Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Prog Transplant. 2019 Dec 16;30(1):13–21. doi: 10.1177/1526924819892921

Impact of Affect on Lung Transplant Candidate Outcomes

Kelly M Pennington 1,2, Roberto P Benzo 1, Terry D Schneekloth 3,4, Marie Budev 5, Satish Chandrashekaran 6, David B Erasmus 7,8, Erika D Lease 8, Deborah J Levine 9, Karin Thompson 1, Elizabeth Stevens 1, Paul J Novotny 10, Cassie C Kennedy 1,2,4
PMCID: PMC7271895  NIHMSID: NIHMS1585084  PMID: 31838950

INTRODUCTION

Median survival following lung transplantation has been stagnant at 5.7 years over the last decade and pales in comparison to other solid organ transplants1. In recent years, increased attention has been applied to pre-transplant candidates in an effort to identify novel extra-pulmonary prognostic factors such as frailty and body composition2. Physical frailty is now increasingly recognized as an important pre-transplant risk factor leading to worse pre- and post-transplant survival35. However, data in geriatrics has suggested novel psychological risk factors (so called “psychological frailty”) may also be important in predicting health related outcomes6. Post-transplant depression and distress at 6 months have been correlated with increased mortality in lung transplant recipients7. Similarly, optimism has been associated with survival in bone marrow transplant8, and pre-transplant psychosocial vulnerability has been linked to worse post-transplant psychosocial outcomes in liver, lung, and bone marrow transplant9. Such factors are relatively understudied as predictors of outcomes in lung transplant candidates.

According to candidate selection guidelines, lung transplant candidates must be able to comply with complex medical regimens and demonstrate adequate psychosocial support to be listed for lung transplantation10. Many transplant centers utilize the Psychosocial Assessment of Candidates for Transplantation (PACT), Transplant Evaluation Rating Scale, or Stanford Integrated Psychosocial Assessment for Transplant (SIPAT) for pre-transplant psychosocial evaluation1114, but a range of practices exist15. These tools are not designed to assess factors contributing to long-term emotional well-being, such as adjustment, resilience, and optimism. While low baseline PACT scores have been associated with increased post-lung transplant mortality, increasing PACT scores in candidates with initial low baseline PACT scores does not seem to reduce mortality16.

A clearer understanding of the factors associated with emotional well-being and psychological frailty surrounding lung transplantation is critical to selecting appropriate candidates and enhancing the benefits of a lung transplant. A balance of positive and negative affect has been described by researchers as essential to emotional health and adjustment17 and may be a useful component in evaluating and optimizing potential lung transplant candidates. Positive affect is the extent that an individual experiences optimism and joy18,19. In contrast, negative affect is the extent that an individual experiences pessimism, unhappiness, anger, nervousness, and sadness18,19. Individuals with approximately three times more positive affect than negative affect, known as the critical positivity ratio, are able to maintain an optimal level of functioning—one that suggests resilience, flexibility, and optimism17.

While optimism has previously been correlated with post-transplant outcomes8,2022, affect and the factors associated with affect have not been previously described in lung transplant candidates. Our objective in this study was to characterize affect in lung transplant candidates. We specifically examined the impact of affect on waitlist death, delisting, and post-transplant death. We also examined clinical factors to determine their association with positive affect, negative affect, and the balance of positive affect and negative affect in lung transplant candidates. Our findings provide a characterization of affect in lung transplant candidates and may serve as a building block for future interventions aimed at reducing psychological frailty and enhance emotional wellbeing in lung transplant patients.

METHODS

This study was approved by the Mayo Clinic Institutional Review Board (IRB) under IRB 15-005378 and adheres to the ethical standards of the Declarations of Helsinki. Individual collaborating sites obtained approval from their respective IRBs.

To examine the association of adult lung transplant candidates’ self-reported affect with transplant-related outcomes, consenting waitlisted candidates from six centers completed questionnaires including the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) annually and post-transplant. Univariate logistic regression analysis was performed to determine the association of baseline affect with outcomes of death or delisting. The details of the study are subsequently outlined.

Study Population

Eligible participants included adult lung or heart-lung transplant candidates on the waiting list at either Mayo Clinic Rochester or Mayo Clinic Florida. In addition, adult lung transplant candidates at one of four participating United States lung transplant centers (University of Florida Gainesville; Cleveland Clinic Foundation; University of Washington; or University of Texas Health Science Center San Antonio) who consented to sharing their name and contact information with Mayo Clinic for the purpose of potential research participation were eligible. Patients were excluded if they required a medical interpreter or did not have an address in the United States. Recruitment began September 16, 2015 and ended (for the purposes of this report) on December 21, 2018.

