Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2020 Jan 23;55(6):685–696. doi: 10.1007/s00127-019-01829-0

Associations between perceived neighborhood environment and cognitive function among middle-aged and older women and men: Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos Sociocultural Ancillary Study

Mayra L Estrella 1, Ramon A Durazo-Arvizu 1,2, Linda C Gallo 3, Carmen R Isasi 4, Krista M Perreira 5, Thanh-Huyen T Vu 6, Elizabeth Vasquez 7, Shruti Sachdeva 1, Donglin Zeng 8, Maria M Llabre 9, Wassim Tarraf 10, Hector M González 11, Martha L Daviglus 1, Melissa Lamar 12
PMCID: PMC7276286  NIHMSID: NIHMS1551502  PMID: 31974810

INTRODUCTION

Socio-ecological [1] and lifecourse models [2] emphasize the role of environmental factors on health outcomes, suggesting that living in neighborhoods with favorable environments (e.g., low social disorder and crime, and strong social ties) may contribute to better cognitive health [3]. Therefore, neighborhood environments, or the characteristics that inform perceptions of these environments, are attractive targets for promoting cognitive health because as people age their ability to move across neighborhoods tends to decline [4, 5]. Perceived neighborhood environments, as proxies for neighborhood-derived psychosocial stressors, may play a role in shaping cognitive health through lifestyle [6], cardiovascular [4], and/or psychological pathways [5, 7].

Evidence indicates that perceptions of one’s neighborhood environment are more strongly associated with various health outcomes including overall well-being [8], walking [9], and sedentary time [10] than objective measures, regardless of age or race/ethnicity. However, only a few studies [11-14] have examined the associations between perceived neighborhood environment and cognitive function, and results have been inconsistent. Some studies have reported associations between favorable perceived neighborhood environment and better cognitive function in older Hispanic immigrants [11], as well as older non-Hispanic white and African Americans [12], but other studies report null associations in older Non-Hispanic white women [13] and older Japanese adults [14]. Most of these studies on the association between perceived neighborhood environment and cognitive function had samples of older non-Hispanic whites and African Americans. To date, replication studies from large samples of middle-aged and older Hispanics/Latinos have not been conducted. This is despite the fact that Hispanics/Latinos living in the U.S. tend to experience earlier onset of progressive declines in cognitive abilities [15] such as verbal learning and memory, and executive function. The current study addresses this gap in the literature.

Using data from the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos (HCHS/SOL) and its Sociocultural Ancillary Study (SCAS), we examined the cross-sectional associations between perceived neighborhood environment and cognitive function among middle-aged and older Hispanic/Latino women and men. A potential reason for the discrepancies reported across previous studies of perceived neighborhood environment and cognition in other race/ethnic groups [11-14] is that associations between perceived neighborhood environment and cognitive function have not been examined by gender. This is particularly important given there exists an extensive body of research documenting that gender differences exist in the impact of the perceived neighborhood environment on health outcomes among non-Hispanic white and African American adults [16-23]. For example, among women but not men, better perceived neighborhood environment has been associated with higher physical activity [16, 22], longer telomere length [21], and lower risk of coronary artery calcification [17], incident depression [18-20], and cardiovascular disease [23].

Thus, based on evidence of stronger associations of perceived neighborhood environment on vascular and metabolic risk factors among women compared to men, coupled with the known sex differences in cognitive function that exist in the HCHS/SOL cohort [24] (i.e., women had higher cognitive scores with the exception of verbal fluency, regardless of key confounders), we hypothesized that more favorable perceived neighborhood environment measures (i.e., higher neighborhood social cohesion, lower neighborhood problems, and higher safety from crime) would each be associated with higher cognitive function among women but not among men. Given that middle-aged and older Hispanics/Latinos are one of the fastest growing segments of the U.S. population [25], there is an urgent need for identifying potentially modifiable factors to promote cognitive health and successful aging in this population [1].

METHODS

Study Design and Analytic Sample

The HCHS/SOL is a multicenter population-based study of cardiovascular diseases and related chronic conditions among adults ages 18–74 years at baseline (2008–2011) of Cuban, Central American, Dominican, Mexican, Puerto Rican, South American, and other/more than one Hispanic/Latino backgrounds [26-28]. Study enrollment (N= 16,415) was conducted from selected households in four U.S. metropolitan areas (i.e., Bronx, NY; Chicago, IL; Miami, FL; and San Diego, CA). The purpose of the SCAS is to examine associations of multiple sociocultural and psychosocial factors with cardiovascular disease and metabolic syndrome prevalence in Hispanics/Latinos. The conceptual framework of the SCAS is based on the Reserve Capacity [29] and Lifespan Biopsychosocial Models [30], which highlight the role of resources and stressors as pathways underlying socioeconomic and ethnic influences on health outcomes. Those HCHS/SOL participants who consented to be contacted for future studies were eligible to participate in the SCAS (2009–2010) [28], which had a 72.6% participation rate. The SCAS sample (N= 5,313) is representative of the HCHS/SOL cohort, although individuals from higher socioeconomic strata were less likely to participate [28]. The HCHS/SOL was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of all sites. All participants provided written informed consent.

The inclusion criteria for this study was defined as SCAS participants eligible to complete neurocognitive assessments because they were ages 45 and older at baseline (n= 3,278). We excluded 171 participants with self-reported history of stroke or heart attack because they are more likely to have cognitive impairment or dementia [31, 32]; if we included these conditions in our analyses, results would be more difficult to interpret within the theoretical framework of perceived neighborhood environment and cognition. We also excluded participants with missing data on any of the cognitive function measures (n= 136), any of the neighborhood environment measures (n= 89), or any of the study covariates (n= 87). The final analytic sample of the current study is comprised of 2,846 participants (1,812 women and 1,034 men). Compared to our analytic sample, excluded HCHS/SOL participants ages 45 and older were more likely to be of higher income (p= 0.002) but did not significantly differ by gender, education, language preference, or field site.

Study Measures

Cognitive Function

Trained interviewers administered cognitive tests in the preferred language of the participant (English or Spanish). Verbal learning and memory were assessed using the Brief Spanish-English Verbal Learning Test (B-SEVLT) [33, 34]. During 3 consecutive trials, the interviewer read all words included in a 15-item list (i.e., list A) and asked the participant to recall them. After reading a distractor list, the interviewer asked participants to recall as many words from list A as possible to assess free-recall post-interference. Verbal learning is the sum of the items correctly recalled from list A across the 3 learning trials (range: 0–45) and memory is the sum of the number of items correctly recalled post-interference (range: 0–15). Verbal fluency was assessed through an adapted version of the Word Fluency Test of the Multilingual Aphasia Examination [35, 36]. Participants were asked to generate as many words as possible in 60 seconds that began with the letter F (first trial) and the letter A (second trial). Verbal fluency score is the sum of the correctly generated words across trials (range: 0–50). Processing speed was assessed using the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised Digit Symbol Substitution Subtest [37]. Participants were asked to write the corresponding symbol for each digit based on the provided key. Processing speed score is the sum of the correctly identified symbols in 90 seconds (range: 0–83). To measure global cognitive function, a composite score was calculated by summing the z-scores (i.e., [individual value - mean value] /SD) for performance across the four cognitive tests (i.e., verbal learning, memory, verbal fluency, and processing speed). Across all tests, higher scores represent better cognitive function.

