COBE‐similarity (CS) group.
2.7. Behavioral differences between groups
Here, we would like to remind the readers that the HCP dataset which is from a healthy population is been dissociated to extract a subset of subjects with dissimilar rs‐fMRI COBE pattern. The purpose is to characterize the traits associated with the dissimilarity. For this, we compared the two groups (MCD and CS) across 62 behavioral measures and 7 physiological parameters from eight categories namely health, alertness, cognition, emotion, personality, psychiatry and life function, sensory, and substance use. The behavioral measures included in each category provided comprehensive information on an individual's physical health, psyche, and lifestyle habits. For example, the personality includes behavioral measure like‐ agreeableness, open to experiences; emotion includes happiness, anger, fear; substance use include his/her intake of alcohol, tobacco, illegal drugs, marijuana; sensory encompasses the sense of odor, taste, pain; cognition involves scores of mini‐mental state examination, sleep, working memory, fluid intelligence; and psychiatric function include the scores of antisocial personality problem, externalizing, internalizing; and physiological parameters include a person's blood pressure, body mass index, and so on (Table S1).
2.8. Code availability
The code of COBE can be downloaded at http://www.bsp.brain.riken.jp/~zhougx/cifa.html. The Schaefer's 400‐region parcellation is available at https://github.com/ThomasYeoLab/CBIG/tree/master/stable_projects/brain_parcellation/Schaefer2018_LocalGlobal. The code relevant to the work can also be obtained from https://github.com/ClinicalBrainLab/Maximizing_Dissimilarity_in_fMRI
3. RESULTS
3.1. Overview
COBE was applied to rs‐fMRI of 788 HCP subjects to obtain their first common component. The common COBE component of each subject was correlated with the common component of other subjects to obtain the 788 x 788 common‐correlation matrix. From this matrix, a subset of subjects that had a dissimilar pattern of common COBE component was extracted to form the MCD group (n = 107) and remaining subjects formed the CS group (n = 681) group. The spatial map of the common component from the MCD and CS group were evaluated and significant differences were found in the default mode network. Finally, we also explored the scores of 62 behavioral measures and 7 physiological parameters between the two groups and found only six behavioral measures to differentiate the two groups.
3.2. Spatial maps of common COBE components
Common COBE components are illustrated in a spatial map in Figure 2 below. Figure 2a,b shows the 400 cortical and 19 subcortical Region of interest (ROI) used to compute the 419 x 1200 rs‐fMRI time courses. Parcel colors correspond to 17 large‐scale‐networks (Yeo et al., 2011). The 17 networks are a finer division of the 7 cortical networks (Temporo Parietal, Default mode, Control, Limbic, Attention, Somato‐motor, and Visual). The distribution of the weights of the common component (419 x C) for C = 1 (i.e., the first common component) for all the subjects across the seven cortical networks (Yeo et al., 2011) of both hemispheres and subcortical areas can be visualized in Figure 2c. In line with previous studies, we find that the pattern with higher weights in the default mode network is consistent across the subjects. However, visual inspection also reveals that in a few subjects the distribution of weights was different. Therefore, we extracted the subjects with a dissimilar pattern of the common COBE component from the common correlation matrix.
Figure 2.
Spatial distribution of common common orthogonal basis extraction (COBE) component (C = 1) across the cortical and subcortical areas. (a) 400 cortical parcels (Schaefer et al., 2018). Parcel colors correspond to 17 large‐scale‐networks (Yeo et al., 2011). The 17 networks are a subdivision of seven networks (temporal parietal, default mode, control, limbic, attention, somato‐motor, and visual). (b) Nineteen subcortical ROIs (Fischl et al., 2002). (c) Spatial distribution of weights of the first common COBE component of 788 subjects across the 7 networks (in both hemispheres) and 19 subcortical areas. Most subjects show higher weights in the default mode network areas, though in few subjects the distribution of weight is different
Interestingly, we also observed the distribution of weights across all the subjects for the second and third common COBE component, that is, for C = 2 and 3, respectively (Supporting Information). We found that as the number of components increases the similarity of the pattern across the subjects decrease. Visual inspection reveals high interindividual variation in the distribution of the second and third common COBE component across the seven rs networks. Overall, the results suggested not to use higher components to classify the subjects.
The MCD group comprised of 107 subjects (max |r| = .74). Consequently, the CS group had 681 subjects (max |r| = .98). The spatial map of the weights of common COBE component for the two groups (averaged across the subjects) can be visualized in Figure 3a. A clear difference between the two groups can be visualized in the default mode network (DMN) areas especially in the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC). Since, Yeo et al. (2011) have subdivided the DMN into DMN‐A, DMN‐B, and DMN‐C (Figure 2a); so we narrowed down our analysis to the subnetworks of DMN and averaged the weights of the common component across the subjects in the two groups for the 17 cortical networks and 19 subcortical areas (Figure 3b). Specifically, we found that the two groups differed in their distribution of the weights only across DMN‐A areas (p < 1e−14).
Figure 3.