Questionnaire Administration

Questionnaires were mailed to all eligible participants (including Mayo Clinic and non-Mayo Clinic participants) with a mechanism for study refusal and a small token of appreciation. Non-responders were mailed a second questionnaire at one month. Continued non-responders one month after the second mailing were contacted by phone and mailed a third questionnaire, unless they chose not to participate or preferred to complete the questionnaire over-the-phone with a study coordinator. Using the same process, questionnaires were re-administered to surviving waitlisted participants annually. All participants were mailed a post-transplant questionnaire three months following the date of the lung transplant surgery. Non-responders were sent another copy of the post-transplant questionnaire every 30 days until a response was received or until post-transplant survey was mailed four times without a response. Post-transplant surveys received greater than one year following transplant were not included. Questionnaire responses were collected and entered into a secured, web-based Research Electronic Data Capture (REDCap) database23 hosted by Mayo Clinic.

Data Collection

Questionnaires included demographic information and relationship of the primary caregiver. Primary institutions for all participants (including Mayo Clinic and non-Mayo Clinic participants) provided reported date of transplant, date of delisting, and date of death. In addition, primary transplant diagnosis and prognostic details at the time of study enrollment including the 6-minute walk distance (6MWD), Karnofsky score, PACT, SIPAT, and lung allocation scores (LAS) were abstracted from the electronic health record of Mayo Clinic participants. This information was not available for non-Mayo Clinic participants. If Mayo Clinic participants received a transplant during the study period, then the post-transplant hospital length of stay and number of days requiring mechanical ventilation post-transplant were also abstracted from the electronic health record.

Survey Instruments

PANAS:

Affect was assessed with the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS), a previously validated 20-item scale18 (Cronbach’s α 0.86 positive affect, 0.84 negative affect). The scale consists of 10 positive and 10 negative words, rated on a Likert scale of 1 to 5 (with 1 indicating that the word applies minimally if at all and 5 indicating it applies very strongly to the respondent). Higher scores for positive words indicate more positive affect and higher scores for negative words indicate more negative affect 18. The minimal clinically important difference was 3.8 for positive and 3.0 for negative affect scores, representing half of a standard deviation (SD). The ratio of positive-to-negative affect is termed the positivity ratio17,24. A positivity ratio of ≥2.9 distinguishes individuals with greater resilience to adversity, more social resources, and better optimal functioning17.

GAD-2.

Generalized Anxiety Disorder Scale-2 (GAD-2) is 2-item, validated screening tool for symptoms of anxiety25 (Cronbach’s α 0.82). A score of 3 or higher is considered positive screening for anxiety with a sensitivity and specificity of 84.4% and 72.8% for generalized anxiety disorder26.

PHQ-2.

The Patient Health Questionaire-2 (PHQ-2) is a validated screening tool for depression27 (Cronbach’s α 0.88). A score of 3 or higher is considered a positive screen for depression with a sensitivity and specificity of 85.7% and 69.2% for major depressive disorder, respectively28.

CRQ.

The Chronic Respiratory Disease Questionnaire (CRQ) is a validated 20-item questionnaire that assesses four domains reflecting a participant’s physical and emotional quality of life: shortness of breath, fatigue, emotional function, and mastery (self-efficacy) (Cronbach’s α 0.82)29. Higher scores indicate better quality of life. The minimal clinically important difference is 0.5.

Data Analysis

Results are presented as mean ± SD unless otherwise indicated. JMP statistical software (version 9.01, SAS, Cary, NC) or SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc.; Cary, NC, USA) was used for data analysis. Our primary predictors were positive affect, negative affect, and positivity ratio at time of study enrollment. Our primary outcomes were death on the waiting list and delisting. Our secondary outcomes included change in PANAS compared to baseline, quality of life by CRQ domains, and duration of mechanical ventilation or hospital length of stay following transplant (available for Mayo Clinic participants only). Longitudinal changes in PANAS scores were analyzed using a one-sample, paired t-test. Primary outcome, other secondary outcomes, and sub-group variables were analyzed with a 2-sided t-test. Strength of relationship between the PANAS scores and psychosocial and functional assessment tools (available for Mayo Clinic participants only) were analyzed via Pearson’s correlation coefficient. Functional assessment tools included LAS, 6 MWD, and Karnofsky scores. These assessments were taken only at the time of enrollment and not followed longitudinally. Univariate logistic regression analysis was performed to determine the association of baseline positive affect, negative affect, or positivity ratio with outcomes of interest. Models were then adjusted by pre-selected variables of age, dichotomous variable of currently married or not, and dichotomous variable of having at least some college education or not. We adjusted for age, marital status and education as we believed those to be the most pertinent possible confounding variables. P-values ≤0.05 were considered significant; no corrections were made for multiple comparisons.

Missing Data

Participants were only included in the analysis if a baseline PANAS was completed in its entirety. We did not analyze partially completed GAD-2, PHQ-2, or CRQ questionnaires.

RESULTS

Patient Demographics

During the recruitment period, 217 transplant-listed patients were sent questionnaires and 169 (77.9%) completed them. The average age at study enrollment was 58.9 ± 9.4 years. Most participants were married (71.0%), Caucasian (93.4%), and completed at least some college (62.1%) (Table 1). Primary caregivers included spouses (63.3%), parents (8.9%), adult children (7.1%), and non-spouse significant others (7.1%). Participants at Mayo Clinic tended to be younger (57.2 ± 9.28 years) than participants from other sites (61.2 ± 8.2 years; p=0.005). Mayo Clinic participants were also more likely to be married (77.8%) than non-Mayo Clinic participants (64.8%, p=0.04). A similar proportion of Mayo Clinic and non-Mayo Clinic participants received transplants, were removed from the waitlist, and died during the study.