Perceived Neighborhood Environment

In the SCAS, perceived neighborhood environment data were collected via interviewer-administered questionnaires originally developed for use in the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis [38]. Neighborhood was defined to the participants as “the area around where you live and around your house. It may include places you shop, religious or public institutions, or a local business district. It is the general area around your house where you might perform routine tasks, such as shopping, going to the park, or visiting with neighbors.”

A 7-item scale [38] was used to assess participants’ perceptions of the neighborhood physical environment (i.e., neighborhood problems) related to disorder including excessive noise, heavy traffic or speeding cars, lack of access to adequate food shopping, lack of parks or playgrounds, trash and litter, no or poorly maintained sidewalks, and violence. Participants indicated the seriousness of each item based a scale ranging from 1 = “very serious problem” to 4 = “not really a problem.” All items were reverse-coded and a total score (range: 7–28) was computed by summing item scores for participants who answered all 7-items (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.79 in our sample). Construct validity of the perceived neighborhood problems measure has been shown in a sample of U.S. Hispanic/Latino adults [39].

Sampson’s Social Cohesion 5-item scale [40] was used to assess perceived neighborhood social cohesion (i.e., social environment characterized by presence of trusting relationships and connection to one’s neighbors). Participants indicated their extent of agreement with each statement based on a scale ranging from 1 = “strongly agree” to 5 = “strongly disagree.” Sample items include: “People around here are willing to help their neighbor” and “People in this neighborhood can be trusted.” Three items were reverse-coded. A total score (range: 5–25) was derived by summing item scores for participants who answered all 5-items (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.68 in our sample). Previous research has shown sufficient reliability, internal consistency, and construct validity for U.S. Hispanics/Latinos [38, 39].

To assess perceived neighborhood safety from crime, participants were asked, “How safe from crime do you consider your neighborhood to be?” Response ranged from 1 = “very safe” to 5 = “not at all safe” and were reverse-coded. Previous research has shown sufficient reliability and internal consistency for Hispanic/Latino adults [39].

Lower neighborhood problems, higher neighborhood social cohesion, and higher neighborhood safety from crime scores represent more favorable perceived neighborhood environments (with the referent category representing least favorable environments). Given that a major goal of our work was to replicate and extend prior studies of the perceived neighborhood environment, health, and cognition in samples of other race/ethnic groups to Hispanics/Latinos [12, 14, 17-19, 22], neighborhood variables were treated as categorical. In these studies, reasons for categorizing the perceived neighborhood environment measures include the potential presence of non-linear relationships, among others, which make categorization a better fit to capture a fine gradation of predictor-outcome associations. Thus, perceived neighborhood problems and social cohesion scores were categorized into quintiles (of roughly equal size based on the distribution of the overall sample). Perceived neighborhood safety from crime was categorized as low (responses= 1–2), medium (response= 3), and high (responses= 4–5) since it was assessed based on a single item.

Covariates

Covariates included self-reported age, Hispanic/Latino background, language preference for baseline examination, field site, years lived in neighborhood, annual household income, education, physical activity, and depressive symptoms. Physical activity in a typical week was assessed using the Global Physical Activity Questionnaire [41] and categorized according to adherence to the 2008 US Guidelines for meeting high or medium activity levels [41]. Adherence included high activity (i.e., >300 minutes/week of moderate-intensity physical activity, or >150 minutes/week of vigorous activity, or combination of both) and medium activity (i.e., 150–300 minutes/week of moderate-intensity activity, or 75–150 minutes/week of vigorous-intensity physical activity, or combination of both). Lack of adherence included low activity (i.e., activity beyond baseline but <150 min/week of moderate-intensity physical activity, or <75 minutes/week of vigorous-intensity activity, or combination of both) and inactivity (i.e., no activity beyond baseline activities of daily living). Depressive symptoms were ascertained with the 10-item version of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression scale [42]. Covariates were selected based on prior empirical studies and because, in preliminary bivariate analyses, they were associated with either the neighborhood environment features in our sample and/or cognitive function.

Statistical Analysis

All analyses were conducted separately among women and men as it has been consistently documented that perceptions of the neighborhood environment are differently associated with health outcomes in women compared to men [16-23]. Descriptive statistics (i.e., percentages and means with standard errors) were generated to characterize women and men of our target population based on study covariates. Differences in participant characteristics by each perceived neighborhood environment measure were examined among women and men using F-tests for continuous variables and chi-square tests for categorical variables. Adjusted linear regression models were used to examine the associations between each perceived neighborhood environment measure and cognitive function in women and men. P-values were computed to examine linear trends across categories of neighborhood perceptions. Model 1 adjusted for sociodemographic characteristics (i.e., age, Hispanic/Latino background, language preference, field site, years lived in neighborhood, annual household income, and education). Model 2 additionally adjusted for depressive symptoms. Further adjustment for physical activity did not change our results (data not shown); thus, physical activity was not considered a confounder in the current study.

All reported values were weighted to account for the complex study design (except sample size which is unweighted). Weights were trimmed to limit precision losses and calibrated to the 2010 Census characteristics by age, sex, and Hispanic/Latino background in each field site’s target population. Data management was performed using SAS 9.4 software (SAS Institute, Cary, NC) and statistical analyses were performed using Stata software Release 15 (Stata Corp LP, College Station, TX). All significance tests were two-sided and significance level was defined as 5%.

RESULTS

Characteristics of Target Population

The mean age of women and men was estimated to be 56 years of age and the target population was predominantly of Mexican background (Table 1). Most women (51.7%) and men (46.7%) reported less than $20,000 as the annual household income, and most women (86.5%) and men (83.4%) reported Spanish as the preferred language for the baseline examination. Women and men lived in their current neighborhood for 10 and 9 years on average, respectively. Approximately, 12.4% of women and 13.9% of men exhibited low mental status as defined as a score of 4 or below on the Six Item Screener. In bivariate analyses, women and men living in neighborhoods with the least favorable levels of perceived neighborhood problems and safety from crime were more likely to report lower annual household income and had higher depressive symptoms (Supplementary Tables 1 and 2). Among women, but not men, all of the perceived neighborhood environment measures were associated with global cognitive function.