Differences in weight of common component across the default mode network areas between the MCD and CS group. (a) Higher weights of common COBE component can be observed in the Posterior cingulate cortex for the CS group. (b) The averaged weights of common COBE component across the 17 cortical networks (Yeo et al., 2011) and subcortical areas is shown for the two groups. * Represents the network that survived Bonferroni correction (p < 1e−14). Clearly, the MCD group has reduced weights in the DMN‐A. (COBE, common orthogonal basis extraction; CS, COBE‐similarity; MCD, maximal‐COBE‐dissimilarity)
3.3. Behavioral differences between the groups
Scores of 62 behavioral measures and 7 physiological parameters were compared between the MCD and CS group (Table S1). We found five behavioral measures to survive the significance level with Bonferroni correction (p < 1e−5). Interestingly, these behaviors belonged to the two categories (a) substance use, and (b) psychiatric and life function. In the first category, we found the behavioral measure that reported a subject's alcohol consumption in past 7 days (Total_Drinks_7_Days) to be higher for MCD (10.11 ± 1.00) than the CS group (4.84 ± 0.26). Similarly, the tobacco consumption in the past 7 days (Total_Any_Tobacco_7_Days) for MCD (18.05 ± 3.26) was also higher than the CS (5.97 ± 0.76). For the ease of representation, we divided the intake of alcohol and tobacco into five categories based on the number of times they (alcohol/tobacco) were reported to have been consumed by a subject in the past 7 days. They are –0 (no intake of alcohol/tobacco), 1–5, 6–10, 11–15, 16–20, and > 20 times (Figure 4a,b). Thirty‐nine percent of subjects in MCD reported ≥ 6 drinks of alcohol/week; which is only 10.7% in the CS. Similarly, 87% in CS group reported to have never taken any kind of tobacco (e.g., cigarettes, cigars, pipes); which in contrast is only 49.5% in MCD.
Figure 4.
Comparison of behavioral measures between MCD and CS group for the use of (a) alcohol (b) tobacco (c) marijuana, and (d) illicit drugs; and for (e) antisocial personality problems
From the first category, we also found the MCD group to be comprised of those subjects who had a high use of Marijuana in their lifetime (4.59 ± 0.04; SSAGA_Mj_Times_Used) compared to the CS group (1.35 ± 0.06). The HCP has divided the use of Marijuana into five categories based on the number of times it was reported to have been used by the subjects in their lifetime. They are 0 (never used), 1–5, 6–10, 11–100, 111–999, >1,000 times. As shown in Figure 4c, all the subjects in MCD reported having used marijuana for more than 100 times; of which, 59% reported a marijuana intake of more than 1,000 times. In contrast, 53% of subjects in the CS group reported to have never used marijuana, and the majority of the remaining subjects (45%) reported to have used it for less than 100 times. Similarly, the use of illicit drugs (cocaine, hallucinogen, opiates, sedatives, and stimulants; SSAGA_Times_Used_Illicits) was less in CS (0.53 ± 0.04) compared to MCD (2.19 ± 0.16). Seventy‐eight percent of the subjects in the CS group reported having never used any illicit drugs. This was only 27% in the MCD (Figure 4d).
In the psychiatric and life function category of behaviors, the score of antisocial personality problems measured by DSM criteria (DSM_Antis_Pct) was marginally higher in MCD than in the CS (Figure 4e). The antisocial personality in HCP is Achenbach Adult Self Report questionnaires for the DSM‐oriented scale that measures a person's aggressiveness, law‐abiding behaviors, and so on. Interestingly, since sex related differences are reported for both substance use (Cooper & Craft, 2018) and antisocial personality problems (Cale & Lilienfeld, 2002), we calculated the number of males and females in each group. MCD group comprised of 74 males and 33 females whereas CS group comprised of 323 males and 358 females, respectively. Chi‐square test revealed gender differences between the groups to be significant (p < 1e−5). Since a large sex difference exists between the two groups we investigated if this factor was driving the between‐group differences in substance use, alcohol and tobacco consumption, and antisocial personality problems. We compared the weights of common COBE component between the MCD group and a randomly selected sex‐matched (RSSM) subset of the CS group. We found this difference to be significant (p < 1e −4) for DMN‐A. Subsequently, we also compared the scores of the five behavioral measures between the MCD and RSSM groups. Only two behavioral measures, namely the use of marijuana (SSAGA_Mj_Times_Used) and illicit drugs (SSAGA_Times_Used_Illicits) remained significantly different between the two groups. The additional analysis provided a concrete basis for evaluating the role of sex in differentiating the MCD and CS group.
Finally, as an auxiliary analysis we also correlated the variation of the common COBE component of each subject in the MCD group with five behavioral measures—marijuana intake, illicit drug use, alcohol and tobacco use, and antisocial personality problems (see Supporting Information).
4. DISCUSSION
In this article, we utilized an alternate way to analyze the multiple scans of rs‐fMRI data from an individual. We applied the COBE technique (Zhou, Cichocki, et al., 2016; Zhou, Zhao, et al., 2016) to decompose the four runs of rs‐fMRI of each HCP subject into a common component space (C = 1) shared by the runs of an individual and run‐specific subspaces. We observed the distribution of the weights of the first common COBE component across the cortical and subcortical areas for all the HCP subjects (n = 788). Consistent with previous rs‐fMRI studies, we found higher weights across the areas of DMN. By visual inspection, distribution of weights across the areas in few subjects appeared dissimilar. Therefore, we attempted to extract the subset of subjects whose common components are as different as possible (Kong et al., 2018). This subset of subjects (n = 107) comprised the MCD, and the remaining subjects (n = 681) formed the CS group. We evaluated the distribution of weights of the common COBE component for the two groups across the 17 rs networks (Yeo et al., 2011) and 19 subcortical areas. We found the weights in the DMN‐A to be lower (p < 1e−14) in MCD compared to the CS group (Figure 2e). To our knowledge, none of the studies have applied COBE to multiple runs of rs‐fMRI of healthy individuals and classified them in subgroups. It is important to mention that appropriate feature extraction is a prerequisite to pattern recognition and classification. While few researchers have used similar techniques like canonical correlation analysis (CCA) to extract features that were hierarchically clustered to identify subtypes of depression (Drysdale et al., 2017), and to delineate positive–negative axis linking various demographic and lifestyle factors (Smith et al., 2015); others have used alternate methods like partial least square (PLS; Krishnan, Williams, McIntosh, & Abdi, 2011) and joint and individual variation explained (JIVE; Yu, Risk, Zhang, & Marron, 2017) to establish the relationship between brain activity and behavioral measure. Although JIVE has been suggested as an improved alternative to PLS and CCA (for details, Yu et al., 2017), COBE was shown to be better in estimating the common components compared to JIVE (for details, Zhou, Cichocki, et al., 2016).
