Abstract
Background
Autistic individuals exhibit atypical patterns of sensory processing that are known to be related to quality of life, but which are also highly heterogeneous. Previous investigations of this heterogeneity have ordinarily used questionnaires and have rarely investigated sensory processing in typical development (TD) alongside autism spectrum development (ASD).
Methods
The present study used hierarchical clustering in a large sample to identify subgroups of young autistic and typically developing children based on the normalized global field power (GFP) of their event-related potentials (ERPs) to auditory stimuli of four different loudness intensities (50, 60, 70, 80 dB SPL): that is, based on an index of the relative strengths of their neural responses across these loudness conditions.
Results
Four clusters of participants were defined. Normalized GFP responses to sounds of different intensities differed strongly across clusters. There was considerable overlap in cluster assignments of autistic and typically developing participants, but autistic participants were more likely to display a pattern of relatively linear increases in response strength accompanied by a disproportionately strong response to 70 dB stimuli. Autistic participants displaying this pattern trended towards obtaining higher scores on assessments of cognitive abilities. There was also a trend for typically developing participants to disproportionately fall into a cluster characterized by disproportionately/nonlinearly strong 60 dB responses. Greater auditory distractibility was reported among autistic participants in a cluster characterized by disproportionately strong responses to the loudest (80 dB) sounds, and furthermore, relatively strong responses to loud sounds were correlated with auditory distractibility. This appears to provide evidence of coinciding behavioral and neural sensory atypicalities.
Limitations
Replication may be needed to verify exploratory results. This analysis does not address variability related to classical ERP latencies and topographies. The sensory questionnaire employed was not specifically designed for use in autism. Hearing acuity was not measured. Variability in sensory responses unrelated to loudness is not addressed, leaving room for additional research.
Conclusions
Taken together, these data demonstrate the broader benefits of using electrophysiology to explore individual differences. They illuminate different neural response patterns and suggest relationships between sensory neural responses and sensory behaviors, cognitive abilities, and autism diagnostic status.
Keywords: Autism, Heterogeneity, Hierarchical clustering, Event-related potentials (ERPs), Subgroups, Sensory processing
Background
Differential sensory processing in autism spectrum development (ASD)1 has historically been under-recognized and under-studied. Sensory processing was not listed as a clinical symptom of autism until the release of DSM-5 in 2013 [2]. However, abundant evidence has emerged to highlight the relevance of differential sensory processing in ASD (see [3, 4] for reviews). For example, there is evidence of hyperacusis in autism [5–8]. The impact of these sensory differences depends upon autistic people’s environments [9], almost always designed with typical development (TD) in mind. Thus, differential sensory processing in ASD is associated with participation in activities [10, 11], adaptive functioning [12–15], anxiety and other affective symptoms [16–19], and even quality of life [20].
Heterogeneity in autistic sensory processing
Sensory processing in ASD is also extremely heterogeneous [21]. First-person accounts demonstrate this diversity within the auditory domain. One autistic individual describes hypersensitivity to sudden, uncontrollable loud sounds like balloons and alarms (p. 69) [22]. Another reports struggling to tolerate soft echoing sounds in large spaces, like the rustling of papers (p. 74) [23], while yet another complains of how the sustained noise of crowds in hallways “seemed to make everything around me ‘echo’” [24]. Thus, different individuals have different patterns of auditory hyper-sensitivity and sensory interests, as well as unique experiences in response to different sounds. Furthermore, autistic people report that their individual sensory experiences can vary based on the degree to which the perceiver can predict or control the sensory stimuli, as well as the perceiver’s levels of anxiety and stress [25, 26]. These illustrations make clear the need for research which takes the heterogeneity of sensory processing in ASD into account.
Caregiver- and self-report questionnaires
In recent years, researchers have started to use caregiver-report questionnaires to explore this heterogeneity, including studies which have clustered data from caregiver-report measures to identify sensory subtypes in ASD (for a systematic review, see [27]). However, two limitations of these studies are that they do not yield information about the neural processes that underlie sensory experiences and that caregivers cannot know directly what their children experience (see also [22], pp. 80–83). Indeed, although self-reported and parent-reported auditory hypersensitivities are significantly correlated in autistic adolescents without intellectual disabilities [28], the relatively low magnitude of the correlation coefficient (.49) obtained between parent- and self-reports on the same measure suggests that there are substantial differences between reporters.
Neurophysiology
At present, relatively few studies using EEG or magnetoencephalography to investigate sensory processing in ASD have investigated heterogeneity. No published neurophysiological studies have attempted to separate autistic individuals into subgroups based neural markers of sensory processing, although some studies have reported associations between neurophysiological responses and various other variables in ASD [29–34], including other measurements of sensory processing, such as questionnaires [35–39].
Of the different ERP paradigms used to study autistic sensory processing, the presentation of tones of differing loudness (as used by [40, 41]), may hold particular promise in the exploration of heterogeneity. This approach was recently used to explore heterogeneity through the identification of differences between autistic participants with and without disproportionate megalencephaly [42]. Reports that some autistic people experience sensitivities to relatively soft sounds, while others describe greater sensitivity to louder sounds, suggest that exploring brain responses to stimuli of different loudness could reveal important information about individual differences in sensory processing in ASD.
Heterogeneity in both ASD and typical sensory processing
It is difficult to fully interpret and contextualize the heterogeneity of sensory processing in ASD without also having some understanding of the heterogeneity of sensory processing in TD—and, in some ways, we know less about sensory heterogeneity in TD than ASD. Little and colleagues [43] explored sensory subtypes using parent reports in both autistic and typically developing participants and using self-reports; Elwin et al. [44] also characterized sensory subtypes in the general population and in ASD. These existing studies seem to suggest the existence of a single relatively “typical” subgroup dominated by typically developing participants (though including some autistic individuals), as well as an unclear number of additional subtypes that include more autistic participants. However, this literature remains limited.
Present study
The present study is part of the Autism Phenome Project (APP) at the UC Davis MIND Institute, a large interdisciplinary study that aims to define subtypes of ASD based on behavioral, biochemical, and neurobiological indices. As part of the APP, electrophysiological responses to auditory stimuli of different intensities were collected from large numbers of participants at the first time-point after study entry. The present study aims to begin exploring neural heterogeneity in autistic and typical sensory processing by using these responses to define clusters of children with similar patterns of intensity-dependent auditory processing. In this study, the autistic and typically developing groups are combined and clustered in the same analysis so that sensory responses in each diagnostic group can be situated and understood in relation to one another. Furthermore, clusters are statistically compared to determine whether levels of different measured variables (such as cognitive ability and caregiver-reported sensory behavior) differ across clusters.
While the present study is exploratory, we made several predictions. First, as the clustering technique explicitly aims to group participants based on relative electrophysiological responses to stimuli of differing intensity, we expect that clusters will differ in their profiles of responses to stimuli of differing intensity. Second, owing to the existence of autistic individuals with sensory processing scores in the TD range in prior work (e.g., [43]), we anticipate that there will be substantial overlap between autistic and typically developing participants in the clusters defined in the analysis. Third, despite the substantial overlap predicted by the second hypothesis, we expect that there will be some separation of autistic and typically developing participants across clusters. Although research suggests that inter-individual variability in intensity-dependent cortical electrophysiological responses is considerable even in TD (e.g., [45, 46]), averaged response patterns in TD show monotonic increases of neural response strength with stimulus intensity [47]. In line with the patterns of subgroups found in questionnaire-based investigations of sensory heterogeneity in ASD and TD [43, 44], it would seem reasonable to anticipate that autistic participants might be more likely to display responses diverging from this monotopic grand-average pattern: that is, autistic participants might fall in clusters with unexpectedly strong responses to weak stimuli or displaying unexpectedly large increases in response strength to loud stimuli. This would also be consistent with some autistic people’s descriptions of particular subjective sensitivity towards the sounds of different loudness. Fourth, we expect that parents of participants in clusters with unexpectedly strong or weak responses to stimuli of particular intensities will report more atypical auditory sensory behaviors. Conversely, we predict that participants in clusters characterized by a relationship between stimulus and neural response strength similar to the overall grand-average will be rated as showing more typical sensory behaviors.
A considerable number of measures were collected as part of the APP. Given that anxiety [16–19], adaptive functioning [12–15], and cognitive ability [48, 49] have all been associated with autistic sensory processing in previous studies, and in light of the extensive literature documenting relationships between chronological age and electrophysiological responses in TD, these variables were also explored in this study.