Table 1.

Characteristics of study population: Women and Men

Women (n= 1,812) Men (n= 1,034)
N (%) or Mean (SE)
Age
 Mean Age in Years 56.5 (0.4) 56.1 (0.3)
Hispanic/Latino Background
 Dominican 198 (11.3%) 78 (8.4%)
 Central or South American 315 (12.8%) 177 (12.1%)
 Cuban 265 (24.3%) 223 (32.9%)
 Mexican 703 (31.3%) 339 (27.6%)
 Puerto Rican 307 (19.4%) 197 (17.4%)
 Other/ More than one Background 24 (1.0%) 20 (1.6%)
Annual Household Income
 <$20,000 939 (51.7%) 480 (46.7%)
 $20,000—$50,000 595 (28.2%) 393 (35.3%)
 >$50,000 107 (8.0%) 116 (12.9%)
 Not Reported 171 (12.1%) 45 (5.1%)
Education
 <High School 726 (39.4%) 391 (36.8%)
 High School 376 (19.0%) 250 (22.2%)
 >High School 710 (41.6%) 393 (41.0%)
Language Preference
 Spanish 1603 (86.5%) 878 (83.4%)
 English 209 (13.5%) 156 (16.7%)
Neighborhood Residence
 Mean Years Living in Neighborhood 9.8 (0.6) 9.1 (0.4)
Field Center
 Bronx 441 (30.7%) 241 (24.9%)
 Chicago 425 (10.8%) 267 (13.0%)
 Miami 460 (35.0%) 311 (40.2%)
 San Diego 486 (23.5%) 215 (21.9%)
Adherence to 2008 US Physical Activity Guidelinesa
 No 860 (51.2%) 333 (35.7%)
 Yes 952 (48.8%) 701 (64.3%)
Depressive Symptoms
 Mean CES-D 10b 8.9 (0.2) 7.1 (0.2)
Perceived Neighborhood Environment
 Quintiles (Q) Neighborhood Problems
  Q1 (Low; range: 0-7) 274 (17.5%) 143 (15.4%)
  Q2 (range: 8-9) 417 (25.0%) 206 (22.3%)
  Q3 (range: 10-11) 346 (17.8%) 195 (17.2%)
  Q4 (range: 12-14) 375 (19.9%) 251 (23.6%)
  Q5 (High, less favorable; range: 15-28) 400 (19.8%) 239 (21.4%)
 Quintiles Neighborhood Social Cohesion
  Q1 (Low; less favorable; range:0-13) 375 (20.3%) 238 (22.0%)
  Q2 (range: 14-15) 394 (20.7%) 216 (18.9%)
  Q3 (range: 16-17) 426 (24.0%) 221 (22.2%)
  Q4 (range: 18-19) 356 (18.5%) 186 (17.6%)
  Q5 (High; range: 20-25) 261 (16.6%) 173 (19.3%)
 Categories Safety from Crime
  Low 485 (22.6%) 270 (26.0%)
  Medium 970 (57.0%) 513 (46.4%)
  High 357 (20.4%) 251 (27.6%)
Cognitive Function
 Mean Global Cognition (z-score) 0.3 (0.1) −0.4 (0.1)
 Mean Verbal Learning 23.4 (0.3) 21.3 (0.2)
 Mean Memory 8.5 (0.1) 7.6 (0.1)
 Mean Verbal Fluency 18.6 (0.4) 18.5 (0.3)
 Mean Processing Speed 35.2 (0.6) 33.7 (0.5)
Low Mental Statusc
 SIS ≤4 213 (12.4%) 152 (13.9%)

Notes: Values in parentheses are standard errors. Sample size is unweighted and all other reported values are weighted to represent the target Hispanic/Latino population.

Q= Quintile; SIS= Six-Item Screener.

a

Physical activity levels were assessed using the Global Physical Activity Questionnaire (GPAP) and were categorized according to the 2008 US Guidelines for meeting high or medium activity levels.

b

Depressive symptoms were assessed using the 10-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D-10).

c

Low mental status was categorized based on as a score of 4 or below on the Six-Item Screener (SIS).

Associations between Perceived Neighborhood Environment and Cognitive Function

Women in the lowest quintile of perceived neighborhood problems (vs. highest quintile) had higher global cognition (β: 0.61, 95% CI: 0.16, 1.05), verbal learning (B: 1.28, 95% CI: 0.20, 2.36), and memory (B: 0.71, 95% CI: 0.22, 1.21) (Model 1; Table 2). After additional adjustment for depressive symptoms, the associations between the lowest quintile of perceived neighborhood problems with higher global cognition and memory persisted (Model 2: β: 0.48, 95% CI: 0.03, 0.94; and B: 0.60, 95% CI: 0.11, 1.09, respectively) but not with verbal learning. We also observed associations between the third quintile of perceived neighborhood problems (vs. highest quintile) and higher processing speed in Models 1 and 2 (Model 1: B: 2.40, 95% CI: 0.46, 4.33; and Model 2: B: 2.19, 95% CI: 0.24, 4.14, respectively). There was evidence of a linear trend in the associations of perceived neighborhood problems with verbal learning and memory in Model 1 (p-trend= 0.045 and 0.021, respectively) but not in any of the final models.

Table 2.

Associations between perceived neighborhood environment and cognitive function among women (n = 1,812)

