Abstract
Pain is a complex phenomenon that is served by neural oscillations and connectivity involving different brain areas and frequencies. Here, we aimed to systematically and comprehensively assess the pattern of neural oscillations and connectivity characterizing the state of tonic experimental pain in humans. To this end, we applied 10‐min heat pain stimuli consecutively to the right and left hand of 39 healthy participants and recorded electroencephalography. We systematically analyzed global and local measures of oscillatory brain activity, connectivity, and graph theory‐based network measures during tonic pain and compared them to a nonpainful control condition. Local measures showed suppressions of oscillatory activity at alpha frequencies together with stronger connectivity at alpha and beta frequencies in sensorimotor areas during tonic pain. Furthermore, sensorimotor areas contralateral to stimulation showed significantly increased connectivity to a common area in the medial prefrontal cortex at alpha frequencies. Together, these observations indicate that the state of tonic experimental pain is associated with a sensorimotor‐prefrontal network connected at alpha frequencies. These findings represent a step further toward understanding the brain mechanisms underlying long‐lasting pain states in health and disease.
Keywords: brain networks, electroencephalography, functional connectivity, graph theory, neuronal oscillations, tonic pain
1. INTRODUCTION
Pain is a complex phenomenon which serves to protect the body. In contrast, ongoing pain in chronic pain syndromes does no longer serve protective functions but represents a pathological condition with detrimental effects on quality of life. In the brain, the experience of pain is associated with the activation of an extended network of brain areas (Baliki & Apkarian, 2015; Garcia‐Larrea & Peyron, 2013). This network yields pain‐related neural oscillations at different frequencies ranging from infra‐slow oscillations below 0.1 Hz to theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–13 Hz), beta (13–29 Hz), and gamma (30–100 Hz) frequencies (Ploner, Sorg, & Gross, 2017). In addition, pain depends on the communication between different brain areas, that is, on functional connectivity (Apkarian & Chialvo, 2006; Kucyi & Davis, 2015; Mano & Seymour, 2015; Tracey, 2005). Functional connectivity can be observed at different frequencies, which are thought to represent complementary, for example, short‐range and long‐range or bottom‐up and top‐down, communication processes (Fries, 2015; Ploner et al., 2017; Siegel, Donner, & Engel, 2012).
So far, most evidence on the cerebral processing of pain refers to brief experimental pain stimuli whose brain mechanisms likely differ from those of tonic pain states (Baliki & Apkarian, 2015; Kuner & Flor, 2017). Comparatively few electroencephalography (EEG), magnetoencephalography (MEG), and intracranial recordings have investigated the brain processing of tonic experimental pain stimuli. They have shown local suppressions of alpha and beta oscillations in sensorimotor cortices (Colon, Liberati, & Mouraux, 2017; Giehl, Meyer‐Brandis, Kunz, & Lautenbacher, 2014; Huishi Zhang, Sohrabpour, Lu, & He, 2016; Nickel, May, Tiemann, Schmidt, et al., 2017; Nir, Sinai, Moont, Harari, & Yarnitsky, 2012; Peng, Hu, Zhang, & Hu, 2014; Schulz et al., 2015) and insula (Liberati et al., 2019) as well as increases of gamma oscillations in prefrontal areas (Nickel, May, Tiemann, Schmidt, et al., 2017; Peng et al., 2014; Schulz et al., 2015). Some studies revealed changes of the peak frequency of neural oscillations (Furman et al., 2018; Nir, Sinai, Raz, Sprecher, & Yarnitsky, 2010). Moreover, a few recent investigations have shown changes of functional connectivity at alpha and beta frequencies during tonic pain (Gram et al., 2017; Huishi Zhang et al., 2016; Levitt, Choo, Smith, LeBlanc, & Saab, 2017; Martel et al., 2017). However, these studies addressed either oscillatory brain activity or functional connectivity and either global, that is, whole‐brain summary measures, or local measures. Thus, a comprehensive picture of local and global neural oscillations and functional connectivity up to the whole‐brain network level associated with tonic experimental pain is lacking so far.
In the present study, we therefore aimed to comprehensively characterize the pattern of neural oscillations and functional connectivity reflecting the state of tonic experimental pain in humans. To this end, we applied tonic heat pain stimuli to the hands of 39 healthy participants and recorded EEG. We systematically analyzed global and local oscillatory brain activity and connectivity and graph theory‐based network measures during tonic pain and compared them to a nonpainful control condition.
2. METHODS
2.1. Participants
Fifty‐one healthy human participants (age 24.7 ± 5.6 years [mean ± SD], 24 female) participated in the study. All participants were right‐handed and gave written informed consent. Due to technical issues with the stimulation device, data sets of 12 participants had to be excluded, resulting in a final sample size of 39 participants (age 24.3 ± 5.6 years, 18 female). The study was approved by the ethics committee of the TUM School of Medicine of the Technical University of Munich. Other analyses of the same data set which addressed the instantaneous pain intensity rather than the state of tonic pain have been published previously (Nickel, May, Tiemann, Postorino, et al., 2017; Nickel, May, Tiemann, Schmidt, et al., 2017).
2.2. Paradigm
The experiment consisted of two tonic pain conditions and two control conditions. In the pain conditions, painful heat stimuli with duration of 10 min were applied to the dorsum of the left (pain left) or the right hand (pain right). Apart from the side of stimulation, the two pain conditions were identical. Left‐ and right‐hand stimulations were performed to test the replicability of findings and to disentangle stimulus location‐dependent and stimulus location‐independent neural processes. In the control conditions, the temperature of the stimulation device was set at 32°C, close to skin temperature. The order of the two pain conditions was counterbalanced across participants and control conditions always followed the respective pain condition. During all conditions, participants wore headphones playing white noise to cancel out ambient noise.
In the pain conditions, painful heat stimuli were applied using a thermode (TSA‐II, Medoc, Ramat Yishai, Israel). While the time course of stimulation was the same for all participants, stimulus intensity varied with a rate of 0.1°C/s between three individually adjusted temperature levels (low, medium, and high) exceeding the individual pain threshold temperature (see below) by 0.5, 0.8, or 1.1°C, respectively. The total stimulation time course consisted of 9 plateaus of 40, 50, or 60 s duration with 3 plateaus for each temperature level. During stimulation, participants continuously rated the perceived pain intensity on a visual analogue scale ranging from no pain to worst tolerable pain (0–100) using a custom‐built finger‐span device with the nonstimulated hand. Data between the start of the first temperature plateau and the end of the last decrease in stimulation temperature was included in the analysis, resulting in an 8.2 min time window for analysis. To determine the pain threshold temperature, participants were asked to adjust the stimulation temperature over the course of 3 min to their individual pain threshold before the first pain condition. The pain threshold was defined as the average stimulation temperature during the last 10 s of this procedure. The hand for which the threshold was assessed was counterbalanced across subjects and the same threshold was used to determine stimulus intensities for both hands. Mean pain threshold temperature was 44.7 ± 1.1°C (mean ± SD), resulting in an average stimulus intensity of 45.5 ± 1.1°C during the 8.2 min window of analysis. Average pain ratings of the pain left and pain right condition were 53.3 ± 23.0 and 49.6 ± 23.4, respectively. In the two control conditions, the thermode remained attached to the same hand as in the respective pain condition, but at a neutral temperature of 32°C. In these conditions, participants performed a rating procedure similar to the pain conditions, tracking the visually displayed and temporally inverted pain ratings of the respective pain conditions with the finger‐span device. For further details of the procedure please refer to Nickel, May, Tiemann, Schmidt, et al. (2017).