Table 1:

Cohort Characteristics

Mean (SD) or n (%)
n 169

Age (yrs) 58.9 (9.4)

Gender
Male 75 (44.4)
Female 76 (45.0)
Unknown 18 (10.7)

BMI 26.2 (4.5)
Underweight (<18.0) 4 (2.4)
Normal weight (18.0-25.0) 58 (34.3)
Overweight (>25.0-30.0) 79 (46.7)
Obese (>30.0) 21 (12.4)
Unknown 7 (4.1)

Marital Status
Never Married 15 (8.8)
Married 120 (71.0)
Widowed 4 (2.4)
Separated or divorced 30 (17.8)

Education
Less than high school graduate 14 (8.3)
High school graduate 35 (20.7)
Trade school 15 (8.9)
Some college 49 (29.0)
Bachelor’s degree 29 (17.2)
Advanced degree 27 (16.0)

Race
Caucasian 158 (93.4)
Other 11 (6.5)

Primary Caregiver
Parent 15 (8.9)
Spouse 107 (63.3)
Significant Other 12 (7.1)
Child 12 (7.1)
Sibling 10 (5.9)
Other 11 (6.5)
Unknown 2 (1.2)

Mayo Clinic Transplant Candidates
n 95

Reason for Transplant
COPD/ Emphysema 28 (29.5)
Pulmonary Fibrosis 33 (34.7)
Sarcoidosis 6 (6.3)
Cystic Fibrosis 6 (6.3)
ILD 10 (10.5)
Pulmonary Hypertension 4 (4.2)
Congenital Heart Disease 3 (3.2)
Other 5 (5.3)

Abbreviations: SD=Standard Deviation; YRS=years; BMI=Body Mass Index; COPD=Chronic Obstructive Pulmonary Disease; ILD=Interstitial Lung Disease

For the 95 participants from Mayo Clinic Rochester and Florida sites: Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD) (29.5%) and pulmonary fibrosis (34.7%) were the most common reasons for transplant listing. PACT reflected acceptable or better psychosocial candidacy for transplant (mean scores 2.6 ± 0.8; N=93). Likewise SIPAT scores reflected low psychosocial risk for transplant (mean score 14.7 ± 7.9, N=62). The mean LAS and Karnofsky scores were 37.4 ± 7.7 and 62.6 ± 13.2. Pre-transplant six-minute walk distance was 1172.5 ± 325.4 feet.

As of December 21, 2018, 93 participants (55.0%) were transplanted, 17 (10.1%) were delisted, 15 (8.9%) died while on the transplant waitlist, and 44 were still waiting. At 1-year and 2-year follow-up, 31 of 34 (91.2%) and 10 of 13 (76.9%) participants still awaiting transplant completed questionnaires, respectively. Of the 93 participants transplanted, 61 (65.6%) completed a post-transplant PANAS questionnaire and median time to completion following transplant was 115 days (IQR: 92,149.5). Of the 95 participants from Mayo Clinic Rochester and Mayo Clinic Florida, 32 (33.7%) were transplanted during the study period. Median duration of mechanical ventilation following transplant was 3.0 days [interquartile range (IQR) 0.7, 5.3] and median post-transplant hospital stay was 16.5 days (IQR 4.6, 28.4).

Positive Affect

The mean baseline positive affect score was 36.0 ± 7.7 (mean positive affect is 33.3 ± 7.2 in healthy college students; higher positive affect score indicates greater positive affect)19. There were no differences in positive affect scores according to sex, marital status, relationship of primary caregiver, race, or reason for transplant. BMI and positive affect were positively correlated (0.20, p=0.01). Participants with an underweight BMI had a significantly lower positive affect score (27.3 ± 6.8) compared to those with normal (34.9 ± 7.8), overweight (37.5 ± 7.2), or obese (34.7 ± 8.3) BMI (p=0.02) (Table 2). As expected, both GAD-2 and PHQ-2 negatively correlated with positive affect scores at baseline (−0.25, p=0.001 and −0.43, p<0.0001, respectively). Interestingly, however, neither PACT (0.02, p=0.83) nor SIPAT (−0.18, p=0.17) scores correlated with positive affect. Between LAS, 6 MWD, and Karnofsky scores, only Karnofsky scores correlated with positive affect (0.20, p=0.04). Positive affect did not correlate with any post-transplant CRQ domain: dyspnea (0.08, p=0.49), fatigue (−0.01, p=0.92), emotional function (0.05, p=0.66), and mastery (0.07, p=0.57). Positive affect was not associated with outcomes of death on the waitlist or delisting (Table 3). After one year of waiting for transplant, 8 of 31 (25.8%) participants had a clinically significant decrease in positive affect, with a 30% decline in those still waiting at 2 years. For the overall cohort the decline in positive affect was clinically and statistically insignificant. Overtime, there was an insignificant increase in mean positive affect score among those still waiting for transplant at 1-year (+0.97 ± 5.9, p=0.37) followed by an insignificant decrease (−2.00 ± 5.6, p=0.29) at 2-years (Table 4). Positive affect was not significantly changed following transplant (−0.70 ± 9.0, p=0.54).