Global Cognition Verbal Learning Memory Verbal Fluency Processing Speed
β (95% CI) B (95% CI) B (95% CI) B (95% CI) B (95% CI)
Neighborhood Problemsa
Model 1
Q1 (Low) 0.61 (0.16, 1.05)** 1.28 (0.20, 2.36)* 0.71 (0.22, 1.21)** 0.87 (−0.63, 2.38) 1.90 (−0.23, 4.02)
Q2 0.17 (−0.28, 0.62) 0.47 (−0.45, 1.39) 0.31 (−0.18, 0.80) −0.13 (−1.77, 1.50) 0.43 (−1.50, 2.36)
Q3 0.37 (−0.03, 0.77) 0.09 (−0.97, 1.16) 0.23 (−0.23, 0.70) 1.23 (0.00, 2.46)* 2.40 (0.46, 4.33)*
Q4 0.44 (−0.07, 0.94) 0.44 (−0.53, 1.41) 0.30 (−0.22, 0.81) 1.58 (−0.74, 3.94) 1.77 (−0.29, 3.83)
Q5 (High) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref)
p-trend 0.064 0.045 0.021 0.989 0.298
Model 2
Q1 (Low) 0.48 (0.03, 0.94)* 0.93 (−0.11, 1.96) 0.60 (0.11, 1.09)* 0.72 (−0.87, 2.31) 1.54 (−0.65, 3.73)
Q2 0.04 (−0.41, 0.48) 0.10 (−0.80, 0.99) 0.19 (−0.28, 0.65) −0.29 (−1.98, 1.40) 0.06 (−1.92, 2.04)
Q3 0.30 (−0.10, 0.69) −0.11 (−1.16, 0.93) 0.17 (−0.30, 0.64) 1.15 (−0.11, 2.40) 2.19 (0.24, 4.14)*
Q4 0.40 (−0.11, 0.92) 0.34 (−0.61, 1.29) 0.26 (−0.25, 0.78) 1.54 (−0.82, 3.90) 1.67 (−0.41, 3.75)
Q5 (High) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref)
p-trend 0.229 0.192 0.060 0.828 0.546
Neighborhood Social Cohesionb
Model 1
Q1 (Low) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref)
Q2 0.09 (−0.31, 0.48) 0.45 (−0.55, 1.45) 0.28 (−0.21, 0.77) −0.02 (−1.39, 1.35) −0.89 (−2.83, 1.04)
Q3 0.10 (−0.40, 0.59) −0.14 (−1.11, 0.83) 0.03 (−0.47, 0.52) 0.82 (−1.29, 2.94) 0.27 (−1.61, 2.16)
Q4 −0.30 (−0.74, 0.15) −0.08 (−0.98, 0.81) −0.04 (−0.61, 0.52) −1.42 (−2.87, 0.04) −1.77 (−3.74, 0.19)
Q5 (High) −0.44 (−0.90, 0.01) −0.71 (−1.68, 0.26) −0.07 (−0.63, 0.48) −1.85 (−3.65, −0.05)* −1.78 (−3.51, −0.04)*
p-trend 0.015 0.077 0.478 0.009 0.025
Model 2
Q1 (Low) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref)
Q2 0.00 (−0.39, 0.39) 0.22 (−0.77, 1.21) 0.21 (−0.28, 0.69) −0.13 (−1.53, 1.27) −1.15 (−3.06, 0.76)
Q3 0.03 (−0.47, 0.54) −0.32 (−1.27, 0.63) −0.03 (−0.53, 0.47) 0.74 (−1.44, 2.92) 0.08 (−1.84, 2.00)
Q4 −0.44 (−0.89, 0.01) −0.48 (−1.38, 0.41) −0.18 (−0.72, 0.37) −1.61 (−3.12, −0.11)* −2.21 (−4.22, −0.21)*
Q5 (High) −0.56 (−1.02, −0.09)* −1.01 (−2.00, −0.03)* −0.17 (−0.74, 0.39) −2.00 (−3.83, −0.16)* −2.11 (−3.87, −0.36)*
p-trend 0.004 0.015 0.273 0.006 0.009
Neighborhood Safety from Crimec
Model 1
Low 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref)
Medium 0.26 (−0.07, 0.58) 0.42 (−0.28, 1.11) 0.11 (−0.24, 0.47) 0.95 (−0.35, 2.24) 0.92 (−0.58, 2.43)
High 0.01 (−0.40, 0.42) 0.21 (−0.75, 1.17) 0.10 (−0.42, 0.61) −0.18 (−1.47, 1.12) −0.47 (−2.30, 1.35)
p-trend 0.965 0.663 0.713 0.790 0.612
Model 2
Low 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref) 0.00 (ref)
Medium 0.18 (−0.14, 0.51) 0.20 (−0.47, 0.86) 0.04 (−0.31, 0.39) 0.88 (−0.48, 2.24) 0.72 (−0.81, 2.24)
High −0.06 (−0.48, 0.36) 0.01 (−0.97, 0.98) 0.03 (−0.51, 0.57) −0.24 (−1.59, 1.11) −0.67 (−2.53, 1.20)
p-trend 0.779 0.989 0.923 0.726 0.483

Note: Sample size is unweighted and all other values are weighted. Q = Quintiles.

a

Perceived neighborhood problems’ score range according to quintile and sample size: Q1: 0-7 (n = 274); Q2: 8-9 (n = 417); Q3: 10-11 (n = 346); Q4: 12-14 (n = 375); Q5: 15-28 (n = 400).

b

Perceived neighborhood social cohesion’s score range according to quintile and sample size: Q1: 5.0-13.0 (n = 375); Q2: 14-15 (n = 394); Q3: 16-17 (n = 426); Q4: 18-19 (n = 356); Q5: 19-25 (n = 261).

c

Perceived neighborhood safety categories’ sample size: Low: 483; Medium: 964; High: 355.

Model 1: Age, Hispanic/Latino background, language preference, field site, years living in neighborhood, annual household income, and education.

Model 2: Model 1 + depressive symptoms.

*

p<0.05

**

p<0.01

***

p<0.001

Women in the highest quintile of perceived neighborhood social cohesion (vs. lowest quintile) had lower verbal fluency (B: −1.85, 95% CI: −3.65, −0.05) and processing speed (B: −1.78, 95% CI: −3.51, −0.04) (Mode1; Table 2). After additional adjustment for depressive symptoms, the association of the highest quintile of perceived neighborhood social cohesion with lower verbal fluency (B: −2.00, 95% CI: −3.83, −0.16) and processing speed (B: −2.11, 95% CI: −3.87, −0.36) remained significant; and, the highest quintile of perceived neighborhood social cohesion became significantly associated with lower global cognition (β: −0.56, 95% CI: −1.02, −0.09) and verbal learning (B: −1.01, 95% CI: −2.00, −0.03). In Model 2, there was evidence of a linear trend in the aforementioned associations of perceived neighborhood social cohesion with global cognition, verbal learning, verbal fluency, and processing speed among women (p-trend= 0.004, 0.015, 0.006, and 0.009, respectively). Finally, there were no associations between perceived neighborhood safety from crime and cognitive function among women. Among men, we observed no associations between any of the perceived neighborhood environment measures and cognitive function regardless of adjustments (Table 3).

Table 3.