Methods
Participants
As part of the APP, attempts were made to collect electrophysiological data from a total of 243 autistic and 96 typically developing children. All autistic participants met DSM-IV/Collaborative Programs for Excellence in Autism criteria for a Pervasive Developmental Disorder and passed cut-off scores on the ADOS-G [50] and, for either Social or Communication subscales, on the ADI-R [51]. Further details regarding the APP and participant recruitment can be found in previous publications (e.g., [52, 53]). Some participants were excluded from the present study due to noisy data, an insufficient number of acceptable-quality trials (< 400), an excessive number of excluded or poor-quality channels (> 6–7), or the presence of neuroanatomical abnormalities revealed by magnetic resonance imaging collected in the APP. One participant entered the study in the typically developing group but was diagnosed with autism at a later APP time-point; this participant’s data are also excluded. The final sample of children with usable electrophysiological data included a total of 81 typically developing participants (52 male) and 132 autistic participants (111 male) (Table 1). Families received a gift card in return for their participation in the study.
Table 1.
TD | ASD | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Mean (SD) | Range | Mean (SD) | Range | |
Chronological age (months) | 37.09 (6.46) | 25.80–56.33 | 38.54 (6.02) | 25.50–54.87 |
MSEL Developmental Quotient (DQ) | 106.36 (11.57) | 79.89–128.62 | 64.83 (20.49) | 30.39–132.45 |
MSEL Verbal DQ (VDQ) | 107.97 (12.70) | 81.26–149.47 | 58.40 (25.55) | 19.31–127.98 |
MSEL Non-Verbal DQ (NVDQ) | 104.75 (13.86) | 71.49–129.96 | 71.26 (18.41) | 36.39–136.93 |
VABS Adaptive Behaviour Composite | 111.22 (12.00) | 82.00–135.00 | 75.35 (11.00) | 53.00–104.00 |
Measures
Mullen Scales of Early Learning
Cognitive and communication assessments, as well as a number of caregiver-report questionnaires, were collected from participants as part of the APP. These measures included the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) [54], a standardized measure of cognitive and motor functioning for children under the age of 68 months. Four MSEL subscales were administered: visual reception, fine motor, expressive language, and receptive language. A ratio developmental quotient (DQ) was calculated (as (mental age/chronological age)×100) for full-scale performance, as well as for verbal (VDQ) and nonverbal (NVDQ) performance. MSEL data were available from all 132 autistic participants with usable electrophysiological data, and for 80 of the 81 typically developing participants.
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales
These measures also included the parent-report form of the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition (VABS-II) [55], a rating scale designed for the assessment of adaptive functioning in populations with developmental disabilities. A standardized composite adaptive behavior score can be calculated. Complete VABS data were available from 117 ASD (96 male) and 69 TD participants (43 male).
Short Sensory Profile
The Short Sensory Profile (SSP) [56] was also collected. The SSP is a 38-item caregiver-report questionnaire that has been used in a number of studies to investigate and characterize autistic sensory processing (e.g., [18, 19, 48, 57, 58]). Higher scores reflect relatively typical sensory behaviors, whereas lower scores are indicative of atypicality. Complete SSP data were available from 99 of the 132 autistic participants, while partial data were available from 108 autistic participants (90 male). Complete SSP data were only available from 65 of the 81 typically developing participants, while some SSP subscales were available from a total of 66 (43 male). In addition to the original seven SSP subscales developed based on a typically-developing sample [56], two studies have explored SSP factors in samples of autistic children [59, 60]. In the present study, SSP items related to auditory sensory processing were of particular interest. Therefore, the nine-factor solution developed by Williams et al. [60], which specifically distinguishes behaviors reflecting auditory sensory sensitivity from those reflecting visual sensitivity, was selected. Given their relevance to the auditory domain, Williams et al.’s subscales for noise distress, auditory distractibility, and hyporesponsiveness to speech were examined in this study, along with the total score on all 38 SSP items.
Childhood Behavior Checklist
The preschool-age form of the Childhood Behavior Checklist (CBCL) was also collected [61]. This 100-item caregiver-report questionnaire aims to assess problematic internalizing and externalizing behaviors in young children. Of particular interest in the present study is the DSM-oriented anxiety problems subscale, given the previous reports of relationships between autistic sensory processing and anxiety. This subscale yields both a raw score and a normed T-score. CBCL DSM-oriented anxiety T-scores were available from 126 autistic (106 male) and 75 typically-developing (48 male) participants.
Electroencephalography (EEG) task
The experimental setup and data collection have been previously described in [42]. Briefly, while seated on a caregiver’s lap in a dimly lit room, participants passively listened to 50 ms (including 5 ms rise and decay time) complex tones (sine waves of equal amplitude overlaid at the following 7 frequencies (musical notes): 249 Hz (B3); 616 Hz (D5), 788 Hz (G5), 1042 Hz (C6), 1410 Hz (F6), 1952 Hz (B6), and 2749 Hz (F7)) randomly presented at different intensities (50 dB, 60 dB, 70 dB, and 80 dB SPL) with a 1–2 s random ISI. The tones were presented via Sony MDR-222KD child-size headphones calibrated with a B&K artificial ear (model 4153) and sound meter (model 2229). Approximately ~1100–1200 trials were collected from each participant. Throughout the EEG recording, children watched a quiet video of their or their caregiver’s choice. Breaks were included as required, including briefly pausing the delivery of stimuli to suit child comfort.
EEG data acquisition and processing
EEG data were collected with a 61-channel EASYCAP system [62] using a Compumedics Neuroscan Synamp II amplifier. Data were sampled at a rate of 1000 Hz with Cz as a reference. Data were then average-referenced and band-pass filtered with a low cutoff of 0.4 Hz offline (12 dB/octave roll-off). Epochs (spanning −200 ms to 900 ms, including 300 ms necessary for subsequent lagged correlations within an independent component analysis step) were screened and extreme amplitudes removed using the artifact scan tool of BESA 5.2 [63]. On average, in the ASD group, 23% of trials were removed in this process, compared to 19% in the TD group (see also Table 2). Given the study’s goal of exploring heterogeneity and individual differences in electrophysiological data, we sought to maximize the ERP signal-to-noise ratio by removing putatively non-neural signal sources from the data. To accomplish this, the remaining epochs were submitted to Second-Order Blind source Identification (SOBI; see [64, 65]). We used a semi-automatic artifact removal tool (SMART, [66]) to identify signal sources from SOBI that were interpreted, on the basis of signal source topography, spectra, autocorrelation and time series, to be of non-neural origin (such as electromyography/EMG, electrooculography/EOG, and blinks). Additional details regarding artifact removal using SOBI and SMART are discussed in [67]. Artifact-free trials were then reconstructed from the putatively neural SOBI signal sources and separate averages for each of the four loudness conditions were computed for each subject. Data from excluded channels were then interpolated using a 3-dimensional spline [68]. Epochs (now spanning 100 ms pre-stimulus onset to 600 ms post-stimulus onset) were filtered (second-order Butterworth with −12 dB/octave roll/off; 0.1 Hz high-pass; 40 Hz low-pass; 60 Hz notch) and baseline-corrected using the pre-stimulus period with the Cartool software [69].
Table 2.
50 dB | 60 dB | 70 dB | 80 dB | |
---|---|---|---|---|
ASD | 220.43 (50.50) | 211.54 (52.00) | 224.76 (49.93) | 216.32 (49.97) |
TD | 240.07 (53.51) | 229.49 (54.05) | 244.00 (54.35) | 234.63 (53.07) |
The global field power (GFP) was used as an index of the strength of the brain’s response for analysis of the electrophysiological data. The GFP is computed as the standard deviation of all electrode channels per sample, and because the overall spatial distribution and gradients of the electrical potential across the montage are independent of the reference electrode, the GFP is reference-independent. Thus, GFP can be regarded as an index of the strength of the brain’s response to the stimulus, with higher GFP values reflecting a stronger neural response overall, independent of the spatial distribution of the electrocortical response [70]. We deemed this study’s focus on GFP appropriate because it allows for an entirely data-driven analysis, based on an index of the total strength of the neural response, that is not dependent upon the a priori selection of ERP components and electrode sites of interest. This may be of particular value in samples with young and neurodivergent children, some of whom may have unusual or idiosyncratic neural responses.
To ensure that the clustering and correlation analyses focused explicitly on differences between loudness conditions (i.e., loudness-dependency) while ignoring the between participant absolute strength of the electrophysiological response across conditions, GFP for each participant in each condition was normalized. In the normalization process, each participant’s GFP value at each time-point in each condition was divided by that participant’s average GFP across all four conditions at the same time-point (see Fig. 1). Thus, the normalized GFP represents the strength of the neural response in a particular condition relative to the other conditions. It should be noted that this process should dramatically reduce the influence of non-neural factors that could contribute to individual differences in electrophysiological responses, such as the thickness of a participant’s skull [71].
