1. Introduction
Research in pain management seeks to offer new approaches to pain treatments that present severe side effects. This endeavor is of increasing importance as opioid misuse and deaths in the United States rise,66 and as health care practitioners and patients are advised to move away from opioid-based pain management.
Virtual reality (VR) and music therapy (MT) have been separately explored as interventions for alleviating pain with relatively consistent levels of success.9,69,85 In this article, we refer to VR as immersive computer-generated environments designed to make a user experience them as real. Music therapy refers to the use of music to promote healing.9 An approach to pain management that combines MT and VR may present novel opportunities for reducing patient pain suffering by presenting a focused aesthetic multisensory stimulation. This might in turn regulate mood, emotions, attention demands, memory, and patients’ engagement. Although many nonpharmacological methods of pain control have been explored (eg, hypnosis and meditation), we have chosen to focus on VR and MT as 2 highly compatible methods, with the intent of addressing our concepts and perspectives as basic scientists, musicians, physicians, and educators.
We conducted a PubMed literature search using the terms: VR and pain; MT and pain. We found 288 and 243 articles, respectively, and reviewed them (E.H. and L.C.). Manually, we also found 37 additional published data-based articles and comprehensive reviews. We presented a total of 53 selected data-based articles (Table 1). Studies were selected that directly investigated the relationship between music or virtual reality and pain in both healthy and pain-afflicted populations using objective and subjective measures of pain as the primary outcome. Studies that assessed multiple interventions beyond virtual reality or music were excluded.
Table 1.
Reference | Intervention | Participants | Sample size |
Study design | Control group(s) |
Measurement type* |
Pain | Measurement outcomes |
Effect size for Pain intensity outcome (Hedges’ g)† |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Ambron et al. (2018)1 | VR | Phantom limb pain patients | n = 2 | Case study | n/a | Subjective | Intensity | Numerical pain scale (NRS) | Missing data |
Carrougher et al. (2009)5 | VR | Burn injuries patients | n = 39 | Within-subjects | One session w/VR, one session w/out VR (order counterbalanced) | Subjective | Intensity (worst pain), time spent thinking about pain, unpleasantness, and opioid equivalents | Graphic rating scale (GRS) | GRS, worst pain: 0.536 Opioid equivalents: −0.171 |
Demeter et al. (2015)11 | VR | Healthy participants | n = 62 | Within-subjects | Baseline, tonic noxious heat stimulation only | Subjective and objective | Intensity and threshold | NPS, latency of intolerability, temperature at which pain was first perceived, and thermal sensory analyzer (TSA) | NPS heat intensity: −0.671 |
Ford et al. (2018)15 | VR | Burn injury patients | n = 10 | Observational study | n/a | Subjective | Intensity | Satisfaction questionnaire (no pain ratings) | Missing data/ |
Gutierrez-Maldonado et al. (2012)25 | VR | Healthy participants | n = 45 | Within-subjects | No VR | Subjective | Threshold, tolerance, intensity, self-efficacy, and catastrophizing | Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS) and Visual Analogue Scale (VAS) | Strongest pain intensity: −0.126 |
Hoffman et al. (2000)27 | VR | Burn injury patients | n = 2 | Within-subjects | Videogame only during wound care | Subjective | Intensity | VAS | Missing data |
Hoffman et al. (2000)28 | VR | Burn injury patients | n = 12 | Within-subjects | Control condition: no distraction during physical therapy | Subjective | Intensity | VAS | Average pain: −1.12 Worst pain: −1.11 |
Hoffman et al. (2004)30 | VR | Healthy participants | n = 8 | Within-subjects | No VR during thermal stimuli | Subjective and objective | Intensity (time spent thinking about pain, unpleasantness, worst pain, amount of fun, amount of nausea, and presence) | GRS and fMRI | Missing data |
Hoffman et al. (2004)33 | VR | Healthy participants | n = 39 | Between-subjects | Low-tech VR | Subjective | Cognitive, sensory, and affective components of pain | Rankings from 0-10 | −1.49 |
Hoffman et al. (2006)32 | VR | Healthy participants | n = 77 | Between-subjects | No distraction | Subjective | Cognitive, affective, and sensory components of pain | Rankings from 0-10 | Missing data |
Hoffman et al. (2007)31 | VR | Healthy participants | n = 9 | Within-subjects | No analgesia, opioids | Subjective, objective | Intensity | GRS, blood oxygen level–dependent assessments of brain activity (fMRI) | Worst pain intensity VR–/opioid– vs VR+/opioid–: –1.32 VR–/opioid– vs VR–/opioid+: −0.367 VR–/opioid– vs VR+/opioid+: −2.46 |
Hoffman et al. (2008)29 | VR | Burn injury patients | n = 11 | Within-subjects | No VR during wound care | Subjective | Worst pain, pain unpleasantness, time spent thinking about pain, and fun | GRS | Worst pain: −1.05 Unpleasant: −1.09 |
Jeffs et al. (2014)37 | VR | Burn injury patients | n = 30 | Between-subjects | Passive distraction and standard care | Subjective | Intensity | Adolescent pediatric pain tool | Missing data |
Jin et al. (2016)39 | VR | Chronic pain patients | n = 20 | Within-subjects | Self-mediated session (regular distracting activities) | Subjective | Intensity | VAS | −0.96 |
Johnson et al. (2016)40 | VR | Healthy participants | n = 32 | Within-subjects | Baseline (no sound, no VR), sound only | Objective | Tolerance | Pain tolerance times | Sound only vs baseline: 0.293†† Head mounted device (HMD) only vs baseline: 1.01†† HMD + sound vs baseline: 1.01†† HMD + sound vs sound only: 0.941†† HMD + sound vs HMD only: 0.430†† |
Jones et al. (2016)41 | VR | Chronic pain patients | n = 30 | Within-subjects | n/a (pre–post pain scores) | Subjective | Intensity | NRS | −0.741 |
Kipping et al. (2012)44 | VR | Burn injuries patients | n = 41 | Between-subjects | Distraction | Subjective, objective | Intensity | VAS, heart rate (HR), oxygen saturation (OS) | Subject VAS, dressing removal: −0.465 Subject VAS, dressing application: −0.420 |
Loreto-Quijada et al. (2014)50 | VR | Healthy participants | n = 77 | Between-subjects | No VR | Subjective, objective | Intensity, tolerance, threshold, time perception, and pain sensitivity range | VAS, PCS | Missing data |
Maani et al. (2011)52 | VR | Burn injury patients | n = 2 | Case study | n/a | Subjective | Intensity/unpleasantness | GRS | Missing data |
Maani et al. (2011)53 | VR | Burn injury patients | n = 12 | Within-subjects | No distraction during wound care | Subjective | Intensity/unpleasantness | GRS | Worst pain ≥7: Worst pain: −1.331‡‡ Unpleasantness: −2.13‡‡ Time thinking about pain: −2.44‡‡ Worst pain <7: Worst pain: −0.40‡‡ Unpleasantness: −0.75‡‡ Time thinking about pain: −1.79‡‡ |
Sano et al. (2015)73 | VR | Phantom limb pain patients | n = 6 | Within-subjects | n/a | Subjective | Pain total score (sensory and affective components) | Short Form McGill Pain Questionnaire | −0.230 |
Schmitt et al. (2011)75 | VR | Burn injury patients | n = 54 | Within-subjects | No VR | Subjective | Sensory, affective, and cognitive components | GRS | −0.516 |
Smith et al. (2017)80 | VR | Healthy participants | n = 25 | Within-subjects | Neutral, pleasant, threatening, socially positive, and socially negative contexts | Subjective, objective | Intensity (threshold) | Pressure pain thresholds (PPTs) and PCS | Missing data |
Sulea et al. (2014)81 | VR | Healthy participants | n = 6 | Within-subjects | No VR | Subjective | Intensity | “Perceived level of pain on a 0-10 scale” | Missing data |
Wiederhold et al. (2014)86 | VR | Chronic pain patients | n = 40 | Within-subjects | Pain focus condition | Subjective and objective | Intensity | HR, skin temperature, “self-report questionnaires… on a scale of 1 to 7” | Mean pain rating: −0.848 (pilot cohort) |
Bradt et al. (2015)4 | Music therapy (MT), music medicine (MM) | Cancer pain patients | n = 31 | Between-subjects | n/a | Subjective | Intensity | NRS | MT, pre-therapy to post-therapy: −0.366 MM, pre-therapy to post-therapy: −0.450 MT vs MM: 0 |
Colwell et al. (2013)10 | MT, MM | Hospitalized pain children | n = 32 | Between-subjects | n/a | Subjective, objective | Pain/anxiety | HR, blood pressure (BP), OS, Wong-Baker FACES Pain Rating Scale | Pain rating score, post-treatment: Music listening vs music composition: 0.21 Music listening vs Orff-based: 0 Music composition vs Orff-based: −0.18 |
Mondanaro et al. (2017)63 | MT | Postspine surgery pain patients | n = 60 | Between-subjects | Standard care | Subjective | Intensity | VAS | VAS, MT preintervention vs postintervention: −0.44 VAS, after intervention: −0.31 |
Bradt et al. (2016)3 | MT | Chronic pain patients | n = 55 | Between-subjects | Waitlist (no music received at time of study) | Subjective | Intensity, interference, and self-efficacy | Westhaven-Yale Multidimensional Pain Inventory (MPI), Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire (PSEQ), NRS | MPI: −0.08, −0.24‡ NRS: −0.61, −0.27‡ |
Gutgsell et al. (2013)24 | MT | Hospital inpatients | n = 200 | Between-subjects | Standard care alone | Subjective | Intensity | NRS, Functional Pain Scale | NRS: −0.69 “Change Score” from baseline |
Madson et al. (2010)54 | MT | Solid organ transplant pain patients | n = 58 | Within-subjects | Pre-test/post-test design | Subjective | Intensity | 10-Point Likert-type scales | −0.30§ |
Ames et al. (2017)2 | MM | Postoperative pain patients | n = 41 | Between-subjects | Controlled non–music listening | Subjective | Intensity | VAS, NRS, and opiate intake | VAS: −0.12║ NRS: 0.00║ Opioid intake, IV: 0.30 Opioid intake, epidural: 0.26 |
Chan et al. (2007)7 | MM | Interventional pain patients (C-clamp procedure) | n = 66 | Between-subjects | 45-min uninterrupted rest period | Subjective and objective | Intensity | Systolic and diastolic pressure (SBP and DBP), HR, respiratory rate (RR), OS, and UCLA Universal Pain Assessment Tool | UCLA Universal Pain Assessment, MT vs control at 45-min timepoint: −1.37 |
Chantawong et al. (2017)8 | MM | Patients undergoing loop electrosurgical excision procedure (LEEP) | n = 150 | Between-subjects | No music listening during procedure | Subjective | Intensity (pain, anxiety, and satisfaction) | VAS | VAS, pain score difference from baseline: −0.26 |
Dobek et al. (2014)12 | MM | Healthy participants | n = 12 | Within-subjects | No music during painful stimulus | Subjective and objective | Intensity | fMRI, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), Crowne-Marlowe Social Desirability Scale, PCS, TSA | Average pain rating: −2.21 Maximum pain rating: −2.57 |
Garza-Villarreal et al. (2015)17 | MM | Fibromyalgia patients | n = 22 | Within-subjects | Pink noise | Subjective, objective | Intensity | PCS, fMRI | Missing data |
Guetin et al. (2012)22 | MM | Lumbar pain, fibromyalgia, inflammatory disease, and neurological disease patients | n = 87 | Between-subjects | Standard treatment only | Subjective | Intensity | VAS | VAS at day 60: −0.79 |
Jafari et al. (2012)36 | MM | Post–cardiac surgery patients | n = 60 | Between-subjects | No music | Subjective | Intensity | NRS | Intensity directly after intervention: −0.64 Intensity 30 minutes after intervention: −0.91 Intensity 1 h after intervention: −0.96 |
Karalar et al. (2016)42 | MM | Renal calculi pain patients | n = 89 | Between-subjects | No headphones or music or music without noise canceling headphones | Subjective | Intensity | VAS | Music+/headphones+vs music–/headphones–: −1.23 music+/headphones+vs music+/headphones–: −0.63 |
McCaffrey et al. (2003)57 | MM | Chronic osteoarthritis pain patients | n = 66 | Between-subjects | No music listening (quiet sitting) | Subjective | Intensity | Short Form McGill Pain Questionnaire (Pain Rating Index, VAS) | PRI: 1.64¶ VAS: 2.05¶ |
McCaffrey et al. (2006)58 | MM | After hip or knee surgery | n = 124 | Between-subjects | Standard postoperative care | Subjective, objective | Intensity | Pain medication intake, NRS | Pain rating, average of days 1-3: −1.12 Pain medications administered: −0.31 |
Meeuse et al. (2010)59 | MM | Interventional pain (sigmoidoscopy) | n = 307 | Between-subjects | Standard of care | Subjective | Intensity | VAS | −0.122 |
Mercadie et al. (2015)61 | MM | Fibromyalgia patients | n = 81 | Between-subjects | Environmental sounds | Subjective | Intensity | VAS | Active situations, music vs sound: −0.02# Passive situations, music vs sound: 0# |
Mitchell et al. (2006)62 | MM | Healthy participants | n = 54 | Within-subjects | White noise | Subjective and objective | Intensity, tolerance, and perceived control | VAS, pain rating index of the McGill Pain Questionnaire | Pain intensity (VAS): Relaxing music vs white noise: −0.340; preferred music vs white noise: −0.760; relaxing music vs preferred music: −0.431 |
Nilsson et al. (2005)64 | MM | Open hernia repair pain patients | n = 75 | Between-subjects | Silence | Subjective, objective | Intensity | Cortisol, blood glucose, immunoglobin A levels, BP, HR, OS, and NRS | Pain NRS; intraoperative MT vs control: −0.74; postoperative MT vs control: −0.97 |
Onieva-Zafra et al. (2013)65 | MM | Fibromyalgia patients | n = 55 | Between-subjects | No music listening | Subjective | Sensory, affective, evaluative, and intensity | McGill Pain Questionnaire Long Form, VAS | VAS after 4 wk: −1.67 |
Özer et al. (2013)67 | MM | Open heart surgery pain patients | n = 87 | Between-subjects | Standard of care | Subjective and objective | Intensity | BP, HR, OS, RR, and unidimensional verbal pain intensity scale | Post-test pain (verbal pain intensity scale): −2.16 |
Schneider et al. (2018)76 | MM | Postoperative (orthopedic) pain | n = 42 (n = 65 for pain logs collected, as several patients completed the exercise multiple times) | Within-subjects | Pre–post intervention pain scores | Subjective | Intensity | NRS | −0.69 |
Shabandokht-Zarmi et al. (2017)77 | MM | Hemodialysis patients, during fistula puncture | n = 114 | Between-subjects | Headphone (no music) or control (no intervention) | Subjective | Intensity | VAS | Music vs control: 1.87**; Music vs headphone-only: 1.96** |
Vaajoki et al. (2012)82 | MM | Laparotomy recovery | n = 168 | Between-subjects | No music listening | Objective | n/a | Analgesic use, length of hospital stay, and adverse events | Missing data |
Villarreal et al. (2012)83 | MM | LHealthy participants | n = 48 | Between-subjects | Environmental sounds, noise | Subjective | Intensity | VAS | Missing data |
Yeo et al. (2013)88 | MM | Cystoscopy related pain patients | n = 70 | Between-subjects | No music during procedure | Subjective | Intensity | VAS, physiological functions (hemodynamic values; mean arterial pressure; heart and respiration rates) | VAS: −1.16 |
Negative values for Hedges’ g indicate a higher value in the control group than in the treatment group. For measures evaluating pain, a negative Hedges’ g reflects a decrease in the treatment compared with the control group. For measures evaluating measures such as threshold, a negative Hedges’ g value indicates a higher pain threshold in controls compared with the treatment group. Hedges’ g: ∣0.2∣ = small effect size; ∣0.5∣ = medium effect size; ∣0.8∣ = large effect size.
“subjective” indicating measurements based on subjects’ self-reported perceptions of their experience; “objective” indicating measurements independent of subject bias. Missing data = case studies or studies in which mean and/or SD were not reported.
Hedges’ g: (mean [tx grp] – mean [control grp])/(combined SD).
(Week 8, follow-up)—data taken from “Change Score” from baseline.
Comparison between pre-MT and post-MT.
Average of 4 time-points.
Average of 3 time-points, reflecting change in pain level (D1, D7, and D14).
Average of 3 time-points.
Calculated using pain score difference before and after intervention.
Using log10-transformed times.
subjects stratified based on “worst pain” in the no VR condition.
fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; VR, virtual reality.
2. Virtual reality and pain
2.1. Neurobiological bases of immersive virtual reality and their relationship with pain
There are several proposed mechanisms for how VR experiences may alleviate pain.30,49,74,87 It has been suggested that VR engages pathways that would otherwise be devoted to pain signaling through distraction.49,86 In this theory, VR creates a positive effect on cognitive variables to both enhance pain control50 and moderate pain signaling pathways through memory, emotions, and other senses including haptic, aural, and visual.20 It is also possible that VR distances patients from their current state through immersion. For example, patients with walking pain may be able to enter a reality where they are not physically moving their limbs but are able to virtually experience walking.16 Efforts are under way to understand the mechanism through which VR functions have clinical relevance.43 Researchers must discover whether the underlying mechanism is distraction,23 in which case, salience is key; whether it acts through fundamentally regulating mood, emotions, and altering how we see and perceive the world around us, in which case, total immersion is paramount; or whether it requires active cognitive engagement, in which case, guided experiences may need to be explored.43
Despite the current paucity of VR equipment that is compatible with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) techniques, a few studies have reported that VR as compared to a no-VR condition decreases the neural activity in regions of interest (ROI) such as the anterior cingulate cortex, primary and secondary somatosensory cortices, insula, and thalamus in healthy participants when thermal painful stimulations are given.30 A follow-up study indicated that the aforementioned ROIs are differently modulated by VR and pharmacological treatments. Healthy participants underwent a thermal painful stimulation in a within-subject design that included (1) control (no analgesia), (2) opioid (4 ng/mL hydromorphone administration), (3) immersive VR, and (4) combined opioid and VR.31 The opioid alone reduced pain unpleasantness (Hedges’ g = −0.367) and blood oxygen level-dependent activity in the insula and thalamus. Virtual reality alone reduced both worst pain intensity (Hedges’ g = −0.367) along with neural activity in the insula, thalamus, and secondary somatosensory cortices. Interestingly, combining opioid and VR interventions resulted in a larger reduction of pain reports compared with opioid alone on all subjective pain measures (Hedges’ g = −2.46). This finding supports the concept that multimodal combined pain therapeutics can be clinically relevant. As MRI-compatible technology improves, future studies with combined VR-MT should explore the mechanisms through which multimodal approaches modulate pain-related and other processes in patients suffering from pain.31
2.2. Acute pain
Virtual reality pain management for acute pain has been frequently studied and reviewed in both healthy subjects11,25,80,81 and pain patients, especially burn victims26,37,44,53,75,79 and patients with phantom limb pain.1,68,73 One pioneering study was conducted on 2 adolescents, showing that they were able to better tolerate painful procedures during burn dressing changes during a VR experience.27 In the years since this study, Hoffman et al. have performed numerous studies with larger sample sizes examining VR interventions as an adjunctive treatment for burn pain.5,52,53,78 For example, adult patients showed both improvement of pain function (eg, motion exercise ranges during physical therapy)28 and pain reduction during wound care.29 All patients reported significantly better outcomes with the immersive VR as compared to no VR.28 Although the study was unblinded, the order of the condition was randomized and counterbalanced. Research that explores different VR features (eg, high-tech VR helmets,32) is aiming to further determine VR’s impact on experimental pain.
A systematic review that assessed studies comparing VR with a control condition or an alternative intervention indicated that VR reduces experimental pain and acute clinical pain associated with burn injury care.55 The review showed that VR works less with needle-related pain, and fully immersive VR-based tools were more likely to provide pain relief. Indovina et al.35 analyzed VR interventions during painful medical procedures, including studies that looked at acute pain and other measures of distress, in varied patient populations. The authors, while confirming the validity of the VR interventions, called for the establishment of predictive factors that would encourage the development of personalized VR experiences.
2.3. Chronic pain
As Indovina et al.35 noted, studies considering VR interventions to treat chronic pain are “in (their) infancy.” However, the numbers of such studies are increasing in recent years. Jones et al.41 explored the efficacy of a 5-minute immersive VR environment (Cool!) as a pain intervention in 30 study participants with moderate chronic pain conditions (ie, cervical spine, lumbar spine, hip, shoulder, abdominal, thoracic pain, and diffuse pain from myalgia or connective tissue disease and neuropathy). During the VR treatment session, participants reported lowered pain by 60% and, after the session, lowered pain by 33% (Hedges’ g = −0.741) as compared to their presession self-report of moderate pain.41 In another chronic pain study, Jin et al. designed and tested the VR game Cryoslide in a randomized, controlled crossover study as an intervention to relieve “spikes” of pain and found a reduction of clinical pain compared with baseline and controls.39 Keefe et al.43 posited several ways in which VR could be used to treat specific chronic pain conditions, including altering pain-related movement patterns and integrating VR with behavioral interventions (eg, hypnosis, meditation, and exposure therapy). Future studies using immersive VR tools that are tailored to the etiology of pain disorders (eg, phantom pain1) are needed.
2.4. Technical applications and audio-based interventions
A wide range of VR interventions have been developed. Well-established applications such as Cool! (DeepStream VR Inc, Seattle, WA, 2014) and Snow World (MultiGen-Paradigm.com, 2001) have provided a sense of immersion through visual and aural components. Other interventions are beginning to explore olfactory19 and tactile21 stimuli. The effectiveness of these interventions has been explored using a variety of subjective measures, including pain scales,44 as well as objective measures such as blood-oxygen–dependent measurements of brain activity,31 vital signs,86 and measures of skin conductance.84 While early VR interventional studies primarily used expensive equipment, commoditization has made lower-cost VR interventions more feasible.15 Virtual reality hardware and software are evolving rapidly, and the viewing systems are becoming less expensive and more portable. At the same time, the library of digital content is rapidly increasing. This might facilitate the use of VR as an adjunct or alternative therapy for the treatment of distinct aspects of pain (sensory and affective components) across pain disorders.
Virtual reality interventions that are considered “higher tech” have been shown to increase the therapeutic effects of VR due to the increase of “presence,” or the illusion of entering and being in the virtual world.33 These higher tech interventions used sound effects, which indicate that incorporating auditory input for interventions may increase the sense of presence and therefore increase efficacy. A recent study directly examined the effect of adding aural input to a VR intervention in healthy subjects participating in a cold-pressor pain study.40 Virtual reality including audio elicited higher pain tolerance as compared to an aural-only condition, a VR-only condition, and a control group (Hedges’ g = 0.43). This suggests that the addition of sound may increase the attention-demanding nature of the experience, providing greater reduction of the perception of pain. The authors note that determining which kind of sounds is most effective in combination with the VR (context-relevant or distracting) may help describe the mechanisms through which VR therapies work.40
3. Music and pain
Music-based therapies have been used to mitigate acute34 and chronic pain9 as observed using subjective measures (eg, pain rating scales) and objective methods (eg, fMRI).13
3.1. Neurobiological bases of music and their relationship with pain
Several studies have sought to explain the neural underpinnings of the human experience of music in general.47 Neural responses to music are centered in the nucleus accumbens, a major reward brain center, and its dopaminergic stimulator, the ventral tegmental area 60,71,72 The activation of the mesolimbic reward system and the release of dopamine in response to music has demonstrated its pleasure-giving capability.45,46 This highlights the unique ability of music to connect and engage with multiple parts of the brain and music-evoked emotions.45 Reybrouck et al. compiled results from 12 studies that used network science algorithms. They concluded that music activates the auditory cortex, the brain reward system, and areas associated with the mind wandering, with distinct changes associated with perceptual, action-related, cognitive, affective, and evaluative processes.70 Studies have also shown that this pathway of anatomical substrates is shared with the perception of pain, indicating that the 2 may be more closely linked than once believed.48
The quantitative understanding of MT treatments as they relate with pain has also been explored. Dobek et al.12 used fMRI to examine neural activity related to painful stimuli in subjects listening to music they enjoyed vs controls who had no music, finding altered neural patterns indicative of decreased pain when music was playing. Garza-Villarreal et al. found that listening to music reduced pain in fibromyalgia through top-down regulation of the modulatory network, with higher connectivity between the left angular gyrus, the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the left caudate (lCau), and decreased connectivity with the right anterior cingulate cortex, the right supplementary motor area, and the precuneus and right precentral gyrus. Pain reduction levels were correlated with the connectivity of the left angular gyrus to the right precentral gyrus.17 If these results are further confirmed, listening to music to activate the pain modulatory systems could open up new strategies for nonpharmacological treatments of pain.
3.2. Acute pain
Many studies have shown MT to be effective in treating acute pain.2,42,54,56,58,63,67,76,77,88 For example, preferred music reduced subjective perception of pain in postcardiac surgical patients compared with controls (P = 0.0001).36 Forty-five minutes of MT reduced heart rate, respiratory rate, oxygen saturation, and pain in patients undergoing C-clamp application after percutaneous coronary intervention in comparison with controls receiving uninterrupted rest.7 However, results regarding MT’s efficacy are conflicting.8,10,59,61 The discrepancy in results may be due to various factors, including inadequate study designs and controls or subjectivity of musical experiences (eg, music anhedonia).
3.3. Chronic pain
Parallel to studies in VR, MT has suggested effectiveness in managing chronic pain.3,14,18,22,24,57,65 For example, Bradt et al.3 explored the feasibility of an 8-week vocal MT treatment program on chronic pain disorders in a population of older Afro-American inner-city adults. The study established the feasibility of the intervention and demonstrated large effect sizes for self-efficacy at weeks 8 and 12, a moderate effect size for pain interference at week 8, and no improvements for general activities and emotional functioning, paving the road for further research in MT.
Music-based therapies have also been explored in the context of opioid use disorders. Some studies found that music reduced opioid intake,64 yet others reported that while the music was enjoyable to the patients, the amount of analgesic used did not differ in music-treated patients vs controls.82 In a recent preliminary research report, Chai et al.6 discussed the future of using music as an adjunct to opioid administration by establishing the feasibility of the intervention and determining experimentally key points of relevance for clinical endpoints (eg, music features, patients’ preferences, motivation, and engagement).
3.4. Technical applications
The range of music-based interventions offered under the umbrella of “Music therapy” is incredibly wide. Activities range from music listening, to vocal therapy, to music production using instruments. It is worth noting that MT, as strictly defined, requires active participation on behalf of the subjects in the presence of a therapist. Most “MT” studies may be more accurately described as music medicine (MM), which usually involves listening to prerecorded music without the presence of a therapist. However, Bradt et al. examined the impacts of MT vs MM and showed that both interventions resulted in equal decreased pain and improved psychological outcomes in cancer patients.4
Overall, these studies bring up the interesting notion that, for music to be an effective aesthetic intervention, listeners must engage with what they are experiencing (eg, vocalization and improvisation). In MT, this is often accomplished by a music therapist acting as a guide. In MM, the unguided nature of the intervention makes this more difficult to control for.
4. Limitations and future directions
Many VR and MT studies miss either the appropriate controls (eg, nonimmersive VR, pink noise vs ambient music, and passive music listening) and/or blinding of both research staff and study participants. Moreover, most of the VR and MT studies focused on pain intensity, when VR and MT may change the nature of affect associated with pain experience (eg, distressing or frightening), pain quality (eg, sharpness of pain) and the effectiveness based on pain location (eg, distinct body representation).38 Magnitude-based inferences related to clinical relevance are based on the examination of outcomes beyond the statistical significance. Based on the studies we reviewed (Table 1), there is a need for additional systematic meta-analyses34 that account for heterogeneity of the studies. Only such approach allows for quantifying the efficacy of MT and VR and their potential to implement their uses routinely to optimize clinical outcomes. Future randomized placebo-controlled clinical trials and comparative effectiveness research will also help define the clinical relevance of VR, MT, and combined VR-MT. Finally, it is important to consider whether and how expectancies and contextual placebo effects may influence the effectiveness of VR-MT interventions.51
Despite these limitations, recent studies have illustrated increased pain tolerance especially in patients who self-selected their musical experience62,83 or were immersed in VR contexts.43 The hypoalgesic properties of both MT and VR could be further explored in the context of combined VR-MT applications. Virtual reality brings the unique opportunity to reach a high level of engagement on multiple sensory and cognitive levels, as well as the specialization necessary for creating MT-based clinically meaningful experiences that lead to pain reduction. This may help bridge the gap between MT and MM. The possibilities of VR-MT interventions range from minimal contact, using speakers or mobile phones, to use of spatial sound-enabled VR headsets. Stereo 360° cinematographic rendering of virtual scenes could transport patients to the stage of a rock concert, where they are able to change positions and alter which instruments they hear best, or place them in the middle of an operatic finale. Although studies have examined the effects of adding sensory components to VR interventions, exploring multiple VR-MT contents may elucidate the differential effects unique to certain pain disorders and patient predictors of beneficial outcomes.
5. Conclusions
Based on the studies discussed above, both VR and MT might contribute to reducing pain through mechanisms that include distraction and demand on attention, mood and emotion regulation, and immersion and engagement. Virtual reality and MT can act as a multimodal pain intervention by activating in turn, sensory-perceptual, action-related, cognitive, affective, and evaluative processes. Future research is needed to explore the mutual contribution of these processes and their effect sizes. At the time of this publication, no studies have been published that assess music as part of the VR intervention for pain management. We believe that with the relative novelty and ongoing development of VR and MT experiences, it is both feasible and logical to introduce VR-MT environments to promote therapeutic hypoalgesic outcomes. Immersive VR-MT presents a unique and promising approach to pain management and could help further our understanding of the complex relationship between music and VR-driven neurobiological healing mechanisms.
Footnotes
Conflict of interest statement
E. Honzel reports personal fees from University of Maryland outside the submitted work. A. Varshney reports grants from National Science Foundation, grants from State of Maryland’s MPower Initiative during the conduct of the study; L. Colloca reports grants from NIDCR (R01DE025946), PCORI, MPower the State, UM Grants, personal fees from Elsevier during the conduct of the study. The remaining authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.
Sponsorships or competing interests that may be relevant to content are disclosed at the end of this article.
References
- [1].Ambron E, Miller A, Kuchenbecker KJ, Buxbaum LJ, Coslett HB. Immersive low-cost virtual reality treatment for phantom limb pain: evidence from two cases. Front Neurol 2018;9:67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [2].Ames N, Shuford R, Yang L, Moriyama B, Frey M, Wilson F, Sundaramurthi T, Gori D, Mannes A, Ranucci A, Koziol D, Wallen GR. Music listening among postoperative patients in the intensive care unit: a randomized controlled trial with mixed-methods analysis. Integr Med Insights 2017;12:1178633717716455. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [3].Bradt J, Norris M, Shim M, Gracely EJ, Gerrity P. Vocal music therapy for chronic pain management in inner-city african Americans: a mixed methods feasibility study. J Music Ther 2016;53:178–206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [4].Bradt J, Potvin N, Kesslick A, Shim M, Radl D, Schriver E, Gracely EJ, Komarnicky-Kocher LT. The impact of music therapy versus music medicine on psychological outcomes and pain in cancer patients: a mixed methods study. Support Care Cancer 2015;23:1261–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [5].Carrougher GJ, Hoffman HG, Nakamura D, Lezotte D, Soltani M, Leahy L, Engrav LH, Patterson DR. The effect of virtual reality on pain and range of motion in adults with burn injuries. J Burn Care Res 2009;30:785–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [6].Chai PR, Carreiro S, Ranney ML, Karanam K, Ahtisaari M, Edwards R, Schreiber KL, Ben-Ghaly L, Erickson TB, Boyer EW. Music as an adjunct to opioid-based analgesia. J Med Toxicol 2017;13:249–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [7].Chan MF. Effects of music on patients undergoing a C-clamp procedure after percutaneous coronary interventions: a randomized controlled trial. Heart Lung 2007;36:431–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [8].Chantawong N, Charoenkwan K. Effects of music listening during loop electrosurgical excision procedure on pain and anxiety: a randomized trial. J Low Genit Tract Dis 2017;21:307–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [9].Cheever T, Taylor A, Finkelstein R, Edwards E, Thomas L, Bradt J, Holochwost SJ, Johnson JK, Limb C, Patel AD, Tottenham N, Iyengar S, Rutter D, Fleming R, Collins FS. NIH/Kennedy center workshop on music and the brain: finding harmony. Neuron 2018;97:1214–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [10].Colwell CM, Edwards R, Hernandez E, Brees K. Impact of music therapy interventions (listening, composition, orff-based) on the physiological and psychosocial behaviors of hospitalized children: a feasibility study. J Pediatr Nurs 2013;28:249–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [11].Demeter N, Josman N, Eisenberg E, Pud D. Who can benefit from virtual reality to reduce experimental pain? A crossover study in healthy subjects. Eur J Pain 2015;19:1467–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [12].Dobek CE, Beynon ME, Bosma RL, Stroman PW. Music modulation of pain perception and pain-related activity in the brain, brain stem, and spinal cord: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Pain 2014; 15:1057–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [13].Economidou E, Klimi A, Vivilaki VG, Lykeridou K. Does music reduce postoperative pain? A review. Health Sci J 2012;6:365–77. [Google Scholar]
- [14].Espi-Lopez GV, Ingles M, Ruescas-Nicolau MA, Moreno-Segura N. Effect of low-impact aerobic exercise combined with music therapy on patients with fibromyalgia. A pilot study. Complement therapies Med 2016;28:1–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [15].Ford CG, Manegold EM, Randall CL, Aballay AM, Duncan CL. Assessing the feasibility of implementing low-cost virtual reality therapy during routine burn care. Burns 2018;44:886–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [16].Garrett B, Taverner T, Masinde W, Gromala D, Shaw C, Negraeff M. A rapid evidence assessment of immersive virtual reality as an adjunct therapy in acute pain management in clinical practice. Clin J Pain 2014; 30:1089–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [17].Garza-Villarreal EA, Jiang Z, Vuust P, Alcauter S, Vase L, Pasaye EH, Cavazos-Rodriguez R, Brattico E, Jensen TS, Barrios FA. Music reduces pain and increases resting state fMRI BOLD signal amplitude in the left angular gyrus in fibromyalgia patients. Front Psychol 2015;6:1051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [18].Garza-Villarreal EA, Pando V, Vuust P, Parsons C. Music-Induced analgesia in chronic pain conditions: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pain Physician 2017;20:597–610. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [19].Gerardi M, Rothbaum BO, Ressler K, Heekin M, Rizzo A. Virtual reality exposure therapy using a virtual iraq: case report. J Trauma Stress 2008; 21:209–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [20].Gold JI, Belmont KA, Thomas DA. The neurobiology of virtual reality pain attenuation. Cyberpsychol Behav 2007;10:536–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [21].Gromala D, Song M, Yim JD, Fox T, Barnes SJ, Nazemi M, Shaw C, Squire P. Proceedings of the 2011 annual conference extended abstracts on human factors in computing systems ACM; 2011:1171–6. Proceedings of e CHI 2011. [Google Scholar]
- [22].Guetin S, Ginies P, Siou DK, Picot MC, Pommie C, Guldner E, Gosp AM, Ostyn K, Coudeyre E, Touchon J. The effects of music intervention in the management of chronic pain: a single-blind, randomized, controlled trial. Clin J Pain 2012;28:329–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [23].Gupta A, Scott K, Dukewich M. Innovative technology using virtual reality in the treatment of pain: does it reduce pain via distraction, or is there more to it? Pain Med 2018;19:151–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [24].Gutgsell KJ, Schluchter M, Margevicius S, DeGolia PA, McLaughlin B, Harris M, Mecklenburg J, Wiencek C. Music therapy reduces pain in palliative care patients: a randomized controlled trial. J Pain Symptom Manage 2013;45:822–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [25].Gutierrez-Maldonado J, Gutierrez-Martinez O, Loreto-Quijada D, Nieto-Luna R. The use of virtual reality for coping with pain with healthy participants. Psicothema 2012;24:516–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [26].Hoffman HG, Chambers GT, Meyer WJ III, Arceneaux LL, Russell WJ, Seibel EJ, Richards TL, Sharar SR, Patterson DR. Virtual reality as an adjunctive non-pharmacologic analgesic for acute burn pain during medical procedures. Ann Behav Med 2011;41:183–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [27].Hoffman HG, Doctor JN, Patterson DR, Carrougher GJ, Furness TA III. Virtual reality as an adjunctive pain control during burn wound care in adolescent patients. PAIN 2000;85:305–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [28].Hoffman HG, Patterson DR, Carrougher GJ. Use of virtual reality for adjunctive treatment of adult burn pain during physical therapy: a controlled study. Clin J Pain 2000;16:244–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [29].Hoffman HG, Patterson DR, Seibel E, Soltani M, Jewett-Leahy L, Sharar SR. Virtual reality pain control during burn wound debridement in the hydrotank. Clin J Pain 2008;24:299–304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [30].Hoffman HG, Richards TL, Coda B, Bills AR, Blough D, Richards AL, Sharar SR. Modulation of thermal pain-related brain activity with virtual reality: evidence from fMRI. Neuroreport 2004;15:1245–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [31].Hoffman HG, Richards TL, Van Oostrom T, Coda BA, Jensen MP, Blough DK, Sharar SR. The analgesic effects of opioids and immersive virtual reality distraction: evidence from subjective and functional brain imaging assessments. Anesth Analg 2007;105:1776–83, Table of contents. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [32].Hoffman HG, Seibel EJ, Richards TL, Furness TA, Patterson DR, Sharar SR. Virtual reality helmet display quality influences the magnitude of virtual reality analgesia. J Pain 2006;7:843–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [33].Hoffman HG, Sharar SR, Coda B, Everett JJ, Ciol M, Richards T, Patterson DR. Manipulating presence influences the magnitude of virtual reality analgesia. PAIN 2004;111:162–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [34].Hole J, Hirsch M, Ball E, Meads C. Music as an aid for postoperative recovery in adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet 2015; 386:1659–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [35].Indovina P, Barone D, Gallo L, Chirico A, De Pietro G, Giordano A. Virtual reality as a distraction intervention to relieve pain and distress during medical procedures: a comprehensive literature review. Clin J Pain 2018; 34:858–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [36].Jafari H, Emami Zeydi A, Khani S, Esmaeili R, Soleimani A. The effects of listening to preferred music on pain intensity after open heart surgery. Iran J Nurs Midwifery Res 2012;17:1–6. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [37].Jeffs D, Dorman D, Brown S, Files A, Graves T, Kirk E, Meredith-Neve S, Sanders J, White B, Swearingen CJ. Effect of virtual reality on adolescent pain during burn wound care. J Burn Care Res 2014;35:395–408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [38].Jensen MP, Karoly P. Self-report scales and procedures for assessing pain in adults In: Melzack DCTaR, editor. Handbook of pain assessment. New York, NY: Guilford Press, 2010. pp. 19–44. [Google Scholar]
- [39].Jin W, Choo A, Gromala D, Shaw C, Squire P. A virtual reality game for chronic pain management: a randomized, controlled clinical study. Stud Health Technol Inform 2016;220:154–60. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [40].Johnson S, Coxon M. Sound can enhance the analgesic effect of virtual reality. R Soc Open Sci 2016;3:150567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [41].Jones T, Moore T, Choo J. The impact of virtual reality on chronic pain. PLoS One 2016;11:e0167523. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [42].Karalar M, Keles I, Dogantekin E, Kahveci OK, Sarici H. Reduced pain and anxiety with music and noise-canceling headphones during shockwave lithotripsy. J Endourol 2016;30:674–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [43].Keefe FJ, Huling DA, Coggins MJ, Keefe DF, Zachary Rosenthal M, Herr NR, Hoffman HG. Virtual reality for persistent pain: a new direction for behavioral pain management. PAIN 2012;153:2163–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [44].Kipping B, Rodger S, Miller K, Kimble RM. Virtual reality for acute pain reduction in adolescents undergoing burn wound care: a prospective randomized controlled trial. Burns 2012;38:650–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [45].Koelsch S Brain correlates of music-evoked emotions. Nat Rev Neurosci 2014;15:170–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [46].Koelsch S Music-evoked emotions: principles, brain correlates, and implications for therapy. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2015;1337:193–201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [47].Koelsch S Investigating the neural encoding of emotion with music. Neuron 2018;98:1075–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [48].Leknes S, Tracey I. A common neurobiology for pain and pleasure. Nat Rev Neurosci 2008;9:314–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [49].Li A, Montano Z, Chen VJ, Gold JI. Virtual reality and pain management: current trends and future directions. Pain Manag 2011;1:147–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [50].Loreto-Quijada D, Gutierrez-Maldonado J, Nieto R, Gutierrez-Martinez O, Ferrer-Garcia M, Saldana C, Fuste-Escolano A, Liutsko L. Differential effects of two virtual reality interventions: distraction versus pain control. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 2014;17:353–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [51].Lunde SJ, Vuust P, Garza-Villarreal EA, Vase L. Music-induced analgesia: how does music relieve pain? PAIN 2019;160:989–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [52].Maani CV, Hoffman HG, Fowler M, Maiers AJ, Gaylord KM, Desocio PA. Combining ketamine and virtual reality pain control during severe burn wound care: one military and one civilian patient. Pain Med 2011;12:673–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [53].Maani CV, Hoffman HG, Morrow M, Maiers A, Gaylord K, McGhee LL, DeSocio PA. Virtual reality pain control during burn wound debridement of combat-related burn injuries using robot-like arm mounted VR goggles. J Trauma 2011;71:S125–130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [54].Madson AT, Silverman MJ. The effect of music therapy on relaxation, anxiety, pain perception, and nausea in adult solid organ transplant patients. J Music Ther 2010;47:220–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [55].Malloy KM, Milling LS. The effectiveness of virtual reality distraction for pain reduction: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev 2010;30:1011–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [56].Matasota P, Christodoulopoulou T, Smyrnioti ME, Pandazi A, Kanellopoulos L, Koursoumi E. Music’s use for anesthesia and analgesia. J Altern Complement Med 2013;19:298–307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [57].McCaffrey R, Freeman E. Effect of music on chronic osteoarthritis pain in older people. J Adv Nurs 2003;44:517–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [58].McCaffrey R, Locsin R. The effect of music on pain and acute confusion in older adults undergoing hip and knee surgery. Holist Nurs Pract 2006;20: 218–24; quiz 225-216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [59].Meeuse JJ, Koornstra JJ, Reyners AK. Listening to music does not reduce pain during sigmoidoscopy. Eur J Gastroenterol Hepatol 2010;22: 942–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [60].Menon V, Levitin DJ. The rewards of music listening: response and physiological connectivity of the mesolimbic system. Neuroimage 2005; 28:175–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [61].Mercadie L, Mick G, Guetin S, Bigand E. Effects of listening to music versus environmental sounds in passive and active situations on levels of pain and fatigue in fibromyalgia. Pain Manag Nurs 2015;16:664–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [62].Mitchell LA, MacDonald RA. An experimental investigation of the effects of preferred and relaxing music listening on pain perception. J Music Ther 2006;43:295–316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [63].Mondanaro JF, Homel P, Lonner B, Shepp J, Lichtensztein M, Loewy JV. Music therapy increases comfort and reduces pain in patients recovering from spine surgery. Am J Orthop (Belle Mead NJ) 2017;46:E13–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [64].Nilsson U, Unosson M, Rawal N. Stress reduction and analgesia in patients exposed to calming music postoperatively: a randomized controlled trial. Eur J Anaesthesiol 2005;22:96–102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [65].Onieva-Zafra MD, Castro-Sanchez AM, Mataran-Penarrocha GA, Moreno-Lorenzo C. Effect of music as nursing intervention for people diagnosed with fibromyalgia. Pain Manag Nurs 2013;14:e39–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [66].Ostling PS, Davidson KS, Anyama BO, Helander EM, Wyche MQ, Kaye AD. America’s opioid epidemic: a comprehensive review and look into the rising crisis. Curr Pain Headache Rep 2018;22:32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [67].Ozer N, Karaman Ozlu Z, Arslan S, Gunes N. Effect of music on postoperative pain and physiologic parameters of patients after open heart surgery. Pain Manag Nurs 2013;14:20–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [68].Perry BN, Mercier C, Pettifer SR, Cole J, Tsao JW. Virtual reality therapies for phantom limb pain. Eur J Pain 2014;18:897–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [69].Pourmand A, Davis S, Marchak A, Whiteside T, Sikka N. Virtual reality as a clinical tool for pain management. Curr Pain Headache Rep 2018;22:53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [70].Reybrouck M, Vuust P, Brattico E. Brain connectivity networks and the aesthetic experience of music. Brain Sci 2018;8:E107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [71].Salimpoor VN, Benovoy M, Larcher K, Dagher A, Zatorre RJ. Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music. Nat Neurosci 2011;14:257–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [72].Salimpoor VN, van den Bosch I, Kovacevic N, McIntosh AR, Dagher A, Zatorre RJ. Interactions between the nucleus accumbens and auditory cortices predict music reward value. Science 2013;340: 216–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [73].Sano Y, Ichinose A, Wake N, Osumi M, Sumitani M, Kumagaya S, Kuniyoshi Y. Reliability of phantom pain relief in neurorehabilitation using a multimodal virtual reality system. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc 2015;2015:2482–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [74].Scapin S, Echevarria-Guanilo ME, Boeira Fuculo PR Junior, Goncalves N, Rocha PK, Coimbra R. Virtual Reality in the treatment of burn patients: a systematic review. Burns 2018;44:1403–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [75].Schmitt YS, Hoffman HG, Blough DK, Patterson DR, Jensen MP, Soltani M, Carrougher GJ, Nakamura D, Sharar SR. A randomized, controlled trial of immersive virtual reality analgesia, during physical therapy for pediatric burns. Burns 2011;37:61–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [76].Schneider MA. The effect of listening to music on postoperative pain in adult orthopedic patients. J Holist Nurs 2018;36:23–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [77].Shabandokht-Zarmi H, Bagheri-Nesami M, Shorofi SA, Mousavinasab SN. The effect of self-selected soothing music on fistula puncture-related pain in hemodialysis patients. Complement Ther Clin Pract 2017;29: 53–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [78].Sharar SR, Miller W, Teeley A, Soltani M, Hoffman HG, Jensen MP, Patterson DR. Applications of virtual reality for pain management in burn-injured patients. Expert Rev Neurother 2008;8:1667–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [79].Small C, Stone R, Pilsbury J, Bowden M, Bion J. Virtual restorative environment therapy as an adjunct to pain control during burn dressing changes: study protocol for a randomised controlled trial. Trials 2015;16: 329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [80].Smith A, Carlow K, Biddulph T, Murray B, Paton M, Harvie DS. Contextual modulation of pain sensitivity utilising virtual environments. Br J Pain 2017;11:71–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [81].Sulea C, Soomro A, Wiederhold BK, Wiederhold MD. Quantifying the effectiveness of virtual reality pain management: a pilot study. Stud Health Technol Inform 2014;199:94–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [82].Vaajoki A, Kankkunen P, Pietila AM, Kokki H, Vehvilainen-Julkunen K. The impact of listening to music on analgesic use and length of hospital stay while recovering from laparotomy. Gastroenterol Nurs 2012;35: 279–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [83].Villarreal EA, Brattico E, Vase L, Ostergaard L, Vuust P. Superior analgesic effect of an active distraction versus pleasant unfamiliar sounds and music: the influence of emotion and cognitive style. PLoS One 2012; 7:e29397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [84].Webb AK, Vincent AL, Jin AB, Pollack MH. Physiological reactivity to nonideographic virtual reality stimuli in veterans with and without PTSD. Brain Behav 2015;5:e00304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [85].Wenger S, Drott J, Fillipo R, Findlay A, Genung A, Heiden J, Bradt J. Reducing opioid use for patients with chronic pain: an evidence-based perspective. Phys Ther 2018;98:424–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [86].Wiederhold BK, Gao K, Sulea C, Wiederhold MD. Virtual reality as a distraction technique in chronic pain patients. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 2014;17:346–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [87].Wiederhold BK, Soomro A, Riva G, Wiederhold MD. Future directions: advances and implications of virtual environments designed for pain management. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 2014;17:414–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [88].Yeo JK, Cho DY, Oh MM, Park SS, Park MG. Listening to music during cystoscopy decreases anxiety, pain, and dissatisfaction in patients: a pilot randomized controlled trial. J Endourol 2013;27:459–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]