Associations between perceived neighborhood environment and cognitive function among men (n = 1,034)

Global Cognition Verbal Learning Memory Verbal Fluency Processing Speed
β (95% CI) B (95% CI) B (95% CI) B (95% CI) B (95% CI)
Neighborhood Problemsb
Model 1
Q1 (Low) 0.07 (−0.57, 0.71) 0.34 (−1.27, 1.95) 0.07 (−0.63, 0.77) 0.18 (−1.49, 1.85) −0.33 (−3.39, 2.74)
Q2 0.17 (−0.35, 0.69) −0.06 (−1.43, 1.32) 0.37 (−0.18, 0.92) 0.18 (−1.11, 1.47) 0.85 (−1.78, 3.47)
Q3 −0.08 (−0.61, 0.46) −0.15 (−1.49, 1.19) −0.02 (−0.63, 0.60) −0.27 (−1.77, 1.24) −0.30 (−2.68, 2.07)
Q4 0.30 (−0.16, 0.76) 0.85 (−0.31, 2.00) 0.23 (−0.35, 0.81) 0.01 (−1.35, 1.37) 1.76 (−0.50, 4.03)
Q5 (High) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref)
p-trend 0.992 0.901 0.726 0.763 0.636
Model 2
Q1 (Low) −0.18 (−0.83, 0.46) −0.21 (−1.85, 1.43) −0.19 (−0.86, 0.49) −0.16 (−1.86, 1.66) −1.31 (−4.41, 1.80)
Q2 −0.06 (−0.58, 0.46) −0.56 (−1.91, 0.79) 0.14 (−0.42, 0.69) −0.13 (−1.45, 1.18) −0.05 (−2.77, 2.66)
Q3 −0.25 (−0.76, 0.26) −0.53 (−1.86, 0.80) −0.19 (−0.81, 0.42) −0.50 (−1.95, 1.03) −0.98 (−3.39, 1.43)
Q4 0.18 (−0.26, 0.62) 0.57 (−0.55, 1.69) 0.11 (−0.46, 0.68) −0.16 (−1.53, 1.15) 1.27 (−0.98, 3.53)
Q5 (High) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref)
p-trend 0.398 0.390 0.650 0.882 0.249
Neighborhood Social Cohesion
Model 1
Q1 (Low) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref)
Q2 0.01 (−0.44, 0.46) 0.32 (−0.72, 1.37) 0.02 (−0.51, 0.55) −0.13 (−1.40, 1.15) −0.43 (−2.89, 2.02)
Q3 0.13 (−0.32, 0.58) 0.95 (−0.24, 2.13) 0.02 (−0.53, 0.58) 0.02 (−1.22, 1.27) −0.22 (−2.52, 2.07)
Q4 −0.09 (−0.56, 0.39) −0.02 (−1.07, 1.03) −0.39 (−1.04, 0.26) 0.38 (−0.98, 1.74) −0.25 (−3.00, 2.49)
Q5 (High) −0.15 (−0.72, 0.42) 0.24 (−1.17, 1.65) 0.16 (−0.77, 0.45) −0.55 (−2.12, 1.01) −1.26 (−3.96, 1.43)
p-trend 0.528 0.928 0.323 0.732 0.469
Model 2
Q1 (Low) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref)
Q2 −0.07 (−0.50, 0.36) 0.16 (−0.85, 1.17) −0.07 (−0.59, 0.55) −0.24 (−1.49, 1.02) −0.75 (−3.21, 1.71)
Q3 0.03 (−0.41, 0.48) 0.75 (−0.42, 1.91) −0.09 (−0.64, 0.47) −0.11 (−1.34, 1.12) −0.60 (−2.97, 1.77)
Q4 −0.23 (−0.70, 0.24) −0.30 (−1.34, 0.74) −0.54 (−1.19, 0.10) 0.19 (−1.18, 1.56) −0.79 (−3.59, 2.00)
Q5 (High) −0.28 (−0.88, 0.32) −0.02 (−1.49, 1.55) −0.30 (−0.91, 0.32) −0.72 (−2.36, 0.92) −1.75 (−4.54, 1.04)
p-trend 0.289 0.762 0.152 0.580 0.289
Neighborhood Safety from Crime
Model 1
Low (Worst) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref)
Medium 0.09 (−0.32, 0.49) 0.30 (−0.77, 1.36) −0.08 (−0.55, 0.39) 0.28 (−0.87, 1.44) 0.57 (−1.17, 2.30)
High 0.11 (−0.41, 0.63) 0.42 (−0.96, 1.79) −0.15 (−0.70, 0.39) −0.24 (−1.79, 1.32) 1.90 (−0.32, 4.12)
p-trend 0.679 0.552 0.577 0.767 0.094
Model 2
Low (Worst) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref) 0.00 (Ref)
Medium 0.01 (−0.37, 0.40) 0.15 (−0.89, 1.18) −0.16 (−0.62, 0.30) 0.18 (−0.95, 1.31) 0.31 (−1.37, 2.00)
High 0.00 (−0.51, 0.51) 0.20 (−1.17, 1.56) −0.27 (−0.82, 0.27) −0.39 (−1.92, 1.15) 1.53 (−0.75, 3.81)
p-trend 0.991 0.777 0.324 0.621 0.189

Note: Sample size is unweighted and all other values are weighted. Q = Quintiles.

a

Perceived neighborhood problems’ score range according to quintile and sample size: Q1: 0-7 (n = 143); Q2: 8-9 (n = 205); Q3: 10-11 (n = 194); Q4: 12-14 (n = 249); Q5: 15-28 (n = 238).

b

Perceived neighborhood social cohesion’s score range according to quintile and sample size: Q1: 5.0-13.0 (n = 236); Q2: 14-15 (n = 216); Q3: 16-17 (n = 221); Q4: 18-19 (n = 185); Q5: 19-25 (n = 171).

c

Perceived neighborhood safety categories’ sample size: Low: 268; Medium: 511; High: 250.

Model 1: Age, Hispanic/Latino background, language preference, field site, years living in neighborhood, annual household income, and education.

Model 2: Model 1 + depressive symptoms.

*

p<0.05

**

p<0.01

***

p<0.001

DISCUSSION

To our knowledge, this is the most comprehensive examination of associations between perceived neighborhood environment and cognitive function among middle-aged and older Hispanic/Latino women and men. Our results revealed that women living in neighborhoods with the lowest perceived problems (versus highest) had higher global cognition and memory, regardless of sociodemographic characteristics and depressive symptoms. Unexpectedly, we also found that women living in neighborhoods with the highest perceived social cohesion (versus lowest) had lower global cognition, verbal learning, verbal fluency, and processing speed. No associations were observed of perceived neighborhood environment and cognitive function among men. Our findings support results from previous research [16-23] on the associations between perceived neighborhood environment and health outcomes among women but not men, and extend these results to include cognitive function as an additional health outcome of interest. The present study also highlights the complexity of these relationships when considering socio-demographic, behavioral, and psychological variables particularly among Hispanic/Latina women.