Hierarchical clustering analysis
The time windows used in the clustering analysis were defined by the criterion of ± 85% peak latency of the grand average of the raw GFP across groups. That is, in every loudness condition, separately, these were the time windows spanning the two time-points on either side of the raw GFP peak where raw GFP values were equal to 85% of the peak raw GFP. These windows, therefore, bracket the period of the strongest neural response. These windows corresponded to 101–152 ms for 50 dB; 90–141 ms for 60 dB; 79–133 ms for 70 dB; and 79–120 ms for 80 dB. Prior literature and visual inspection suggest that these periods correspond to the auditory P1, a large cortical auditory event-related response observed around ~100–150 ms in young children [72–74].
Next, the normalized GFP in the windows defined by 85% fractional peak latency was entered into a hierarchical clustering analysis. In this analysis, the normalized GFP values of each participant from each time-point in each loudness condition were clustered in R according to Ward’s agglomerative hierarchical method and visualized using heatmaps. Ward’s method aims to identify clusters based on distance in multivariate Euclidean space, by minimizing the variance within each cluster. That is, in Ward’s method, pairs of clusters are selected for merging based on the criterion that their merger should make the smallest possible increase in within-cluster variability. Initially, each cluster is a single participant, but clusters are then merged until all participants are grouped together. This generates a dendrogram which reflects the hierarchy of the clusters; participants appearing together in the lower branches are relatively less distant from (or more similar to) each other, in terms of the pattern of differences between loudness conditions, than participants that are widely separated in the dendrogram. Similar to K-means, Ward’s method utilizes object function- minimization of the within-cluster sum of squared error. However, Ward’s appears to be slightly more accurate than K-means in finding the number of clusters in datasets and uncovering relations among the clusters [75, 76]. Furthermore, in relation to the alternative hierarchical approach of single-linkage clustering, Ward’s method appears less susceptible to noise and less likely to produce elongated clusters [76, 77], although complete-linkage clustering has been shown to yield results more similar to Ward’s method [75].
While clustering solutions are often selected on the basis of an optimization algorithm, Fushing and McAssey [78] demonstrate that the question of exactly how many clusters exist in a dataset is ill-posed. At least within (multi-)dimensional data clouds (rather than more naturally categorical data clouds with convex, well-separated clusters), different clusters can be said to exist at different hierarchical levels; it is hard to see how any single level necessarily yields the only valid, optimal solution. When clustering is used as a technique for exploring and describing dimensional data (as the authors believe loudness-dependent auditory responses should be regarded), a better question might be the question of what hierarchical level allows for the clearest description of the present dataset for the purposes of the present study. As such, the level of the dendrogram used for the determination of final cluster groups was manually selected based on the hierarchies formed in this analysis. A level of yielding four clusters was chosen (see Fig. 2). To further validate our clusters and offer at least a preliminary examination of their replicability albeit within the limits of the present dataset, subsamples of participants were repeatedly drawn and probabilities of reassignment alongside other participants from each original cluster were calculated (see Supplementary Methods and Results, Figures S1–S2).
Statistical comparisons of clusters.
A chi-square test of homogeneity was used to determine whether autistic participants were statistically more likely to fall into different clusters than typically developing participants, as well as vice versa. Omnibus effects were probed using adjusted standardized residuals (ASRs), which were obtained from each cell of the chi-square table and corrected for multiple comparisons using the Bonferroni-Holm procedure. With two diagnostic groups, comparisons in any row of the chi-square table perfectly mirrored one another, so the number of possible comparisons equaled the number of clusters.
To statistically compare the normalized GFP patterns found in different clusters, a parametric mixed ANOVA with loudness and cluster as factors was fitted. Interaction effects were explored further with follow-up one-way parametric ANOVAs separately in the 50 dB, 60 dB, 70 dB, and 80 dB conditions; results were corrected for multiple comparisons using the Bonferroni-Holm procedure. Afterwards, Welch’s t-tests, with a Bonferroni-Holm correction, were used to probe significant one-way ANOVA results. All results that reached significance prior to correction for multiple comparisons are reported with their corrected p values, but results that did not attain statistical significance before or after correction for multiple comparisons are not reported. Generalized eta-squared () and Cohen’s d were used to estimate effect size.
Finally, to compare clusters and diagnostic groups on measured variables such as DQ and SSP scores, two-way between-groups ANOVA was used, unless assumptions of this parametric test were violated. If assumptions were violated, one-way Kruskal-Wallis tests were used, with no multiple comparison correction, to compare clusters separately in each diagnostic group. Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney tests with Bonferroni-Holm corrections were used to determine the significance of post-hoc comparisons, which are reported using the effect size δ [79].
Contiguity-based permutation correlation analyses
Due to the limitations of analyses involving comparison of levels of continuous variables across categorical subgroups defined on the basis of other continuous variables (see [80]), continuous associations were examined between normalized GFP and those variables which significantly differed across clusters in each diagnostic group, namely (as discussed below in the results): SSP total scores, SSP auditory distractibility scores, MSEL DQ, and MSEL NVDQ in ASD, as well as MSEL VDQ in TD. In each loudness condition, and at each time-point between 79 ms and 152 ms—that is, each time point falling into one of the windows used for the clustering analyses—Spearman’s rank-based correlation coefficient was used to examine the association between normalized GFP values and these variables. No multiple comparison correction was used to control for the number of different variables examined, so these tests are clearly exploratory. However, a contiguity-based permutation testing approach2 [81] was used to correct for the multiple comparisons entailed by separately examining effects at numerous consecutive time-points. First, all test statistics exceeding an initial two-tailed probability threshold of .05 were grouped into different temporally contiguous series of time points. Obtained statistics from these contiguous series were summed. To determine final statistical significance, summed statistics from the obtained contiguous series were compared against the null distribution of summed contiguous series statistics generated through 10,000 random permutations of the data. Essentially, this procedure discards any associations that are not stronger and/or more temporally sustained than would be expected from chance alone.
Results
Hierarchical clustering analysis
Grand-averaged raw and normalized GFP across both groups are depicted in Fig. 1, visual inspection of which shows that intensity systematically modulated neural responses, as well as supplementary Figures S3–S6, while the results of the hierarchical clustering analysis are displayed in Fig. 2. Groups are referred to as “C1” (referring to “Cluster 1”) through “C4”. Generally, analyses of cluster replicability (albeit within the present dataset) based on drawing and clustering repeated subsamples suggest the present clusters are fairly replicable, with participants being substantially more likely to be clustered alongside other participants from their original clusters than alongside those outside their original clusters (Supplementary Tables S2–S3), although it should be noted that one subgroup of participants within C1 showed some propensity to move back and forth between C1 and C2 on resampling (Supplementary Figures S1–S2).
Diagnostic group membership
The distributions of autistic and typically-developing participants across the clusters differed significantly, X2 (3, N = 213) = 8.42, p = .04 (Table 3). C1 had significantly more autistic participants than expected based on the proportion of ASD participants in the study, ASR = 2.70, corrected p = .03. Furthermore, at a trend level, C3 had more typically developing participants than expected, ASR = 2.18, corrected p = .06.
Table 3.
C1 | C2 | C3 | C4 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
ASD | 53 (74.65%) | 24 (58.54%) | 32 (50.79%) | 23 (60.53%) |
TD | 18 (25.35%) | 17 (41.46%) | 31 (49.21%) | 15 (39.47%) |
Cluster electrophysiological patterns
There was a significant between-subjects main effect of the cluster on normalized GFP, F(3, 209) = 3.86, p = .01, = .003, there was a significant within-subjects effect of loudness, F(3, 627) = 78.23, p < .0001, = .26, and there was a significant cluster by loudness interaction, F(9, 627) = 55.09, p < .0001, = .43 (Figs. 3 and 4). This robust interaction confirmed that the hierarchical clustering analysis succeeded in defining clusters that differed in the loudness-dependency of their electrophysiological responses to auditory stimuli.
In the 50 dB condition, normalized GFP significantly differed across the clusters, F(3, 209) = 49.09, corrected p < .0001, = .41. The brain’s electrophysiological response to 50 dB sounds was significantly stronger in C4 than in any other cluster group (compared to C1, Welch’s t(96.90) = 13.91, corrected p < .0001, d = 2.55; compared to C2, Welch’s t(63.42) = −11.36, corrected p < .0001, d = 2.50; and compared to C3, Welch’s t(98.99) = 10.58, corrected p < .0001, d = 1.93). There was also a trend for a stronger 50 dB response in C3 compared to C2, but this was not significant after applying the Holm-Bonferroni correction, Welch’s t(81.40) = 2.45, corrected p = .11, d = 0.50.
In the 60 dB condition, normalized GFP significantly differed across the clusters, F(3, 209) = 85.54, corrected p < .0001, = .55. The brain’s electrophysiological response to 60 dB sounds was significantly stronger in C3 than any other group (compared to C1, Welch’s t(131.85) = 10.04, corrected p < .0001, d = 1.72; compared to C2, Welch’s t(76.02) = 14.30, corrected p < .0001, d = 2.97; and compared to C4, Welch’s t(83.34) = 12.91, corrected p < .0001, d = 2.60). Furthermore, the brain’s response to 60 dB sounds was stronger in C1 than C2, Welch’s t(83.44) = 5.64, corrected p < .0001, d = 1.11, and it was stronger in C1 than C4, Welch’s t(92.12) = 3.15, corrected p = .02, d = 0.59. A trend for the 60 dB response to be stronger in C4 than C2 did not survive post-hoc correction, Welch’s t(75.05) = 2.77, corrected p = .06, d = 0.62.
In the 70 dB condition, normalized GFP significantly differed across the clusters, F(3, 209) = 45.57, corrected p < .0001, = .40. The brain’s electrophysiological response to 70 dB sounds was significantly stronger in C1 than any other group (compared to C2, Welch’s t(88.25) = 5.48, corrected p < .0001, d = 1.06; compared to C3, Welch’s t(124.08) = 11.90, corrected p < .0001, d = 2.01; and compared to C4, Welch’s t(82.60) = 6.62, corrected p < .0001, d = 1.29). Furthermore, the brain’s response to 70 dB sounds was significantly weaker in C3 than in C2, Welch’s t(67.23) = –4.20, corrected p = .0009, d = −0.90, and the 70 dB response was significantly weaker in C3 than C4, Welch’s t(62.34) = −2.91, corrected p = .05, d = −0.64.
In the 80 dB condition, normalized GFP significantly differed across the clusters, F(3, 209) = 42.00, corrected p < .0001, = .38. The brain’s electrophysiological response to 80 dB sounds was significantly stronger in C2 than any other group (compared to C1, Welch’s t(76.23) = 9.74, corrected p < .0001, d = 1.97; compared to C3, Welch’s t(87.61) = 9.13, corrected p < .0001, d = 1.82; and compared to C4, Welch’s t(74.44) = 9.13, corrected p < .0001, d = 2.06). These effects were robust to the removal of the three outlying participants in C2 (based on the criterion of 3× the median absolute deviation) visible in Fig. 3.
Measures and demographics
Caregiver-reported sensory symptoms
Total scores on the SSP showed a non-normal distribution, Shapiro-Wilk W = .96, p = .0003, as did scores on the three SSP auditory factors defined by Williams et al. [60], Shapiro-Wilk p ≤ .0001. Furthermore, combining across clusters, autistic participants had significantly lower (i.e., more atypical) SSP total scores than typically developing participants, Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney W = 746.5, nASD = 99, nTD = 65, p < .0001, δ = −.88, and the same pattern was observed with all three auditory subscores, p ≤ .008. Therefore, one-way non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis tests were used to compare clusters on SSP scores and subscores separately in each diagnostic group. Among autistic participants, total SSP scores differed between clusters, H(3) = 8.67, p = .03 (Table 4, Fig. 5a). Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney tests indicated that sensory processing trended towards being more atypical in C2 than C3, W = 108.5, nC2 = 18, nC3 = 23, corrected p = .06, δ = −.48. Furthermore, among autistic participants, scores on the SSP Auditory Distractibility factor significantly differed between clusters, H(3) = 10.31, p = .02 (Fig. 5b). Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney tests indicated that more auditory distractibility was reported in C2 than C4, W = 90.5, nC2 = 19, nC4 = 19, Bonferroni-Holm corrected p = .05, δ = −.50; there was also a trend for more auditory distractibility in C2 than C3, W = 139.5, nC2 = 19, nC3 = 26, corrected p = .07, δ = −.44. Trends in the ASD sample for effects of the cluster on Noise Distress did not attain significance. In the typically developing sample, one-way Kruskal-Wallis tests revealed no significant differences between clusters on the SSP total score or any of the auditory subscores (Supplementary Table S1).
Table 4.
Cluster means (standard deviations) | Kruskal-Wallis test | Cliff’s δ1 | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
C1 | C2 | C3 | C4 | H(3) | p | C1, C2 | C1, C3 | C1, C4 | C2, C3 | C2, C4 | C3, C4 | |
Auditory Distractibility | 11.15 (2.56) | 10.21 (2.70) | 12.19 (1.92) | 12.47 (2.37) | 10.31 | .02 | .21 | −.23 | −.31 | −.44 | −.50* | −.13 |
Hyporesponsiveness to Speech | 4.81 (1.52) | 4.38 (2.11) | 4.65 (1.38) | 5.21 (1.65) | 3.89 | .27 | .22 | .05 | −.10 | −.19 | −.35 | −.15 |
Noise Distress | 7.19 (2.33) | 6.24 (2.61) | 8.08 (1.79) | 7.68 (1.92) | 7.21 | .07 | .22 | −.21 | −.10 | −.43 | −.34 | .13 |
Total SSP Score | 135.08 (20.08) | 128.50 (24.51) | 145.43 (16.69) | 141.42 (18.75) | 8.67 | .03 | .19 | −.30 | −.21 | −.48 | −.37 | .15 |
1Values of δ are indicated with * if corresponding the Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney p value is < .05 after Bonferroni-Holm correction for six comparisons
Chronological age
There was no main effect of cluster group, F(3, 204) = 1.02, p = .38, = .01, or diagnostic group, F(1, 204) = 2.19, p = .14, = .01 on chronological age, nor was there an interaction of cluster and diagnostic groups, F(3, 204) = 0.90, p = .44, = .01.
Cognitive ability
MSEL DQ had a non-normal distribution in ASD, Shapiro-Wilk W = .96, p = .0008. Furthermore, autistic participants had significantly lower DQ than typically developing participants (Table 1). Therefore, one-way non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis tests were used to compare clusters on DQ, VDQ, and NVDQ scores separately in each diagnostic group. Among autistic participants, DQ scores differed between clusters, H(3) = 8.28, p = .04 (Table 5, Fig. 5c). Similar effects were found in NVDQ scores, and a similar trend was seen in VDQ scores, although in each case post-hoc comparisons failed to locate significant differences after strict corrections for multiple comparisons were applied. Among typically developing participants, VDQ scores significantly differed between clusters (Table 6, Fig. 5d), H(3) = 9.08, p = .03; after correction, scores in C2 were lower than C4, W = 56.0, nC2 = 17, nC3 = 15, corrected p = .04, δ = –.56. No effects of full-scale DQ and NVDQ were observed in TD.
Table 5.
Cluster means (Standard deviations) | Kruskal-Wallis test | Cliff’s δ1 | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
C1 | C2 | C3 | C4 | H(3) | p | C1, C2 | C1, C3 | C1, C4 | C2, C3 | C2, C4 | C3, C4 | |
MSEL DQ | 71.89 (23.96) | 56.73 (14.31) | 60.88 (16.92) | 62.54 (17.50) | 8.28 | .04 | .37 | .25 | .22 | −.11 | −.19 | −.05 |
MSEL NVDQ | 77.38 (21.21) | 64.83 (12.81) | 68.16 (17.16) | 68.19 (14.43) | 9.00 | .03 | .36 | .28 | .27 | −.10 | −.11 | −.11 |
MSEL VDQ | 66.40 (29.09) | 48.63 (18.92) | 53.59 (22.05) | 56.87 (23.11) | 7.64 | .05 | .35 | .25 | .19 | −.13 | −.19 | −.08 |
1Values of δ are indicated with * if corresponding the Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney p value is < .05 after Bonferroni-Holm correction for six comparisons
Table 6.
Cluster means (Standard deviations) | Kruskal-Wallis test | Cliff’s δ1 | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
C1 | C2 | C3 | C4 | H(3) | p | C1, C2 | C1, C3 | C1, C4 | C2, C3 | C2, C4 | C3, C4 | |
MSEL DQ | 106.41 (11.80) | 100.30 (11.75) | 108.63 (10.75) | 108.63 (11.33) | 5.62 | .13 | .24 | −.13 | −.13 | −.39 | −.41 | −.01 |
MSEL NVDQ | 101.73 (13.13) | 100.22 (13.29) | 107.65 (13.08) | 107.71 (15.99) | 4.72 | .19 | .08 | −.23 | −.25 | −.33 | −.30 | −.03 |
MSEL VDQ | 111.09 (15.53) | 100.38 (12.55) | 109.62 (11.31) | 109.55 (9.05) | 9.08 | .03 | .45 | .02 | −.01 | −.45 | −.56* | −.02 |
1Values of δ are indicated with * if corresponding the Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney p value is < .05 after Bonferroni-Holm correction for six comparisons
Adaptive behavior
Although the overall distribution of VABS composite scores was non-normal, distributions did not violate Shapiro-Wilk tests in each group separately (p ≥ .07). Therefore, one-way ANOVAs were used to compare clusters on VABS composite scores separately in each diagnostic group. VABS scores did not differ between clusters in the ASD sample, F(3, 113) = 2.04, p = .11. Among typically developing participants, the VABS composite did not differ between clusters, F(3, 65) = 1.48, p = .23.
Anxiety
CBCL DSM-oriented anxiety T-scores had a non-normal distribution, Shapiro-Wilk W = .63, p < .0001. Furthermore, autistic participants had significantly higher anxiety levels than typically developing participants, Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney W = 6043.5, nASD = 126, nTD = 75, p = .0003, δ = .28. Therefore, one-way non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis tests were used to compare clusters on anxiety scores separately in each diagnostic group. Anxiety levels did not differ between clusters among autistic participants, H(3) = 0.25, p = .97, or typically developing participants, H(3) = 0.97, p = .81.
Cluster-based permutation correlation analyses
Caregiver-reported sensory symptoms
In ASD, there was a significant negative Spearman correlation between SSP total scores and normalized GFP to 70 dB sounds in a contiguous series of time points spanning between 97 and 131 ms, p = .009: that is, autistic participants with strong responses to 70 dB sounds in this approximate time period had, overall, more atypical caregiver-reported sensory processing features (Fig. 6a). There were no significant associations between SSP total scores and normalized GFP in any other loudness condition in ASD.
In ASD, there was a significant positive correlation between SSP auditory distractibility scores and normalized GFP to 50 dB sounds in a contiguous series of points spanning between 87 and 128 ms, p = .01 (Fig. 6b). There was no significant association between SSP auditory distractibility scores and normalized GFP to 60 dB sounds in ASD. However, there was a significant negative association between SSP auditory distractibility scores and normalized GFP to 70 dB sounds between 101 and 132 ms, p = .02. Furthermore, in ASD, there was a significant negative association between SSP auditory distractibility scores and normalized GFP to 80 dB sounds between 79 and 114 ms, p = .02. In other words, autistic participants with relatively weak responses to soft 50 dB sounds and relatively strong responses to louder 70 dB and 80 dB sounds were reported by caregivers to have more auditory distractibility problems.
Cognitive ability
In ASD, there were no significant associations between normalized GFP and MSEL DQ or MSEL NVDQ in any loudness condition or diagnostic group. Furthermore, in TD, there were no significant associations between normalized GFP and MSEL VDQ in any loudness condition or diagnostic group.
Discussion
Clustering and individual differences research with ERPs
This study demonstrates that it is possible to use the hierarchical clustering of electrophysiological data to group autistic and typically developing participants into clusters based on the loudness-dependency of their brain responses to auditory tones. As predicted by the first hypothesis, with normalized GFP as a dependent variable, there were robust differences in the pattern of loudness dependency between clusters. Visual inspection of the averaged normalized GFP across clusters (Fig. 3) as well as the waveforms in each cluster (Fig. 4) clearly reveals large, inter-cluster differences in the magnitude of the brain’s electrophysiological responses to stimuli of differing loudness. Thus, the clustering method was able to identify what we take to be meaningful differences in loudness-dependent response profiles. Indeed, it is important to note, as discussed further below, that the clusters defined on the basis of ERP responses differed from one another in other variables as well: notably, diagnostic group, performance on cognitive measures, and caregiver-reported sensory symptoms, which emphasizes the meaningfulness of the clusters. Interestingly, however, the clusters did not differ in the chronological ages of their participants, which suggests that developmental changes in auditory evoked responses do not affect the loudness-dependency of overall response strength in the time window of the present study.
Indeed, it is not clear at this point what neural mechanism might be responsible for these individual differences in loudness-dependent neural responses. It has been suggested that individual differences in intensity-dependent N1/P2 auditory responses in adults reflect variation in serotonergic neurotransmission [82]. However, even in adults, empirical evidence regarding this hypothesis is mixed, as is evidence regarding associations between loudness-dependent auditory responses and dopamine [83]. Given the excitation-inhibition balance hypothesis of autism [84, 85], we are intrigued by the possibility that loudness-dependent responses might relate to neural excitation and inhibition.
Overlap between diagnostic groups
As predicted by the second hypothesis, there was considerable overlap between the diagnostic groups in the clusters defined in the present analysis. Although the proportions of autistic and typically developing participants did differ significantly across clusters, these differences were modest. All clusters contained substantial numbers of both autistic and typically developing participants. This overlap across diagnostic groups appears to emphasize the complexity of the individual differences in neural processes that underlie both the autistic and typically developing auditory processing.
Description of clusters and associations with other measures
The results of the present study are not consistent with the third hypothesis, which predicted that typically developing participants would be more likely to fall into clusters characterized by a pattern of neural responses increasing in strength monotonically as the loudness of the tones increased, while autistic participants might be more likely to appear in clusters with either non-monotonic patterns or unexpectedly strong increases in neural response strength with intensity. As can be seen in Fig. 3, the only groups which approximate monotonicity are C1 and C2. However, the only cluster in the observed data that trended to have a disproportionate number of typically developing participants was not C1 but C3, and contrary to prediction, C3’s participants actually displayed an unexpectedly strong, non-monotonic responses to softer, 60 dB sounds.
On the other hand, C1 had a disproportionately large number of autistic participants: this cluster exhibited a response to 70 dB sounds that was stronger than that found in any other cluster, as well as a stronger 60 dB response than C2 or C4. To further characterize this cluster, one can turn to the various measures and assessments collected in the APP. Curiously, although autistic participants were more likely to fall into C1 than other clusters, autistic participants in C1 did not appear to have more conspicuously atypical phenotypes than autistic participants in other clusters. Indeed, the data reveal the opposite. Among autistic participants, full-scale and nonverbal DQ on the MSEL did significantly differ between clusters, and a similar effect of verbal DQ fell just short of statistical significance. While results of follow-up tests no longer attained statistical significance after a robust correction for multiple comparisons, trends suggest that the significant effects were driven by higher scores in C1. Replication and further research are needed to confirm and fully understand these findings, in part because, as noted in supplementary materials, a subgroup of participants from C1 would often be reclassified into C2 when subsamples were repeatedly drawn and re-clustered. However, it is interesting to note that, in ASD, there were no significant continuous associations between MSEL scores and normalized GFP in any loudness condition. This perhaps suggests that the high cognitive abilities of autistic participants in C1 are driven not by the strength of their responses to any given intensity (such as their strong 70 dB response) but by their overall pattern of response monotonicity across loudness conditions.
Meanwhile, the other approximately monotonic cluster, C2, is characterized primarily by an extremely strong response to the very loudest, 80 dB stimuli. Autistic and typically developing participants appeared to have roughly equal probabilities of being classified into C2, but membership in C2 appeared to be linked to sensory behaviors. The fourth hypothesis suggested that auditory sensory behaviors would be more typical in clusters characterized by a pattern of neural responses increasing in strength in a monotonic fashion similar to the typical grand-average pattern, but C2 does not appear to meet these criteria. Responses appear roughly monotonic, but from visual inspection, the strength of the 80 dB response seems to greatly exceed the overall grand-average. Indeed, among autistic participants, exploratory analyses indicated that caregiver-reported problems with auditory distractibility were significantly greater in C2 than C4. Furthermore, although the post-hoc effect was no longer significant after correction for multiple comparisons, auditory distractibility problems strongly trended towards being greater among autistic participants in C2 than C3. Similarly, continuous associations suggested that auditory distractibility problems were greater in autistic participants exhibiting a pattern of relative neural hyper-responsiveness to loud 70 dB and 80 dB sounds and relative neural hypo-responsiveness to soft 50 dB sounds.
These findings should be interpreted with care owing to the ambiguity of the “auditory distractibility” factor; for example, it seems possible to interpret the SSP items loading on this factor (“Is distracted or has trouble functioning if there is a lot of noise around,” “Can’t work with background noise,” and “Has trouble completing tasks when the radio is on”) as signs of general difficulty functioning in noisy environments rather than distractibility per se. Thus, neural over-responsiveness to loud sounds in at least a subgroup of autistic participants appears to be related to abnormal behavior—distractibility and/or difficulty functioning—in noisy environments. The possibility that these results could be related to hyperacusis, which has been observed in ASD [5–8], appears intriguing. However, such an interpretation must be considered tentative as the present study does not assess loudness discomfort levels. Furthermore, similar neural responses in TD participants from C2 were not linked to any pattern of caregiver-reported auditory sensory behaviors, indicating that the presence of these relatively stronger responses to loud sounds is not a sufficient condition for auditory sensory sensitivity.
Verbal cognitive ability among typically developing participants was lower in C2 than C4, with similar but nonsignificant trends for VDQ to be lower in C2 than C1 and C3. However, although C2 appeared to be characterized by abnormally strong responses to 80 dB sounds, the mean VDQ differences between C2 and other clusters in TD were surprisingly not accompanied by significant continuous associations between normalized GFP to 80 dB sounds and MSEL VDQ in TD, complicating their interpretation. Replication and further research appear necessary before this effect can be confirmed and understood.
Limitations
The present study has a number of strengths. It is based on a large, well-characterized sample of autistic and typically developing participants drawn from a relatively narrow chronological age range. Many trials were collected from each participant, and data were subjected to an intensive processing pipeline, allowing us to have considerable confidence in individuals’ observed average responses in each loudness condition. Furthermore, the focus on loudness-dependent normalized responses allows the analysis to circumvent individual differences in biophysical factors such as skull thickness, while the use of GFP gives the study an overall metric of neural response strength that avoids the need for a priori decisions about analyzing particular components or electrode sites.
Along with these strengths, the present study has limitations. One is the use of a brief, 38-item caregiver report of sensory behaviors that was not originally designed for use in autism. Future studies could use other parent-report measures or, when participants’ verbal abilities permit, self-reports. It should also be noted that our analyses examined a large number of variables like MSEL DQ and SSP sensory behaviors in an exploratory manner.
Another limitation of the present study is that there is naturally a loss of information in any procedure (such as the clustering procedure employed here) that reduces the complexity of the data. To address this limitation, future analyses will cluster participants based on the latency of ERP responses. The present analysis also ignores the topographic distribution of neural responses. The authors plan in future analyses to define clusters not only based on timing but also on the topographic distribution of neural responses over pre-defined scalp regions.
We also acknowledge that the present study did not involve the collection of hearing acuity measures. This reflected the difficulty of obtaining reliable estimates of hearing acuity in young children, especially in groups with diverse language and cognitive abilities. However, prior research suggests that there is variability in hearing acuity within ASD [6, 7]. While the within-participants normalization of GFP across loudness intensities in the present study could offer some protection against any individual differences in overall hearing acuity, this procedure does not account for non-linearities in the relations among GFP strength, stimulus intensity, and hearing thresholds. It is thus unclear how hearing acuity might relate to the individual differences observed in this study.
More generally, one might argue that the ultimate goal of projects examining neural sensory responses—such as the present study—is to gather information related to the internal sensory experience of an individual [86]. However, the cortical auditory response from ~80–150 ms post-stimulus onset is only one contributor to these internal sensory experiences, which are likely to be emergent properties of many neural processes. Thus, two individuals with cortical neural responses of equal strength in the 80–150 ms window might nevertheless have different sensory experiences. Furthermore, one must consider that brief auditory complex tones differing in loudness are only one of the numerous types of stimuli that exist in the auditory domain alone. This should not be interpreted as an argument against electrophysiology and other neuroimaging techniques, but as a buttress to existing suggestions (e.g., by [21]) that multiple methods should be employed to understand individual differences in sensory experience.
Conclusions
We believe that this study demonstrates that clustering can be used to meaningfully describe the variability of neurophysiological event-related responses in sufficiently large datasets and that normalization of responses across conditions may be valuable in focusing on neural variations in response strength across these conditions. As confirmed by chi-square analysis, autistic and typically developing participants were distributed significantly differentially across clusters. Additional analyses suggested that participants in a cluster characterized by neural hyper-responsiveness to loud sounds exhibited auditory distractibility/filtering problems; similarly, in continuous associations, relative neural hyper-responsiveness to loud sounds was associated with auditory distractibility. Furthermore, autistic participants in a cluster characterized by relatively monotonic responses seemed to exhibit higher total cognitive abilities, although post-hoc comparisons fell short of significance after correction for multiple comparisons. We also observed the effects of cognitive ability in typical development, although these findings may require replication and further research to be understood. Overall, however, the existence of relationships between cluster membership and other variables suggests that the present clusters constitute and describe meaningful variability, though we draw no conclusion to the effect that the clusters represent categorically discrete populations. We suggest that clustering analyses similar to those used in this study may be valuable tools in future research describing additional dimensions of neural heterogeneity.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We wish to gratefully acknowledge all of the children and families who generously devoted considerable time and effort to participate in this large study, which included many other components besides ERP data collection. We warmly acknowledge the MIND Institute APP implementation and assessment team for their neuropsychological screening work and for coordinating the logistics of ERP appointments with participants’ families. We thank all of the research assistants and junior specialists for their help with EEG data collection and processing (including Sarah Abedi, Margarita Beransky, Costanza Columbi, Sam Cheyette, Sharon Corina, Tucker Fisher, Sevan K. Harootonian, David Horton, Ryan Hubbard, Anne Kalomiris, Sarabeth Maciey, Lindsey Marcelino, Joshua Martin, Saloni Mathur, Thomas McLennan, Tracy Riggins, and Ashley Stark). We also thank Manish Saggar and Iman Mohammadrezazadeh for software development, and Yukari Takarae for scientific support. We thank Dr. Tal Kenet, of the Harvard Medical School Department of Pediatric Neurology, who provided the stimuli used in this study. This study was funded by the UC Davis MIND Institute, by the Robert Shoes Fund, by Scott & Jennifer Fearon, by the UC Davis Deans’ Distinguished Graduate Fellowship, by the NIH (1R01 MH089626-01), the Swiss National Science Foundation (project P2LAP3_164911), the NIMH (U24MH081810), and by an Autism Center of Excellence grant awarded by the NICHD (P50 HD093079).
Abbreviations
- APP
-
Autism Phenome Project
ASD
Autism spectrum development
ASR
Adjusted standardized residual
CBCL
Childhood Behavior Checklist
DQ
Developmental quotient
EEG
Electroencephalography
RP
Event-related potential
GFP
Global field power
MSEL
Mullen Scales of Early Learning
NVDQ
Non-Verbal Developmental Quotient
SMART
Semi-Automatic Artifact Removal Tool
SOBI
Second-Order Blind source Identification
SSP
Short Sensory Profile
TD
Typical development
VABS
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales
VDQ
Verbal Developmental Quotient
Authors’ contributions
CDS and SMR were responsible for designing the electrophysiological experiment that forms the basis of the present analysis. RDM-M processed or supervised the processing of the electrophysiological data included in the present manuscript. The idea for the clustering method was contributed by FH, but all authors contributed to planning the statistical analyses described in the present study, which were performed by PD. XW provided code that was employed in the clustering analysis. PD drafted the manuscript of the present study, which was edited and approved by all authors.
Funding
This study was funded by the UC Davis MIND Institute, by the Robert Shoes Fund, by Scott & Jennifer Fearon, by the UC Davis Deans’ Distinguished Graduate Fellowship, by the NIH (1R01 MH089626-01), the Swiss National Science Foundation (project P2LAP3_164911), the NIMH (U24MH081810), and by an Autism Center of Excellence grant awarded by the NICHD (P50 HD093079). If internal funding from the UC Davis MIND Institute is excluded, these funding bodies had no role in the design of the study.
Availability of data and materials
The data from which classifications and statistical results in the present paper were derived will be made available in an appropriate repository—either OSF or NDAR subsequent to publication.
Ethics approval and consent to participate
All procedures performed in this study were approved by the UC Davis Institutional Review Board (reference number 220915).
Consent for publication
Not applicable—no information in this manuscript identifies any individual person.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests relevant to the present study.
Footnotes
Note on terminology: In light of research [1] suggesting that the majority of autistic-people prefer identity-first language (e.g., “autistic”) to person-first language (e.g., “person with autism”), the authors of the present paper have chosen to use identity-first language. Furthermore, since this study [1] also found that only a minority of autistic people endorsed the use of the terms “disorder” or “condition” to describe autism, and out of a desire to avoid unnecessarily embedding value judgements into research terminology, the authors have decided to use “autism spectrum development” as an alternative.
This statistical test is often referred to as a “cluster-based permutation test.” However, we have chosen to use the term “contiguity-based” to avoid confusion with the hierarchical clustering analysis also employed in this paper.
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Clifford D. Saron and Susan M. Rivera share senior authorship.
Contributor Information
Patrick Dwyer, Email: patricksrdwyer@gmail.com.
Xiaodong Wang, Email: xidwang@ucdavis.edu.
Rosanna De Meo-Monteil, Email: rosanna.demeo@gmail.com.
Fushing Hsieh, Email: fhsieh@ucdavis.edu.
Clifford D. Saron, Email: cdsaron@ucdavis.edu
Susan M. Rivera, Email: srivera@ucdavis.edu
Supplementary information
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1186/s13229-020-00352-3.
References
- 1.Kenny L, Hattersley C, Molins B, Buckley C, Povey C, Pellicano E. Which terms should be used to describe autism? Perspectives from the UK autism community. Autism. 2016. 10.1177/1362361315588200. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 2.American Psychiatric Association. Neurodevelopmental disorders. In: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. 2013. 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.dsm01.
- 3.Robertson CE, Baron-Cohen S. Sensory perception in autism. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2017. 10.1038/nrn.2017.112. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 4.Rogers SJ, Ozonoff S. What do we know about sensory dysfunction in autism? A critical review of the empirical evidence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01431.x. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 5.Khalfa S, Bruneau N, Rogé B, Georgieff N, Veuillet E, Adrien J-L, et al. Increased perception of loudness in autism. Hear Res. 2004. 10.1016/j.heares.2004.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 6.Danesh AA, Lang D, Kaf W, Andreassen WD, Scott J, Eshraghi AA. Tinnitus and hyperacusis in autism spectrum disorders with emphasis on high functioning individuals diagnosed with Asperger’s Syndrome. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2015. 10.1016/j.ijporl.2015.07.024. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 7.Rosenhall U, Nordin V, Sandström M, Ahlsén G, Gillberg C. Autism and hearing loss. J Autism Dev Disord. 1999. 10.1023/A:1023022709710. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 8.Wilson US, Sadler KM, Hancock KE, Guinan JJ, Lichtenhan JT. Efferent inhibition strength is a physiological correlate of hyperacusis in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Neurophysiol. 2017. 10.1016/j.ijporl.2015.07.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 9.Mostafa M. Architecture for autism: Autism ASPECTSS(TM) in school design. ArchNet iJAR Int J Archit Res. 2014;8(1):143–158. doi: 10.26687/archnet-ijar.v8i1.314. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ismael N, Lawson LM, Hartwell J. Relationship between sensory processing and participation in daily occupations for children with autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review of studies that used Dunn’s sensory processing framework. Am J Occup Ther. 2018. 10.5014/ajot.2018.024075. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 11.Little LM, Ausderau K, Sideris J, Baranek GT. Activity participation and sensory features among children with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015. 10.1007/s10803-015-2460-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 12.Ausderau KK, Sideris J, Little LM, Furlong M, Bulluck JC, Baranek GT. Sensory subtypes and associated outcomes in children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 2016. 10.1002/aur.1626. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 13.Lane AE, Young RL, Baker AEZ, Angley MT. Sensory processing subtypes in autism: Association with adaptive behavior. J Autism Dev Disord. 2010. 10.1007/s10803-009-0840-2. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 14.Tomchek SD, Little LM, Dunn W. Sensory pattern contributions to developmental performance in children with autism spectrum disorder. Am J Occup Ther. 2015. 10.5014/ajot.2015.018044. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 15.Williams KL, Kirby AV, Watson LR, Sideris J, Bulluck J, Baranek GT. Sensory features as predictors of adaptive behaviors: A comparative longitudinal study of children with autism spectrum disorder and other developmental disabilities. Res Dev Disabil. 2018. 10.1016/j.ridd.2018.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 16.Ben-Sasson A, Cermak SA, Orsmond GI, Tager-Flusberg H, Kadlec MB, Carter AS. Sensory clusters of toddlers with autism spectrum disorders: Differences in affective symptoms. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2008. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01899.x. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 17.Green SA, Ben-Sasson A, Soto TW, Carter AS. Anxiety and sensory over-responsivity in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders: Bidirectional effects across time. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012. 10.1007/s10803-011-1361-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 18.Mazurek MO, Vasa RA, Kalb LG, Kanne SM, Rosenberg D, Keefer A, et al. Anxiety, sensory over-responsivity, and gastrointestinal problems in children with autism spectrum disorders. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2013. 10.1007/s10802-012-9668-x. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 19.Uljarević M, Lane A, Kelly A, Leekam S. Sensory subtypes and anxiety in older children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2016. 10.1002/aur.1602. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 20.Lin L-Y, Huang P-C. Quality of life and its related factors for adults with autism spectrum disorder. Disabil Rehabil. 2019. 10.1080/09638288.2017.1414887. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 21.Uljarević M, Baranek G, Vivanti G, Hedley D, Hudry K, Lane A. Heterogeneity of sensory features in autism spectrum disorder: Challenges and perspectives for future research. Autism Res. 2017. 10.1002/aur.1747. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 22.Grandin T, Panek R. The autistic brain: Helping different kinds of minds succeed. New York: Mariner Books; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Jackson L. Freaks, geeks & Asperger syndrome: A user guide to adolescence. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Lesko A. Chapter 7: Anita Lesko. In: Grandin T, editor. Different…Not less: Inspiring stories of achievement and successful employment from adults with autism, Asperger’s, and ADHD. Arlington: Future Horizons; 2012. pp. 183–208. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Robertson AE, Simmons DR. The sensory experiences of adults with autism spectrum disorder: A qualitative analysis. Perception. 2015. 10.1068/p7833. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 26.Smith RS, Sharp J. Fascination and isolation: A grounded theory exploration of unusual sensory experiences in adults with Asperger Syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013. 10.1007/s10803-012-1633-6. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 27.DeBoth KK, Reynolds S. A systematic review of sensory-based autism subtypes. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2017. 10.1016/j.rasd.2017.01.005.
- 28.Keith JM, Jamieson JP, Bennetto L. The importance of adolescent self-report in autism spectrum disorder: Integration of questionnaire and autonomic measures. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2019. 10.1007/s10802-018-0455-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 29.Carter Leno V, Chandler S, White P, Yorke I, Charman T, Pickles A, et al. Alterations in electrophysiological indices of perceptual processing and discrimination are associated with co-occurring emotional and behavioural problems in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Mol Autism. 2018. 10.1186/s13229-018-0236-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 30.Dunn MA, Gomes H, Gravel J. Mismatch negativity in children with autism and typical development. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008. 10.1007/s10803-007-0359-3. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 31.Iwanami A, Okajima Y, Ota H, Tani M, Yamada T, Yamagata B, et al. P300 component of event-related potentials in persons with Asperger disorder. J Clin Neurophysiol. 2014. 10.1097/WNP.0000000000000080. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 32.Port RG, Edgar JC, Ku M, Bloy L, Murray R, Blaskey L, et al. Maturation of auditory neural processes in autism spectrum disorder - A longitudinal MEG study. NeuroImage Clin. 2016. 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.03.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 33.Vlaskamp C, Oranje B, Madsen GF, Møllegaard Jepsen JR, Durston S, Cantio C, et al. Auditory processing in autism spectrum disorder: Mismatch negativity deficits. Autism Res. 2017. 10.1002/aur.1821. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 34.Weismüller B, Thienel R, Youlden AM, Fulham R, Koch M, Schall U. Psychophysiological correlates of developmental changes in healthy and autistic boys. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015. 10.1007/s10803-015-2385-x. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 35.Chien YL, Hsieh MH, Gau SSF. Mismatch negativity and P3a in adolescents and young adults with autism spectrum disorders: Behavioral correlates and clinical implications. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017. 10.1007/s10803-017-3426-4. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 36.Donkers FCL, Schipul SE, Baranek GT, Cleary KM, Willoughby MT, Evans AM, et al. Attenuated auditory event-related potentials and associations with atypical sensory response patterns in children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015. 10.1007/s10803-013-1948-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 37.Hudac CM, DesChamps TD, Arnett AB, Cairney BE, Ma R, Webb SJ, et al. Early enhanced processing and delayed habituation to deviance sounds in autism spectrum disorder. Brain Cogn. 2018. 10.1016/j.bandc.2018.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 38.Karhson DS, Golob EJ. Atypical sensory reactivity influences auditory attentional control in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 2016. 10.1002/aur.1593. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 39.Ludlow A, Mohr B, Whitmore A, Garagnani M, Pulvermüller F, Gutierrez R. Auditory processing and sensory behaviours in children with autism spectrum disorders as revealed by mismatch negativity. Brain Cogn. 2014. 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 40.Bruneau N, Bonnet-Brilhault F, Gomot M, Adrien JL, Barthélémy C. Cortical auditory processing and communication in children with autism: Electrophysiological/behavioral relations. Int J Psychophysiol. 2003. 10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00149-1. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 41.Bruneau N, Roux S, Adrien JL, Barthélémy C. Auditory associative cortex dysfunction in children with autism: Evidence from late auditory evoked potentials (N1 wave-T complex). Clin Neurophysiol. 1999. 10.1016/S1388-2457(99)00149-2. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 42.De Meo-Monteil R, Nordahl CW, Amaral DG, Rogers SJ, Harootonian SK, Martin J, et al. Differential altered auditory event-related potential responses in young boys on the autism spectrum with and without disproportionate megalencephaly. Autism Res. 2019. 10.1002/aur.2137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 43.Little LM, Dean E, Tomchek SD, Dunn W. Classifying sensory profiles of children in the general population. Child Care Health Dev. 2017. 10.1111/cch.12391. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 44.Elwin M, Schröder A, Ek L, Wallsten T, Kjellin L. Sensory clusters of adults with and without autism spectrum conditions. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017. 10.1007/s10803-016-2976-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 45.Buchsbaum M. Self-regulation of stimulus intensity: Augmenting/reducing and the average evoked response. In: Schwartz GE, editor. Consciousness and Self-Regulation: Advances in Research. New York: Plenum Press; 1976. pp. 101–135. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Prescott J, Connolly JF, Gruzelier JH. The augmenting/reducing phenomenon in the auditory evoked potential. Biol Psychol. 1984. 10.1016/0301-0511(84)90008-5. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 47.Tlumak A, Durrant J, Delgado R. The effect of stimulus intensity and carrier frequency on auditory middle- and long-latency evoked potentials using a steady-state-response approach. Am J Audiol. 2016. 10.1044/2016_AJA-15-0061. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 48.Lane AE, Molloy CA, Bishop SL. Classification of children with autism spectrum disorder by sensory subtype: A case for sensory-based phenotypes. Autism Res. 2014. 10.1002/aur.1368. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 49.Leekam SR, Nieto C, Libby SJ, Wing L, Gould J. Describing the sensory abnormalities of children and adults with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007. 10.1007/s10803-006-0218-7. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 50.Lord C, Risi S, Linda L, Cook EH Jr, Leventhal BL, Di Lavore PC, et al. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule - Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. . 10.1023/A:1005592401947. [PubMed]
- 51.Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994. 10.1007/BF02172145. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 52.Libero LE, Nordahl CW, Li DD, Ferrer E, Rogers SJ, Amaral DG. Persistence of megalencephaly in a subgroup of young boys with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2016. 10.1002/aur.1643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 53.Nordahl CW, Lange N, Li DD, Barnett LA, Lee A, Buonocore MH, et al. Brain enlargement is associated with regression in preschool-age boys with autism spectrum disorders. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2011. 10.1073/pnas.1107560108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 54.Mullen EM. Mullen scales of early learning. AGS ed. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1995.
- 55.Sparrow SS, Cichetti DV, Balla DA. Vineland adaptive behavior scales. 2nd ed. Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson; 2005.
- 56.McIntosh DN, Miller LJ, Shyu V. Development and validation of the Short Sensory Profile. In: Dunn W, editor. Sensory Profile: User’s manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1999. pp. 59–73. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Hand BN, Dennis S, Lane AE. Latent constructs underlying sensory subtypes in children with autism: A preliminary study. Autism Res. 2017. 10.1002/aur.1787. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 58.Tomchek SD, Dunn W. Sensory processing in children with and without autism: A comparative study using the Short Sensory Profile. Am J Occup Ther. 2007. 10.5014/ajot.61.2.190. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 59.Tomchek SD, Huebner RA, Dunn W. Patterns of sensory processing in children with an autism spectrum disorder. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2014. 10.1007/s10803-006-0218-7.
- 60.Williams ZJ, Failla MD, Gotham KO, Woynaroski TG, Cascio C. Psychometric evaluation of the Short Sensory Profile in youth with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2018. 10.1007/s10803-018-3678-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 61.Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA preschool forms and profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 62.EASYCAP. https://www.easycap.de/. Accessed 8 December 2019.
- 63.BESA. http://www.besa.de/. Accessed 8 December 2019.
- 64.Belouchrani A, Abed-Meraim K, Cardoso J-F, Moulines E. A blind source separation technique using second-order statistics. IEEE Trans Signal Process. 1997. 10.1109/78.554307.
- 65.Tang AC, Sutherland MT, McKinney CJ. Validation of SOBI components from high-density EEG. Neuroimage. 2005. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.027. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 66.Semi-Automatic Artifact Removal Tool (SMART). https://stanford.edu/~saggar/Software.html. Accessed 8 December 2019.
- 67.Saggar M, King BG, Zanesco AP, MacLean KA, Aichele SR, Jacobs TL, et al. Intensive training induces longitudinal changes in meditation state-related EEG oscillatory activity. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012. 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 68.Perrin F, Pernier J, Bertrand O, Giard M, Echallier J. Mapping of scalp potentials by surface spline interpolation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1987. 10.1016/0013-4694(87)90141-6. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 69.Brunet D, Murray MM, Michel CM. Spatiotemporal analysis of multichannel EEG: CARTOOL. Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience. 2011. 10.1155/2011/813870. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 70.Skrandies W. Global field power and topographic similarity. Brain Topogr. 1990. 10.1007/BF01128870. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 71.Frodl T, Meisenzahl EM, Müller D, Leinsinger G, Juckel G, Hahn K, et al. The effect of the skull on event-related P300. Clin Neurophysiol. 2001. 10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00587-9. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 72.Čeponienė R, Rinne T, Näätänen R. Maturation of cortical sound processing as indexed by event-related potentials. Clin Neurophysiol. 2002. 10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00078-0. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 73.Shafer VL, Yu YH, Wagner M. Maturation of cortical auditory evoked potentials (CAEPs) to speech recorded from frontocentral and temporal sites: Three months to eight years of age. Int J Psychophysiol. 2015. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.08.1390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 74.Ponton C, Eggermont J, Khosla D, Kwong B, Don M. Maturation of human central auditory system activity: Separating auditory evoked potentials by dipole source modeling. Clin Neurophysiol. 2002. 10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00733-7. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 75.Keuiper FK, Fisher L. A Monte Carlo comparison of six clustering procedures. Biometrics. 1975. 10.2307/2529565.
- 76.Mojena R. Hierarchical grouping methods and stopping rules: an evaluation. Comput J. 1977. 10.1093/comjnl/20.4.359.
- 77.de Amorim RC. Feature relevance in Ward’s hierarchical clustering using the Lp Norm. J Classif. 2015. 10.1007/s00357-015-9167-1.
- 78.Fushing H, McAssey MP. Time, temperature, and data cloud geometry. Phys Rev. 2010. 10.1103/PhysRevE.82.061110. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 79.Cliff N. Dominance statistics: ordinal analyses to answer ordinal questions. Quant Methods Psychol. 1993. 10.1037/0033-2909.114.3.494.
- 80.MacCallum RC, Zhang S, Preacher KJ, Rucker DD. On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychol Methods. 2002. 10.1037//1082-989X.7.1.19. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 81.Maris E, Oostenveld R. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. J Neurosci Methods. 2007. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 82.Hegerl U, Juckel G. Intensity dependence of auditory evoked potentials as an indicator of central serotonergic neurotransmission: A new hypothesis. Biol Psychiatry. 1993. 10.1016/0006-3223(93)90137-3. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 83.O’Neill BV, Croft RJ, Nathan PJ. The loudness dependence of the auditory evoked potential (LDAEP) as an in vivo biomarker of central serotonergic function in humans: Rationale, evaluation and review of findings. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2008. 10.1002/hup.940. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 84.Rubenstein JLR, Merzenich MM. Model of autism: increased ratio of excitation / inhibition in key neural systems. Brain. 2003. 10.1046/j.1601-183X.2003.00037.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 85.Sohal VS, Rubenstein JLR. Excitation-inhibition balance as a framework for investigating mechanisms in neuropsychiatric disorders. Mol Psychiatry. 2019. 10.1038/s41380-019-0426-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 86.Ward J. Individual differences in sensory sensitivity: A synthesising framework and evidence from normal variation and developmental conditions. Cogn Neurosci. 2019. 10.1080/17588928.2018.1557131. [DOI] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data from which classifications and statistical results in the present paper were derived will be made available in an appropriate repository—either OSF or NDAR subsequent to publication.