Stimulus presentation and timing was controlled using MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, MA) and the Psychophysics Toolbox (http://psychtoolbox.org/).
2.3. Recordings and preprocessing
EEG data were recorded using an electrode montage of 64 electrodes consisting of all 10–20 system electrodes and the additional electrodes Fpz, CPz, POz, Oz, Iz, AF3/4, F5/6, FC1/2/3/4/5/6, FT7/8/9/10, C1/2/5/6, CP1/2/3/4/5/6, TP7/8/9/10, P5/6, and PO1/2/9/10, plus two electrodes below the outer canthus of each eye (Easycap, Herrsching, Germany) and BrainAmp MR plus amplifiers (Brain Products, Munich, Germany). All electrodes were referenced to FCz and grounded at AFz. The EEG was sampled at 1000 Hz (0.1 μV resolution) and band‐pass filtered between 0.016 and 250 Hz. Impedances were kept below 20 kΩ.
Preprocessing was performed using BrainVision Analyzer software (Brain Products). EEG data were downsampled to 500 Hz. To detect artifacts and to compute independent component (IC) weights, a 1 Hz high‐pass filter and a 50 Hz notch filter removing line noise were applied. IC analysis was applied to the filtered EEG data after concatenation of all conditions and ICs representing eye movements and muscle artifacts were identified (Jung et al., 2000). Data segments of 400 ms around data points with amplitudes exceeding ±100 μV and data jumps exceeding 50 μV were marked for rejection. Subsequently, the identified ICs were subtracted from the unfiltered EEG and all electrodes were re‐referenced to the average reference. Remaining artifacts were identified by visual inspection and manually marked for rejection. No significant differences in relative portions of rejected data were found between conditions (pain left/right, 5.7 ± 3.9%, 5.2 ± 4.8%, control left/right, 5.0 ± 3.6%, 5.9 ± 5.1%, one‐way repeated measures ANOVA, F [2, 86] = 0.95, p > .4, Greenhouse–Geisser corrected).
All further analysis steps were performed in MATLAB using the FieldTrip toolbox (Oostenveld, Fries, Maris, & Schoffelen, 2011), the Brain connectivity toolbox (Rubinov & Sporns, 2010), and custom‐written scripts. In FieldTrip, previously marked artifacts were rejected and data were downsampled to 250 Hz to accelerate further analysis. The continuous EEG data were segmented into 2 s epochs with 50% overlap which served as basis for all following analyses except for the peak frequency analysis, in which case 5 s epochs with 50% overlap were used to increase frequency resolution. All data were analyzed separately for each condition. Frequency bands were defined as follows: theta, 4–8 Hz; alpha, 8–13 Hz; beta, 14–30 Hz; gamma, 60–100 Hz. For the gamma band, frequencies between 60 and 100 Hz were chosen as previous studies (Nickel, May, Tiemann, Schmidt, et al., 2017; Peng et al., 2014; Schulz et al., 2015) showed strongest effects related to tonic pain at these frequencies.
2.4. Analysis—Overview
Figure 1 gives an overview of the analysis, which comprised assessments of oscillatory brain activity and functional connectivity. For both, we define two categories of analyses, global and local analyses. Global analyses include analyses which average or summarize across electrodes or voxels, such as power spectra, peak frequency, and all global graph measures. Hence, global analyses are spatially unspecific. In contrast, local analyses comprise the comparison of topographies of power, strength of connectivity, and degree as local graph measure on electrode and source level. Thus, these analyses are spatially specific.
2.5. Analysis of oscillatory brain activity
All analyses of oscillatory brain activity were performed on electrode level. We first assessed global measures of oscillatory brain activity at frequencies between 1 and 100 Hz. To calculate global power spectra, a power spectrum with a frequency resolution of 0.5 Hz was computed using a fast Fourier transformation and Slepian multitapers with ±1 Hz frequency smoothing for each epoch and electrode. To suppress line noise, a band‐stop filter of 48–52 Hz was applied. Subsequently, the power spectra were averaged across epochs and electrodes, resulting in a global power spectrum for each condition and participant. Furthermore, we computed relative power spectra by dividing the absolute power spectra by the summed power across the whole frequency range (1–100 Hz) for each participant in order to enhance sensitivity with respect to frequency band‐specific changes in oscillatory brain activity. To determine global peak frequencies of oscillatory brain activity, a frequency analysis similar to the global absolute power spectra but with 0.2 Hz frequency resolution was performed in an extended alpha band range (6–14 Hz) (Bazanova & Vernon, 2014). Peak frequencies were determined for each epoch by detecting the frequency between 6 and 14 Hz which showed a maximum in power and subsequently averaged across epochs for each condition and participant to avoid a bias toward epochs with high alpha power (Furman et al., 2018).
To assess local measures of oscillatory brain activity, we calculated topographies of absolute and relative power for theta, alpha, beta, and gamma frequency bands for each condition and participant. Topographies of relative power were computed by dividing the absolute power spectra by the summed power across the whole frequency range (1–100 Hz) for each electrode and participant. Relative power spectra were averaged across the respective frequency ranges to obtain the relative power of each frequency band.
2.6. Analysis of functional connectivity
Functional connectivity was assessed using the phase locking value (PLV) (Lachaux et al., 1999), which is defined as
Φx(j) − Φy(j) indicates the phase difference between two sites x and y of epoch j. N signifies the number of epochs across time. The PLV is well‐established, highly sensitive and captures zero‐phase lag and nonzero‐phase lag connectivity. In addition, we calculated the debiased weighted phase lag index (dwPLI) (Vinck et al., 2011), which is defined between two sites x and y as
Im{Sxyj} and Im{Sxyk} indicate the imaginary part of the cross‐spectral density S of epochs j and k, respectively. N signifies the number of 2 s epochs across time. The dwPLI captures nonzero‐phase lag connectivity only. Therefore, the dwPLI is not susceptible to volume conduction but has a reduced sensitivity, since real synchrony at zero‐phase lag is also discarded.