Among women, as hypothesized, we found that the most favorable level of perceived neighborhood problems (first quintile) was associated with better global cognitive function and memory in the fully adjusted model. Overall, such finding is consistent with theories positing that adverse neighborhood conditions can act as psychosocial stressors that are associated with worst mental health outcomes [43]. It is consistent with a study reporting an association between lower perceived neighborhood disorder and lower cognitive decline over time in older African-American adults [12] and a study reporting that living in more advantaged neighborhoods (based on objective assessments) is associated with better cognitive function in older non-Hispanic white women [44]. There were no significant linear trends in the neighborhood problems-cognition associations. Most significant associations were only present when comparing the highest to the lowest quintiles, which may be partly due to low variability across neighborhoods since communities with a high proportion of Hispanics/Latinos were oversampled in HCHS/SOL [46].

Surprisingly, among women, the most favorable level of perceived neighborhood social cohesion (i.e., fifth quintile) was associated lower global cognition, verbal learning, verbal fluency, and processing speed scores. There was evidence of significant linear trends in the neighborhood social cohesion-cognition associations, where cognitive function scores decreased as social cohesion was higher. This unexpected direction may simply suggest that women with better cognitive function have greater capacity to comprehensively assess and develop nuanced understandings of the quality of social relations in their neighborhood. Alternatively, it may be that neighborhood social cohesion may not promote collective resources and actions but rather serve as a stressor in neighborhoods of lower socioeconomic status [45, 46]. In fact, inconsistent findings have been reported across previous studies [47] with a study reporting an association between better perceived neighborhood social climate and better cognitive function [11] but other studies [13, 14] reporting no associations. These inconsistent findings across studies may be due, in part, to the cross-sectional design of the available studies, cross-cultural differences in the samples used, and/or differences in the measures employed to assess the neighborhood environment. Longitudinal research on the associations between perceived neighborhood problems and social cohesion with changes in cognitive function particularly among women is warranted.

Overall, the null associations in men but not women suggest that perceptions of one’s neighborhood environment may play a greater role in shaping cognitive function among women compared to men; a possibility that may be related to gender differences in neighborhood-based activities and/or social relationships. For example, compared to men, women tend to report larger social network size [48-50] and seek more social support outside of their family [49, 50] which may result in better quality of social relations with their neighbors. Alternatively, a potential explanation for the largely null findings observed among men is that they had lower cognitive function scores (compared to women) which could contribute to lower ability to ascertain and report nuances in their neighborhood environment. However, it is important to note that although we describe some statistically significant associations present for women and not men, post hoc analyses showed that only the association between perceived neighborhood social cohesion and processing speed had a significant interaction term (p-interaction= 0.038) in the fully adjusted model. Future research with a larger sample size is needed to confirm such finding.

Additionally, the lack of associations between perceived neighborhood safety from crime and cognitive function in women and men were may reflect that these associations operate through multiple indirect pathways rather than directly as examined in the current study. For example, higher perceived neighborhood safety from crime has been associated with higher levels of neighborhood-based walking [51] which in turn is associated with increased opportunities for social engagement with neighbors [51], an important protective factor for cognitive function [52]. In the present study, however, there was no evidence of bivariate associations between perceived neighborhood safety from crime and adherence to the 2008 U.S. physical activity guidelines in either women or men. More work is needed investigating the complex interplay of perceived neighborhood safety from crime and associated healthy behaviors that may relate to the cognitive function of middle-aged and older Hispanics/Latinos.

This study has several limitations that need to be considered when interpreting our results, particularly the cross-sectional nature of our study. Not only do findings from this cross-sectional study require additional confirmation, longitudinal research is also needed to examine direction and mediators of these associations [3]. While empirical studies have demonstrated that adverse neighborhood environments (assessed using objective measures) are associated with faster rates of cognitive decline [53, 54], there is a need for research on the associations between perceived neighborhood environment and cognitive decline. Such focus on cognitive trajectories will help to address some of the limitations of this cross-sectional study. Moreover, future studies are needed to examine the role of early life neighborhood context on cognitive function by applying a lifecourse approach [55]. Further, to date, it remains unknown whether depressive symptoms are a mediator of the associations between perceived neighborhood environment and cognitive function (as opposed to being a confounder). Thus, longitudinal research examining depressive symptoms as a mediator of the neighborhood-cognition associations is warranted [56]. Interestingly, we found strong bivariate associations between worse perceptions of the neighborhood environment and higher depressive symptoms among women and men, which will be relevant to consider in future research. Although consideration of physical activity may be important in the associations examined in the current study (because it could be either a confounder or mediator of the associations), adding physical activity to our final model did not change our findings. Potential reasons for this unexpected finding is the lack of bivariate association between neighborhood activity and physical activity in our study and that physical activity (objectively assessed) is not associated with cognitive function in our sample [57]. Another limitation of our study is that perceived neighborhood safety from crime was assessed using a single question, while it has been shown to have statistical validity [39], this limitation may nonetheless have affected our findings. While response bias [58] is another potential limitation of our study, e.g., participants with worse cognitive function may report poorer rating of their neighborhood, the combined relatively young age and low prevalence of lower mental status in our cohort may lessen this possibility. Finally, Hispanics/Latinos living in suburban or rural areas were not included in the HCHS/SOL study and our cohort is not representative of those populations. Nevertheless, most of the U.S. Hispanic/Latino population lives in urban areas [59] (including those sampled in the HCHS/SOL). An important strength of this study is that we included a diverse cohort of middle age and older U.S. Hispanics/Latinos and we highlight the importance of exploring gender differences in the associations between perceived neighborhood features and cognitive function. Additionally, we employed well-recognized and validated measures to assess various perceived neighborhood environmental features, and validated tests of cognitive function (both as individual test items and as a measure of global cognitive function). Future studies should examine whether Hispanic/Latino background moderates the neighborhood–cognition associations and whether objective neighborhood environment measures (e.g., green spaces, crime rates, and sociocultural environment) are associated with cognitive function among Hispanics/Latinos. We were able to adjust our models by duration of exposure to the neighborhood environment (approximately 9 years on average) which is important to consider when investigating relationships between neighborhood environments and individual health.

CONCLUSION

This study provides cross-sectional evidence suggesting that lower levels of perceived neighborhood problems (as a proxy for neighborhood-derived psychosocial stressors) is associated with higher global cognitive function and memory scores among middle-aged and older Hispanic/Latina women but not among men. Future research should include longitudinal measures and consider mechanistic pathways to aim to better understand the role of perceived neighborhood environment on cognitive function among middle-aged and older Hispanics/Latinos. A better understanding of these associations is warranted given the need to identify public health approaches that may aid in the promotion of cognitive health among Hispanics/Latinos, an understudied yet rapidly growing segment of the U.S. population [25] at increased risk for developing early onset of cognitive impairment [15].