Table 2:

Factors associated with positive affect, negative affect, and positivity ratio

Positive Affect Negative Affect Positivity Ratio
Variables n Mean ± SD P-Value Mean ± SD P-Value Mean ± SD P-Value
Gender 0.57 0.26 0.33
Female 76 36.3 ± 7.7 17.8 ± 5.6 2.31 ± 1.06
Male 75 35.5 ± 7.7 16.7 ± 6.7 2.48 ± 1.05
Unknown 18

BMI 0.02* 0.90 0.23
Underweight 4 27.3 ± 6.8 20.0 ± 9.0 1.70 ± 1.12
Normal weight 58 34.9 ± 7.8 17.3 ± 6.3 2.33 ± 1.08
Overweight 79 37.5 ± 7.2 16.7 ± 5.4 2.51 ± 1.01
Obese 21 34.7 ± 8.3 17.7 ± 6.5 2.31 ± 1.78
Unknown 7

Marital Status 0.66 0.83 0.98
Single 49 36.4 ± 7.2 17.1 ± 5.3 2.39 ± 1.06
Married 120 35.9 ± 7.9 17.3 ± 6.4 2.39 ± 1.06

Education 0.90 0.93 0.97
No college 64 35.9 ± 7.9 17.2 ± 6.4 2.39 ± 1.07
Some college or higher 105 36.1 ± 7.6 17.3 ± 5.9 2.39 ± 1.05

Race 0.21 0.04* 0.09
Caucasian 158 35.8 ± 7.7 17.5 ± 6.2 2.36 ± 1.06
Other 11 38.9 ± 7.4 14.5 ± 4.1 2.85 ± 0.83

Primary Caregiver 0.83 0.58 0.86
Non-spouse 60 35.8 ± 7.9 17.0 ± 5.2 2.37 ± 1.07
Spouse 107 36.1 ± 7.6 17.5 ± 6.6 2.40 ± 1.06
Unknown 2

Reason for Transplant** 0.70 0.77 0.95
COPD/ Bronchiectasis 34 36.0 ± 8.0 16.8 ± 6.4 2.45 ± 1.01
ILD 49 35.6 ± 8.5 17.5 ± 6.8 2.41 ± 1.22
Other 12 38.9 ± 7.8 16.8 ± 4.0 2.52 ± 1.10

GAD-2 0.11 <0.0001* <0.0001*
Positive 25 33.7 ± 7.5 24.7 ± 7.2 1.57 ± 0.82
Negative 140 36.4 ± 7.7 15.9 ± 4.8 2.54 ± 1.03
Incomplete 4

PHQ-2 0.02* 0.0004* <0.0001*
Positive 23 31.6 ± 9.3 22.7 ± 7.2 .56 ± 0.76
Negative 143 36.7 ± 7.2 16.4 ± 5.5 2.51 ± 1.04
Incomplete 3

Received transplant during study 0.97 0.25 0.24
Yes 93 36.0 ± 8.0 16.8 ± 6.1 2.47 ± 1.11
No 76 36.1 ± 7.2 17.9 ± 6.0 2.28 ± 0.97

Died on Waitlist 0.17 0.07 0.02*
Yes 15 32.9 ± 8.8 20.7 ± 7.3 1.82 ± 0.92
No 154 36.3 ± 6.3 16.9 ± 5.9 2.45 ± 1.05

Removed from transplant list 0.29 0.77 0.86
Yes 17 38.1 ± 6.3 17.0 ± 3.7 2.36 ± 0.72
No 152 35.8 ± 7.8 17.3 ± 6.3 2.39 ± 1.09

Died Post-transplant*** 0.94 0.58 0.96
Yes 9 35.9 ± 10.3 15.9 ± 4.5 2.51 ± 1.26
No 84 36.2 ± 7.8 16.8 ± 6.3 2.49 ± 1.11

Abbreviations: SD=Standard Deviation; COPD=Chronic Obstructive Pulmonary Disease; ILD=Interstitial Lung Disease; GAD-2=Generalized Anxiety Disorder Scale-2; PHQ-2=Patient Health Questionnaire-2

*

Indicates statistical significance

**

Includes only Mayo Clinic participants (total n=95)

***

Includes only patients that received a transplant during the study period (total n=93)

Table 3:

Multivariable logistic regression models univariate and adjusted for age, married, and some college education

Endpoint Variable OR p-value Adjusted OR Adjusted p-value
Removed from transplant list Positive Affect 1.04 0.23 1.05 0.24
Negative Affect 0.99 0.85 0.99 0.89
Positivity Ratio 0.97 0.90 0.96 0.87
Died on Waitlist Positive Affect 0.94 0.10 0.95 0.15
Negative Affect 1.09 0.03* 1.09 0.05*
Positivity Ratio 0.50 0.03* 0.51 0.06
Received transplant during study Positive Affect 1.00 0.10 1.00 0.92
Negative Affect 0.97 0.25 0.97 0.27
Positivity Ratio 1.19 0.24 1.18 0.27

Abbreviations: OR=Odds Ratio;

*

Indicates statistical significance.

