Abstract
Human cognition is not always tethered to events in the external world. Laboratory and real world experience sampling studies reveal that attention is often devoted to self-generated mental content rather than to events taking place in the immediate environment. Recent studies have begun to explicitly examine the consistency between states of off-task thought in the laboratory and in daily life, highlighting differences in the psychological correlates of these states across the two contexts. Our study used neuroimaging to further understand the generalizability of off-task thought across laboratory and daily life contexts. We examined (1) whether context (daily life versus laboratory) impacts on individuals’ off-task thought patterns and whether individual variations in these patterns are correlated across contexts; (2) whether neural correlates for the patterns of off-task thoughts in the laboratory show similarities with those thoughts in daily life, in particular, whether differences in cortical grey matter associated with detail and off-task thoughts in the para-hippocampus, identified in a prior study on laboratory thoughts, were apparent in real life thought patterns. We also measured neural responses to common real-world stimuli (faces and scenes) and examined how neural responses to these stimuli were related to experiences in the laboratory and in daily life - finding evidence of both similarities and differences. There were consistent patterns of off-task thoughts reported across the two contexts, and both patterns had a commensurate relationship with medial temporal lobe architecture. However, compared to real world off-task thoughts, those in the laboratory focused more on social content and showed a stronger correlation with neural activity when viewing faces compared to scenes. Overall our results show that off-task thought patterns have broad similarities in the laboratory and in daily life, and the apparent differences may be, in part, driven by the richer environmental context in the real world. More generally, our findings are broadly consistent with emerging evidence that shows off-task thoughts emerge through the prioritisation of information that has greater personal relevance than events in the here and now.
Keywords: Off-task thought, Vivid detailed experience, Face processing, Fusiform cortex, Real-world neuroscience, Mind-wandering
Highlights
-
•
Laboratory and daily life patterns of thought show both similarities and differences.
-
•
Thoughts from both contexts have similar associations with medial temporal lobe grey matter.
-
•
Laboratory off-task thoughts have stronger emphasis on content related to other people.
-
•
Laboratory off-task thoughts are linked to greater neural activity when viewing faces.
1. Introduction
Human cognition is flexible and we often use imagination to focus on information pertinent to ourselves rather than events in the here and now. Often our minds wander to thoughts of a holiday, our family and friends, or to work related problems (Smallwood and Schooler, 2006, 2015). Experience sampling studies in both the laboratory and in daily life settings indicate we spend substantial amount of time engaging in patterns of thought that are, at best, loosely related to ongoing events in the external world (Kane et al., 2007; Killingsworth and Gilbert, 2010; Klinger, 1978). These self-generated thoughts often involve personally relevant topics, such as other people or other times and places (Poerio et al., 2015; Ruby et al., 2013; Smallwood, 2013; Smallwood et al., 2016). In the laboratory, studies suggest that self-generated experiences increase when the demands of the outside world decline (Smallwood et al., 2008; Teasdale et al., 1993), when we find the external environment uninteresting or lacking change (Giambra, 1993; Smallwood et al., 2009), or when we lack the motivation to engage with the task at hand (Seli et al., 2015). Recent evidence suggests that the capacity to self-generate patterns of thought unrelated to the external environment occurs through a process of prioritisation rooted in the neural architecture of the saliency network, including the dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex, and the anterior insula (Kam et al., 2019; Turnbull et al., 2019b).
Nevertheless, most existing research on self-generated thought relies on data gathered in laboratory settings with less meaningful semantic content, compared to the richness of the real world environment. An important question, therefore, is to what extent results derived from assessment of thoughts collected in the laboratory can be generalised to those in daily life. A small but growing number of studies have begun to explore and compare these two contexts, yielding evidence of both similarities and differences. For instance, in laboratory settings there is typically a robust negative association between working memory capacity and on-task thought during demanding tasks (e.g. McVay and Kane, 2009; Mrazek et al., 2012). However, the relationships with working memory and other executive functions are more complex when experience is sampled in daily life. In the real world, the link between executive abilities and off-task thoughts is less robust and varies as a function of concentration, that is, the ability to maintain attention to the ongoing task (Kane et al., 2007, 2017). More recently, Linz and colleagues (Linz et al., 2019) demonstrated that aspects of experiential content are correlated from laboratory and daily life (e.g. the degree of self-relevance or the focus on the future), whereas the degree of task focus was not. These studies, combined with theoretical accounts (Smallwood and Andrews-Hanna, 2013), emphasise that a more complete understanding of ongoing thoughts requires accounting for the broader context in which experience emerges. Our study leveraged the tools of cognitive neuroscience to further understand the similarities and differences between patterns of experiences observed in the laboratory and in the more complex context of daily life. This endeavour is important for developing ecologically valid neurocognitive accounts of self-generated thoughts, and also more generally for the emerging area of ‘Real-world Neuroscience’ (Hamilton and Huth, 2018; Matusz et al., 2019).
1.1. Study design
Fig. 1 illustrates our study design. We sampled experiences across two situations: (i) in the laboratory while participants performed simple tasks in which they made spatial decisions about inanimate shapes (triangles, circles and squares) and (ii) in daily life where thoughts occurred in an ecologically valid context. We used a relatively large sample (n = 77) recruited from a larger cohort of participants for whom we have previously documented thought patterns and associated neural correlates (for prior publications, see Ho et al., 2019; Turnbull et al., 2019a; Turnbull et al., 2019b; Wang et al., 2018b). In the same group of participants, we sought to establish patterns of experiences in the context of minimal content (the laboratory) and in the more ecological context of daily life. We first compared the patterns of experience obtained from these two contexts to quantify their similarities and differences. We also examined how individual variations in thought patterns from the real world and the laboratory are associated with measures of brain structure and function. In a previous study (Ho et al., 2019), we mapped patterns of laboratory ongoing thoughts to whole brain cortical thickness in a large cohort of individuals (those in the current study are a sub-sample of this cohort). We found that more detailed external thought patterns were associated with greater thickness in a region of the posterior hippocampus, whereas an adjacent anterior region was associated with more on-task thoughts. In the present study we examined whether these findings replicate with the ecologically valid thought patterns measured in the real world. Next, motivated by the importance of using meaningful stimuli in understanding cognition (Hamilton and Huth, 2018; Matusz et al., 2019), we examined whether individual differences in patterns of laboratory and daily life experiences were associated with neural responses to ecologically valid stimuli that are likely to be encountered outside laboratory contexts. Specifically, we measured neural activity while viewing faces and indoor or outdoor scenes. Scrambled versions of both sets of stimuli were included to act as controls.
2. Material and methods
2.1. Participants
Participants were healthy, right-handed, native English speakers, with normal or corrected-to-normal vision and no history of psychiatric or neurological illness. They were recruited as volunteers from the undergraduate and postgraduate student bodies at the University of York who were either paid or given course credits. The study was approved by the Department of Psychology and York Neuroimaging Centre, University of York ethics committees. All participants gave their written informed consent prior to taking part in any task or fMRI scanning, and were debriefed after completion of the study.
This study was a part of a larger project with a large cohort of individuals. Among these participants, seventy-seven (female = 57; Age: Mean = 19.66, SD = 1.62, range = 18–27) completed the experience sampling questionnaires in daily life. All but one of these participants, together with a bigger subgroup, completed the experience sampling questionnaires in the laboratory, totalling 199 participants (female = 128; Age: Mean = 20.11, SD = 2.24, range = 18–31). These data regarding experience sampling in the laboratory have been previously published with extensive descriptions of the neural correlates of different patterns of experience (see Ho et al., 2019; Turnbull et al., 2019a; Turnbull et al., 2019b; Wang et al., 2018b). For the purpose of comparison, we examined structural (cortical thickness) and functional (content localizer task) neuroimaging data for participants who had experience sampling data from both context. After excluding one participant with problematic structural brain data, valid brain data from seventy-three participants (55 females; Age: Mean = 19.63, SD = 1.65. Range = 18–27) were used to quantify brain structural correlates of experience sampling in daily life. Only sixty-one participants successfully completed the functional neuroimaging task (content localizer task), three of whom were excluded because of missing data, leaving valid data for fifty-eight participants (female = 43; Age: Mean = 19.62, SD = 1.69, range = 18–27) for functional neuroimaging data analysis. Finally, we used resting state functional imaging data to embed any region identified from the content localizer analyses in a whole brain context, hence, we included all valid data for resting-state functional connectivity analysis. Of the initial 181 individuals who had scanned for resting state data, seven were excluded with missing or problematic data and another nine were excluded for excessive movement (see details in 2.5.2. Resting state fMRI under 2.5. Neuroimaging Data Preprocessing), leaving a total of 165 subjects (female = 105; Age: Mean = 20.18, SD = 2.36, range = 18–31). Please note that both the real world experience sampling data and the neural response to real world stimuli have not been previously published in any form. Due to time limitations we were not able to measure experience during the functional imaging session with passive viewing (content localizer task). Supplementary Table S1 provides a summary of the participant information for each of these groups.
2.2. Procedures
Laboratory experience sampling data were collected from participants who took part in a comprehensive set of behavioural assessments that captured different aspects of cognition, including the experiential assessment task, as well as other experimental tasks which were not analyzed in the current study. These tasks were completed over two to three sessions on different days, with the order of sessions counterbalanced across participants. Daily life experience sampling data were collected using participants’ own smartphones, which would alert them to respond to the experience sampling questionnaire five times a day for 7 days at quasi-random intervals as they went about their daily lives. Neuroimaging data was collected at the York Neuroimaging Centre.
2.3. Behavioural data
2.3.1. Experiential assessment in the laboratory
The contents of ongoing thoughts in the laboratory were assessed using a 0-/1-back task, which were detailed in other studies from our laboratory (Sormaz et al., 2018; Turnbull et al., 2019a). Briefly, it is a simple visual decision making paradigm in which memory load is manipulated across alternate blocks of low-load (0-back) and high-load (1-back) conditions. At the end of each run of 2–8 (Mean = 5) non-target trials, participants are required to respond to a target trial by deciding whether the probe shape (black-coloured circle, square or triangle) appears in the middle matches with the shape presented on the left or right hand side of the current (0-back) or previous (1-back) screen. In 20% of these target trials, instead of probes for shape, the “Thoughts ?” prompt appears and participants are then required to provide the subjective ratings of their ongoing thoughts on a four-point scale, from “Not at all” to “Completely”, for all 13 questions in the multidimensional experience sampling (MDES) questionnaire (see Table 1, rows marked as ‘L’ under column ‘Content’). Participants always rated their level of task focus first and then described their thoughts at the moment before the probe on a further 12 dimensions. These MDES probes occurred on a quasi-random basis to minimise the likelihood that participants could anticipate the occurrence of a probe. A similar approach has been used in our previous studies of on-going thoughts (e.g. Ruby et al., 2013; Smallwood et al., 2016).
Table 1.
Dimensions | Context [a] | Questions | 1 | 4 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Task | D, L | My thoughts were related to my current activity and/or external environment. | Not at all | Completely | |
Future | D, L | My thoughts were about the future. | Not at all | Completely | |
Past | D, L | My thoughts were about the past. | Not at all | Completely | |
Self | D, L | My thoughts involved myself. | Not at all | Completely | |
Person [b] | D | My thoughts involved other people close to me. | Not at all | Completely | |
D | My thoughts involved other people NOT close to me | Not at all | Completely | ||
Person | L | My thoughts involved other people. | Not at all | Completely | |
Emotion | D, L | My thoughts were …. | Very Negative | Very Positive | |
Images | D, L | My thoughts were in the form of images. | Not at all | Completely | |
Words | D, L | My thoughts were in the form of words. | Not at all | Completely | |
Vivid | D, L | My thoughts were vivid. | Not at all | Completely | |
Detailed | D, L | My thoughts were detailed and specific. | Not at all | Completely | |
Habit | D, L | My thoughts had recurrent themes similar to those that I have had before. | Not at all | Completely | |
Evolving | D, L | My thoughts tended to evolve in a series of steps. | Not at all | Completely | |
Deliberate | D, L | My thoughts were: | Completely Spontaneous | Completely Deliberate | |
NA [c] | D | My thoughts were conflicting/interfering with what I am trying to achieve right now | Not at all | Completely | |
NA | D | My thoughts were helpful for goals that I am trying to achieve right now | Not at all | Completely | |
NA | D | My thoughts were helpful for goals that I am trying to achieve (or avoid) in the future | Not at all | Completely | |
NA | D | The content of my thoughts is important to me (i.e., it deals with something important in my life) | Not at all | Completely | |
NA | D | I was trying to control the progression of my thoughts | Not at all | Completely | |
NA | D | I wanted to have my thoughts | Not at all | Completely |
Note:
For a full list of the PCA scores, see Supplementary Information Tables S2 and S3
Denotes the setting in which question appeared: D = Daily Life; L = Laboratory.
Average of the two items from daily life data entered as ‘Person’ dimension.
NA = Not Available; these questions appeared in the Daily Life setting only, therefore, were not included in the PCA.
2.3.2. Experiential assessment in daily life
Participants reported the content and form of their naturally occurring thoughts five times daily for seven days. This daily life experience sampling was administered using participants’ own smartphones in conjunction with SurveySignal Software (Hofmann and Patel, 2015). Participants received each signal as a text message with a link to an online survey created in Qualtrics. This survey required participants to consider the content of their thought immediately before answering the survey on various dimensions (see Table 1, rows marked as ‘D’ under column ‘Content’). We sought to assess the degree of similarity between the patterns of thought observed in the laboratory and daily life settings so we focused only on items that were presented in both contexts. We excluded 6 questions from the daily life questionnaire that had no analogue in the laboratory situation and averaged the ratings for the two questions related to the social content of thought that was present in the real world (MDES Dimension = ‘Person’, see Table 1 Note [2]). Signals occurred at random intervals between 09:00 and 21:00 with at least 30 min between adjacent signals. Participants were given two hours to answer the survey before the link expired, after that they were no longer able to provide a response (overall actual response rate = 77.18%).
2.3.3. Experiential assessment data analysis
To identify the internal structure of reports of ongoing thought content, principal components analysis (PCA) with varimax rotation was applied to the two MDES datasets using IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 25). Component scores were averaged for individual participants and transformed into z-scores, with outliers (>2.5) replaced with mean values (total outliers ≤ 2.44%).
Two analyses were adopted to assess the similarity of the PCA components from the laboratory and from daily life: (i) We conducted a single PCA which included thoughts in both contexts and calculated the correlations between an individual’s loadings in both situations and (ii) we calculated two separate PCAs in each context and the compared the similarity of the patterns produced using these independent analyses, as well as correlation of the component scores in the two contexts by individual participant. In the latter analysis, one of the components (emotion) showed similar relationships between items but the phase of these relationships was reversed in the two data sets (i.e. ‘Past’ was positive and ‘Emotion’ was negative in daily life, while ‘Emotion’ was positive and ‘Past’ was negative in the laboratory). Since PCA solutions are symmetrical, to facilitate interpretation, we reversed the scores in daily life for all of the analysis reported in the rest of this paper. Please note that the variation in patterns of experience across the 0-back and 1-back task have previously been described elsewhere (Turnbull et al., 2019a).
2.4. Neuroimaging data acquisition
Neuroimaging data were acquired at the York Neuroimaging Centre, York with a 3T GE HDx Excite MRI scanner using an eight-channel phased array head coil. First, a T1-weighted structural scan with 3D fast spoiled gradient echo (TR = 7.8 s, TE = minimum full, flip angle = 20°, matrix size = 256 x 256, 176 slices, voxel size = 1.13 x 1.13 × 1 mm3) and a FLAIR sequence were acquired. Next, participants were asked to take a 9-min resting state fMRI (open eye) using single-shot 2D gradient-echo-planar imaging (TR = 3 s, TE = minimum full, flip angle = 90°, matrix size = 64 x 64, 60 slices, voxel size = 3 x 3 × 3 mm3, 180 vol).
For some participants, this was followed by a content localizer task with single-shot 2D gradient-echo-planar imaging (TR = 3 s, TE = minimum full, flip angle = 90°, matrix size = 64 x 64, 60 slices, voxel size = 3 x 3 × 3 mm3, 260 vol). Each participant, who had also participated in a semantic localizer task which has been analyzed previously (Zhang et al., 2019), completed one functional run (average running time: 8 min 35 s) for a single block of 312 trials. The task began with written instructions requesting the participants to passively view a series of stimuli presented at the centre of a grey screen (900 ms), each being separated by an inter-stimulus interval (ISI; 600 ms) when a small white fixation cross was displayed. At random intervals (10% trials), the fixation cross would change colour (white to green) and participants were required to press a button. This did not affect the presentation timing but was used to help participants maintain attentional focus.
The stimuli, presented in a quasi-random order, would either be a NULL stimulus (a normal fixation cross, 60 trials), or a grey-scale image (standardized in size of 256 x 256 pixel) of faces and scenes that are deemed to be commonly encountered in everyday life. Face images (non-scrambled = 126, scrambled = 42) were Caucasian adult faces selected from the Radboud Faces Database (Langner et al., 2010), with balanced number of positive (happy), negative (disgusted, sad and fearful) and neutral facial expression, gender (male vs female), as well as fixed proportion of gaze direction (frontal: left: right = 3:2:2). Scene images (non-scrambled = 42, scrambled = 42) consisted of equal number of scenes related to city, indoor and nature (see Supplementary Information Fig. S2 for a sample of the experimental stimuli). Details of the paradigm is shown in Fig. 2.
2.5. Neuroimaging Data Preprocessing
2.5.1. Cortical thickness
Cortical thickness is computed as the closest distance between the grey/white boundary and pial surface at each vertex across the entire cortex. This measure was derived from the T1-weighted images using models generated by FreeSurfer (Version 5.3.0; https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) (Fischl, 2012) and preprocessed using the standard pipeline (for details, please refer to Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999). Briefly, these steps include intensity normalization, removal of non-brain tissues, tissue classification, and surface extraction. We then visually inspected and, if necessary, manually corrected the cortical surface for each participant before registering surface data to an average spherical representation, fsaverage5, to improve the correspondence of locations across subjects. A surface-based smoothing with a 20 mm full-width-at-half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel was applied to reduce measurement noise without forgoing the capacity for anatomical localization (Lerch and Evans, 2005). Similar kernels have been used by our previous studies focussing on cortical morphology (Bernhardt et al., 2009, 2010, 2014; Valk et al., 2017).
2.5.2. Resting state fMRI
Preprocessing steps for the MRI data were carried out using the SPM software package (Version 12.0) (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) based on the MATLAB platform (Version 16. a) (https://uk.mathworks.com/products/matlab.html). After removing the first three functional volumes to account for the magnetization equilibrium, the remaining data were corrected for motion using six degrees of freedom (x, y, z translations and rotations), and adjusted for differences in slice-time. Subsequently, the high-resolution structural image was co-registered to the mean functional image via rigid-body transformation, segmented into grey/white matter and cerebrospinal fluid probability maps, and were spatially normalized to the MNI space alongside all functional volumes using the segmented images and a priori templates (Ashburner and Friston, 2005). All the functional images were then smoothed using an 8 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. We have also applied the MRI data denoising procedure to remove potential confounds of motion and other artefacts; and both of these steps were carried out using the Conn functional connectivity toolbox (Version 17. f) (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/conn) (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon, 2012). The denoising procedure was done by employing an extensive motion-correction procedure and denoising step, comparable to those reported in the literature (Ciric et al., 2017); and then entering the six realignment parameters and their second-order derivatives, a linear detrending term and the CompCor method that removed five principle components of the signal from white matter and cerebrospinal fluid (Behzadi et al., 2007) into linear model (Friston et al., 1996) as covariates of no interest. Volumes with excessive motion were also identified and scrubbed based on the conservative settings of motion greater than 0.5 mm and global signal changes larger than z = 3 and participants who had more than 15% of their data affected by motion were excluded (Power et al., 2014). Finally, a band-pass filter between 0.009 Hz and 0.08 Hz was employed in order to focus on low frequency fluctuations (Fox et al., 2005).
2.5.3. Task-based fMRI (content localizer)
Functional data were preprocessed and analyzed using a standard pipeline by FMRI Expert Analysis Tool (FEAT) from the FMRIB Software Library (FSL Version 6.0). Individual FLAIR and T1-weighted structural brain images were extracted using FSL’s Brain Extraction Tool (BET). Structural images were registered to the MNI152 template using FMRIB’s Linear Image Registration Tool (FLIRT). Spatial preprocessing of the functional data included motion correction via MCFLIRT, slice-timing correction with Fourier space time-series phase-shifting, and spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel of FWHM 6 mm. A high-pass temporal filtering (sigma = 100 s) was also applied in order to remove temporal signal drift and participants with excessive motions were excluded.
2.6. Neuroimaging data analysis
2.6.1. Cortical thickness
Cortical thickness data were analyzed using SurfStat for matlab [http://www.math.mcgill.ca/keith/surfstat/] (Worsley et al., 2009). One of our recent studies revealed that cortical thickness of two adjacent areas at the medial temporal lobe (MTL) were associated with individual variations of detailed and off-task thoughts based on PCAs derived from laboratory experience sampling data (Ho et al., 2019). This indicated that patterns of ongoing thought might be related to stable features of brain anatomy. To extend this investigation regarding the association of stable neural features to patterns of ongoing thoughts in daily life, we extracted cortical thickness from clusters in our previous study, and used an ANOVA with repeated measures (Greenhouse-Geisser corrected) to test whether cortical thickness of these brain regions would be associated with patterns of ongoing thought in daily life. Consistent with our prior study, we included both age and gender as covariates of no-interest.
2.6.2. Resting state fMRI
We used the resting state data to embed any region identified through the content localizer analyses in a whole brain context. Seed-based functional connectivity analyses were conducted based on the binarised seed ROIs that were highlighted through the content localizer task. Individual connectivity map for each participant were obtained by extracting the average BOLD signal within the time series of the ROI and then correlated with the time course of each individual voxel in the rest of the brain. These correlation results were then transformed to Fisher’s Z-scores to produce the standardized functional connectivity. Group-level inferences on the connectivity map were made based on one-sample t-tests, thresholded at Z = 3.1 (Eklund et al., 2016). The resulting brain networks were visualized using Connectome Workbench (v1.3.2). [https://www.humanconnectome.org/software/connectome-workbench].
2.6.3. Task-based fMRI (content localizer)
This analysis included a total of 58 participants for whom there were valid localizer data, as well as measures of experience in both the laboratory and daily life. For our first-level analysis, we included four explanatory variables namely, faces, scrambled faces, scenes, scrambled scenes, each modelled for the time period of 900 ms during which the respective type of image was presented. This was convolved with the hemodynamic response function and contrasts were created to assess brain activity related to main effects of faces (faces > scrambled faces) and scenes (scenes > scrambled scenes), as well as comparisons of these two types of stimuli ((faces > scrambled faces) > (scenes > scrambled scenes) and vice versa). Please note that we assessed faces with different emotional expressions (positive, neutral and negative). These were modelled separately and analysis indicated that there were no effects specific to a particular facial expression, accordingly we do not report these analyses in the main body of the text.
Group-level analysis was modelled by entering the PCA parameters, that is, averages (mean PCA scores from daily life and laboratory) and differences (PCA scores from daily life minus scores from laboratory) for all relevant PCAs (see 2.6.1 Experience sampling analysis), into a linear model as explanatory variables for predicting neural activity represented by the contrast maps generated from the first-level modelling. All spatial maps were thresholded at Z = 3.1 (Eklund et al., 2016). The resulting brain networks were visualized using Connectome Workbench (v1.3.2). [https://www.humanconnectome.org/software/connectome-workbench].
2.7. NeuroSynth reverse inference (meta-analysis)
Automated meta-analyses were conducted using the NeuroSynth decoder (http://neurosynth.org/decode; Yarkoni et al., 2011) to make quantitative inferences on the results identified from fMRI analysis. Unthresholded connectivity maps obtained from group analysis were submitted to NeuroSynth, which then computed the spatial correlations between each of these maps and a large database of meta-analytic maps (n = 11,406) for different cognitive terms (e.g., autobiographic, memory, task, emotional regulation). The top 15 terms (singular and plural forms of the same term were combined), excluding neuroanatomical terms, exhibiting the highest positive correlation with each unthresholded map submitted were extracted and presented as word clouds. Size and colour of the font reflects the strength of the relationship. This analysis allows us to quantify the reverse inferences that would be drawn from the functional maps by the larger neuroimaging community.
3. Results
3.1. Experiential assessment
Our first analysis combined MDES results obtained from both contexts and decomposed these in a single PCA. (see Supplementary Information Fig. S2 for the scree plot). The decompositions can be summarized along four dimensions: (i) ‘Vivid Evolving and Detailed’ - describing patterns of thought with the highest loadings on ‘Detailed’ and ‘Evolving’, (ii) ‘Off-Task and Self-Relevant’ with low loadings on ‘Task’ and higher loadings on ‘Self’, (iii) ‘Deliberate and Task-relevant Positive Emotion’ and (iv) ‘Modality’ with high loadings on ‘Images’ and low loadings on ‘Words’. Word clouds representing loadings for each component is shown in Fig. 3 (top panel). To assess how consistent these were across the two contexts, we summarized each individual’s PCA scores by the laboratory and daily life situations and correlated these values. We found that all components, except the ‘Deliberate and Task-relevant Positive Emotion’ component were correlated across the two situations (Fig. 3, bottom panel). Inspection into the summary statistics of the individual MDSE questions (see Supplementary Information Table S4) showed that there were relatively larger variabilities of “Emotion” and “Deliberate” thoughts, which may be the driving cause of this non-correlation of the third component.
Next we examined the PCA structure independently across the two situations. For consistency across contexts we calculated four components in both cases (see Supplementary Information Fig. S2 for the scree plots) which explained 59.83% and 54.85% of the total variance in ongoing thought contents in the laboratory and in daily life respectively.
Representation of the four components by word clouds (Fig. 4) revealed that independent decompositions of the datasets from these two settings showed broadly similar internal structures of ongoing thought patterns, which are also broadly consistent with the structure revealed by the combined analysis using single PCA. These four dimensions are: (i) ‘Vivid Evolving and Detailed’ (ii) ‘Off-Task and Self-Relevant’ (iii) ‘Modality’ and (iv) ‘Emotion’ with opposite loadings on ‘Emotion’ and ‘Past’ (i.e. positive loadings emotion and negative loadings on the past). Compared to daily life thought patterns, ‘Vivid Evolving and Detailed’ thoughts in the laboratory were more self-focused and recurrent, and tended to be past or future oriented; whereas ‘Off-Task and Self-Relevant’ laboratory thoughts were more focused on other people and the past, but were less deliberate and recurrent (see bottom panel of Fig. 4 for further comparisons).
To quantify the relationship between thought patterns recorded across the two contexts, we correlated both PCA loadings and individual PCA scores obtained from both contexts. Significant positive correlations were observed for PCA loadings (internal structure of thought contents summarized by group ratings in the two settings) of all four components (Fig. 5, top panel), and moderate correlations were found for individual PCA scores (individual participants’ actual thought component ratings across the two settings) in three of these four components (‘Vivid Evolving & Detailed’, ‘Off-Task & Self-Relevant’, ‘Modality’ but not ‘Emotion’) (Fig. 5, middle panel). These results suggested that ongoing thought contents across contexts were broadly consistent in structure (PCA loadings) and were correlated across participants. Similar to the combined analysis, the component showing the strongest loading for emotion was the least well correlated across contexts.
Having established both similarities and differences in the patterns of ongoing thoughts in the laboratory and in daily life, we next examined their neural correlates using the results from independent analyses (as they are not constrained by the assumption that thoughts in the two contexts are having the same internal structure). Eight parameters were derived from the independent PCA results: four averages of the PCA loadings across contexts and four differences for daily life minus laboratory PCA loadings. These parameters were transformed into z-scores, with outlying values (>2.5) replaced by the mean (overall percentage of outliers < 3.23%). They were then used to examine the neural associations with patterns of ongoing thoughts and both anatomical and functional indices of brain organisation.
3.2. Cortical thickness
Our cortical thickness analysis examined whether there are similarities between the structural correlates associated with patterns of thought in the laboratory and those observed in daily life. To achieve this, mean cortical thickness was extracted from the left medial temporal regions associated with ‘Vivid Evolving & Detailed’ thought and ‘On-Task’ (negatively associated with ‘Off-task & Self-relevant’) thoughts from the larger laboratory sample reported in our prior study (Ho et al., 2019). We then ran a repeated measures ANOVA in which the two averages of the cortical thickness in these two regions of the para-hippocampus were dependent variables, while the two average loadings on Detailed and Off-task components from the real-world experience sampling were explanatory variables. Consistent with our prior study, we included both age and gender as covariates of no-interest. We found two patterns of associations between cortical thickness and ongoing experience. First, there was a main effect of the Detailed component (F(1, 68) = 8.01, p < .01) which reflected a positive correlation between Detailed thoughts in daily life and cortical thickness across both regions of the para-hippocampus (r = 0.32, p = .005). In addition, a region by Off-task interaction (F (1, 68) = 7.88, p < .01) indicated that a greater on-task focus was linked to greater thickness in the anterior relative to the posterior region of the left para-hippocampus (r = −0.28, p < .05) (Fig. 6).
Together these patterns are broadly consistent with our previous findings using laboratory based thought sampling (Ho et al., 2019). They indicate that task focused thoughts are more closely linked to greater thickness in the anterior part of the left para-hippocampus, whereas detailed thoughts are more closely associated with the posterior region. These analyses, therefore, have established a broad similarity in the ‘brain structure – experience’ relationship, regardless of whether thoughts were sampled in the laboratory or in the more ecologically valid context of one’s daily life.
3.3. Face and scene processing related neural activity
Next, to understand whether patterns of ongoing experience are related to the neural responses to faces and scenes, we analyzed the neural activity using a generalised linear model (GLM). First, we formulated a high level contrast (Faces > Scrambled Faces) > (Scenes > Scrambled Scenes) to identify neural activity specific to either type of content. We then regressed this contrast, within a single linear model, against (i) overall levels of experience for both situations, as well as (ii) differences in these experiences across the two contexts. We found a significant cluster that showed a pattern of activity consistent with an interaction between stimulus type and the difference in off-task thoughts across contexts. Greater neural activity was observed in a cluster near the right fusiform gyrus (including the right inferior and superior lateral occipital cortex, occipital pole and fusiform gyrus) when viewing faces compared to viewing scenes. Neural activity in this region increased with levels of ‘Off-task & Self-relevant’ thoughts in the laboratory, but not for those in daily life (shown by scatterplots in Fig. 7). Decoding this region using NeuroSynth confirmed a pattern of neural responses seen for faces and other objects (see left hand word cloud). Together these results show that individuals who tended to have more ‘Off-task & Self-relevant’ thought in the laboratory also tended to show greater activity in a region of right fusiform cortex when viewing faces compared to scenes. Importantly, an apparent bias linked to social information is also reflected in the loadings of off-task thoughts, that is, more thoughts on ‘Person’ are associated with off-task thoughts in the laboratory than in daily life (summarized in the word clouds on the right panel in Fig. 7.)
Finally, to embed the region of right fusiform cortex in a whole brain functional context, we conducted a seed-based resting state functional connectivity analysis using the region of interest at the right fusiform cortex (see Methods). We identified a network of regions that had a similar time course. These included both unimodal regions (i.e. visual and motor cortex), and transmodal regions including lateral temporal cortex, anterior insula and the tempo parietal junction. Decoding of this spatial map using Neurosynth identified terms confirming that this pattern of neural activity is often seen when individuals actively engage with the external environment (terms like “visual”, “faces”, “objects”, “reading”) (Fig. 8).
4. Discussion
We leveraged the tools of neuroimaging to gain a better understanding of differences and similarities between patterns of ongoing thoughts across laboratory and naturalistic real world contexts. Application of principal components analysis (PCA) to experience sampling data obtained from both laboratory and daily life contexts revealed similar patterns of personal, temporal and social thought content during off-task states. We also found similarities between the architecture of the medial temporal lobe and patterns of thought in the laboratory and in daily life. Extending recent work by Linz et al. (2019), these findings suggest that laboratory-based measures provide a reasonable approximation for real world patterns of thought. However, we also found important differences across contexts: laboratory off-task thoughts were focused to a greater degree on other people compared to thoughts sampled in real world settings. This difference in experiential content across context was mirrored at a neural level: individuals who tended to experience more off-task thoughts in the laboratory showed heighted activity in a region of right fusiform cortex when processing external social information (viewing faces).
From a methodological viewpoint, our study provides insight into the generalizability between cognition, as measured in the controlled context of the laboratory, and in the more contextually rich real world (Hamilton and Huth, 2018; Matusz et al., 2019). We found broad similarities between the patterns of experiences across contexts in terms of: (i) their patterns of loadings, (ii) their variations across individuals and (iii) their associations with grey matter structures in the para-hippocampus. Given the role of the medial temporal lobe in memory, this latter finding provides initial evidence that memory systems play a vital role in driving patterns of imaginative experience in the real world as it is assumed to play in laboratory settings (Corballis, 2015).
Despite broad similarities, we also identified differences across contexts. Notably, off-task laboratory thoughts were more social and occurred more spontaneously than those experienced in the real world. These differences were reflected in the functional brain activity patterns such that individuals who experienced more off-task thoughts in the laboratory responded more strongly to similarly social external stimuli (faces); while similar relationship was not observed for real world off-task thoughts. Our findings add to the small but growing body of evidence highlighting how differences between off-task thoughts across contexts relate to other aspects of cognition such as attentional control (Kane et al., 2017). Compared to the highly controlled conditions of a laboratory task, daily life provides people with a much richer and engaging environment within which their ongoing thoughts unfold. We also found differences in the coupling between positive emotion and the degree of intention in on-going experience between the two settings, with daily life thinking characterised by positive deliberate thought more than in the laboratory. We speculate that this may occur because the laboratory is relatively devoid of opportunities to engage in activities that participants enjoy performing. This finding provides a novel insight into evidence that motivation is especially important for the experience of off-task thoughts under laboratory conditions (Seli et al., 2015, 2016, 2018). It is possible, therefore, that in the real world individuals often engage in tasks that they enjoy performing, which are often lacking from laboratory situations, and so may explain why single questions regarding task focus does not generalise across the two situations (Linz et al., 2019).
Although there are differences in thought patterns assessed across laboratory and daily life contexts, our analysis does not preclude the study of ongoing experience under controlled settings. Instead, our study highlights that generalising from laboratory findings to daily life should be done cautiously, and ideally, future studies would assess experiences across both contexts. Laboratory measures of experience, especially using techniques such as MRI, provide a greater degree of precision when investigating underlying cognitive mechanisms, something that can be enhanced by their integration with sampling in ecological real world situations. We have shown here that it is possible to combine the detailed measures of cognitive neuroscience, with the richer, more naturalistic measurements obtained in daily life in a way that can help constrain theoretical accounts of ecologically valid aspects of cognition (Hamilton and Huth, 2018; Matusz et al., 2019). A useful approach, therefore, would be to combine task-based neuroimaging studies with experience-sampling studies that measure naturally occurring experience outside the laboratory. This would allow the identification of the specific patterns of thought that are most meaningful in the real world. Moreover, in combination with neuroimaging work, it would set the foundations for an integrative approach in which these everyday thought patterns would simultaneously enhance the ecological validity of neuroimaging work and provide a mechanistic and neural explanation for whether, when, and how on-going experience would help or hinder navigation of the social world.
While our study used neuroimaging to provide evidence of both similarities and differences in the patterns of thought observed in the laboratory and in daily life, it nonetheless leaves open several important questions. One such issue concerns the mechanism that underpins the observed association between elevated neural responses to faces and off-task thought in the laboratory. Our data show a neurocognitive association across individuals between laboratory thoughts and visual processing of faces, and there are several different mechanisms that could explain this. One possibility is that it could reflect a process through which social information attracts attention, perhaps at an early perceptual level. Our prior studies have highlighted that patterns of thought are prioritised across tasks in a contextually appropriate manner by regions of the ventral attention or saliency network (as defined by Yeo et al., 2011), and in particular the dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex (Turnbull et al., 2019a, 2019b). This neural system is important for prioritising different types of relevant internal and external stimuli (Seeley et al., 2007), which may, in part, enabled by this network’s ability to causally influence other large scale networks (Bonnelle et al., 2012; Goulden et al., 2014). Alternatively, studies have shown that regions of fusiform cortex are activated during imagination of the future and the past (Addis et al., 2007; St. Jacques et al., 2008). An alternative implication of our data, therefore, is that individuals who prioritise thoughts about other people in the laboratory possess more elaborate social representations in memory, which in turn could lead to the greater neural response when viewing faces observed in our study. More generally, we speculate that the implementation of motivationally salient information in self-generated thinking, may be influenced by the saliency network’s ability to prioritise goal relevant information, a question that will be an important focus for future work.
It is important to note that our observation that off-task thought in the laboratory and neural activation while viewing faces are associated should not be seen as an alternative to accounts of off-task thought which emphasise the need to maintain attention on external tasks. For example, there is a large body of evidence demonstrating that off-task thought is related to an inability to constrain attention to external task-relevant information. This perspective is often referred to as the executive-failure view (McVay and Kane, 2010, 2012; Robison and Unsworth, 2017, 2018) (see Randall et al., 2014 for a meta-analysis review) and is supported by a growing body of neurocognitive evidence (Turnbull et al., 2019a, 2019b; Wang et al., 2018a). Rather than framing these accounts as being in competition, the heterogeneous nature of on-going thought encourages us to consider them as complementary accounts which describe different features of the complex landscape of experience (see also Christoff et al., 2016). In this context, our study is important because it highlights that constructs such as ‘off-task thought’ can share broad similarities at both self-report and neural levels, as well as important contextual differences. Moving forward, it should be a priority for future work to focus on understanding how different features of our neurocognitive architecture influence experience in different contexts, and how they are implicated in different types of self-generated content (Smallwood and Andrews-Hanna, 2013). For example, in the context of face processing, Denkova et al. (2018) found that off-task thought led to an attenuation of face relevant event related components in the scalp electroencephalogram (including the n170). It would be informative in the future to compare both neural and experience sampling data in laboratory contexts using tasks with more ecological content to understand in greater detail whether the neural correlates of momentary changes in attention vary across different contexts.
We close by considering the broader implications of our results for accounts of off-task thought, particularly with respect to the role they play in social life (Poerio and Smallwood, 2016). Contemporary neuroscientific enquiry has documented the breadth of neural regions engaged during social cognition (Bzdok et al., 2016; Mars et al., 2012), observations that confirm the importance of social cognition from a neuro-scientific perspective (Schilbach et al., 2008). Likewise, prior experience sampling and other psychological work has shown that patterns of ongoing thought are often social (e.g. Ruby et al., 2013) and can have important implications for health and well-being (Vatansever et al., 2019). For example, positive thoughts about other people can improve mood (Poerio et al., 2015), reduce loneliness (Poerio et al., 2016a), promote social and emotional adjustment (Poerio et al., 2016b), and facilitate personal goal development (Medea et al., 2018). Overall, the prevalence of social information during self-generated thought, and its ability to predict success and failure in the social world, is consistent with the idea that we use ongoing thoughts to focus on personally important information and contextually relevant goals, a view known as the current concerns account (Klinger, 1978; Klinger and Cox, 1987). Our results are broadly in line with this perspective because they suggest that individuals who prioritise imagining socially relevant information during a simple laboratory task show heightened processing of faces in selective face processing regions. Given the importance of social processing to our species, a more thorough neuro-cognitive account of how individuals use their imagination in daily life may help us to understand the conditions under which we can survive and thrive in the complex social landscape of daily life.
CRediT authorship contribution statement
Nerissa Siu Ping Ho: Conceptualization, Methodology, Validation, Formal analysis, Writing - original draft, Writing - review & editing, Visualization. Giulia Poerio: Conceptualization, Methodology, Investigation, Writing - review & editing. Delali Konu: Investigation. Adam Turnbull: Investigation. Mladen Sormaz: Methodology, Investigation. Robert Leech: Resources. Boris Bernhardt: Methodology, Writing - review & editing. Elizabeth Jefferies: Writing - review & editing. Jonathan Smallwood: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing - review & editing, Supervision, Funding acquisition.
Acknowledgements
This project was supported by European Research Council Consolidator awarded to JS (WANDERINGMINDS – 646927).
Footnotes
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116765.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
The following is the Supplementary data to this article:
References
- Addis D.R., Wong A.T., Schacter D.L. Remembering the past and imagining the future: common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:1363–1377. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashburner J., Friston K.J. Unified segmentation. Neuroimage. 2005;26:839–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behzadi Y., Restom K., Liau J., Liu T.T. A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. Neuroimage. 2007;37:90–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernhardt B.C., Bernasconi N., Concha L., Bernasconi A.J.N. Cortical thickness analysis in temporal lobe epilepsy: reproducibility and relation to outcome. Neurol. 2010;74:1776–1784. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181e0f80a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernhardt B.C., Smallwood J., Tusche A., Ruby F.J., Engen H.G., Steinbeis N., Singer T. Medial prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortical thickness predicts shared individual differences in self-generated thought and temporal discounting. Neuroimage. 2014;90:290–297. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernhardt B.C., Worsley K., Kim H., Evans A., Bernasconi A., Bernasconi N.J.N. Longitudinal and cross-sectional analysis of atrophy in pharmacoresistant temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurol. 2009;72:1747–1754. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000345969.57574.f5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonnelle V., Ham T.E., Leech R., Kinnunen K.M., Mehta M.A., Greenwood R.J., Sharp D.J. Salience network integrity predicts default mode network function after traumatic brain injury. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. Unit. States Am. 2012;109:4690–4695. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113455109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bzdok D., Hartwigsen G., Reid A., Laird A.R., Fox P.T., Eickhoff S.B. Left inferior parietal lobe engagement in social cognition and language. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2016;68:319–334. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.02.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christoff K., Irving Z.C., Fox K.C., Spreng R.N., Andrews-Hanna J.R. Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a dynamic framework. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2016;17:718–731. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ciric R., Wolf D.H., Power J.D., Roalf D.R., Baum G.L., Ruparel K., Shinohara R.T., Elliott M.A., Eickhoff S.B., Davatzikos C., Gur R.C., Gur R.E., Bassett D.S., Satterthwaite T.D. Benchmarking of participant-level confound regression strategies for the control of motion artifact in studies of functional connectivity. Neuroimage. 2017;154:174–187. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corballis M.C. University of Chicago Press; 2015. The Wandering Mind: what the Brain Does when You’re Not Looking. [Google Scholar]
- Dale A.M., Fischl B., Sereno M.I. Cortical surface-based analysis: I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage. 1999;9:179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denkova E., Brudner E.G., Zayan K., Dunn J., Jha A.P. Attenuated face processing during mind wandering. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 2018;30:1691–1703. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eklund A., Nichols T.E., Knutsson H. Cluster failure: why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. Unit. States Am. 2016;113:7900–7905. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1602413113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B. FreeSurfer. Neuroimag. 2012;62:774–781. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B., Sereno M.I., Dale A.M. Cortical surface-based analysis: II: inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage. 1999;9:195–207. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox M.D., Snyder A.Z., Vincent J.L., Corbetta M., Van Essen D.C., Raichle M.E. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. Unit. States Am. 2005;102:9673–9678. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston K.J., Williams S., Howard R., Frackowiak R.S., Turner R. Movement-related effects in fMRI time-series. Magn. Reson. Med. 1996;35:346–355. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910350312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giambra L.M. The influence of aging on spontaneous shifts of attention from external stimuli to the contents of consciousness. Exp. Gerontol. 1993;28:485–492. doi: 10.1016/0531-5565(93)90073-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goulden N., Khusnulina A., Davis N.J., Bracewell R.M., Bokde A.L., McNulty J.P., Mullins P.G. The salience network is responsible for switching between the default mode network and the central executive network: replication from DCM. Neuroimage. 2014;99:180–190. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton L.S., Huth A.G. The revolution will not be controlled: natural stimuli in speech neuroscience. Lang. Cognit. Neurosci. 2018:1–10. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2018.1499946. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ho N.S.P., Wang X., Vatansever D., Margulies D., Bernhardt B., Jefferies E., Smallwood J. Neuroimage; 2019. Individual Variation in Patterns of Task Focused, and Detailed, Thought Are Uniquely Associated within the Architecture of the Medial Temporal Lobe; p. 116045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hofmann W., Patel P.V. SurveySignal: a convenient solution for experience sampling research using participants’ own smartphones. Soc. Sci. Comput. Rev. 2015;33:235–253. [Google Scholar]
- Kam J.W., Lin J.J., Solbakk A.-K., Endestad T., Larsson P.G., Knight R.T. Default network and frontoparietal control network theta connectivity supports internal attention. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2019:1–8. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0717-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kane M.J., Brown L.H., McVay J.C., Silvia P.J., Myin-Germeys I., Kwapil T.R. For whom the mind wanders, and when: an experience-sampling study of working memory and executive control in daily life. Psychol. Sci. 2007;18:614–621. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01948.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kane M.J., Gross G.M., Chun C.A., Smeekens B.A., Meier M.E., Silvia P.J., Kwapil T.R. For whom the mind wanders, and when, varies across laboratory and daily-life settings. Psychol. Sci. 2017;28:1271–1289. doi: 10.1177/0956797617706086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Killingsworth M.A., Gilbert D.T. A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science. 2010;330 doi: 10.1126/science.1192439. 932-932. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klinger E. Dimensions of thought and imagery in normal waking states. J. Altered States Conscious. 1978;4:97–113. [Google Scholar]
- Klinger E., Cox W.M. Dimensions of thought flow in everyday life. Imagin., Cognit. Pers. 1987;7:105–128. [Google Scholar]
- Langner O., Dotsch R., Bijlstra G., Wigboldus D.H., Hawk S.T., Van Knippenberg A. Presentation and validation of the Radboud faces database. Cognit. Emot. 2010;24:1377–1388. [Google Scholar]
- Lerch J.P., Evans A.C. Cortical thickness analysis examined through power analysis and a population simulation. Neuroimage. 2005;24:163–173. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linz R., Pauly R., Smallwood J., Engert V. Mind-wandering content differentially translates from the lab to daily life and relates to subjective stress experience. Psychol. Res. 2019 doi: 10.1007/s00426-019-01275-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mars R.B., Neubert F.-X., Noonan M.P., Sallet J., Toni I., Rushworth M.F. On the relationship between the “default mode network” and the “social brain”. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012;6:189. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matusz P.J., Dikker S., Huth A.G., Perrodin C. Are we ready for real-world neuroscience? J. Cognit. Neurosci. 2019;31:327–338. doi: 10.1162/jocn_e_01276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McVay J.C., Kane M.J. Conducting the train of thought: working memory capacity, goal neglect, and mind wandering in an executive-control task. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cognit. 2009;35:196. doi: 10.1037/a0014104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McVay J.C., Kane M.J. Does mind wandering reflect executive function or executive failure? Comment on Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008) Psychol. Bull. 2010;136:188–197. doi: 10.1037/a0018298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McVay J.C., Kane M.J. Why does working memory capacity predict variation in reading comprehension? On the influence of mind wandering and executive attention. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2012;141:302–320. doi: 10.1037/a0025250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Medea B., Karapanagiotidis T., Konishi M., Ottaviani C., Margulies D., Bernasconi A., Bernasconi N., Bernhardt B.C., Jefferies E., Smallwood J. How do we decide what to do? Resting-state connectivity patterns and components of self-generated thought linked to the development of more concrete personal goals. Exp. Brain Res. 2018;236:2469–2481. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4729-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mrazek M.D., Smallwood J., Franklin M.S., Chin J.M., Baird B., Schooler J.W. The role of mind-wandering in measurements of general aptitude. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2012;141:788. doi: 10.1037/a0027968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poerio G.L., Smallwood J. Daydreaming to navigate the social world: what we know, what we don’t know, and why it matters. Soc. Personal.Psychol. Compass. 2016;10:605–618. [Google Scholar]
- Poerio G.L., Totterdell P., Emerson L.-M., Miles E. Love is the triumph of the imagination: daydreams about significant others are associated with increased happiness, love and connection. Conscious. Cognit. 2015;33:135–144. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poerio G.L., Totterdell P., Emerson L.-M., Miles E. Helping the heart grow fonder during absence: daydreaming about significant others replenishes connectedness after induced loneliness. Cognit. Emot. 2016;30:1197–1207. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1049516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poerio G.L., Totterdell P., Emerson L.-M., Miles E. Social daydreaming and adjustment: an experience-sampling study of socio-emotional adaptation during a life transition. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Power J.D., Mitra A., Laumann T.O., Snyder A.Z., Schlaggar B.L., Petersen S.E. Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage. 2014;84:320–341. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Randall J.G., Oswald F.L., Beier M.E. Mind-wandering, cognition, and performance: a theory-driven meta-analysis of attention regulation. Psychol. Bull. 2014;140:1411–1431. doi: 10.1037/a0037428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robison M.K., Unsworth N. Working memory capacity and mind-wandering during low-demand cognitive tasks. Conscious. Cognit. 2017;52:47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2017.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robison M.K., Unsworth N. Cognitive and contextual correlates of spontaneous and deliberate mind-wandering. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cognit. 2018;44:85–98. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruby F.J., Smallwood J., Engen H., Singer T. How self-generated thought shapes mood—the relation between mind-wandering and mood depends on the socio-temporal content of thoughts. PloS One. 2013;8 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077554. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schilbach L., Eickhoff S.B., Rotarska-Jagiela A., Fink G.R., Vogeley K. Minds at rest? Social cognition as the default mode of cognizing and its putative relationship to the “default system” of the brain. Conscious. Cognit. 2008;17:457–467. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seeley W.W., Menon V., Schatzberg A.F., Keller J., Glover G.H., Kenna H., Reiss A.L., Greicius M.D. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seli P., Cheyne J.A., Xu M., Purdon C., Smilek D. Motivation, intentionality, and mind wandering: implications for assessments of task-unrelated thought. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cognit. 2015;41:1417–1425. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seli P., Kane M.J., Smallwood J., Schacter D.L., Maillet D., Schooler J.W., Smilek D. Mind-wandering as a natural kind: a family-resemblances view. Trends Cognit. Sci. 2018;22:479–490. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.03.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seli P., Risko E.F., Smilek D. On the necessity of distinguishing between unintentional and intentional mind wandering. Psychol. Sci. 2016;27:685–691. doi: 10.1177/0956797616634068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smallwood J. Distinguishing how from why the mind wanders: a process–occurrence framework for self-generated mental activity. Psychol. Bull. 2013;139:519–535. doi: 10.1037/a0030010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smallwood J., Andrews-Hanna J. Not all minds that wander are lost: the importance of a balanced perspective on the mind-wandering state. Front. Psychol. 2013;4:441. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smallwood J., Karapanagiotidis T., Ruby F., Medea B., de Caso I., Konishi M., Wang H.-T., Hallam G., Margulies D.S., Jefferies E. Representing representation: integration between the temporal lobe and the posterior cingulate influences the content and form of spontaneous thought. PloS One. 2016;11 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0152272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smallwood J., McSpadden M., Schooler J.W. When attention matters: the curious incident of the wandering mind. Mem. Cognit. 2008;36:1144–1150. doi: 10.3758/MC.36.6.1144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smallwood J., Nind L., O’Connor R.C. When is your head at? An exploration of the factors associated with the temporal focus of the wandering mind. Conscious. Cognit. 2009;18:118–125. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smallwood J., Schooler J.W. The restless mind. Psychol. Bull. 2006;132:946–958. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smallwood J., Schooler J.W. The science of mind wandering: empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2015;66:487–518. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sormaz M., Murphy C., Wang H.-t., Hymers M., Karapanagiotidis T., Poerio G., Margulies D.S., Jefferies E., Smallwood J. Default mode network can support the level of detail in experience during active task states. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. Unit. States Am. 2018;115:9318–9323. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1721259115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- St Jacques P., Rubin D.C., LaBar K.S., Cabeza R. The short and long of it: neural correlates of temporal-order memory for autobiographical events. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 2008;20:1327–1341. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teasdale J.D., Proctor L., Lloyd C.A., Baddeley A.D. Working memory and stimulus-independent thought: effects of memory load and presentation rate. Eur. J. Cognit. Psychol. 1993;5:417–433. [Google Scholar]
- Turnbull A., Wang H.-T., Schooler J.W., Jefferies E., Margulies D.S., Smallwood J. The ebb and flow of attention: between-subject variation in intrinsic connectivity and cognition associated with the dynamics of ongoing experience. Neuroimage. 2019;185:286–299. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turnbull A., Wang H., Murphy C., Ho N., Wang X., Sormaz M., Karapanagiotidis T., Leech R., Bernhardt B., Margulies D. Left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex supports context-dependent prioritisation of off-task thought. Nat. Commun. 2019;10:1–10. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-11764-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valk S.L., Bernhardt B.C., Trautwein F.-M., Böckler A., Kanske P., Guizard N., Collins D.L., Singer T.J.S.A. Structural plasticity of the social brain: differential change after socio-affective and cognitive mental training. Sci. Adv. 2017;3 doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1700489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vatansever D., Karapanagiotidis T., Margulies D.S., Jefferies E., Smallwood J. 2019. Distinct Patterns of Thought Mediate the Link between Brain Functional Connectome and Psychological Well-Being; p. 762344. bioRxiv. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang H.-T., Bzdok D., Margulies D., Craddock C., Milham M., Jefferies E., Smallwood J. Patterns of thought: population variation in the associations between large-scale network organisation and self-reported experiences at rest. Neuroimage. 2018;176:518–527. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.04.064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang H.-T., Poerio G., Murphy C., Bzdok D., Jefferies E., Smallwood J. Dimensions of experience: exploring the heterogeneity of the wandering mind. Psychol. Sci. 2018;29:56–71. doi: 10.1177/0956797617728727. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitfield-Gabrieli S., Nieto-Castanon A. Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect. 2012;2:125–141. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Worsley K.J., Taylor J., Carbonell F., Chung M., Duerden E., Bernhardt B., Lyttelton O., Boucher M., Evans A. NeuroImage Organisation for Human Brain Mapping 2009 Annual Meeting; 2009. A Matlab Toolbox for the Statistical Analysis of Univariate and Multivariate Surface and Volumetric Data Using Linear Mixed Effects Models and Random Field Theory; p. S102. [Google Scholar]
- Yarkoni T., Poldrack R.A., Nichols T.E., Van Essen D.C., Wager T.D. Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nat. Methods. 2011;8:665–670. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeo B., Krienen F., Sepulcre J., Sabuncu M., Lashkari D., Hollinshead M., Roffman J., Smoller J., Zöllei L., Polimeni J. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 2011;106:1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang M., Savill N., Margulies D.S., Smallwood J., Jefferies E. Distinct individual differences in default mode network connectivity relate to off-task thought and text memory during reading. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-52674-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.