Altogether our COBE‐based approach highlighted that there exists heterogeneity between participants even in healthy control populations. Such heterogeneities are often ignored in comparison studies (healthy vs. disease, control vs. patient, etc.) because the behavioral manifestations of the observed heterogeneities are not well mapped. To this, we looked for the traits that are different between the two groups. We compared the two groups for 62 behavioral measures and 7 physiological parameters spanning eight categories that include an individual's lifestyle habits, cognitive skills, and physical health. Four behavioral measures (use of marijuana, illicit drugs, alcohol, and tobacco) from the substance use category, and one behavioral measure (antisocial personality problem) from the psychiatric and life function category had significant difference (p < 1e−5) between the two groups. The findings suggested that high substance use and problems of antisocial personality might alter the pattern of common COBE component shared by multiple scans of rs‐fMRI. However, it is equally plausible that the observed alterations in common COBE component might indicate vulnerability toward substance use and antisocial personality problems. Interestingly, we also found sex differences between the two groups, with percentage of males being higher in the MCD. Never the less, the existence of subtypes within healthy subjects highlighted that the general categorization of subjects based on external symptoms (e.g., healthy vs. diseased, control vs. patient, etc.) might need to consider aspects of a subject's lifestyle habits, and psychological factors as they have an impact on the rs‐fMRI features.
It was interesting to find that MCD group reported higher use of marijuana, illicit drugs, alcohol, and tobacco. This was coherent with our observation of reduced weight of the common COBE components in the PCC constituting the core region of DMN‐A. Task‐based fMRI meta‐analysis has reported PCC areas in the DMN to be affected in substance seekers (Blest‐Hopley, Giampietro, & Bhattacharyya, 2018; Yanes et al., 2018). The rs‐fMRI analysis revealed that longer use of cannabis is associated with decreased low frequency power of the DMN (Thijssen et al., 2017). The PCC, a key region in the DMN is responsible for self‐consciousness and self‐referential mental thoughts (Andrews‐Hanna, Reidler, Sepulcre, Poulin, & Buckner, 2010; Andrews‐Hanna, Smallwood, & Spreng, 2014; Buckner & Carroll, 2007; Cavanna & Trimble, 2006; Fransson & Marrelec, 2008; Leech & Sharp, 2013; Raichle et al., 2001; Spreng, Mar, & Kim, 2008). Marijuana users were also observed to have alteration in the PCC (Kim et al., 2011). Similarly, individuals with alcohol use disorder, (Chanraud, Pitel, Pfefferbaum, & Sullivan, 2011) reported desynchronized low frequency rs‐fMRI signals from the PCC and cerebellum. Nicotine (a key ingredient of tobacco) was also associated with decreased activity in the regions of DMN (Tanabe et al., 2011) and low overall functional connectivity (Cheng et al., 2019). From the vast pool of scientific studies, it can be inferred that tobacco and drugs intake, along with marijuana and alcohol use are associated with differences in rs time courses (Cheng et al., 2014), and are possibly making adolescents more vulnerable to psychiatric and other disorders (Thijssen et al., 2017).
The subjects in the MCD were found to have a predisposition toward antisocial personality problems. It is important to mention here that the interrelationship between substance use, tobacco‐intake, alcoholism, and antisocial behavior is complex (Helstrom, Bryan, Hutchison, Riggs, & Blechman, 2004). These factors are highly correlated both cross‐sectionally and across the lifespan, share common risk factors, and are predictive of negative life outcomes (Compton, Conway, Stinson, Colliver, & Grant, 2005; Kendler, Prescott, Myers, & Neale, 2003; Krueger, Markon, Patrick, Benning, & Kramer, 2007; Moffitt, 2017; Windle, 1990). About 40–50% of individuals with a substance use disorder meet the criteria for antisocial personality disorder and approximately 90% of individuals diagnosed with an antisocial personality disorder also have a co‐occurring substance use disorder (Messina, Farabee, & Rawson, 2003). Results have also suggested that smoking and alcohol use act as mediators between externalizing problems and marijuana and other drug use (Helstrom et al., 2004). Adolescents with antisocial behavior have reported reduced connectivity in the DMN (Broulidakis et al., 2016; Zhou, Yao, et al., 2016). This is not surprising because DMN has an indispensable role in social understanding of others (Li et al., 2014) and the areas responsible for social cognition partly overlap with the DMN (Corbetta, Patel, & Shulman, 2008; Mars et al., 2012; Schilbach, Eickhoff, Rotarska‐Jagiela, Fink, & Vogeley, 2008).