Supplementary Material

127_2019_1829_MOESM1_ESM

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors would like to thank the HCHS/SOL and its SCAS participants as well as the staff for their commitment to this study. Mayra L. Estrella was partially supported by the NIH- National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (NHLBI) T32-HL125294 and by a Diversity Supplement from the NIH-NHLBI for HCHS/SOL Chicago Field Center (75N92019D00012). Dr. Hector González was supported by the NIH- National Aging Institute (5R01AG048642-04). The HCHS/SOL was carried out as a collaborative study supported by contracts from the NHLBI to the University of North Carolina (N01-HC65233), University of Miami (N01-HC65234), Albert Einstein College of Medicine (N01-HC65235), University of Illinois at Chicago (HHSN268201300003I), Northwestern University (N01-HC65236), and San Diego State University (N01-HC65237). The following Institutes/Centers/Offices contribute to the HCHS/SOL through a transfer of funds to the NHLBI: National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities, National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders, National Institute of Dental and Craniofacial Research, National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, NIH Institution Office of Dietary Supplements. The views expressed in this manuscript are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute; the National Institutes of Health; or the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.

Footnotes

CONFLICT OF INTEREST STATEMENT

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Hill C V, Pérez-Stable EJ, Anderson NA, Bernard MA (2015) The National Institute on Aging Health Disparities Research Framework. Ethn Dis 25:245–54. 10.18865/ed.25.3.245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Glymour MM, Manly JJ (2008) Lifecourse social conditions and racial and ethnic patterns of cognitive aging. Neuropsychol Rev 18:223–254. 10.1007/s11065-008-9064-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Wu Y-T, Prina AM, Brayne C (2015) The association between community environment and cognitive function: A systematic review. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 50:351–362. 10.1007/s00127-014-0945-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Besser LM, McDonald NC, Song Y, et al. (2017) Neighborhood environment and cognition in older adults: A systematic review. Am J Prev Med 53:241–251. 10.1016/j.amepre.2017.02.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Aneshensel CS, Ko MJ, Chodosh J, Wight RG (2011) The urban neighborhood and cognitive functioning in late middle age. J Health Soc Behav 52:163–179. 10.1177/0022146510393974 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Gorelick PB, Furie KL, Iadecola C, et al. (2017) Defining optimal brain health in adults: a Presidential Advisory from the American Heart Association/American Stroke Association. Stroke 48:e00–e00. 10.1161/STR.0000000000000148 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Kit BK, Flegal KM (2014) Prevalence of Childhood and Adult Obesity in the United States, 2011-2012. JAMA 311:806 10.1001/jama.2014.732 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Townley G, Kloos B (2014) Mind Over Matter? The Role of Individual Perceptions in Understanding the Social Ecology of Housing Environments for Individuals with Psychiatric Disabilities. Am J Community Psychol 54:205–218. 10.1007/s10464-014-9664-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Koohsari MJ, Badland H, Sugiyama T, et al. (2015) Mismatch between Perceived and Objectively Measured Land Use Mix and Street Connectivity: Associations with Neighborhood Walking. J Urban Heal 92:242–252. 10.1007/s11524-014-9928-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Ahuja C, Ayers C, Hartz J, et al. (2018) Examining relationships between perceptions and objective assessments of neighborhood environment and sedentary time: Data from the Washington, D.C. Cardiovascular Health and Needs Assessment. Prev Med reports 9:42–48. 10.1016/j.pmedr.2017.12.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Brown SC, Mason CCA, Spokane AR, et al. (2009) The relationship of neighborhood climate to perceived social support and mental health in older Hispanic immigrants in Miami, Florida. Aging Heal 21:431–459. 10.1177/0898264308328976 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Boardman J, Barnes L, Wilson R, Evans D (2012) Social disorder, APOE-E4 genotype, and change in cognitive function among older adults living in Chicago. Soc Sci [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Magaziner J, Cadigan DA (1989) Community resources and mental health of older women living alone. J Aging Health 1:35–49. 10.1177/089826438900100103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Murayama H, Nishi M, Matsuo E, et al. (2013) Do bonding and bridging social capital affect self-rated health, depressive mood and cognitive decline in older Japanese? A prospective cohort study. Soc Sci Med 98:247–252. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.09.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Alzheimer’s Association (2017) 2017 Alzheimer’s Disease Facts and Figures. Alzheimer’s Dement 13:325–373. 10.1016/j.jalz.2017.02.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Strong LL, Reitzel LR, Wetter DW, McNeill LH (2013) Associations of perceived neighborhood physical and social environments with physical activity and television viewing in African-American men and women. Am J Health Promot 27:401–9. 10.4278/ajhp.120306-QUAN-127 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Kim D, Diez Roux A V., Kiefe CI, et al. (2010) Do Neighborhood Socioeconomic Deprivation and Low Social Cohesion Predict Coronary Calcification?: The CARDIA Study. Am J Epidemiol 172:288–298. 10.1093/aje/kwq098 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Mair C, Diez Roux A V., Shen M, et al. (2009) Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Associations of Neighborhood Cohesion and Stressors with Depressive Symptoms in the Multiethnic Study of Atherosclerosis. Ann Epidemiol 19:49–57. 10.1016/j.annepidem.2008.10.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Mair C, Diez Roux A V., Morenoff JD (2010) Neighborhood stressors and social support as predictors of depressive symptoms in the Chicago Community Adult Health Study. Health Place 16:811–819. 10.1016/J.HEALTHPLACE.2010.04.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Choi NG, Kim J, DiNitto DM, Marti CN (2015) Perceived Social Cohesion, Frequency of Going Out, and Depressive Symptoms in Older Adults: Examination of Longitudinal Relationships. Gerontol Geriatr Med 1:2333721415615478. 10.1177/2333721415615478 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Gebreab SY, Riestra P, Gaye A, et al. (2016) Perceived neighborhood problems are associated with shorter telomere length in African American women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 69:90–97. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.03.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Suminski RR, Poston WSC, Petosa RL, et al. (2005) Features of the neighborhood environment and walking by U.S. adults. Am J Prev Med 28:149–55. 10.1016/j.amepre.2004.09.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Barber S, Hickson DA, Wang X, et al. (2016) Neighborhood Disadvantage, Poor Social Conditions, and Cardiovascular Disease Incidence Among African American Adults in the Jackson Heart Study. Am J Public Health 106:2219–2226. 10.2105/AJPH.2016.303471 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.González HM, Tarraf W, Gouskova N, et al. (2015) Neurocognitive function among middle-aged and older Hispanic/Latinos: results from the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 30:68–77. 10.1093/arclin/acu066 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Passel JCD (2008) U.S. Population Projections: 2005-2050. Washington, DC [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Sorlie PD, Avilés-Santa LM, Wassertheil-Smoller S, et al. (2010) Design and implementation of the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos. Ann Epidemiol 20:629–641. 