All connectivity analyses were performed in source space as contrasts between tonic pain and respective control conditions which reduces possible confounds by volume conduction and field spread (Schoffelen & Gross, 2009). We used linearly constrained minimum variance beamforming (Van Veen, van Drongelen, Yuchtman, & Suzuki, 1997) to project band‐pass filtered EEG data from electrode into source space. EEG data were band‐pass filtered in the predefined frequency bands using a fourth‐order Butterworth filter (forward and backward). Spatial filters were computed based on the covariance matrices of the band‐pass filtered data for each frequency band and a lead field matrix. A three‐dimensional grid with a 1 cm resolution covering the brain was defined. The lead field was constructed for each voxel using a realistically shaped three‐shell boundary‐element volume conduction model based on the template Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) brain. We used a regularization parameter of 5% of the covariance matrix and chose the dipole orientation of most variance using singular value decomposition. Finally, the preprocessed, band‐pass filtered EEG data were projected through the spatial filter to extract the time series of neuronal activity of each frequency band at each voxel.
For global connectivity analyses, the connectivity between every pair of voxels was computed resulting in a 2020 × 2020 voxels connectivity matrix representing the strength of connection for each pair of voxels over the complete 8.2 min analysis window. To describe brain networks, we applied a graph theory‐based approach. Graph theory is a valuable framework to better understand the characteristics of complex systems, including brain networks, as it allows to describe and quantify key features of such systems in a few measures. A graph is a mathematical construct consisting of nodes and edges connecting the nodes to each other. Here, nodes were defined as voxels and edges as connectivity between voxels. To reduce the computational load, the adjacency matrix, which defines all edges between the nodes, was thresholded to the 10% strongest connections and binarized (Mano et al., 2018; Mansour et al., 2016), resulting in an unweighted and undirected graph.
In principle, graph measures can characterize either complete networks or single nodes of the network. Three complementary measures of the complete network were included in the analysis: the global clustering coefficient, global efficiency, and small‐worldness. The global clustering coefficient is the average of the local clustering coefficients of all nodes, that is, the fraction of the node's neighbors which are also connected to each other (Watts & Strogatz, 1998). It captures the prevalence of clustered connections around single nodes in a network. This is commonly interpreted as a measure of functional segregation, indicating specialized subregions of the brain which are densely connected (Rubinov & Sporns, 2010). Global efficiency is the inverse of the average shortest path length, that is, the minimum number of edges necessary to connect any pair of nodes in a network. It reflects long‐range connections which play a pivotal role for the global integration of a network (Stam, 2014). Small‐worldness describes the ratio of clustering coefficient and global efficiency in comparison to random networks (Rubinov & Sporns, 2010). Brain networks characteristically show small‐world properties, that is, clustered local connectivity with relatively few long‐range connections which renders the network efficient and minimizes wiring costs (Achard & Bullmore, 2007; Bullmore & Sporns, 2012).
To assess local connectivity, we first determined the average connectivity of each voxel to all other voxels, that is, the strength of connectivity. Second, we investigated the graph measure degree. The degree quantifies the number of nodes connected to an individual node by edges and thus, reflects the importance and the connection strength of a node in a network (Rubinov & Sporns, 2010). In the present study, edges were defined as the 10% strongest connections. Third, we analyzed to which brain regions selected brain areas, which show increased connectivity during tonic pain, are connected to in a seed‐based approach. As we found increased connectivity and degree in the contralateral sensorimotor area in both pain conditions and in light of previous findings (Nickel, May, Tiemann, Schmidt, et al., 2017; Schulz et al., 2015), we defined one voxel located in the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) contralateral to the stimulation (MNIxyz = [30/−30, −30, 60] mm [Bradley, Bastuji, & Garcia‐Larrea, 2017]) as seed voxel and computed the seed‐based strength of connectivity. Moreover, we computed the individual mean and group mean phase differences in the alpha band between oscillatory activity in contralateral S1‐voxels and a region‐of‐interest in the medial frontal cortex (MNIxyz = [0, −10, 60]) obtained as the result of the conjunction analysis with maximal t value (see below). Finally, to establish the dominant directionality of the seed‐based connectivity, we calculated partial directed coherence (PDC) (Baccala & Sameshima, 2001) in the alpha band as a measure of directed connectivity between the aforementioned seed voxels in the contralateral S1 and the region‐of‐interest in the medial frontal cortex.
2.7. Statistical analysis
Statistical analysis was carried out using FieldTrip (Oostenveld et al., 2011) and custom‐written MATLAB scripts. We used cluster‐based permutation tests with 10,000 permutations to compare each pain condition to the respective control condition (Maris & Oostenveld, 2007), either clustering across frequencies for the power spectra as part of the global analysis or clustering across space (electrodes or voxels) for all local analyses. We contrasted pain and control conditions using dependent‐samples t tests for every frequency/electrode/voxel and applied a cluster‐threshold at p < .05. Neighboring t values which exceeded this p value were summed up. Subsequently, single‐subject data were randomly assigned to the pain and control conditions and the aforementioned procedure was repeated. For each of the 10,000 permutations, the maximum summed t value was kept building the distribution under the null hypothesis that pain and control conditions did not differ. Statistical significance was tested with an alpha level of .05 for one‐ and two‐tailed testing. One‐tailed testing was used for local power and the seed‐based analyses only, as directed hypotheses existed, such as a decrease in contralateral alpha activity when heat pain was applied. Except for the power spectra, all global measures were compared using a nonparametric permutation test permuting single‐subject values 10,000 times between pain conditions and the respective control conditions, computing a dependent‐samples t test and obtaining a null distribution of t values to which the t value of the original data was compared. The alpha level was also set to .05 (two‐tailed). Bonferroni corrections were performed across the four frequency bands. With regard to the seed‐based analysis, we additionally performed a conjunction analysis (Nichols, Brett, Andersson, Wager, & Poline, 2005) of left and right‐hand stimulations to assess brain areas whose connectivity are consistently changed during tonic pain regardless of stimulation side. Phase differences between voxels in the contralateral S1 (MNIxyz = [30/−30, −30, 60]) and medial frontal cortex (MNIxyz = [0, −10, 60]) were statistically compared against zero using the circ_mtest.m function of the CircStat toolbox for circular statistics (Berens, 2009). Moreover, we compared PLV‐based connectivity between these voxels while controlling for alpha power at central electrodes C1/C3 and C2/C4 contralateral to stimulation to test whether the connectivity effect can be explained by alpha power. We performed repeated measures analyses of covariance (rmANCOVAs) including the factor condition (pain vs. control condition), the covariate alpha power and the dependent variable connectivity. Separate rmANCOVAs were calculated for the pain left and pain right contrasts. For this analysis, connectivity and power values were z‐transformed across pain left and control left conditions and across pain right and control right conditions. Finally, to test the dominant directionality of seed‐based connectivity, we contrasted the PDC between both directions (S1 to medial frontal cortex and medial frontal cortex to S1) for each pain condition separately using a nonparametric permutation test permuting single‐subject values 10,000 times between both connectivity directions.