Even though we identified the common component shared by multiple runs of rs‐fMRI of an individual and observed its distribution across all networks; it is difficult to conclude whether the lower weights in the DMN‐A areas accounted for the observed behavioral differences. In a longitudinal study from a large sample (n = 1,293) of subjects, it was found that adolescents who reported more antisocial behavior at age 14 were more likely to smoke daily, to drink heavily, and to use illicit drugs at age 17 (Adalbjarnardottir & Rafnsson, 2002). Valuing the association between antisocial personality and other behaviors reported in existing literature, we investigated the correlation between the scores of antisocial personality with other four behavioral measures and found weak correlation for all the pairs (see Supporting Information). Never the less, considering that such causal relationship between antisocial behavior and addiction is strong, future studies could investigate whether pre‐existing differences in DMN leads to antisocial behavior in individuals or whether the differences in DMN are the effect of antisocial behavior. With that said, the present study was an exploratory analysis that found meaningful differences in rs‐fMRI features within subgroups of healthy individuals that had the distinctions translated to behavioral measures. The groups differed in their ratio of number of males to females, clearly reflecting that the MCD group comprised more males (see Table S1). Although this might indicate that the rate of substance and antisocial personality problems are higher in males (Cale & Lilienfeld, 2002; Cooper & Craft, 2018; Petker et al., 2019), we cross‐validated the findings to determine if the gender variable could be driving the between‐group (MCD and CS) differences in the behaviors. For this, we created the RSSM group and accounted for the behaviors that are different between MCD and RSSM. The differences in the two groups (RSSM and MCD) remained significant for use of marijuana and illicit drugs only, and not for tobacco consumption, alcohol intake, and antisocial personality problems. This additional analysis had an impact on our overall inference regarding the effect of sex on the behavioral differences between MCD and CS group. The results show that it is unlikely that the between‐group difference (MCD and CS) in marijuana and illicit drug use is related to sex. Similarly, the behaviors for which differences were nonsignificant suggested that there exists a group in which an interesting sex difference might also relate to the elevated rates of tobacco consumption, alcohol intake, and antisocial personality problems seen in men.
Here, we used COBE for analyzing multisession rsfMRI to identify subtypes in healthy population. A natural question therefore arises as to why individuals with dissimilar COBEs themselves formed a coherent subgroup? To understand this, we explored the variation of the common COBE component within the subjects in the MCD group with the five behavioral measure (see Supporting Information). A weaker correlation observed between the behaviors and variance of the common COBE component made it difficult to comprehend the reason behind such dissociation. However, it does confirm that maximizing the dissimilarity in the extracted features holds the potential to accentuate a coherent subgroup. Never the less, our findings highlighted the importance of considering between‐subject heterogeneity even in healthy control populations (Kebets et al., 2019); and conveys a subtle message to the comparison studies (healthy vs. disease, control vs. patient, etc.) to recruit healthy/control subjects based not only on the features relevant to their study, but to also consider the factors that might lead to disparateness in subject's habit and psyche.
Here, we would also like to point out few limitations of the study. From the data perspective, it would have been interesting to obtain additional information about the last use of substance, marijuana as well as tobacco and alcohol. We mention this because an individual's substance use status (dose, duration, etc.) at the time of scanning can affect the COBE indices. In the absence of such vital information, it is difficult to rationalize the state versus trait effects of substance use on the common COBE component of a subject. From the approach perspective, we would like to point out that only cortex‐based parcellation was used in our analysis. Subcortical structures as well as cerebellum were considered as 19 individual nodes in the analysis. We cannot deny the crucial role these vital structures have in substance use and personality disorders (Manza, Tomasi, & Volkow, 2018). We would therefore advise future studies to use integrated parcellations that detail the cortex, cerebellum, and the subcortical structures. Keeping these limitations in mind, we look forward to applying the present method on patient groups to identify meaningful disorder subtypes (Drysdale et al., 2017; Zhang et al., 2016).
Finally, it is important to note that the results obtained by our present study are consistent with the results obtained by Smith et al. (2015), though they adopted a different methodology. They applied CCA on the resting state functional connectivity matrices of 461 HCP subjects with 158 behavioral measures to obtain a single mode of brain‐behavior covariation. They found that vast majority of the positive behaviors (e.g., education, income, IQ, life‐satisfaction, etc.) correlate positively with this mode, and similarly sets of negative behaviors (e.g., anger, sleep quality, alcohol intake, substance abuse, Marijuana use, etc.) correlate negatively. Interestingly, they also found DMN regions mainly the medial frontal and parietal cortex in the temporo‐parietal junction and in anterior insula and frontal operculum to contribute strongly. It cannot be denied that the CCA mode provided a distinct overview of the brain‐behavior organization, however, the ability to generate explainable insights at the individual level (subject or behavior specific) was limited. For example, the set of areas belonging to DMN were found to be involved across a wide range of behaviors that surfaced from the first CCA mode; however, association of certain behaviors (reading, aggression, working memory, etc.) with these exclusive areas might be of debate. In contrast, our study had a tapered point of view. Using the common signal from the multiple fMRI runs, we focused to delineate subgroups within healthy individuals whose rs pattern (mainly in the PCC area of DMN) was dissimilar. The dissimilarity translated to significant differences across six (out of 69) negative behaviors (including gender), which were also a part of the behavioral measures found significant in the CCA mode (Smith et al., 2015). It is noteworthy to mention that two exploratory studies reporting similar resting‐state fMRI‐behavior relationship by using different methodologies emphasize the reliability of the findings. Last but not least, the reproducibility of the prime findings is also one of the important purposes of generating big public datasets.
5. CONCLUSION
In this work, we decomposed the rs‐fMRI runs of a single HCP subject to extract the common component shared by their multiple runs. We classified those subjects whose common component is as different as possible. In those subjects, we found that the strength of the common component was reduced in the DMN areas. Interestingly, we also found these subjects to have higher usage of marijuana, illicit drugs, alcohol, and tobacco, and have a predisposition toward antisocial personality problems. The existence of subtypes within healthy individuals that have meaningful differences in their resting‐state patterns conveys that the general categorization of subjects based only on external symptoms (e.g., healthy vs. diseased, control vs. patient, etc.) should also consider aspects of a healthy subject's lifestyle habits and psyche. Overall, the exploratory approach and the findings convey the importance of considering the factors associated with between‐subject heterogeneity in healthy control populations.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest.