Table 4:

Change in positive affect, negative affect, and positivity ratio over time on the waitlist and post-transplant

Positive Affect Negative Affect Positivity Ratio
n Mean ± SD p-Value Mean ± SD p-Value Mean ± SD p-Value
Baseline 169 36.0 ± 7.7 17.3 ± 6.1 2.39 ± 1.06
1-year follow-up (waitlisted) 31 +0.97 ± 5.9 0.37 −0.13 ± 7.9 0.93 +0.04 ± 0.96 0.80
2-years follow-up (waitlisted) 10 −2.00 ± 5.6 0.29 +2.90 ± 2.9 0.34 −0.70 ± 1.07 0.07
Post-transplant 61 −0.70 ± 9.0 0.54 +1.67 ± 6.3 0.04* −0.11 ± 1.23 0.48

Abbreviations: SD=Standard Deviation;

P-Value is a comparison from baseline mean.

*

Indicates statistical significance.

Negative Affect

The mean baseline negative affect score was 17.3 ± 6.1 (mean negative affect is 17.4 ± 6.2 in healthy college students; higher negative affect score indicates greater negative affect)19. Similar to the above, there was no difference in negative affect scores according to sex, marital status, relationship with primary caregiver, or reason for transplant listing. However, Caucasian participants had a significantly higher negative affect score compared to non-Caucasian participants (17.5 ± 6.2 versus 14.5 ± 4.1; p=0.04). As expected, GAD-2 and PHQ-2 scores positively correlated with negative affect (0.67, p<0.0001 and 0.51, p<0.0001, respectively). Negative affect did not correlate with 6 minute walk distance (−0.18, p=0.08), LAS (−0.02, p=0.87), or Karnofsky score (0.01, p=0.90). Negative affect did not correlate with the post-transplant CRQ domains of dyspnea (−0.16, p=0.17), emotional function (−0.16, p=0.19), fatigue (−0.21, p=0.08), or mastery (−0.22, p=0.06).

Negative affect did not correlate with PACT (−0.05, p=0.63), but positively correlated with SIPAT (0.25, p=0.05). Negative affect was not different among participants who were transplanted (compared to those not), delisted compared to those not, or died following transplant compared to those who survived. Negative affect was higher in patients that died awaiting transplant compared to those who survived, although this did not reach statistical significance (20.7 ± 7.3 versus 16.9 ± 5.9, respectively; p=0.07). (Table 2) Negative affect was associated with increased death on the waiting list by univariate analysis (OR 1.09; 95% confidence interval (CI) 1.01 to 1.18; p=0.029). This association remained (OR 1.10; 95% CI 1.02 to 1.20; p=0.021) despite adjustment for age, marital status, and level of education. (Table 3) After one year of waiting, 9 of 31 (29%) participants had a clinically significant increase (≥3) in the negative affect; after 2 years of waiting 5 of 10 had a clinically significant increase. Negative affect did not significantly change over time on the waitlist compared to baseline (1-year: −0.13 ± 7.9, p=0.93; 2-years: −2.90 ± 2.9, p=0.34) (Table 4). However, negative affect increased post-transplant (1.67 ± 6.3, p=0.04), but this did not meet the threshold of clinical significance.

Positivity Ratio

The mean baseline positivity ratio was 2.39 ± 1.06. A minority of participants had a positivity ratio ≥2.9 indicating flourishing well-being (51 or 169, 30.2%). There was no difference in positivity ratio based on sex, marital status, relationship with primary caregiver, race, or reason for transplant listing. BMI and positivity ratio were positively correlated (0.20, p=0.009), but positivity ratio did not vary with BMI category (Table 2). As expected, positivity ratio was lower in participants with a positive baseline GAD-2 (1.57 ± 0.82) and PHQ-2 (1.56 ± 0.76) compared to participants with a negative baseline GAD-2 (2.54 ± 1.03, p<0.0001) and PHQ-2 (2.51 ± 1.04, p<0.0001). Positivity ratio negatively correlated with GAD-2 (−0.52, p<0.0001) and PHQ-2 (−0.50, p<0.0001). PACT, SIPAT, LAS, or Karnofsky scores; CRQ subdomains, or 6 MWD did not correlate with positivity ratio. Participants who died on the waitlist (1.82 ± 0.92) had a lower positivity ratio compared to those who survived (2.45 ± 1.05, p=0.02). Likewise, a higher positivity ratio was associated with decreased death on the waiting list by univariate analysis OR 0.50 (95% CI 0.26 – 0.95; p=0.033) and following adjustment for age, sex, and education OR 0.45 (95% CI 0.23 – 0.92; p=0.027). Positivity ratio did not significantly change while on the waiting list (1-year: +0.04 ± 0.96, p=0.80; 2-year: −0.70 ± 1.07, p=0.07) or post-transplant (−0.11 ± 1.23, p=0.48) compared to baseline (Table 4).