3. RESULTS
3.1. Oscillatory brain activity during the tonic pain state
We first assessed global changes of oscillatory brain activity during tonic pain. To this end, we compared global power spectra and peak frequencies between pain and control conditions (Figure 2). We did not observe significant differences of absolute power spectra (p min > .08) or relative power spectra (p min > .07) between pain and control conditions. Similarly, peak frequencies did not differ between pain left and control left (9.29 ± 1.10 Hz and 9.38 ± 0.99 Hz, respectively, p > .32) or between pain right and control right (9.30 ± 1.09 Hz and 9.41 ± 0.99 Hz, respectively, p > .14).
We next analyzed local changes of oscillatory brain activity during tonic pain. We calculated topographies of absolute and relative power for pain and control conditions at theta, alpha, beta, and gamma frequency bands. Topographies of absolute power did not differ between pain and control conditions (Figure 3a; p min > .06, Bonferroni‐corrected; see also Figure S1a). Topographies of relative power showed a significant decrease in alpha power over bilateral central electrodes covering the sensorimotor areas in pain left and pain right conditions (Figure 3b; left, p = .026, Cohen's d [d] = −0.31; right, p = .044, d = −0.42; Bonferroni‐corrected; see also Figure S1b). Relative theta power at frontal and central electrodes was significantly reduced in the pain left condition (p = .018, d = −0.40, Bonferroni‐corrected). However, we found no significant changes of relative theta power in the pain right condition (p > .21). No other significant changes in relative power were observed (p min > .12, Bonferroni‐corrected).
3.2. Functional connectivity during the tonic pain state
To investigate global changes of functional connectivity at theta, alpha, beta, and gamma frequencies during tonic pain, we performed graph theory‐based network analysis and contrasted different global graph measures between pain and control conditions. Using the PLV as connectivity measure, the global clustering coefficient (gCC), global efficiency (gEff), and small‐worldness (S) did not significantly differ between pain and control conditions in any frequency band (Figure 4a; gCC, p min > .5; gEff, p min > .7; S, p min > .1, Bonferroni‐corrected). Using the dwPLI as connectivity measure (Figure 4b), the gCC in the alpha frequency band was significantly increased in both pain conditions (gCC, left, p = .0016, d = 0.58; right, p = .029, d = 0.45; other frequency bands, p min > .7). Furthermore, small‐worldness in the alpha frequency was significantly enhanced in the pain left condition (S, left, p = .007, d = 0.51, Bonferroni‐corrected), but not in the pain right condition (S, right, p > .1, other frequency bands, p > .5, Bonferroni‐corrected), thus, not consistent across pain conditions. No significant differences in gEff were observed (gEff, p min > .2, Bonferroni‐corrected).
We next assessed local changes of functional connectivity. We first calculated the strength of connectivity by averaging the connectivity of each voxel to all other voxels. Using the PLV, we observed an increase in connectivity in the beta frequency band predominantly contralateral to the stimulated hand in both pain conditions (Figure 5a; left, p = .0056, d = 0.89; right, p = .0056, d = 0.74, Bonferroni‐corrected; see also Figure S2a). No effect was observed in any other frequency band (p min > .27, Bonferroni‐corrected). Using the dwPLI, which excludes zero‐phase lag connectivity, we did not observe any significant differences (p min > .07, Bonferroni‐corrected; data not shown). To further investigate local changes in connectivity, we next computed the local graph measure degree and compared it between pain and control conditions. The degree of a node is the number of connections to other nodes after thresholding to the 10% strongest connections and therefore, conceptually related to the strength of connectivity. As shown by Figure 5b (see also Figure S2b), we found an increased degree, based on the PLV, in sensorimotor areas contralateral to the stimulation in alpha (left, p = .025, d = 0.62; right, p = .013, d = 0.98, Bonferroni‐corrected) and beta frequency bands (left, p < .001, d = 1.06; right, p < .001, d = 0.79, Bonferroni‐corrected) in both pain conditions. In the theta frequency band, we observed an increase in degree predominantly in the bilateral prefrontal cortices (left, p = .0048, d = 0.75; right, p = .026, d = 0.79, Bonferroni‐corrected). Moreover, significant negative occipital clusters were revealed in the alpha (right, p = .038, d = −0.65) and beta (left, p < .001, d = −0.87; right, p = .0016, d = −0.81) frequency bands, indicating fewer strong connections in the visual cortex during tonic pain as compared to the control condition. We found no significant differences at gamma frequencies (p min > .41, Bonferroni‐corrected) or for the degree based on the dwPLI (p min > .37, Bonferroni‐corrected; data not shown).
Furthermore, we investigated to which regions brain areas with increased connectivity strength were connected during tonic pain. To this end, we used the contralateral sensorimotor area as a seed region and compared seed‐based connectivity assessed by PLV between pain and control conditions. We observed increased connectivity to the medial prefrontal cortex and the frontal cortex contralateral to the stimulation side in the alpha frequency band (Figure 6). These effects were consistent across stimulation sides (left, p = .030, d = 0.65; right, p < .001, d = 0.80, Bonferroni‐corrected). In addition, we found increased connectivity to the frontal cortex in the beta frequency band (p = .0044, d = 1.11, Bonferroni‐corrected) in the pain left but not in the pain right condition. No difference was detected at any other frequency band (p min > .05, Bonferroni‐corrected). To determine whether the sensorimotor cortex of both hemispheres connects to a common brain area independent of stimulation side, we applied a conjunction analysis of seed‐based connectivity from both sensorimotor areas. The results revealed that both sensorimotor areas connected to the medial prefrontal cortex in the alpha frequency band during tonic pain (Figure 6, t max = 4.7, MNIxyz = [0, −10, 60]). The analysis of phase differences showed that they did not or only marginally differ from zero (pain right, 3.3 ± 15.3°, mean ± SD, p > .07; pain left, 4.6 ± 11.2°, p = .017). Using the center frequency of 10.5 Hz of the alpha band (8–13 Hz), the phase differences equaled time lags of 0.9 and 1.2 ms, respectively, indicating that connectivity was essentially observed with a phase lag close to zero.
We further investigated whether these connectivity changes between S1 and medial frontal cortex could be explained by power changes contralateral to stimulation. We therefore calculated repeated measures analysis of covariance including the factor condition, the covariate alpha power and the dependent variable connectivity. The analyses revealed significantly enhanced functional connectivity in pain left compared to control left (F [1, 36] = 26.3, p < .001, partial η 2 = 0.42) and in pain right compared to control right (F [1, 36] = 30.9, p < .001, partial η 2 = 0.46) independent of alpha power.
Finally, to investigate if the increased seed‐based connectivity in the alpha band assessed by PLV predominantly reflects bottom‐up or top‐down processes, we compared PDC as a measure of directed connectivity from S1 to the medial frontal cortex to the reverse direction in both pain conditions. We observed significantly increased PDC from S1 to the medial frontal cortex (pain left, p < .001, d = 0.76, pain right, p < .001, d = 1.28).
3.3. Summary
Taken together, we found a decrease of oscillatory brain activity at alpha frequencies associated with an increase of functional connectivity at alpha and beta frequencies in the contralateral sensorimotor cortex during tonic pain compared to the control condition. Furthermore, we found that the contralateral sensorimotor cortex showed a stronger connection to the medial prefrontal cortex during tonic pain than during the control condition at alpha frequencies.
4. DISCUSSION
In the present study, we systematically and comprehensively investigated the pattern of neural oscillations and connectivity characterizing the state of tonic experimental pain in healthy human participants. We observed local suppressions of oscillatory brain activity at alpha frequencies over the sensorimotor cortices during tonic pain. These suppressions were associated with increases of functional connectivity at alpha frequencies between sensorimotor cortices contralateral to the stimulated hand and the medial prefrontal cortex. Directed connectivity analysis suggests bottom‐up signaling of nociceptive information in the alpha frequency band from somatosensory areas to the medial prefrontal cortex. Together, these findings indicate that tonic pain is associated with local changes in oscillations and increased connectivity in a sensorimotor‐prefrontal network synchronized at alpha frequencies.
The absence of global changes of oscillatory brain activity reported here contrasts with previous studies reporting an increase (Nir et al., 2010) or a decrease (Furman et al., 2018) of the peak alpha frequency during tonic pain. Instead, our results suggest that tonic pain is more closely related to local changes of alpha oscillations over the sensorimotor cortex (see below) than to changes of global brain activity. Besides, the discrepancies might also be due to differences in control conditions. The previous studies used passive control conditions whereas we used an active control condition. As cognitive load can influence the peak alpha frequency (Haegens, Cousijn, Wallis, Harrison, & Nobre, 2014; Mierau, Klimesch, & Lefebvre, 2017), a comparison of pain to different control conditions can yield different results. In the present study, we chose an active control condition whose cognitive load was intended to match that of the pain condition.
Furthermore, we did not find global changes of functional connectivity captured by PLV‐based graph theory‐based network measures during tonic pain. This is in accordance with previous studies showing that, in healthy human participants, functional connectivity networks assessed by EEG are stable across time (Chu et al., 2012; Kramer et al., 2011). We, however, found an increased dwPLI‐based global clustering coefficient in the alpha frequency band, showing an increased functional segregation during tonic pain. Such significant changes of electrophysiological global network measures have been observed in different neurological and psychiatric disorders such as Alzheimer's disease (Stam, Jones, Nolte, Breakspear, & Scheltens, 2007; Yu et al., 2017), multiple sclerosis, and epilepsy (Stam, 2014). Although such changes have not directly been shown in chronic pain so far, fMRI studies reporting a global reorganization of the brain in chronic pain (Mano et al., 2018; Mansour et al., 2016) suggest that global changes of functional connectivity networks might also manifest in electrophysiological recordings. The present findings indicate that 10 min of experimental pain only induce changes in the dwPLI‐based global clustering coefficient. However, we only analyzed a subset of important complementary graph measures based on selected connectivity measures. Differences in other graph and connectivity measures can therefore not be ruled out.
The observation of a local suppression of alpha oscillations in sensorimotor cortices during tonic pain is in good accordance with previous studies (Colon et al., 2017; Giehl et al., 2014; Gram, Graversen, Olesen, & Drewes, 2015; Huishi Zhang et al., 2016; Nir et al., 2012; Peng et al., 2014; Schulz et al., 2015). These suppressions likely indicate activation and increased excitability (Klimesch, 2012) of the sensorimotor cortex during the processing of tonic pain. Intriguingly, our findings revealed that these local suppressions of alpha oscillations are associated with local increases of functional connectivity in the same brain areas and at the same frequencies. Compared to the small effect sizes of the changes in oscillatory brain activity, medium to large effect sizes of connectivity changes, as shown in the present study, support that connectivity plays a crucial role in the cerebral processing of pain (Apkarian & Chialvo, 2006; Kucyi & Davis, 2015; Mano & Seymour, 2015; Tracey, 2005).
The present findings further revealed that the sensorimotor cortex more strongly connected to the medial prefrontal cortex at alpha frequencies during tonic pain compared to the control condition. In light of previous findings (Nickel, May, Tiemann, Schmidt, et al., 2017), this suggests that the stimulation side‐dependent representation of stimulus intensity in sensorimotor cortex connects to a stimulation side‐independent representation of pain intensity in the medial prefrontal cortex by synchronizing at alpha frequencies. Synchrony at alpha and beta frequencies has been suggested to serve long‐range communication in the brain (Donner & Siegel, 2011). Although these frequencies have been implicated in the top‐down signaling of predictions (Fries, 2015; Michalareas et al., 2016; Ploner et al., 2017), the present findings indicate that the alpha connectivity between sensorimotor and prefrontal cortex might reflect increased bottom‐up rather than top‐down signaling of potentially nociceptive information as our recent work suggests (Nickel, May, Tiemann, Schmidt, et al., 2017). These results are in line with previous findings showing that directed functional connectivity is enhanced between somatosensory and medial frontal cortex during pain (Huishi Zhang et al., 2016). Moreover, the findings are in accordance with mounting evidence for an important role of the medial prefrontal cortex for the encoding of chronic (Baliki et al., 2006; Hashmi et al., 2013; Vachon‐Presseau et al., 2016) and tonic experimental pain (Nickel, May, Tiemann, Schmidt, et al., 2017; Schulz et al., 2015). Hub‐like properties of the medial frontal cortex with representations of pain, cognitive control, and negative emotion (Kragel et al., 2018) render it suitable for the integration of sensory and contextual information into a coherent percept and appropriate behavioral responses. The comparison of the topography of the connectivity effects in the prefrontal cortex in the present study (Figure 6) with the topography of pain‐specific fMRI activations in a recent cross‐study approach (Kragel et al., 2018) reveals an intriguing similarity of these effects. Anatomically, functional connectivity between sensorimotor and medial prefrontal cortex might be subserved by structural connections between the sensorimotor cortex and the orbital/medial prefrontal cortex as revealed by retrograde tracing studies in macaque monkeys (Carmichael & Price, 1995; Price, 2007).
Moreover, increases in connectivity were also observed in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) which plays an important role in pain processing (Seminowicz & Moayedi, 2017). The DLPFC is involved in many cognitive and regulatory functions and connected to different functional brain networks (Seminowicz & Moayedi, 2017). Regarding tonic experimental pain, modulatory effects of the DLPFC on the excitability of the sensorimotor cortex were reported (De Martino, Seminowicz, Schabrun, Petrini, & Graven‐Nielsen, 2018). The present observation of increased connectivity between sensorimotor and lateral prefrontal cortex would be, in principle, well compatible with these observations of an important role of dorsolateral prefrontal‐sensorimotor connectivity in tonic pain.
Some limitations apply to the present findings and their interpretation. EEG‐based connectivity analyses using the PLV can be influenced by volume conduction and field spread effects complicating the distinction between artifactual and true connectivity effects. This particularly applies to zero‐phase lag connectivity. However, experimental (Roelfsema, Engel, Konig, & Singer, 1997; Vicente, Gollo, Mirasso, Fischer, & Pipa, 2008) and modeling (Gollo, Mirasso, Sporns, & Breakspear, 2014; Viriyopase, Bojak, Zeitler, & Gielen, 2012) evidence has indicated that physiological long‐range connectivity can occur with zero‐phase lag. Moreover, several arguments indicate that the present findings reflect physiological rather than spurious connectivity (Bastos & Schoffelen, 2016; Cohen, 2014; Palva & Palva, 2012; Schoffelen & Gross, 2009). First, we performed all connectivity analyses in source space which reduces volume conduction confounds. Second, the present results are based on contrasts between pain and control conditions which diminishes the risk of volume conduction effects (see, however, below for limitations of the control condition). Third, connectivity between sensorimotor and prefrontal cortex significantly differed between pain and control conditions even when controlling for power changes indicating that connectivity effects cannot be fully explained by power effects. Nevertheless, we cannot completely rule out alternative explanations for this connectivity pattern that might involve common input by one or more additional sources in combination with volume conduction effects, an inherent confound and limitation of connectivity analyses of scalp EEG recordings. Another limitation is that the control condition does not perfectly control for all possible confounds. In particular, pain and control conditions differ in attentional focus (body‐centered vs. object‐centered), somatosensory stimulation and salience. Hence, it is impossible to unequivocally disentangle effects of tonic pain on the one hand and attentional focus, somatosensory stimulation, and salience on the other hand (Legrain, Iannetti, Plaghki, & Mouraux, 2011). Furthermore, in the pain condition finger movements may have a stronger self‐initiated component than in the control condition in which subjects are asked to track the visually displayed pain ratings of the respective pain conditions. As such differences in self‐initiation can influence alpha and beta oscillations in sensorimotor cortex and the coherence between bilateral sensorimotor cortices (Wang et al., 2017), a confound by differences in self‐initiation cannot be ultimately ruled out.
5. CONCLUSIONS
In summary, the present findings show that tonic pain is associated with decreases of local oscillations and increases of connectivity at alpha frequencies in the sensorimotor cortex. These stimulation side‐specific changes of oscillations and connectivity occur along with increased connectivity at alpha frequencies to a stimulation side‐independent representation in the medial prefrontal cortex. Although inherent limitations in the interpretation of scalp EEG‐based connectivity analyses remain to be resolved, these findings suggest a core network for the processing of tonic pain which includes sensorimotor and medial prefrontal cortex and their synchronization at alpha frequencies. This network might be associated with the transformation of objective stimulus information into the subjective experience of pain. Together, these observations represent a step further toward understanding the cerebral representation of long‐lasting pain states and might help to identify targets for novel chronic pain treatment approaches such as noninvasive brain stimulation and neurofeedback (Jensen, Day, & Miro, 2014; Polania, Nitsche, & Ruff, 2018).
Supporting information
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The study was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (PL 321/10‐1, PL 321/10‐2, PL 321/11‐1, PL 321/11‐2), the Bavarian State Ministry of Education, Science and the Arts and the Wellcome Trust (098433). We thank Martina Postorino for support in data acquisition. The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Nickel MM, Ta Dinh S, May ES, et al. Neural oscillations and connectivity characterizing the state of tonic experimental pain in humans. Hum Brain Mapp. 2020;41:17–29. 10.1002/hbm.24784
Funding information Bayerisches Staatsministerium für Bildung und Kultus, Wissenschaft und Kunst; Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, Grant/Award Numbers: PL321/10‐1, PL321/10‐2, PL321/11‐1, PL321/11‐2; Wellcome Trust, Grant/Award Number: 098433
REFERENCES
- Achard, S. , & Bullmore, E. (2007). Efficiency and cost of economical brain functional networks. PLoS Computational Biology, 3(2), e17 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0030017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Apkarian, A. V. , & Chialvo, D. R. (2006). The shadows of pain. Pain, 123(3), 221–222. 10.1016/j.pain.2006.04.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baccala, L. A. , & Sameshima, K. (2001). Partial directed coherence: A new concept in neural structure determination. Biological Cybernetics, 84(6), 463–474. 10.1007/PL00007990 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baliki, M. N. , & Apkarian, A. V. (2015). Nociception, pain, negative moods, and behavior selection. Neuron, 87(3), 474–491. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.06.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baliki, M. N. , Chialvo, D. R. , Geha, P. Y. , Levy, R. M. , Harden, R. N. , Parrish, T. B. , & Apkarian, A. V. (2006). Chronic pain and the emotional brain: Specific brain activity associated with spontaneous fluctuations of intensity of chronic back pain. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26(47), 12165–12173. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3576-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bastos, A. M. , & Schoffelen, J. M. (2016). A tutorial review of functional connectivity analysis methods and their interpretational pitfalls. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 9, 175 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00175 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bazanova, O. M. , & Vernon, D. (2014). Interpreting EEG alpha activity. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 44, 94–110. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.05.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berens, P. (2009). CircStat: A MATLAB toolbox for circular statistics. Journal of Statistical Software, 31(10), 1–21. [Google Scholar]
- Bradley, C. , Bastuji, H. , & Garcia‐Larrea, L. (2017). Evidence‐based source modeling of nociceptive cortical responses: A direct comparison of scalp and intracranial activity in humans. Human Brain Mapping, 38(12), 6083–6095. 10.1002/hbm.23812 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bullmore, E. , & Sporns, O. (2012). The economy of brain network organization. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 13(5), 336–349. 10.1038/nrn3214 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carmichael, S. T. , & Price, J. L. (1995). Sensory and premotor connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of macaque monkeys. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 363(4), 642–664. 10.1002/cne.903630409 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chu, C. J. , Kramer, M. A. , Pathmanathan, J. , Bianchi, M. T. , Westover, M. B. , Wizon, L. , & Cash, S. S. (2012). Emergence of stable functional networks in long‐term human electroencephalography. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(8), 2703–2713. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5669-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, M. X. (2014). Analyzing neural time series data. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Colon, E. , Liberati, G. , & Mouraux, A. (2017). EEG frequency tagging using ultra‐slow periodic heat stimulation of the skin reveals cortical activity specifically related to C fiber thermonociceptors. NeuroImage, 146, 266–274. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Martino, E. , Seminowicz, D. A. , Schabrun, S. M. , Petrini, L. , & Graven‐Nielsen, T. (2018). High frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex modulates sensorimotor cortex function in the transition to sustained muscle pain. NeuroImage, 186, 93–102. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.10.076 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donner, T. H. , & Siegel, M. (2011). A framework for local cortical oscillation patterns. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(5), 191–199. 10.1016/j.tics.2011.03.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fries, P. (2015). Rhythms for cognition: Communication through coherence. Neuron, 88(1), 220–235. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Furman, A. J. , Meeker, T. J. , Rietschel, J. C. , Yoo, S. , Muthulingam, J. , Prokhorenko, M. , … Seminowicz, D. A. (2018). Cerebral peak alpha frequency predicts individual differences in pain sensitivity. NeuroImage, 167, 203–210. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garcia‐Larrea, L. , & Peyron, R. (2013). Pain matrices and neuropathic pain matrices: A review. Pain, 154(Suppl 1), S29–S43. 10.1016/j.pain.2013.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giehl, J. , Meyer‐Brandis, G. , Kunz, M. , & Lautenbacher, S. (2014). Responses to tonic heat pain in the ongoing EEG under conditions of controlled attention. Somatosensory & Motor Research, 31(1), 40–48. 10.3109/08990220.2013.837045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gollo, L. L. , Mirasso, C. , Sporns, O. , & Breakspear, M. (2014). Mechanisms of zero‐lag synchronization in cortical motifs. PLoS Computational Biology, 10(4), e1003548 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003548 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gram, M. , Erlenwein, J. , Petzke, F. , Falla, D. , Przemeck, M. , Emons, M. I. , … Drewes, A. M. (2017). The cortical responses to evoked clinical pain in patients with hip osteoarthritis. PLoS One, 12(10), e0186400 10.1371/journal.pone.0186400 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gram, M. , Graversen, C. , Olesen, S. S. , & Drewes, A. M. (2015). Dynamic spectral indices of the electroencephalogram provide new insights into tonic pain. Clinical Neurophysiology, 126(4), 763–771. 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.07.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haegens, S. , Cousijn, H. , Wallis, G. , Harrison, P. J. , & Nobre, A. C. (2014). Inter‐ and intra‐individual variability in alpha peak frequency. NeuroImage, 92, 46–55. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.049 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hashmi, J. A. , Baliki, M. N. , Huang, L. , Baria, A. T. , Torbey, S. , Hermann, K. M. , … Apkarian, A. V. (2013). Shape shifting pain: Chronification of back pain shifts brain representation from nociceptive to emotional circuits. Brain, 136(Pt 9), 2751–2768. 10.1093/brain/awt211 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huishi Zhang, C. , Sohrabpour, A. , Lu, Y. , & He, B. (2016). Spectral and spatial changes of brain rhythmic activity in response to the sustained thermal pain stimulation. Human Brain Mapping, 37(8), 2976–2991. 10.1002/hbm.23220 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jensen, M. P. , Day, M. A. , & Miro, J. (2014). Neuromodulatory treatments for chronic pain: Efficacy and mechanisms. Nature Reviews. Neurology, 10(3), 167–178. 10.1038/nrneurol.2014.12 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jung, T. P. , Makeig, S. , Humphries, C. , Lee, T. W. , McKeown, M. J. , Iragui, V. , & Sejnowski, T. J. (2000). Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation. Psychophysiology, 37(2), 163–178. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch, W. (2012). Alpha‐band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(12), 606–617. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kragel, P. A. , Kano, M. , Van Oudenhove, L. , Ly, H. G. , Dupont, P. , Rubio, A. , … Wager, T. D. (2018). Generalizable representations of pain, cognitive control, and negative emotion in medial frontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 21(2), 283–289. 10.1038/s41593-017-0051-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kramer, M. A. , Eden, U. T. , Lepage, K. Q. , Kolaczyk, E. D. , Bianchi, M. T. , & Cash, S. S. (2011). Emergence of persistent networks in long‐term intracranial EEG recordings. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(44), 15757–15767. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2287-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kucyi, A. , & Davis, K. D. (2015). The dynamic pain connectome. Trends in Neurosciences, 38(2), 86–95. 10.1016/j.tins.2014.11.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuner, R. , & Flor, H. (2017). Structural plasticity and reorganisation in chronic pain. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 18(2), 113 10.1038/nrn.2017.5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lachaux, J. P. , Rodriguez, E. , Martinerie, J. , & Varela, F. J. (1999). Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Human Brain Mapping, 8(4), 194–208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Legrain, V. , Iannetti, G. D. , Plaghki, L. , & Mouraux, A. (2011). The pain matrix reloaded: A salience detection system for the body. Progress in Neurobiology, 93(1), 111–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levitt, J. , Choo, H. J. , Smith, K. A. , LeBlanc, B. W. , & Saab, C. Y. (2017). Electroencephalographic frontal synchrony and caudal asynchrony during painful hand immersion in cold water. Brain Research Bulletin, 130, 75–80. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2016.12.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liberati, G. , Algoet, M. , Santos, S. F. , Ribeiro‐Vaz, J. G. , Raftopoulos, C. , & Mouraux, A. (2019). Tonic thermonociceptive stimulation selectively modulates ongoing neural oscillations in the human posterior insula: Evidence from intracerebral EEG. NeuroImage, 188, 70–83. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.11.059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mano, H. , Kotecha, G. , Leibnitz, K. , Matsubara, T. , Nakae, A. , Shenker, N. , … Seymour, B. (2018). Classification and characterisation of brain network changes in chronic back pain: A multicenter study. Wellcome Open Research, 3, 19 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.14069.1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mano, H. , & Seymour, B. (2015). Pain: A distributed brain information network? PLoS Biology, 13(1), e1002037 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mansour, A. , Baria, A. T. , Tetreault, P. , Vachon‐Presseau, E. , Chang, P. C. , Huang, L. , … Baliki, M. N. (2016). Global disruption of degree rank order: A hallmark of chronic pain. Scientific Reports, 6, 34853 10.1038/srep34853 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maris, E. , & Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG‐ and MEG‐data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 164(1), 177–190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martel, M. , Harvey, M. P. , Houde, F. , Balg, F. , Goffaux, P. , & Leonard, G. (2017). Unravelling the effect of experimental pain on the corticomotor system using transcranial magnetic stimulation and electroencephalography. Experimental Brain Research, 235(4), 1223–1231. 10.1007/s00221-017-4880-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michalareas, G. , Vezoli, J. , van Pelt, S. , Schoffelen, J. M. , Kennedy, H. , & Fries, P. (2016). Alpha‐Beta and Gamma rhythms subserve feedback and feedforward influences among human visual cortical areas. Neuron, 89(2), 384–397. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mierau, A. , Klimesch, W. , & Lefebvre, J. (2017). State‐dependent alpha peak frequency shifts: Experimental evidence, potential mechanisms and functional implications. Neuroscience, 360, 146–154. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.07.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nichols, T. , Brett, M. , Andersson, J. , Wager, T. , & Poline, J. B. (2005). Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic. NeuroImage, 25(3), 653–660. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nickel, M. M. , May, E. S. , Tiemann, L. , Postorino, M. , Ta Dinh, S. , & Ploner, M. (2017). Autonomic responses to tonic pain are more closely related to stimulus intensity than to pain intensity. Pain, 158(11), 2129–2136. 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nickel, M. M. , May, E. S. , Tiemann, L. , Schmidt, P. , Postorino, M. , Ta Dinh, S. , … Ploner, M. (2017). Brain oscillations differentially encode noxious stimulus intensity and pain intensity. NeuroImage, 148, 141–147. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nir, R. R. , Sinai, A. , Moont, R. , Harari, E. , & Yarnitsky, D. (2012). Tonic pain and continuous EEG: Prediction of subjective pain perception by alpha‐1 power during stimulation and at rest. Clinical Neurophysiology, 123(3), 605–612. 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.08.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nir, R. R. , Sinai, A. , Raz, E. , Sprecher, E. , & Yarnitsky, D. (2010). Pain assessment by continuous EEG: Association between subjective perception of tonic pain and peak frequency of alpha oscillations during stimulation and at rest. Brain Research, 1344, 77–86. 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.05.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oostenveld, R. , Fries, P. , Maris, E. , & Schoffelen, J. M. (2011). FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience, 2011, 156869–156869. 10.1155/2011/156869 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palva, S. , & Palva, J. M. (2012). Discovering oscillatory interaction networks with M/EEG: Challenges and breakthroughs. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 219–230. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.02.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peng, W. , Hu, L. , Zhang, Z. , & Hu, Y. (2014). Changes of spontaneous oscillatory activity to tonic heat pain. PLoS One, 9(3), e91052 10.1371/journal.pone.0091052 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ploner, M. , Sorg, C. , & Gross, J. (2017). Brain rhythms of pain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(2), 100–110. 10.1016/j.tics.2016.12.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polania, R. , Nitsche, M. A. , & Ruff, C. C. (2018). Studying and modifying brain function with non‐invasive brain stimulation. Nature Neuroscience, 21(2), 174–187. 10.1038/s41593-017-0054-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price, J. L. (2007). Definition of the orbital cortex in relation to specific connections with limbic and visceral structures and other cortical regions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1121, 54–71. 10.1196/annals.1401.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roelfsema, P. R. , Engel, A. K. , Konig, P. , & Singer, W. (1997). Visuomotor integration is associated with zero time‐lag synchronization among cortical areas. Nature, 385(6612), 157–161. 10.1038/385157a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubinov, M. , & Sporns, O. (2010). Complex network measures of brain connectivity: Uses and interpretations. NeuroImage, 52(3), 1059–1069. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoffelen, J. M. , & Gross, J. (2009). Source connectivity analysis with MEG and EEG. Human Brain Mapping, 30(6), 1857–1865. 10.1002/hbm.20745 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schulz, E. , May, E. S. , Postorino, M. , Tiemann, L. , Nickel, M. M. , Witkovsky, V. , … Ploner, M. (2015). Prefrontal gamma oscillations encode tonic pain in humans. Cerebral Cortex, 25, 4407–4414. 10.1093/cercor/bhv043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seminowicz, D. A. , & Moayedi, M. (2017). The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in acute and chronic pain. The Journal of Pain, 18(9), 1027–1035. 10.1016/j.jpain.2017.03.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegel, M. , Donner, T. H. , & Engel, A. K. (2012). Spectral fingerprints of large‐scale neuronal interactions. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 13(2), 121–134. 10.1038/nrn3137 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stam, C. J. (2014). Modern network science of neurological disorders. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 15(10), 683–695. 10.1038/nrn3801 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stam, C. J. , Jones, B. F. , Nolte, G. , Breakspear, M. , & Scheltens, P. (2007). Small‐world networks and functional connectivity in Alzheimer's disease. Cerebral Cortex, 17(1), 92–99. 10.1093/cercor/bhj127 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tracey, I. (2005). Functional connectivity and pain: How effectively connected is your brain? Pain, 116(3), 173–174. 10.1016/j.pain.2005.05.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vachon‐Presseau, E. , Tetreault, P. , Petre, B. , Huang, L. , Berger, S. E. , Torbey, S. , … Apkarian, A. V. (2016). Corticolimbic anatomical characteristics predetermine risk for chronic pain. Brain, 139(Pt 7), 1958–1970. 10.1093/brain/aww100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Veen, B. D. , van Drongelen, W. , Yuchtman, M. , & Suzuki, A. (1997). Localization of brain electrical activity via linearly constrained minimum variance spatial filtering. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 44(9), 867–880. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vicente, R. , Gollo, L. L. , Mirasso, C. R. , Fischer, I. , & Pipa, G. (2008). Dynamical relaying can yield zero time lag neuronal synchrony despite long conduction delays. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(44), 17157–17162. 10.1073/pnas.0809353105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vinck, M. , Oostenveld, R. , van Wingerden, M. , Battaglia, F. , & Pennartz, C. M. (2011). An improved index of phase‐synchronization for electrophysiological data in the presence of volume‐conduction, noise and sample‐size bias. NeuroImage, 55(4), 1548–1565. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Viriyopase, A. , Bojak, I. , Zeitler, M. , & Gielen, S. (2012). When long‐range zero‐lag synchronization is feasible in cortical networks. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 6, 49 10.3389/fncom.2012.00049 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang, B. A. , Viswanathan, S. , Abdollahi, R. O. , Rosjat, N. , Popovych, S. , Daun, S. , … Fink, G. R. (2017). Frequency‐specific modulation of connectivity in the ipsilateral sensorimotor cortex by different forms of movement initiation. NeuroImage, 159, 248–260. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.07.054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watts, D. J. , & Strogatz, S. H. (1998). Collective dynamics of ‘small‐world’ networks. Nature, 393(6684), 440–442. 10.1038/30918 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu, M. , Engels, M. M. A. , Hillebrand, A. , van Straaten, E. C. W. , Gouw, A. A. , Teunissen, C. , … Stam, C. J. (2017). Selective impairment of hippocampus and posterior hub areas in Alzheimer's disease: An MEG‐based multiplex network study. Brain, 140(5), 1466–1485. 10.1093/brain/awx050 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.