Supporting information
Appendix S1: Supplemental Results
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This work was supported by Nanyang Technological University Start‐Up Grant (NTU‐SUG). The work was also funded by National University of Singapore Strategic Research (DPRT/944/09/14), NUS SOM Aspiration Fund (R185000271720), Singapore NMRC (CBRG/0088/2015), NUS YIA, and the Singapore National Research Foundation (NRF) Fellowship (Class of 2017). Our research also utilized resources provided by the Center for Functional Neuroimaging Technologies, P41EB015896 and instruments supported by 1S10RR023401, 1S10RR019307, and 1S10RR023043 from the Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging at the Massachusetts General Hospital. Our computational work was partially performed on resources of the National Supercomputing Centre, Singapore (https://www.nscc.sg). Data were provided by the Human Connectome Project, WU‐Minn Consortium (Principal Investigators: David Van Essen and Kamil Ugurbil; 1U54MH091657) funded by the 16 NIH Institutes and Centers that support the NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research; and by the McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience at Washington University.
Kashyap R, Bhattacharjee S, Yeo BTT, Chen SHA. Maximizing dissimilarity in resting state detects heterogeneous subtypes in healthy population associated with high substance use and problems in antisocial personality. Hum Brain Mapp. 2020;41:1261–1273. 10.1002/hbm.24873
Funding information Singapore National Research Foundation (NRF) Fellowship; NUS YIA; Singapore NMRC, Grant/Award Number: CBRG/0088/2015; NUS SOM Aspiration Fund, Grant/Award Number: R185000271720; National University of Singapore Strategic Research, Grant/Award Number: DPRT/944/09/14; Nanyang Technological University Start‐Up Grant (NTU‐SUG)
Contributor Information
Rajan Kashyap, Email: rajankashyap6@gmail.com.
S. H. Annabel Chen, Email: annabelchen@ntu.edu.sg.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
Human Connectome Project data is widely available to researchers. The relevant code written in MATLAB can be obtained from https://github.com/ClinicalBrainLab/Maximizing_Dissimilarity_in_fMRI.
REFERENCES
- Adalbjarnardottir, S. , & Rafnsson, F. D. (2002). Adolescent antisocial behavior and substance use: Longitudinal analyses. Addictive Behaviors, 27(2), 227–240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrews‐Hanna, J. R. , Reidler, J. S. , Sepulcre, J. , Poulin, R. , & Buckner, R. L. (2010). Functional‐anatomic fractionation of the brain's default network. Neuron, 65(4), 550–562. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrews‐Hanna, J. R. , Smallwood, J. , & Spreng, R. N. (2014). The default network and self‐generated thought: Component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1316(1), 29–52. 10.1111/nyas.12360 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bertolero, M. A. , Yeo, B. T. T. , Bassett, D. S. , & D'Esposito, M. (2018). Strong hubs facilitate network modularity and cognitive performance. Nature Human Behavior, 2, 765–777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bijsterbosch, J. , Harrison, S. , Duff, E. , Alfaro‐Almagro, F. , Woolrich, M. , & Smith, S. (2017). Investigations into within‐ and between‐subject resting‐state amplitude variations. NeuroImage, 159, 57–69. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.07.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biswal, B. , Yetkin, F. Z. , Haughton, V. M. , & Hyde, J. S. (1995). Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo‐planar mri. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 34(4), 537–541. 10.1002/mrm.1910340409 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blest‐Hopley, G. , Giampietro, V. , & Bhattacharyya, S. (2018). Regular cannabis use is associated with altered activation of central executive and default mode networks even after prolonged abstinence in adolescent users: Results from a complementary meta‐analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 96, 45–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broulidakis, M. J. , Fairchild, G. , Sully, K. , Blumensath, T. , Darekar, A. , & Sonuga‐Barke, E. J. (2016). Reduced default mode connectivity in adolescents with conduct disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(9), 800–808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner, R. L. , Andrews‐Hanna, J. R. , & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain's default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124(1), 1–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner, R. L. , & Carroll, D. C. (2007). Self‐projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 49–57. 10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner, R. L. , Krienen, F. M. , & Yeo, B. T. (2013). Opportunities and limitations of intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Nature Neuroscience, 16(7), 832–837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgess, G. C. , Kandala, S. , Nolan, D. , Laumann, T. O. , Power, J. D. , Adeyemo, B. , … Barch, D. M. (2016). Evaluation of denoising strategies to address motion‐correlated artifacts in resting‐state functional magnetic resonance imaging data from the human connectome project. Brain Connectivity, 6(9), 669–680. 10.1089/brain.2016.0435 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cale, E. M. , & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2002). Sex differences in psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder: A review and integration. Clinical Psychology Review, 22(8), 1179–1207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavanna, A. E. , & Trimble, M. R. (2006). The precuneus: A review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain, 129(3), 564–583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chanraud, S. , Pitel, A.‐L. , Pfefferbaum, A. , & Sullivan, E. V. (2011). Disruption of functional connectivity of the default‐mode network in alcoholism. Cerebral Cortex, 21(10), 2272–2281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng, H. , Skosnik, P. D. , Pruce, B. J. , Brumbaugh, M. S. , Vollmer, J. M. , Fridberg, D. J. , … Newman, S. D. (2014). Resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging reveals distinct brain activity in heavy cannabis users—A multi‐voxel pattern analysis. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 28(11), 1030–1040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng, W. , Rolls, E. T. , Robbins, T. W. , Gong, W. , Liu, Z. , Lv, W. , … Quinlan, E. B. (2019). Decreased brain connectivity in smoking contrasts with increased connectivity in drinking. eLife, 8, e40765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole, M. W. , Bassett, D. S. , Power, J. D. , Braver, T. S. , & Petersen, S. E. (2014). Intrinsic and task‐evoked network architectures of the human brain. Neuron, 83(1), 238–251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Compton, W. M. , Conway, K. P. , Stinson, F. S. , Colliver, J. D. , & Grant, B. F. (2005). Prevalence, correlates, and comorbidity of DSM‐IV antisocial personality syndromes and alcohol and specific drug use disorders in the United States: Results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 66(6), 677–685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper, Z. D. , & Craft, R. M. (2018). Sex‐dependent effects of cannabis and cannabinoids: A translational perspective. Neuropsychopharmacology, 43(1), 34–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbetta, M. , Patel, G. , & Shulman, G. L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: From environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58(3), 306–324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drysdale, A. T. , Grosenick, L. , Downar, J. , Dunlop, K. , Mansouri, F. , Meng, Y. , … Schatzberg, A. F. (2017). Resting‐state connectivity biomarkers define neurophysiological subtypes of depression. Nature Medicine, 23(1), 28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dubois, J. , Galdi, P. , Han, Y. , Paul, L. K. , & Adolphs, R. (2018). Resting‐state functional brain connectivity best predicts the personality dimension of openness to experience. Personality Neuroscience, 1, e6. 10.1017/pen.2018.8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finn, E. S. , Shen, X. , Scheinost, D. , Rosenberg, M. D. , Huang, J. , Chun, M. M. , … Constable, R. T. (2015). Functional connectome fingerprinting: Identifying individuals using patterns of brain connectivity. Nature Neuroscience, 18(11), 1664–1671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl, B. , Salat, D. H. , Busa, E. , Albert, M. , Dieterich, M. , Haselgrove, C. , … Klaveness, S. (2002). Whole brain segmentation: Automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron, 33(3), 341–355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox, M. D. , & Raichle, M. E. (2007). Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(9), 700–711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fransson, P. , & Marrelec, G. (2008). The precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex plays a pivotal role in the default mode network: Evidence from a partial correlation network analysis. NeuroImage, 42(3), 1178–1184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glasser, M. F. , Sotiropoulos, S. N. , Wilson, J. A. , Coalson, T. S. , Fischl, B. , Andersson, J. L. , … Polimeni, J. R. (2013). The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. NeuroImage, 80, 105–124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gorgolewski, K. J. , Mendes, N. , Wilfling, D. , Wladimirow, E. , Gauthier, C. J. , Bonnen, T. , … Cozatl, R. (2015). A high resolution 7‐Tesla resting‐state fMRI test‐retest dataset with cognitive and physiological measures. Scientific Data, 2, 140054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gratton, C. , Laumann, T. O. , Nielsen, A. N. , Greene, D. J. , Gordon, E. M. , Gilmore, A. W. , … Schlaggar, B. L. (2018). Functional brain networks are dominated by stable group and individual factors, not cognitive or daily variation. Neuron, 98(2), 439–452. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffanti, L. , Salimi‐Khorshidi, G. , Beckmann, C. F. , Auerbach, E. J. , Douaud, G. , Sexton, C. E. , … Mackay, C. E. (2014). ICA‐based artefact removal and accelerated fMRI acquisition for improved resting state network imaging. NeuroImage, 95, 232–247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gusnard, D. A. , & Raichle, M. E. (2001). Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(10), 685–694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hampson, M. , Driesen, N. R. , Skudlarski, P. , Gore, J. C. , & Constable, R. T. (2006). Brain connectivity related to working memory performance. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(51), 13338–13343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helstrom, A. , Bryan, A. , Hutchison, K. E. , Riggs, P. D. , & Blechman, E. A. (2004). Tobacco and alcohol use as an explanation for the association between externalizing behavior and illicit drug use among delinquent adolescents. Prevention Science, 5(4), 267–277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson, M. , Bannister, P. , Brady, M. , & Smith, S. (2002). Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. NeuroImage, 17(2), 825–841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kashyap, R. , Kong, R. , Bhattacharjee, S. , Li, J. , Zhou, J. , & Yeo, B. T. (2019). Individual‐specific fMRI‐subspaces improve functional connectivity prediction of behavior. NeuroImage, 189, 804–812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kebets, V. , Holmes, A. J. , Orban, C. , Tang, S. , Li, J. , Sun, N. , … Yeo, B. T. (2019). Somatosensory‐motor dysconnectivity spans multiple transdiagnostic dimensions of psychopathology. Biological Psychiatry, 86, 779–791. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.06.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler, K. S. , Prescott, C. A. , Myers, J. , & Neale, M. C. (2003). The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance use disorders in men and women. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(9), 929–937. 10.1001/archpsyc.60.9.929 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim, D.‐J. , Skosnik, P. D. , Cheng, H. , Pruce, B. J. , Brumbaugh, M. S. , Vollmer, J. M. , … Newman, S. D. (2011). Structural network topology revealed by white matter tractography in cannabis users: A graph theoretical analysis. Brain Connectivity, 1(6), 473–483. 10.1089/brain.2011.0053 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kong, R. , Li, J. , Orban, C. , Sabuncu, M. R. , Liu, H. , Schaefer, A. , … Eickhoff, S. B. (2018). Spatial topography of individual‐specific cortical networks predicts human cognition, personality, and emotion. Cerebral Cortex, 29(6), 2533–2551. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krienen, F. M. , Yeo, B. T. , & Buckner, R. L. (2014). Reconfigurable task‐dependent functional coupling modes cluster around a core functional architecture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1653), 20130526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krishnan, A. , Williams, L. J. , McIntosh, A. R. , & Abdi, H. (2011). Partial least squares (PLS) methods for neuroimaging: A tutorial and review. NeuroImage, 56(2), 455–475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krueger, R. F. , Markon, K. E. , Patrick, C. J. , Benning, S. D. , & Kramer, M. D. (2007). Linking antisocial behavior, substance use, and personality: An integrative quantitative model of the adult externalizing spectrum. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(4), 645–666. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leech, R. , & Sharp, D. J. (2013). The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain, 137(1), 12–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li, J. , Kong, R. , Liégeois, R. , Orban, C. , Tan, Y. , Sun, N. , … Yeo, B. T. T. (2019). Global signal regression strengthens association between resting‐state functional connectivity and behavior. NeuroImage, 196, 126–141. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.04.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li, W. , Mai, X. , & Liu, C. (2014). The default mode network and social understanding of others: What do brain connectivity studies tell us. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manza, P. , Tomasi, D. , & Volkow, N. D. (2018). Subcortical local functional hyperconnectivity in cannabis dependence. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3(3), 285–293. 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.11.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mars, R. B. , Neubert, F.‐X. , Noonan, M. P. , Sallet, J. , Toni, I. , & Rushworth, M. F. S. (2012). On the relationship between the “default mode network” and the “social brain.”. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 189. 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00189 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGonigle, D. J. , Howseman, A. M. , Athwal, B. S. , Friston, K. J. , Frackowiak, R. S. J. , & Holmes, A. P. (2000). Variability in fMRI: An examination of intersession differences. NeuroImage, 11(6), 708–734. 10.1006/nimg.2000.0562 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mennes, M. , Kelly, C. , Zuo, X.‐N. , Di Martino, A. , Biswal, B. B. , Castellanos, F. X. , & Milham, M. P. (2010). Inter‐individual differences in resting‐state functional connectivity predict task‐induced BOLD activity. NeuroImage, 50(4), 1690–1701. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Messina, N. , Farabee, D. , & Rawson, R. (2003). Treatment responsivity of cocaine‐dependent patients with antisocial personality disorder to cognitive‐behavioral and contingency management interventions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71(2), 320–329. 10.1037/0022-006X.71.2.320 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moffitt, T. E. (2017). Adolescence‐limited and life‐course‐persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy In Biosocial Theories of Crime (pp. 69–96). Routledge. 10.4324/9781315096278-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petker, T. , Owens, M. M. , Amlung, M. T. , Oshri, A. , Sweet, L. H. , & MacKillop, J. (2019). Cannabis involvement and neuropsychological performance: Findings from the Human Connectome Project. Journal of Psychiatry Neuroscience, 44(5), 1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Power, J. D. , Barnes, K. A. , Snyder, A. Z. , Schlaggar, B. L. , & Petersen, S. E. (2012). Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. NeuroImage, 59(3), 2142–2154. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raichle, M. E. , MacLeod, A. M. , Snyder, A. Z. , Powers, W. J. , Gusnard, D. A. , & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(2), 676–682. 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenberg, M. D. , Finn, E. S. , Scheinost, D. , Papademetris, X. , Shen, X. , Constable, R. T. , & Chun, M. M. (2016). A neuromarker of sustained attention from whole‐brain functional connectivity. Nature Neuroscience, 19(1), 165–171. 10.1038/nn.4179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salimi‐Khorshidi, G. , Douaud, G. , Beckmann, C. F. , Glasser, M. F. , Griffanti, L. , & Smith, S. M. (2014). Automatic denoising of functional MRI data: Combining independent component analysis and hierarchical fusion of classifiers. NeuroImage, 90, 449–468. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaefer, A. , Kong, R. , Gordon, E. M. , Laumann, T. O. , Zuo, X.‐N. , Holmes, A. J. , … Yeo, B. T. T. (2018). Local‐global parcellation of the human cerebral cortex from intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Cerebral Cortex, 28(9), 3095–3114. 10.1093/cercor/bhx179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schilbach, L. , Eickhoff, S. B. , Rotarska‐Jagiela, A. , Fink, G. R. , & Vogeley, K. (2008). Minds at rest? Social cognition as the default mode of cognizing and its putative relationship to the default system of the brain. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(2), 457–467. 10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shirer, W. R. , Ryali, S. , Rykhlevskaia, E. , Menon, V. , & Greicius, M. D. (2012). Decoding subject‐driven cognitive states with whole‐brain connectivity patterns. Cerebral Cortex, 22(1), 158–165. 10.1093/cercor/bhr099 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegel, J. S. , Mitra, A. , Laumann, T. O. , Seitzman, B. A. , Raichle, M. , Corbetta, M. , & Snyder, A. Z. (2017). Data quality influences observed links between functional connectivity and behavior. Cerebral Cortex, 27(9), 4492–4502. 10.1093/cercor/bhw253 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith, S. M. , Beckmann, C. F. , Andersson, J. , Auerbach, E. J. , Bijsterbosch, J. , Douaud, G. , … Glasser, M. F. (2013). Resting‐state fMRI in the Human Connectome Project. NeuroImage, 80, 144–168. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith, S. M. , Beckmann, C. F. , Ramnani, N. , Woolrich, M. W. , Bannister, P. R. , Jenkinson, M. , … McGonigle, D. J. (2005). Variability in fMRI: A re‐examination of inter‐session differences. Human Brain Mapping, 24(3), 248–257. 10.1002/hbm.20080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith, S. M. , Fox, P. T. , Miller, K. L. , Glahn, D. C. , Fox, P. M. , Mackay, C. E. , … Beckmann, C. F. (2009). Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States, 106(31), 13040–13045. 10.1073/pnas.0905267106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith, S. M. , Nichols, T. E. , Vidaurre, D. , Winkler, A. M. , Behrens, T. E. J. , Glasser, M. F. , … Miller, K. L. (2015). A positive‐negative mode of population covariation links brain connectivity, demographics and behavior. Nature Neuroscience, 18(11), 1565–1567. 10.1038/nn.4125 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spreng, R. N. , Mar, R. A. , & Kim, A. S. N. (2008). The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: A quantitative meta‐analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(3), 489–510. 10.1162/jocn.2008.21029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanabe, J. , Nyberg, E. , Martin, L.F. , Martin, J. , Cordes, D. , Kronberg, E. & Tregellas, J.R. (2011). Nicotine effects on default mode network during resting state. Psychopharmacology, 216(2), 287–295. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tavor, I. , Jones, O. P. , Mars, R. B. , Smith, S. M. , Behrens, T. E. , & Jbabdi, S. (2016). Task‐free MRI predicts individual differences in brain activity during task performance. Science, 352(6282), 216–220. 10.1126/science.aad8127 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thijssen, S. , Rashid, B. , Gopal, S. , Nyalakanti, P. , Calhoun, V. D. , & Kiehl, K. A. (2017). Regular cannabis and alcohol use is associated with resting‐state time course power spectra in incarcerated adolescents. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 178, 492–500. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.05.045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Heuvel, M. P. , & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2010). Exploring the brain network: A review on resting‐state fMRI functional connectivity. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 20(8), 519–534. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.03.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Essen, D. C. , Ugurbil, K. , Auerbach, E. , Barch, D. , Behrens, T. E. J. , Bucholz, R. , … Yacoub, E. (2012). The Human Connectome Project: A data acquisition perspective. NeuroImage, 62(4), 2222–2231. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang, C. , Ong, J. L. , Patanaik, A. , Zhou, J. , & Chee, M. W. L. (2016). Spontaneous eyelid closures link vigilance fluctuation with fMRI dynamic connectivity states. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(34), 9653–9658. 10.1073/pnas.1523980113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Windle, M. (1990). A longitudinal study of antisocial behaviors in early adolescence as predictors of late adolescent substance use: Gender and ethnic group differences. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99(1), 86–91. 10.1037/0021-843X.99.1.86 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yanes, J. A. , Riedel, M. C. , Ray, K. L. , Kirkland, A. E. , Bird, R. T. , Boeving, E. R. , … Sutherland, M. T. (2018). Neuroimaging meta‐analysis of cannabis use studies reveals convergent functional alterations in brain regions supporting cognitive control and reward processing. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 32(3), 283–295. 10.1177/0269881117744995 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeo, B. T. T. , Krienen, F. M. , Eickhoff, S. B. , Yaakub, S. N. , Fox, P. T. , Buckner, R. L. , … Chee, M. W. L. (2015). Functional specialization and flexibility in human association cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 25(10), 3654–3672. 10.1093/cercor/bhu217 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeo, B. T. T. , Krienen, F. M. , Sepulcre, J. , Sabuncu, M. R. , Lashkari, D. , Hollinshead, M. , … Buckner, R. L. (2011). The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106(3), 1125–1165. 10.1152/jn.00338.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeo, B. T. T. , Tandi, J. , & Chee, M. W. L. (2015). Functional connectivity during rested wakefulness predicts vulnerability to sleep deprivation. NeuroImage, 111, 147–158. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu, Q. , Risk, B. B. , Zhang, K. , & Marron, J. S. (2017). JIVE integration of imaging and behavioral data. NeuroImage, 152, 38–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang, X. , Mormino, E. C. , Sun, N. , Sperling, R. A. , Sabuncu, M. R. , Yeo, B. T. T. , & Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative . (2016). Bayesian model reveals latent atrophy factors with dissociable cognitive trajectories in Alzheimer's disease. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(42), E6535–E6544. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou, G. , Cichocki, A. , Zhang, Y. , & Mandic, D. P. (2016). Group component analysis for multiblock data: Common and individual feature extraction. IEEE Transactions on Neural Networks and Learning Systems, 27(11), 2426–2439. 10.1109/TNNLS.2015.2487364 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou, G. , Zhao, Q. , Zhang, Y. , Adalı, T. , Xie, S. , & Cichocki, A. (2016). Linked component analysis from matrices to high‐order tensors: Applications to biomedical data. Proceedings of the IEEE, 104(2), 310–331. 10.1109/JPROC.2015.2474704 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou, J. , Yao, N. , Fairchild, G. , Cao, X. , Zhang, Y. , Xiang, Y.‐T. , … Wang, X. (2016). Disrupted default mode network connectivity in male adolescents with conduct disorder. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 10(4), 995–1003. 10.1007/s11682-015-9465-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Appendix S1: Supplemental Results
Data Availability Statement
Human Connectome Project data is widely available to researchers. The relevant code written in MATLAB can be obtained from https://github.com/ClinicalBrainLab/Maximizing_Dissimilarity_in_fMRI.