DISCUSSION

Affect refers to the emotions we experience, our emotional expressions, and the influence of our emotions on our actions and sense of emotional well-being. Negative affect and positive affect are not dichotomous. A single experience—such as being listed for lung transplant—can generate both strong negative and strong positive emotional responses. We found that those with a higher negative affect and a lower positivity ratio had higher odds of mortality on the waitlist. This association persisted despite adjustments for potential confounding variables. Affect may therefore represent a novel risk factor for wait list mortality in lung transplant candidates.

Our findings enhance prior research demonstrating the importance of psychosocial risk factors in solid organ transplant patients2022,30,31. Prior work in solid organ transplant has shown patients’ dispositional optimism is associated with coping in kidney transplant 21. Additionally, pre-transplant optimism was associated with post-transplantation quality of life in heart transplant recipients22. In lung transplant specifically, prior work has demonstrated associations between psychiatric conditions (e.g. persistent depression) or psychosocial risk factors (e.g. adverse childhood events or low baseline PACT score) and post-transplant mortality7,16,32,33. Our findings are consistent with previous research demonstrating an association of affect with adverse events. For example, Maxwell et al. demonstrated that increased negative affect (as measured by PANAS) was associated with increased severity of illness in a cohort of 353 subjects suffering acute respiratory infections34. This finding could be particularly relevant to lung transplant candidates with limited respiratory reserve while waiting for transplantation. Reduced positive affect has also been associated with risk of death or myocardial infarction, while increased positive affect was protective against cardiac events35,36. Such findings lend credence to the possible association between affect and mortality in our study.

We do not have cause of death data for those who died during our study and therefore it is unclear how a negative affect or a reduced positivity might have influenced mortality on the waiting list for our cohort. However, Benzo et al. has demonstrated that affect is a mediator of self-management37. Furthermore, Kessing et al. previously demonstrated an association between higher positive affect and improved self-care in patients with congestive heart failure38. Certainly if patients’ emotional well-being improves their ability to manage their chronic diseases, one could imagine it could lead to improved outcomes in lung transplant candidates.

Despite prior studies reporting decreased emotional well-being and increased depression and anxiety in lung transplant candidates39,40, mean positive affect was higher in our study compared to college students and living kidney donors19,41 and negative affect was similar to college students and living kidney donors19,41. However, we observed a mean positivity ratio (2.39 ± 1.06) lower than 2.9, the threshold previously described to distinguish individuals with optimal emotional functioning and resilience17. These findings indicate that many of the lung transplant candidates in our cohort were not at an optimal state of emotional well-being and interventions to improve affect may be helpful.

Affect can change, and positive affect has been the focus of prior interventions for improvement of emotional well-being in non-lung transplant populations42. Positive affect did not significantly change over time in our cohort, but following a lung transplant negative affect increased. This is consistent with prior studies that have demonstrated increased depression and psychological distress from the pre to post-transplant periods43,44. This increase in negative affect did not meet the minimally clinically significant threshold of 3.0, but may represent the emotional distress of post-transplant recovery and further reinforces the need for ongoing psychological care for patients after transplant even if post-transplant survival is not associated with pre-transplant emotional wellbeing, as in our cohort.

Limitations

Our study had limited post-transplant deaths during the study period and as such we were likely underpowered to demonstrate an association with post-transplant mortality. Although not excluded from our study, the sickest lung transplant candidates’ participation was likely limited both due to decreased capacity to complete the surveys and increased transplant urgency with decreased waiting time. Additionally, despite our large, multi-center sample, our recruited population was fairly homogenous in regards to race and education potentially limiting the applicability of our findings to other populations. Our ability to adjust for confounding variables was limited by the number of outcome events; therefore, we were unable to adjust for disease severity factors. In addition, our surveys could not be completed by the sickest patients (e.g. who were intubated and sedated or expeditiously transplanted prior to survey completion). These factors limit drawing definitive conclusions regarding the association of affect with peri-transplant mortality. A larger study to further define the predictive ability on affect should be performed to confirm our findings.

Implications for future research

Our study describes affect and potential associated factors in lung transplant candidates. Further understanding of how affect can be modified and whether modification improves waitlist survival are key topics for future investigation. Moreover longer duration and larger studies evaluating the impact of affect on post-transplant outcomes are needed.

CONCLUSIONS

Affect was similar in lung transplant candidates compared to population norms. High negative affect and low positivity ratio were associated with death on the waitlist, despite adjustment for some potential confounding variables. A limitation of this finding, however, is our inability to adjust for disease severity given the small number of events during the study period. Affect may be a useful adjunctive measure for emotional well-being and may represent a future target for psychological optimization in lung transplant candidates.

Acknowledgments

This study was approved by the Mayo Clinic Institutional Review Board and approved under IRB 15-005378.