10.1016/j.annepidem.2010.03.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.LaVange LM, Kalsbeek WD, Sorlie PD, et al. (2010) Sample design and cohort selection in the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos. Ann Epidemiol 20:642–649. 10.1016/j.annepidem.2010.05.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Gallo LC, Penedo FJ, Carnethon M, et al. (2014) The Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos Sociocultural Ancillary Study: sample, design, and procedures. Ethn Dis 24:77–83 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Gallo LC, Penedo FJ, Espinosa de los Monteros K, Arguelles W (2009) Resiliency in the Face of Disadvantage: Do Hispanic Cultural Characteristics Protect Health Outcomes? J Pers 77:1707–1746. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00598.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Myers HF (2009) Ethnicity- and socio-economic status-related stresses in context: an integrative review and conceptual model. J Behav Med 32:9–19. 10.1007/s10865-008-9181-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Deckers K, Schievink SHJ, Rodriquez MMF, et al. (2017) Coronary heart disease and risk for cognitive impairment or dementia: Systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One 12:e0184244 10.1371/journal.pone.0184244 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Levine DA, Wadley VG, Langa KM, et al. (2018) Risk Factors for Poststroke Cognitive Decline: The REGARDS Study (Reasons for Geographic and Racial Differences in Stroke). Stroke 49:987–994. 10.1161/STROKEAHA.117.018529 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.González HM, Mungas D, Reed BR, et al. (2001) A new verbal learning and memory test for English- and Spanish-speaking older people. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 7:544–55 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.González HM, Mungas D, Haan MN (2002) A verbal learning and memory test for English- and Spanish-speaking older Mexican-American adults. Clin Neuropsychol (Neuropsychology, Dev Cogn Sect D) 16:439–451. 10.1076/clin.16.4.439.13908 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Lezak M, Howieson DBLD (2004) Neuropsychological Assessment. NY [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Benton AL HK (1989) Multilingual Aphasia Examination, 2nd ed. Iowa City [Google Scholar]
  • 37.D W (1981) WAIS-R Manual. San Antonio [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Mujahid MS, Diez Roux A V., Morenoff JD, Raghunathan T (2007) Assessing the measurement properties of neighborhood scales: From psychometrics to ecometrics. Am J Epidemiol 165:858–67. 10.1093/aje/kwm040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Echeverria SE, Diez-Roux A V, Link BG (2004) Reliability of Self-Reported Neighborhood Characteristics. J Urban Heal Bull New York Acad Med 81:682–701. 10.1093/jurban/jth151 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Sampson RJ (1997) Neighborhoods and violent crime: A Multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science (80- ) 277:918–924. 10.1126/science.277.5328.918 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Armstrong T, Bull F (2006) Development of the World Health Organization Global Physical Activity Questionnaire (GPAQ). J Public Health (Bangkok) 14:66–70. 10.1007/s10389-006-0024-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Andresen EM, Malmgren JA, Carter WB, Patrick DL (1993) Screening for depression in well older adults: evaluation of a short form of the CES-D (Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale). Am J Prev Med 10:77–84. 10.4236/health.2013.53a078 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Cutrona CE, Wallace G, Wesner KA (2006) Neighborhood Characteristics and Depression. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 15:188–192. 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00433.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Shih RA, Ghosh-Dastidar B, Margolis KL, et al. (2011) Neighborhood Socioeconomic Status and Cognitive Function in Women. Am J Public Health 101:1721–1728. 10.2105/AJPH.2011.300169 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Echeverría S, Diez-Roux A V, Shea S, et al. (2008) Associations of neighborhood problems and neighborhood social cohesion with mental health and health behaviors: the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis. Health Place 14:853–65. 10.1016/j.healthplace.2008.01.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Berkman L, Glass T (2000) Social integration, social networks, social support, and health In: Berkman LF, Kawachi I (eds) Social Epidemiology. Oxford University Press, New York, NY, pp 137–173 [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Kuiper JS, Zuidersma M, Zuidema SU, et al. (2016) Social relationships and cognitive decline: a systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Int J Epidemiol 45:dyw089 10.1093/ije/dyw089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.McLaughlin D, Vagenas D, Pachana NA, et al. (2010) Gender Differences in Social Network Size and Satisfaction in Adults in Their 70s. J Health Psychol 15:671–679. 10.1177/1359105310368177 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Antonucci TC, Akiyama H (1987) An examination of sex differences in social support among older men and women. Sex Roles 17:737–749. 10.1007/BF00287685 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Cornwell B, Laumann EO, Schumm LP (2008) The Social Connectedness of Older Adults: A National Profile. Am Sociol Rev 73:185–203 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Stafford M, Chandola T, Marmot M (2007) Association between fear of crime and mental health and physical functioning. Am J Public Health 97:2076–81. 10.2105/AJPH.2006.097154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Krueger KR, Wilson RS, Kamenetsky JM, et al. (2009) Social Engagement and Cognitive Function in Old Age. Exp Aging Res 35:45–60. 10.1080/03610730802545028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Clarke PJ, Weuve J, Barnes L, et al. (2015) Cognitive decline and the neighborhood environment. Ann Epidemiol 25:849–54. 10.1016/j.annepidem.2015.07.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Kovalchik SA, Slaughter ME, Miles J, et al. (2015) Neighbourhood racial/ethnic composition and segregation and trajectories of cognitive decline among US older adults. J Epidemiol Community Health 69:978–984. 10.1136/jech-2015-205600 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Liu S, Jones RN, Glymour MM (2010) Implications of lifecourse epidemiology for research on determinants of adult disease. Public Health Rev. 32:489–511 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Blair A, Ross NA, Gariepy G, Schmitz N (2014) How do neighborhoods affect depression outcomes? A realist review and a call for the examination of causal pathways. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 49:873–887. 10.1007/s00127-013-0810-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Vásquez E, Strizich G, Isasi CR, et al. (2017) Is there a relationship between accelerometer-assessed physical activity and sedentary behavior and cognitive function in US Hispanic/Latino adults? The Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos (HCHS/SOL). Prev Med (Baltim) 103:43–48. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2017.07.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Macleod J, Davey Smith G, Heslop P, et al. (2002) Psychological stress and cardiovascular disease: empirical demonstration of bias in a prospective observational study of Scottish men. BMJ 324:1247–51. 10.1136/bmj.324.7348.1247 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Krogstad JM (2016) Key facts about how the U.S. Hispanic population is changing [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

127_2019_1829_MOESM1_ESM

RESOURCES