Financial Support: This research and CCK’s time was supported by the NHLBI grant K23 HL128859 from the National Institutes of Health. KMP is supported by the Robert D. and Patricia E. Kern Center for the Science of Health Care Delivery, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN. RB is supported by the NHLBI grant K24 HL138150. The project utilized REDCap for data management; REDCap was supported by NIH/NCRR/NCATS CTSA Grant Number UL1 TR002377. The manuscript’s contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official view of NIH.

Abbreviations:

BMI

Body Mass Index

COPD

Chronic Obstructive Pulmonary Disease

CRQ

Chronic Respiratory Disease Questionnaire

GAD-2

Generalized Anxiety Disorder Scale-2

LAS

Lung Allocation Score

PACT

Psychosocial Assessment of Candidates for Transplantation

PANAS

Positive and Negative Affect Schedule

PHQ-2

Patient Health Questionnaire-2

SIPAT

Stanford Integrated Psychosocial Assessment for Transplant

6MWD

Six-minute Walking Distance

CI

Confidence Interval

IQR

interquartile range

SD

standard deviation

Footnotes

Conflict of interest: None of the authors have any conflict of interest. All authors have reviewed and contributed to this manuscript.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Valapour M, Lehr CJ, Skeans MA, et al. OPTN/SRTR 2016 Annual Data Report: Lung. Am J Transplant. 2018;18 Suppl 1:363–433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hook JL, Lederer DJ. Selecting lung transplant candidates: where do current guidelines fall short? Expert Rev Respir Med. 2012;6(1):51–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Wilson ME, Vakil AP, Kandel P, Undavalli C, Dunlay SM, Kennedy CC. Pretransplant frailty is associated with decreased survival after lung transplantation. J Heart Lung Transplant. 2016;35(2):173–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Singer JP, Diamond JM, Gries CJ, et al. Frailty Phenotypes, Disability, and Outcomes in Adult Candidates for Lung Transplantation. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2015;192(11):1325–1334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kobashigawa J, Dadhania D, Bhorade S, et al. Report from the American Society of Transplantation on frailty in solid organ transplantation. Am J Transplant. 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Fitten LJ. Psychological Frailty in the Aging Patient. Nestle Nutr Inst Workshop Ser. 2015;83:45–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Smith PJ, Blumenthal JA, Trulock EP, et al. Psychosocial Predictors of Mortality Following Lung Transplantation. Am J Transplant. 2016;16(1):271–277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Lee SJ, Loberiza FR, Rizzo JD, Soiffer RJ, Antin JH, Weeks JC. Optimistic expectations and survival after hematopoietic stem cell transplantation. Biol Blood Marrow Transplant. 2003;9(6):389–396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Goetzmann L, Klaghofer R, Wagner-Huber R, et al. Psychosocial vulnerability predicts psychosocial outcome after an organ transplant: results of a prospective study with lung, liver, and bone-marrow patients. J Psychosom Res. 2007;62(1):93–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Weill D, Benden C, Corris PA, et al. A consensus document for the selection of lung transplant candidates: 2014--an update from the Pulmonary Transplantation Council of the International Society for Heart and Lung Transplantation. J Heart Lung Transplant. 2015;34(1):1–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Lewandowski AN, Skillings JL. Who gets a lung transplant? Assessing the psychosocial decision-making process for transplant listing. Glob Cardiol Sci Pract. 2016;2016(3):e201626. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Erim Y, Beckmann M, Marggraf G, Senf W. Psychosomatic evaluation of patients awaiting lung transplantation. Transplant Proc. 2009;41(6):2595–2598. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Olbrisch MLJ, Hamer R. The PACT: a rating scale for the study of clinical decision making in psychosocial screening of organ transplant candidates. Clinical Transplant. 1989;3:164–169. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Maldonado JR, Dubois HC, David EE, et al. The Stanford Integrated Psychosocial Assessment for Transplantation (SIPAT): a new tool for the psychosocial evaluation of pre-transplant candidates. Psychosomatics. 2012;53(2):123–132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Dew MA, DiMartini AF, Dobbels F, et al. The 2018 ISHLT/APM/AST/ICCAC/STSW recommendations for the psychosocial evaluation of adult cardiothoracic transplant candidates and candidates for long-term mechanical circulatory support. J Heart Lung Transplant. 2018;37(7):803–823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hitschfeld MJ, Schneekloth TD, Kennedy CC, et al. The Psychosocial Assessment of Candidates for Transplantation: A Cohort Study of its Association With Survival Among Lung Transplant Recipients. Psychosomatics. 2016;57(5):489–497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Fredrickson BL, Losada MF. Positive affect and the complex dynamics of human flourishing. Am Psychol. 2005;60(7):678–686. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Crawford JR, Henry JD. The positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS): construct validity, measurement properties and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br J Clin Psychol. 2004;43(Pt 3):245–265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54(6):1063–1070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Amonoo HL, Barclay ME, El-Jawahri A, Traeger LN, Lee SJ, Huffman JC. Positive Psychological Constructs and Health Outcomes in Hematopoietic Stem Cell Transplantation Patients: A Systematic Review. Biol Blood Marrow Transplant. 2019;25(1):e5–e16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Costa-Requena G, Cantarell-Aixendri MC, Parramon-Puig G, Seron-Micas D. Dispositional optimism and coping strategies in patients with a kidney transplant. Nefrologia. 2014;34(5):605–610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Jowsey SG, Cutshall SM, Colligan RC, et al. Seligman’s theory of attributional style: optimism, pessimism, and quality of life after heart transplant. Prog Transplant. 2012;22(1):49–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Harris PA, Taylor R, Thielke R, Payne J, Gonzalez N, Conde JG. Research electronic data capture (REDCap)--a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J Biomed Inform. 2009;42(2):377–381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bradburn N The structure of psychological well-being. 1969.
  • 25.Skapinakis P The 2-item Generalized Anxiety Disorder scale had high sensitivity and specificity for detecting GAD in primary care. Evid Based Med. 2007;12(5):149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Seo JG, Park SP. Validation of the Generalized Anxiety Disorder-7 (GAD-7) and GAD-2 in patients with migraine. J Headache Pain. 2015;16:97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Arroll B, Goodyear-Smith F, Crengle S, et al. Validation of PHQ-2 and PHQ-9 to screen for major depression in the primary care population. Ann Fam Med. 2010;8(4):348–353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Wang L, Lu K, Li J, Sheng L, Ding R, Hu D. [Value of patient health questionnaires (PHQ)-9 and PHQ-2 for screening depression disorders in cardiovascular outpatients]. Zhonghua Xin Xue Guan Bing Za Zhi. 2015;43(5):428–431. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Vodanovich DA, Bicknell TJ, Holland AE, et al. Validity and Reliability of the Chronic Respiratory Disease Questionnaire in Elderly Individuals with Mild to Moderate Non-Cystic Fibrosis Bronchiectasis. Respiration. 2015;90(2):89–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Rosenberger EM, Dew MA, Crone C, DiMartini AF. Psychiatric disorders as risk factors for adverse medical outcomes after solid organ transplantation. Curr Opin Organ Transplant. 2012;17(2):188–192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Chacko RC, Harper RG, Gotto J, Young J. Psychiatric interview and psychometric predictors of cardiac transplant survival. Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153(12):1607–1612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Evon DM, Burker EJ, Galanko JA, Dedert E, Egan TM. Depressive symptoms and mortality in lung transplant. Clin Transplant. 2010;24(5):E201–206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Kennedy CC, Zubair A, Clark MM, Jowsey-Gregoire S. Childhood Abuse Is Associated With Worse Survival Following Lung Transplantation. Prog Transplant. 2016;26(2):178–182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Maxwell L, Barrett B, Chase J, Brown R, Ewers T. Self-Reported Mental Health Predicts Acute Respiratory Infection. WMJ. 2015;114(3):100–104. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Davidson KW, Mostofsky E, Whang W. Don’t worry, be happy: positive affect and reduced 10-year incident coronary heart disease: the Canadian Nova Scotia Health Survey. Eur Heart J. 2010;31(9):1065–1070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Denollet J, Pedersen SS, Daemen J, de Jaegere P, Serruys PW, van Domburg RT. Reduced positive affect (anhedonia) predicts major clinical events following implantation of coronary-artery stents. J Intern Med. 2008;263(2):203–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Benzo RP, Abascal-Bolado B, Dulohery MM. Self-management and quality of life in chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD): The mediating effects of positive affect. Patient Educ Couns. 2016;99(4):617–623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kessing D, Pelle AJ, Kupper N, Szabo BM, Denollet J. Positive affect, anhedonia, and compliance with self-care in patients with chronic heart failure. J Psychosom Res. 2014;77(4):296–301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Finlen Copeland CA, Vock DM, Pieper K, Mark DB, Palmer SM. Impact of lung transplantation on recipient quality of life: a serial, prospective, multicenter analysis through the first posttransplant year. Chest. 2013;143(3):744–750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Parekh PI, Blumenthal JA, Babyak MA, et al. Psychiatric disorder and quality of life in patients awaiting lung transplantation. Chest. 2003;124(5):1682–1688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Hsieh CY, Chien CH, Liu KL, Wang HH, Lin KJ, Chiang YJ. Positive and Negative Affects in Living Kidney Donors. Transplant Proc. 2017;49(9):2036–2039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Sin NL, Lyubomirsky S. Enhancing well-being and alleviating depressive symptoms with positive psychology interventions: a practice-friendly meta-analysis. J Clin Psychol. 2009;65(5):467–487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Dew MA, DiMartini AF, DeVito Dabbs AJ, et al. Onset and risk factors for anxiety and depression during the first 2 years after lung transplantation. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2012;34(2):127–138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Stilley CS, Dew MA, Stukas AA, et al. Psychological symptom levels and their correlates in lung and heart-lung transplant recipients. Psychosomatics. 1999;40(6):503–509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES