Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2020 Jun 15;15(6):e0228976. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0228976

Seasonal and social factors associated with spacing in a wild territorial electric fish

Lucía Zubizarreta 1,2, Laura Quintana 2, Daniel Hernández 3, Franco Teixeira de Mello 4, Mariana Meerhoff 4,5, Renato Massaaki Honji 6, Renata Guimarães Moreira 7, Ana Silva 2,8,*
Editor: Melissa J Coleman9
PMCID: PMC7295226  PMID: 32542049

Abstract

In this study, we focused on the seasonal variation of the determinants of territory size in the weakly electric fish Gymnotus omarorum. This species is a seasonal breeder that displays year-round territorial aggression. Female and male dyads exhibit indistinguishable non-breeding territorial agonistic behavior and body size is the only significant predictor of contest outcome. We conducted field surveys across seasons that included the identification of individual location, measurements of water physico-chemical variables, characterization of individual morphometric and physiological traits, and their correlation to spatial distribution. G. omarorum tolerates a wide range of dissolved oxygen concentration, and territory size correlated positively with dissolved oxygen in both seasons. In the non-breeding season, territory size was sexually monomorphic and correlated only with body size. In the breeding season, territory size no longer correlated with body size but differed between sexes: (i) the overall spatial arrangement was sexually biased, (ii) territory size depended on gonadal hormones in both sexes, which was expected for males, but not previously reported in females, (iii) female territory size showed a positive relationship with gonadal size, and (iv) females showed relatively larger territories than males. This study demonstrates seasonal changes in the determinants of territory size and thus contributes to the understanding of the mechanisms underlying the behavioral plasticity natural territorial behavior.

Introduction

The mechanisms underlying behavioral plasticity, by which animals respond to dynamic environmental and social contexts, are not fully understood [1]. The study of the modulation of territorial behavior in wild species is especially suited for this aim. In territorial behavior, animals first detect the environmental and social clues that determine territory quality, then compare this information to their individual requirements and fighting abilities, and finally decide whether to compete over an area. Therefore, the distribution of territorial animals in space provides insights into how animals integrate individual traits with environmental and social factors. Variation in the ability or motivation to obtain and defend a territory can generate differences in territory size because traits such as body mass, sex, and reproductive state are known to influence resource holding potential and resource value [26]. Within a population, body size is associated with territory size in most species, as it directly determines metabolic requirements. Moreover, body size strongly impacts contest outcome and territory size [710]. In species that display territoriality in both sexes, asymmetries in fighting abilities and motivational factors may cause sex differences in territory size. For example, in red squirrels (Sciurus vulgaris), males often hold larger territories than females although this species is sexually monomorphic in body size [11]. In stripped plateau lizards (Sceloporus virgatus), females are more territorial than males for motivational reasons [12].

Many species show territorial behavior only during the breeding season [13]. On the other hand, some species show robust territorial aggression all year round even though they are seasonal breeders. Non-breeding territoriality is found in diverse taxa, including birds [1417], mammals [1820], reptiles [21], and fish [22,23]. These species offer a valuable opportunity to study the seasonality of environmental features and individual traits, and their relation to territory size in the natural habitat. During the breeding season, male territorial aggression depends largely on gonadal steroids across vertebrates [2426] and, in particular, androgen levels have been related to territory size in the wild [2730]. By contrast, in breeding females, there are few studies on the association between circulating estrogen (E2) levels and territorial aggression in free-living conditions [3133], and, to our knowledge, there are no studies reporting the association between circulating E2 and territory size.

The South American weakly electric fish, Gymnotus omarorum [34], is a seasonal breeder that displays male and female territorial aggression all year and thus is an interesting model system to study the seasonal control of territoriality and its sex differences [35]. Previous laboratory results showed that this species presents a remarkably robust non-breeding territorial aggression (described initially in [22]). In these well-characterized agonistic behavioral displays, fish modulate their electric organ discharge (EOD) to signal a submissive [3638] or dominant status that persists for at least 36 hours [39]. Under experimental laboratory conditions, male-male [40] and female-female [41] dyads that display non-breeding territorial behavior have no differences in contest outcome, temporal dynamics of the agonistic encounter, levels of aggression, or submissive signaling [35]. Moreover, the only significant predictor of contest outcome is body size [22], and none of the features of agonistic encounters depend on circulating gonadal hormones [40].

In this study, we evaluated seasonal variations in the ecological, morphometric, and physiological correlates of territory size in free-living Gymnotus omarorum. Based on previous results, we predicted that, in the non-breeding season when gonads are regressed and circulating gonadal hormones are low, territory size would be sexually monomorphic and explained mostly by body size. For fish in the breeding season, when motivational aspects of territoriality may be confounded with the reproductive drive, we expected territory size and/or its determinants to be sexually dimorphic.

Materials and methods

Study location and sampling seasons

Fieldwork was performed in the Laguna de los Cisnes, Uruguay (205 ha, 34° 48′ S, 55° 18′ W), which consists of a three-part interconnected shallow system of freshwater (maximum depth 5 m) with no inputs of salt or brackish water (Fig 1A, [42]). The study species, Gymnotus omarorum, is the sole species of weakly electric fish present in the study area. The littoral area of the lake is blanketed by a dense strip (5–40 m width) of free-floating aquatic macrophytes that cover the sampling area (Fig 1B), with G. omarorum typically resting among the roots of these plants [34].

Fig 1. Study site and sampling method.

Fig 1

A. The study site is located in Maldonado, Uruguay, in Laguna de los Cisnes. B. The shores of the lake have water hyacinths creating extensive floating mats that constitute the sampling area. C. Census unit illustrating individual spots. Fish location in individual spots was achieved by carrying out an electric census (Survey 1). Once a fish was located, water dissolved O2 concentration, temperature, and fish EOD rate were measured in each spot. In Survey 2, individual spots were revisited in sequence, fish were collected, and individual traits measured.

Following Quintana et al. (2004), who identified the breeding season for weakly electric fish species in this region from December to February (austral summer), we collected field data during December, corresponding to the early-breeding season before the appearance of offspring [43], and from June to August (austral winter), corresponding to the non-breeding season.

Sampling

Sampling area was homogeneous in depth, distance to shore, and vegetation composition. The sampling area was divided into adjacent areas referred to as census units (Fig 1B and 1C, defined below), which were each studied on different days, without repeating sites. Sampling was performed during the day, which is the resting phase in this species [44], in two periods: the first one (Survey 1) during the morning (0800-1200h), and the second one (Survey 2) in the afternoon (1300-1800h).

All research procedures complied with ASAP/ABS Guidelines for the Use of Animals in Research and were approved by the Institutional Ethical Committee (Facultad de Ciencias, Universidad de la República, Comisión Honoraria de Experimentación Animal, Exp: 240011-002308-14).

Field survey 1: Electric census and environmental variables

To achieve an initial estimate of the spatial arrangement of individuals, we performed an electric census during the resting phase of the animals and measured the distance between each fish and its nearest neighbor (distance to the nearest neighbor, DNN) in the two seasons. To calculate DNN, we considered each focal fish distance to the nearest neighbor. DNN was used as a proxy of territory size. Although this is a simplified approach, animals which present territorial aggression display a decrease in defense as distance increases from center points (10, reviewed in [45]). This study was carried out during the resting phase of G. omarorum and fish have been shown to display site fidelity, which suggests they are located in the central parts of their territory. We performed the electric census by locating individual fish by means of an audio amplifier connected to a pair of electrodes, as described elsewhere [46] and individual fish were localized by their EODs when the sound is maximal (detection range is 60 cm). Monitoring the EOD rate allowed us to confirm that fish remain undisturbed during the measurement of parameters since EOD rate increases transiently when fish are alerted by novel stimuli (reviewed in [47]). To perform the electric census, two experienced researchers waded up to 1.2 m depth in the water, to slowly and carefully access areas 1 m away from individual fish. Once a fish was located in an individual spot, the plant above it was tagged (Fig 1C). Water conductivity, dissolved oxygen concentration (O2, mg/l) and temperature (T,°C) were measured 30 cm below the surface (using TDSu Testr 3, Cole Parmer for conductivity, and OxyWard, Handy Polaris for O2 and T). After taking physico-chemical measurements, the EOD of each fish was recorded in situ for 10 s through two electrodes lowered into the vicinity of the animal and connected to an amplifier located on the shore of the lake (World Precision Instruments Inc., Sarasota, FL. DAM-50, AC-coupled). After amplification, EOD signals were recorded on a portable computer, captured by the audio card, and stored for further analysis. To normalize the potential effect of water temperature on EOD rate, values were corrected to a constant 20°C using the Q10 value of 1.5 as calculated for electric fish, from the equation: Q10 = EOD rate x T / EOD rate x (T + 10) [48,49].

During each sampling day, the measurement of physico-chemical water parameters and EOD rate was conducted for all the fish located within a census unit. A census unit was defined as the area with all fish detected by 12 AM, or the area where we detected a group of fish surrounded by at least 6 m of water uninhabited by the species (Fig 1C). Field survey 1 was performed both, during the breeding and non-breeding seasons.

Field survey 2: Quantification of individual traits

In addition to environmental variables, individual morphological and physiological traits can influence spatial distribution and vary seasonally. Therefore, based on Survey 1, we characterized individual traits of captured fish in both seasons, and then analyzed their correlation with spatial distribution. Individual spots were revisited in sequence, and each fish located under the tagged plants was collected using a net, without disturbing nearby tagged sites. Netting desorganize plants, thus if the first netting was unsuccessful no other attempts were made in that site and fish was reported as “not recovered”. Fish were weighed, measured for length, sacrificed by quick decapitation and their gonads were visually inspected for sex determination. In the breeding season, fish were anesthetized immediately after netting by immersion in a fast-acting eugenol solution (1.2 mg/l, first dissolved in alcohol 70%) and blood was collected from the caudal vein with a heparinized syringe within 3 min of capture, which is the time range usually used to avoid a stress response due to manipulation [5052]. Blood was placed in tubes on ice in the field and later centrifuged (3000rpm, 10 min) in the laboratory and stored at -80°C. Dissected gonads were stored in dry ice in the field, and then weighed in the laboratory for gonadosomatic index (GSI) calculation ([Gonad Weight / Total Tissue Weight] x 100 [53]).

Hormone assays

17-β Estradiol (E2) levels were quantified in breeding females, and 11-Ketotestosterone (11-KT) in breeding males by enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) using commercial kits (IBL International, Hamburg, Germany for E2 and Cayman Chemical Company, MI, USA for 11-KT). The analyses were performed according to the manufacturer's instructions and a standard curve was run for each ELISA plate. In all cases, samples were assayed in duplicate and analyses were performed on samples whose coefficients of variation were below 20% [54]. Intra-assay variation was 3.95% for E2 (detection limit: 25 pg/ml) and 6.2% for 11-KT (detection limit: 1.56 pg/ml). Pilot assays using three different dilutions of 8 samples (4 samples per sex) were performed to establish the appropriate working dilutions, which were 1:2 for breeding female E2 and 1:30 for breeding male 11-KT. The assays were validated with standards provided in the kit, indicating that each assay effectively detects G. omarorum E2 and 11-KT.

Data analysis and statistics

All data were subjected first to D'Agostino & Pearson normality test. If data fit a Gaussian distribution, they were analyzed with parametric tests; otherwise non-parametric comparisons were used. Parametric, non-parametric statistical analyses, and simple linear regressions were carried out with PAST [55], and generalized linear models (GLMs) and Binomial tests with software R [56] using RStudio interface.

Data analysis was performed in three steps. The first step of analysis used data from Survey 1 to explore environmental heterogeneity. Environmental variables (O2 and T) were compared between seasons by Mann-Whitney U test. To analyze O2 and T heterogeneity we calculated the coefficient of variation (CV, SD/mean*100) for each census unit and then the mean CV and standard deviation per season. Finally, we performed a linear regression between two individual traits, DNNs and EOD basal rate, and O2 content, both in the breeding and non-breeding seasons (Fig 2).

Fig 2. Fish spatial distribution based on environmental variables (Survey 1).

Fig 2

The breeding season is represented in green and the non-breeding season in gray; dark green sections implies overlap of both seasons. A. Frequency distribution versus DNN(in cm). B. Frequency distribution versus O2 concentration (in mg/l) measured at 30 cm from the surface in each individual spot. C. Linear regression of DNN and O2 concentration in individual spots (Log transformed). Breeding season: R2 = 0.44, p = 1 exp -4, N = 35; non-breeding season: R2 = 0.21, p = 1 exp-4, N = 50. D. Linear regression of EOD rate and water oxygen concentration in individual spots. Breeding season: R2 = 0.47, p = 5 exp-4, N = 22; non-breeding season: R2 = 1 exp-3, p = 0.54, N = 39.

The second step of the analysis was based on the dataset obtained in Survey 2, which entailed fish collection. We explored whether individual morphometric and physiological traits correlated with DNN in both the non-breeding and breeding seasons. Individual traits (body size and EOD rate), DNN, and DNN relative to body length were compared between sexes within each season by Student t-tests (Table 1). Because body weight and body length were strongly correlated (R = 0.91, N = 47), and because we had more measurements of body length than of body weight, we used body length as an indicator of body size throughout. Body length, EOD rate, and DNN were compared seasonally by Student t-test for females and males together.

Table 1. Sexual comparison of individual traits and DNN in the breeding season and in the non-breeding season (Survey 2).

Individual traits: Body length and EOD rate corrected by water temperature. Distance to the nearest neighbor (DNN in cm), and relative DNN (DNN/body length). Values are expressed as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM), and statistical comparisons were performed by t-test.

Body size (cm) EOD rate temp DNN (cm) DNN / body length
A- Breeding season 22.4 ± 0.5 n = 13 24.5 ± 0.9 n = 15 41.0 ± 1.1 n = 13 39.3 ± 0.9 n = 17 220 ± 20 n = 13 230 ± 30 n = 18 9.8 ± 0.8 n = 13 9.9 ± 1.4 n = 17
♀ vs ♂ p = 0.2 ♀ vs ♂ p = 0.3 ♀ vs ♂ p = 0.8 ♀ vs ♂ p = 0.9
B- Non-breeding season 16.9 ± 0.9 n = 26 17.4 ± 0.8 n = 27 20.0 ± 1.2 n = 18 19.3 ± 0.95 n = 18 113 ± 10 n = 23 120 ± 10 n = 23 7.0 ± 0.6 n = 23 7.8 ± 0.7 n = 23
♀ vs ♂ p = 0.7 ♀ vs ♂ p = 0.6 ♀ vs ♂ p = 0.5 ♀ vs ♂ p = 0.4

To examine the effects of individual traits (see below) on DNN as the dependent variable, weused GLMs; [57], within each season analyzed separately. For the breeding season, we first ran a model with body length, EOD rate, and sex as explanatory variables. Because the initial model was non-significant, we ran one model for females with body length, EOD rate, and circulating E2 as explanatory variables, and a second model for males with body length and EOD rate as explanatory variables (Table 2). For the non-breeding season, the model included females and males together, and the explanatory variables were body length, EOD rate, and sex (Table 3). For each season separately, initial models contained all single effects and pairwise interactions of the explanatory variables.

Table 2. GLM models that presented the best adjustment to explain DNN in females during the breeding season with data obtained in Survey 2.

Model intercept and explanatory variables are expressed as Mean (SD). For each parameter, the value is shown in bold if statistically significant (p<0.05), in italic if marginal (p<0.1), and expressed as NS if non-significant. Model 1 and Model 2 did not present significant differences by the Akaike information criterion (AIC).

Model Intercept Circulating E2 EOD rate N P model Adjusted R2 AIC
M1 452 (141) 0.14 (0.03) -0.51 (2.7) 13 0.02 0.44 20.24
M2 188 (18) 0.1 (0.04) NS 13 0.03 0.31 22.21

Table 3. GLM model that presented the best adjustment to explain the distance to the nearest neighbor (DNN) in both sexes during the non-breeding season with data obtained in Survey 2.

Model intercept and explanatory variable are expressed as Mean (SD). For each parameter the value is shown in bold if statistically significant (p<0.05).

Intercept Body size N P model Adjusted R2 AIC
36.4 (34) 5.2 (2) 48 0.01 0.11 79.75

To select the most parsimonious GLM, we used the command bestglm [58] for a maximum of three simultaneous variables, and considered up to second order interactions. Initial models were simplified by the stepwise deletion of the least significant terms in a model and compared successive steps of model simplification by the Akaike information criterion (AIC), deleting a term whenever there was a difference of more than two units between alternative models until arriving to the most parsimonious model that could be fitted. The selection of the best model included the AIC criterion as well as the number and statistical significance of the estimated parameters, discarding models with improvements in the AIC but non-significant parameters. All models were subjected to the customary residual analysis [59].

The final step of analysis aimed to address three points. The first point was to further evaluate how sexually dimorphic traits, such as gonad size and sexual hormones, impacted on DNN particularly during breeding (Fig 3). To do this, we compared DNN relative to body size between non-breeding and breeding season separately for each sex. In addition, to analyze the relative importance of gonadal mass on individual spacing, we performed linear regressions of DNN versus GSI, and body weight–gonad weight, both in breeding females and males. The second point was to analyze whether oxygen concentration, besides affecting DNN, showed association with morphological (GSI) and physiological individual traits (EOD rate and E2 levels). The third point was to evaluate whether the sex of the nearest neighbor differed from an expected random distribution, and for that we used Binomial tests [60]. For the analysis, the focal fish was only considered if the nearest neighbor was captured and its sex confirmed. The proportion of sexes expected for a random distribution was deduced from the empirical sex ratio observed in each season (Fig 4).

Fig 3. Emergence of sex dimorphism during the breeding season.

Fig 3

The plots show values for females (left panels, represented as squares) and males (right panels, represented as circles), from data obtained in Survey 2 (Step 3 of analysis). A- Relative DNN (cm / cm body length). Dots represent individual values, and in A and B horizontal lines represent mean values, and error bars represent SEM. For each sex, breeding values are shown in the left (green) and non-breeding values in the right (grey). * indicate statistically significance (p < 0.05) t-test. B- Linear regression between body size (cm) and DNN (cm) in the breeding season. Females: p = 0.8, R2 = 7 exp-3, N = 13; males p = 0.7, R2 = 0.01, N = 15. C- Linear regression between GSI and DNN (cm) in the breeding season. Females: p = 1 exp-3, R2 = 0.8, N = 9; males: p = 0.14, R2 = 0.25, N = 10. D- Linear regression between body weight–gonad weight (g) and DNN (cm) in the breeding season. Females: p = 0.7, R2 = 0.02, N = 9; males: p = 0.6, R2 = 0.03, N = 10. E- Linear regression between water O2 concentration (mg/l) and E2 (pg/ml) in breeding females: p = 0.03, R2 = 0.41, N = 11. The same relation is shown for 11-KT in breeding males with no statistical test due to small sample size (N = 5).

Fig 4. Seasonality in the spatial distribution of sexes (from data obtained in Survey 2).

Fig 4

Sex of the nearest neighbor (expressed in percentage) when the focal fish is a female or a male, both in the breeding season (A, top), and in the non-breeding season (B, bottom). Dashed line represents the expected percentage to have a female as nearest neighbor for a random distribution, according to the empirical sex ratio (41% in the breeding season, and 50.9% in the non-breeding season). * indicates statistical significance (p < 0.05) according to the binomial exact test.

Results

In both seasons, individual Gymnotus omarorum were typically located in water depths of ~ 30 cm among the dense roots of extensive floating mats of vegetation along the littoral area across seasons (Fig 1B; i.e., in the breeding season during the austral summer, and in the non-breeding season during the austral winter). The free-floating water hyacinth Eichhornia crassipes dominated both surface and underwater areas accounting for 86% of the total subaquatic biomass. Associated vegetation was composed of submerged Egeria densa and Miriophyllum aquaticum, free-floating Salvinia auriculata, and the rooted but partly emergent Ludwigia elegans, and Hydrocotyle criptocarpa. This vegetation, with the same composition, was present all year, although overall coverage was lower during the non-breeding season.

As expected in the subtropical region, water temperature and oxygen concentration showed significant differences across seasons. Water temperature was higher during the breeding than during the non-breeding season (27.3 ± 0.1°C, N = 36 vs 11.3 ± 0.5°C, N = 60; p = 1 exp-4, Mann Whitney U test), whereas O2 concentration was significantly lower in the breeding compared to the non-breeding season (5.6 ± 0.9 mg/l, N = 36, vs 8.4 ± 0.4 mg/l, N = 65; p = 4 exp-3, Mann Whitney U test). By contrast, water conductivity remained consistent (< 150 μS/cm) throughout the year.

Field survey 1: Fish spatial distribution and environmental variables

Our first step of analysis aimed to relate environmental data to individual spacing and EOD rates of fish detected by electrical census. Fish occurred under the central area of the floating mats, and absent from the edge near the open water (Fig 1C). In both seasons, fish were distributed evenly, and not aggregated with other conspecifics. The distribution of DNNs was asymmetrical, skewed with a mode at 150 cm (Fig 2A). The DNN mean value was significantly higher in the breeding season than in the non-breeding season (230 ± 15 cm, t-test, T = 5.9, p = 1 exp-4, N = 47 vs 140 ± 7 cm, N = 73).

Water temperature showed low variability among individual spots within each census unit (breeding season mean T CV = 2.0 ± 2.4%, non-breeding season mean T CV = 6.5 ± 5.5%). In contrast, O2 concentration showed high variability within each census unit (breeding season mean O2 CV = 20.4 ± 9.3%, non-breeding season mean O2 CV = 25.9 ± 14.6%). Consequently, we explored whether O2 may contribute to fish spatial patterns. During the breeding season, the distribution of O2 concentration ranged from 0 to 15 mg/l and was positively skewed (Fig 2B, green), with a higher frequency of low values (mode at 1 mg/l). However, during the non-breeding season, O2 distribution was negatively skewed, showing a single mode at 11 mg/l (Fig 2B, gray). Oxygen concentration and DNN showed a positive and significant association both in the breeding (R2 = 0.44, p = 1 exp -4, N = 31, Fig 2C green) and in the non-breeding season (R2 = 0.21, p = 1 exp -4, N = 50, Fig 2C gray). Interestingly, O2 showed a positive linear relationship with individual EOD rate during the breeding but not the non-breeding season (breeding: R2 = 0.47, p = 5 exp -4, N = 22, Fig 2D green; non-breeding: R2 = 1 exp -3, p = 0.54, N = 39, Fig 2D gray).

Field survey 2: Fish spatial distribution based on individual traits

The second step of analysis explored the impact of individual morphological and physiological characteristics of fish on DNN in both the non-breeding and breeding seasons. Across census units, 71 ± 5.8% of fish located under tagged sites in Survey 1 were recovered in Survey 2. There were no sexual differences in body length, EOD rate, DNN, nor DNN relative to body length, in both the breeding and the non-breeding seasons (Table 1). During breeding, females showed mean circulating E2 levels of 293.7 ± 97.7 pg/ml and males had mean circulating 11-KT levels of 399.1 ± 140.9 pg/ml. Mean adult body size was significantly larger in the breeding season than in the non-breeding season (t-test, T = 6.6, p = 1 exp-4, N breeding = 28, N non-breeding = 53). In addition, EOD rate was higher (t-test, T = 18.8, p = 1 exp-4, N breeding = 30, N non-breeding = 36), DNN was larger (t-test, T = 5.6, p = 1 exp-4, N breeding = 31, N non-breeding = 46) and relative DNN (DNN/body length) was also larger during the breeding season (t-test, T = 2.7, p = 9 exp-3, N breeding = 30, N non-breeding = 46).

We evaluated the determinants of DNN separately in the breeding and non-breeding seasons. Because the first GLM including breeding females and males, with body length, EOD rate, and sex as explanatory variables was not significant, we separated sexes into two different models and included circulating E2 levels as an explanatory variable for females. We obtained two significant models in females, both equivalent according to the AIC criterion (Table 2). In the model with the best adjustment (model 1), DNN showed a positive correlation with circulating E2, and a marginal negative correlation with EOD rate. In model 2, EOD rate was not a significant explanatory variable for DNN, and only E2 had a significant positive correlation.

For males, we found no correlation between the independent variables tested and DNN. Although androgen levels were not included in the model (due to a low number of valid samples), it is worth mentioning that circulating 11-KT levels showed a positive trend with DNN, although the dataset was too small for sufficiently powerful statistical analysis. In the non-breeding season, individual traits were the same for males and females, thus we were able to include both sexes in a single GLM to test the influence of individual traits on DNN. We explored if individual sex, body size, and EOD rate correlated with DNN, and found that body size, but not sex nor EOD rate, correlated positively with DNN (Table 3).

To assess sexual differences in territorial features during breeding, we performed a third step of analysis. We compared relative territory size between seasons and evaluated the relationship between individual and environmental traits. Although both absolute and relative DNN were larger in the breeding season in both sexes, an analysis in which males and females were separated showed that this seasonal difference is only significant in females (Females: breeding 9.8 ± 0.8, N = 13 vs non-breeding 7.0 ± 0.6, N = 23; t-test, T = 2.7, p = 0.01, Males: breeding 9.9 ± 1.4, N = 17 vs non-breeding 7.5 ± 0.7, N = 22; t-test, T = 1.4, p = 0.12, Fig 3A). In accordance with the breeding GLM results, body size showed no association with DNN in the breeding season in either sex (Females: p = 0.8, R2 = 7 exp-3, N = 13. Males: p = 0.7, R2 = 0.01, N = 15; Fig 3B). With respect to the relative importance of gonadal maturity on spacing, we found that GSI was significantly higher in the breeding than in the non-breeding season in females (Females: 1.1 ± 0.22% N = 9 vs 0.6 ± 0.09% N = 11; t-test, T = 2.3, p = 0.02,. Males: 0.24 ± 0.03% N = 10 vs 0.23 ± 0.03, N = 16; t-test, T = 0.3, p = 0.87); and as expected GSI showed a positive relationship with E2 in breeding females (p = 2 exp-3, R2 = 0.76, N = 9, data not shown). In addition, GSI showed a positive relationship with DNN only in breeding females (Females: p = 1 exp-3, R2 = 0.8, N = 9. Males: p = 0.14, R2 = 0.25, N = 10, Fig 3C); and DNN showed no relation with body weight after the subtraction of gonad weight in breeding (Females: p = 0.7, R2 = 0.02, N = 9. Males: p = 0.6, R2 = 0.03, N = 10; Fig 3D). Further, water O2 content showed a positive relationship with circulating E2 in breeding females (p = 0.03, R2 = 0.41, N = 11, Fig 3E) and O2 showed a marginal relationship with female GSI (p = 0.08, R2 = 0.5, N = 7, data not shown); while male 11-KT showed no association with O2, although the dataset was too small for statistical analysis (N = 5, Fig 3E).

In the breeding season, fish were distributed non-randomly according to sex. The percentage of females with a female as the nearest neighbor was significantly lower than the random distribution (p = 0.03, N = 11; Binomial exact test, Fig 4A), and the percentage of females with a male as the nearest neighbor was significantly higher than random (p = 0.04, N = 16; Binomial exact test, Fig 4A). By contrast, in the non-breeding season, the spatial configuration of the population was random with respect to sex. The probability of having a female as the nearest neighbor did not differ significantly from the random distribution for both females (p = 0.82, N = 20; Binomial exact test, Fig 4B) and males (p = 0.12, N = 15; Binomial exact test, Fig 4B).

Discussion

This study examined seasonal and sexual differences in the determinants of territory size in a teleost species. First, we found that Gymnotus omaroum has a spatial distribution in the natural habitat consistent with territoriality across seasons and that O2 concentration had a significant positive correlation with territory size both in the breeding and in the non-breeding season. In addition, we found that: a) In the non-breeding season, territory size was sexually monomorphic and determined partially by individual body size; and b) there was a seasonal emergence of sexual dimorphism in relative territory size and in its determinants.

In many species, body mass is strongly associated with a variety of physiological and ecological attributes (e.g., home range and metabolic rate) and is key to understanding how animals use the environment [6164]. The distribution of differently sized animals can be shaped by behavioral interactions such as resource defense [65]. These ideas, put forth initially to analyze interspecific interactions [66], can be also applied to interpret distribution of individuals of the same species, as in this study. We found that body size predicted DNN in the non-breeding season but not in the breeding season (Tables 2 and 3). This suggests that, during reproduction, other physiological, behavioral, and motivational aspects may override body size in predicting DNN, a proxy for territory size.

Oxygen is a limiting physico-chemical variable in aquatic ecosystems, and hypoxia has been related to fish mortality [67]. Interestingly, Gymnotus has been shown to tolerate a wide range of dissolved O2 concentrations and can survive in hypoxic environments [68]. Gymnotus can breathe air at the water surface and has metabolic adaptations that compensate for any potential damages hypoxia may cause [69]. In our study, individual locations showed O2 concentration correlated with territory size in both seasons (Fig 2C). This suggests that higher levels of dissolved O2 may enable fish to defend large territories because their capacity for aerobic respiration is enhanced. Interestingly, O2 concentration correlated positively with EOD rate only during the breeding season, when EOD rate was significantly higher and O2 content was significantly lower (Fig 2). This is consistent with the metabolic requirement of EOD production [70], which can be specially demanding when EOD rate is high, in the challenging environment of summer hypoxia.

Non-breeding spacing: Body size dependent and sex independent

In teleosts, year round territoriality has been examined mostly in populations of coral reef communities (for example Stegastes fuscus, [71]). Here, we evaluated the seasonal spatial organization in a subtropical freshwater fish in the field and found that spatial distribution during winter was consistent with territoriality. Why would Gymnotus defend non-breeding territories? The acquisition and maintenance of territories are known to be mediated by agonistic behavior [72]. In particular, previous reports of laboratory experiments demonstrated that Gymnotus omarorum exhibits non-breeding agonistic behavior that mediates territory access [39] and that body size is the main determinant of the fight outcome [22].

The generation of the EOD in electric fish imposes a high basal metabolic cost [70], which requires an additional constant need to forage. In line with this, we found that larger fish hold larger territories, in both males and females (Table 3). Body size is sexually monomorphic in this species and behavioral experiments showed that agonistic behavior is non-sex-biased [22,35]. Here, we confirmed that non-breeding males and females hold sexually monomorphic territory sizes in the wild (Table 1), probably to cope with energetic requirements that are not expected to differ by sex during winter.

Electric fish use electric communication signals as behavioral displays. Since the EOD encodes information about body size and physiological state [7377], a fish may maintain territory boundaries in the wild by remotely assessing the EOD of neighbors. EOD rate signals dominant status when fish are kept in close quarters after conflict resolution, but not when allowed to distance themselves [39]. Thus, we expected no correlation between EOD rate and territory size in the field, and this was confirmed by our data (Table 3). Dominance status based on EOD rate may be needed to reinforce submission when a subordinate individual is unable to escape from the dominant as in confined laboratory conditions. However, under the population densities observed in this study, dominance status is not expected.

Breeding spacing: The emergence of sexual dimorphism in territory determinants

While competition for reproductive opportunities is usually sexually dimorphic, competition over non-sexual resources is usually equal between males and females [78,79]. Moreover, many species with male and female territoriality are sexually monomorphic in body size and signal traits [8082]. Consistent with this pattern, we found that during the breeding season both absolute and relative territory sizes were sexually monomorphic, which parallels the lack of sex dimorphism in body size (Table 1). However, during breeding, body size did not correlate with territory size (Fig 3B), suggesting that other factors beyond body size influence habitat resource acquisition and maintenance.

Interestingly, a closer analysis (Step 3) showed that although there were no sex differences in relative territory size in each season (Table 1), they emerged when comparing between seasons. Females seemed to use relatively larger territories in the breeding season compared to the non-breeding season (Fig 3A). This result can be interpreted in the context of energetic cost. Female GSI was higher in the breeding season and correlated positively to circulating E2. Moreover, GSI was also an excellent predictor of female territory size (Fig 3C). This suggests that females may establish relatively larger territories in the breeding season related to foraging needs imposed by ovarian maturation. In line with this, circulating E2 positively correlated with territory size in females (Table 2), a result obtained in both breeding female GLM models. EOD rate, on the other hand, is less reliable in its influence upon territory size, as it presented a marginal correlation to territory in only one GLM model.

Steroid hormones are strong modulators of behavioral plasticity [1] as hormones are used to orchestrate organismal-level physiological and behavioral responses to social cues [83,84]. In females, the fact that both GSI and E2 correlated with territory size, combined with reports in which E2 promotes female aggression [8587], suggests that ovarian E2 modulates territorial behavior in G. omarorum. We hypothesize that E2 secretion integrates female metabolic requirements with the social environment, through the expression of territorial behavior. A closer look at the relationship between water O2 concentration and female individual traits, shows that O2 levels correlate positively with circulating E2 (Fig 3E) and marginally with GSI. These data allow us to further speculate that breeding females compete for territories with high O2, which would allow them to better confront energetically expensive processes. In males, we identified a positive trend between circulating 11-KT and territory size, which, although not statistically significant, may be expected given the well documented relationship between androgens and male territoriality [2426].

The fact that individual body size was larger in the breeding than in the non-breeding season, most likely depends on the physical growth inherent to G. omarorum life history. However, we cannot assume that the non-breeding and breeding population remain demographically unchanged. Although this study was not conceived to track population seasonal changes, differences in fish size between seasons may reflect demographic changes. For example, large mature fish may displace smaller adults not only to exploit valuable territories for their own use, but also to access sites where offspring have higher survival as observed, for example, in the cichlid Lobochilotes labiatus [88].

Motivation for territory defense in the breeding season may involve not only the value of the territory per se (territory size, O2 concentration) but also the sex of the nearest neighbor. The results presented in Fig 4 suggest that G. omarorum can assess territory features and use this information to shift positions seasonally. In the non-breeding season individual fish had a closest neighbor that was random with respect to sex, whereas in the breeding season, it was significantly more likely to have an opposite-sex closest neighbor (Fig 4). This evidence supports the idea that the sex of the nearest neighbor becomes a relevant factor for territory value, but only during the breeding season. This can be considered a good example of behavioral plasticity by which individuals respond differently to the same social stimulus (e.g., sex of the nearest neighbor), depending on variations in their internal state (sexually dimorphic hormones).

Concluding remarks

Gymnotus omarorum offers the opportunity to analyze seasonal changes in year-round territoriality. We found no differences in absolute nor relative territory size between sexes across the year. In the non-breeding season, only body size and water O2 concentration influenced territory size. By contrast, in the breeding season, sex became relevant for territorial behavior: a) the sex of neighbors became important; b) territory size was related to gonadal hormones in both sexes, which was expected for males, but not previously reported in females; and c) breeding females used relatively larger territories than non-breeding females, which reflects particular female metabolic demands related to ovarian maturation. This study helps bridge the gap between behavioral plasticity of natural territorial behavior and its underlying mechanisms.

Acknowledgments

We are very grateful to Adriana Migliaro, Rossana Perrone, Carlos Passos, Federico Reyes, and Bettina Tassino for their useful discussions during the BERTA Workshop, Cerro del Toro, Piriápolis, Uruguay. We also thank the anonymous reviewers for their constructive suggestions that greatly improved this article.

Data Availability

Raw data are available from the https://osf.io/dashboard database Seasonal and social factors associated with spacing in a wild territorial electric fish (DOI 10.17605/OSF.IO/65D8A).

Funding Statement

This research was funded by Agencia Nacional de Investigación e Innovación (ANII), Fondo Clemente Estable grants to AS (ANII_FCE_6180; FCE_4272) and LQ (ANII_FCE_136381). Programa de Desarrollo de las Ciencias Básicas gave support to LZ, MM, FTM, LQ and AS and Universidad de la República gave support to AS, MM and FTM.

References

  • 1.Oliveira RF. Social behavior in context: Hormonal modulation of behavioral plasticity and social competence. Integr Comp Biol. 2009. July 21;49(4):423–40. 10.1093/icb/icp055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Smith JM, Parker GA. The logic of asymmetric contests. Anim Behav. 1976. February 1;24(1):159–75. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hurd PL. Resource holding potential, subjective resource value, and game theoretical models of aggressiveness signalling. J Theor Biol. 2006. August 7;241(3):639–48. 10.1016/j.jtbi.2006.01.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Piper WH, Mager JN, Walcott C, Furey L, Banfield N, Reinke A, et al. Territory settlement in common loons: no footholds but age and assessment are important. Anim Behav. 2015. June 1;104:155–63. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Metcalfe NB, Van Leeuwen TE, Killen SS. Does individual variation in metabolic phenotype predict fish behaviour and performance? J Fish Biol. 2016. January;88(1):298–321. 10.1111/jfb.12699 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Nolen ZJ, Allen PE, Miller CW. Seasonal resource value and male size influence male aggressive interactions in the leaf footed cactus bug, Narnia femorata. Behav Processes. 2017. May 1;138:1–6. 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.01.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Elliott JM. Mechanisms Responsible for Population Regulation in Young Migratory Trout, Salmo trutta. III. The Role of Territorial Behaviour. J Anim Ecol. 1990;59(3):803–18. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Butchart SHM, Seddon N, Ekstrom JMM. Polyandry and competition for territories in bronze-winged jacanas. J Anim Ecol. 1999;68(5):928–39. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Keeley ER. An experimental analysis of territory size in juvenile steelhead trout. Anim Behav. 2000. March 1;59(3):477–90. 10.1006/anbe.1999.1288 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Adams ES. Approaches to the Study of Territory Size and Shape. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 2001. November 1;32(1):277–303. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Wauters L, Dhondt AA. Spacing behaviour of red squirrels, Sciurus vulgaris: variation between habitats and the sexes. Anim Behav. 1992. February 1;43(2):297–311. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Smith DC. Home range and territory in the striped plateau lizard (Sceloporus virgatus). Anim Behav. 1985. May 1;33(2):417–27. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Adkins-Regan E. Hormones and Animal Social Behavior. Princeton University Press; 2005. 431 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Logan CA, Wingfield JC. Autumnal territorial aggression is independent of plasma testosterone in mockingbirds. Horm Behav. 1990. December 1;24(4):568–81. 10.1016/0018-506x(90)90042-v [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Gwinner E, Rödl T, Schwabl H. Pair Territoriality of Wintering Stonechats: Behaviour, Function and Hormones. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1994;34(5):321–7. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Wingfield JC, Monk D. Behavioral and Hormonal Responses of Male Song Sparrows to Estradiol-Treated Females during the Non-breeding Season. Horm Behav. 1994. June 1;28(2):146–54. 10.1006/hbeh.1994.1012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Soma KK, Sullivan K, Wingfield J. Combined Aromatase Inhibitor and Antiandrogen Treatment Decreases Territorial Aggression in a Wild Songbird during the Nonbreeding Season. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 1999. September 1;115(3):442–53. 10.1006/gcen.1999.7334 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Caldwell G, Glickman S, Smith E. Seasonal aggression independent of seasonal testosterone in wood rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1984. August;81(16):5255–7. 10.1073/pnas.81.16.5255 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Jasnow AM, Huhman KL, Bartness TJ, Demas GE. Short-Day Increases in Aggression Are Inversely Related to Circulating Testosterone Concentrations in Male Siberian Hamsters (Phodopus sungorus). Horm Behav. 2000. September 1;38(2):102–10. 10.1006/hbeh.2000.1604 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Demas GE, Cooper MA, Albers HE, Soma KK. Novel Mechanisms Underlying Neuroendocrine Regulation of Aggression: A Synthesis of Rodent, Avian, and Primate Studies In: Lajtha A, Blaustein JD, editors. Handbook of Neurochemistry and Molecular Neurobiology [Internet]. Boston, MA: Springer US; 2007. [cited 2019 Nov 15]. p. 337–72. Available from: http://link.springer.com/10.1007/978-0-387-30405-2_8 [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Moore MC, Marler CA. Effects of testosterone manipulations on nonbreeding season territorial aggression in free-living male lizards, Sceloporus jarrovi. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 1987. February 1;65(2):225–32. 10.1016/0016-6480(87)90170-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Batista G, Zubizarreta L, Perrone R, Silva A. Non-sex-biased dominance in a sexually monomorphic electric fish: fight structure and submissive electric signalling. Ethology. 2012. April 1;118(4):398–410. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Vullioud P, Bshary R, Ros AFH. Intra- and interspecific aggression do not modulate androgen levels in dusky gregories, yet male aggression is reduced by an androgen blocker. Horm Behav. 2013. August 1;64(3):430–8. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.06.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Monaghan EP, Glickman SE. Hormones and aggressive behavior. Behav Endocrinol. 1992;134:692–694. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Nelson RJ. An introduction to behavioral endocrinology, 3rd ed Sunderland, MA, US: Sinauer Associates; 2005. xvii, 822. (An introduction to behavioral endocrinology, 3rd ed.). [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Simon NG, Lu S-F. Androgens and aggression. In: Biology of aggression. Oxford University Press, USA; 2006. p. 211–30. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Watson A, Parr R. Hormone Implants Affecting Territory Size and Aggressive and Sexual Behaviour in Red Grouse. Ornis Scand Scand J Ornithol. 1981;12(1):55–61. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Cardwell JR, Liley NR. Androgen control of social status in males of a wild population of stoplight parrotfish, Sparisoma viride (Scaridae). Horm Behav. 1991. March 1;25(1):1–18. 10.1016/0018-506x(91)90035-g [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Sinervo B, Miles DB, Frankino WA, Klukowski M, DeNardo DF. Testosterone, Endurance, and Darwinian Fitness: Natural and Sexual Selection on the Physiological Bases of Alternative Male Behaviors in Side-Blotched Lizards. Horm Behav. 2000. December 1;38(4):222–33. 10.1006/hbeh.2000.1622 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Mougeot F, Redpath SM, Moss R, Matthiopoulos J, Hudson PJ. Territorial behaviour and population dynamics in red grouse Lagopus lagopus scoticus. I. Population experiments. J Anim Ecol. 2003. November 1;72(6):1073–82. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Woodley SK, Moore MC. Ovarian Hormones Influence Territorial Aggression in Free-Living Female Mountain Spiny Lizards. Horm Behav. 1999. June 1;35(3):205–14. 10.1006/hbeh.1999.1514 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Rubenstein DR, Wikelski M. Steroid hormones and aggression in female Galápagos marine iguanas. Horm Behav. 2005. September;48(3):329–41. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.04.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Pärn H, Lindström KM, Sandell M, Amundsen T. Female Aggressive Response and Hormonal Correlates—An Intrusion Experiment in a Free-Living Passerine. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2008;62(10):1665–77. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Richer-de-Forges MM, Crampton WGR, Albert JS. A New Species of Gymnotus (Gymnotiformes, Gymnotidae) from Uruguay: Description of a Model Species in Neurophysiological Research. Copeia. 2009;2009(3):538–44. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Quintana L, Zubizarreta L, Jalabert C, Batista G, Perrone R, Silva A. Building the case for a novel teleost model of non-breeding aggression and its neuroendocrine control. Journal of Physiology-Paris. 2016. October 1;110(3, Part B):224–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Silva AC, Perrone R, Zubizarreta L, Batista G, Stoddard PK. Neuromodulation of the agonistic behavior in two species of weakly electric fish that display different types of aggression. J Exp Biol. 2013. July 1;216(13):2412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Zubizarreta L, Stoddard PK, Silva A. Aggression Levels Affect Social Interaction in the Non-Breeding Territorial Aggression of the Weakly Electric Fish, Gymnotus omarorum. Ethology. 2015. January 1;121(1):8–16. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Perrone R, Silva AC. Status-Dependent Vasotocin Modulation of Dominance and Subordination in the Weakly Electric Fish Gymnotus omarorum. Front Behav Neurosci. 2018;12:1 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Perrone R, Pedraja F, Valiño G, Tassino B, Silva A. Non-breeding territoriality and the effect of territory size on aggression in the weakly electric fish, Gymnotus omarorum. Acta Ethologica. 2019. June 1;22(2):79–89. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Jalabert C, Quintana L, Pessina P, Silva A. Extra-gonadal steroids modulate non-breeding territorial aggression in weakly electric fish. Horm Behav. 2015. June 1;72:60–7. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.05.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Zubizarreta L, Silva AC, Quintana L. The estrogenic pathway modulates non-breeding female aggression in a teleost fish. Physiol Behav. 2020. June 1;220:112883 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.112883 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Marroni S, Iglesias C, Mazzeo N, Clemente J, Teixeira de Mello F, Pacheco JP. Alternative food sources of native and non-native bivalves in a subtropical eutrophic lake. Hydrobiologia. 2014. September 1;735(1):263–76. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Quintana L, Silva A, Berois N, Macadar O. Temperature induces gonadal maturation and affects electrophysiological sexual maturity indicators in Brachyhypopomus pinnicaudatus from a temperate climate. J Exp Biol. 2004. May 1;207(11):1843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Migliaro A, Moreno V, Marchal P, Silva A. Daily changes in the electric behavior of weakly electric fish naturally persist in constant darkness and are socially synchronized. Biol Open. 2018. December 15;7(12):bio036319 10.1242/bio.036319 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Brown JL, Orians GH. Spacing Patterns in Mobile Animals. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 1970;1:239–62. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Silva A, Quintana L, Galeano M, Errandonea P. Biogeography and Breeding in Gymnotiformes from Uruguay. Environ Biol Fishes. 2003. April 1;66(4):329–38. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Moller P. Electric fishes: history and behavior. Vol. 17 Springer; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Dunlap KD, Smith GT, Yekta A. Temperature Dependence of Electrocommunication Signals and Their Underlying Neural Rhythms in the Weakly Electric Fish, Apteronotus leptorhynchus. Brain Behav Evol. 2000;55(3):152–62. 10.1159/000006649 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Silva A, Perrone R, Macadar O. Environmental, seasonal, and social modulations of basal activity in a weakly electric fish. Incl Spec Sect Chronobiol Asp Sleep—Wake Cycle Thermoreregulation. 2007. February 28;90(2):525–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Fox HE, White SA, Kao MHF, Fernald RD. Stress and Dominance in a Social Fish. J Neurosci. 1997. August 15;17(16):6463 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-16-06463.1997 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Dunlap KD, Smith GT, Yekta A. Temperature Dependence of Electrocommunication Signals and Their Underlying Neural Rhythms in the Weakly Electric Fish, Apteronotus leptorhynchus. Brain Behav Evol. 2000;55(3):152–62. 10.1159/000006649 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Salazar VL, Stoddard PK. Sex differences in energetic costs explain sexual dimorphism in the circadian rhythm modulation of the electrocommunication signal of the gymnotiform fish Brachyhypopomus pinnicaudatus. J Exp Biol. 2008. March 15;211(6):1012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Anderson RO, Gutreuter SJ, Nielsen L, Johnson D. Fisheries techniques In: Fish Farmin, Handbook. American Fisheries Society, Bethesda Maryland. Brown; 1983. p. 237–337. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Sink TD, Lochmann RT, Fecteau KA. Validation, use, and disadvantages of enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay kits for detection of cortisol in channel catfish, largemouth bass, red pacu, and golden shiners. Fish Physiol Biochem. 2008. March;34(1):95–101. 10.1007/s10695-007-9150-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Hammer Ø, Harper DA, Ryan PD. PAST: Paleontological statistics software package for education and data analysis. Palaeontol Electron. 2001;4(1):art 4. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Zuur A, Ieno EN, Smith GM. Analyzing Ecological Data. Springer; 2007. 686 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.McLeod A, Xu C. bestglm: Best Subset GLM. 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.McCullagh P, Nelder JA. Generalized linear models. 2nd ed London New York: Chapmann and Hall; 1989. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Conover WJ. Practical Nonparametric Statistics. USA: John Wiley & Sons, Inc; 1971. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Peters R. The ecological implications of body size [Internet]. Cambridge University Press; 1983. [cited 2019 Nov 12]. Available from: https://scholar.google.com/scholar?hl=en&as_sdt=0%2C5&q=Peters+RH.+1983.+The+ecological+implications+of+body+size.+Cambridge+University+Press%2C+USA&btnG= [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Brown JH, Gillooly JF, Allen AP, Savage VM, West GB. Toward a metabolic theory of ecology. Ecology. 2004. July 1;85(7):1771–89. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Woodward G, Ebenman B, Emmerson M, Montoya JM, Olesen JM, Valido A, et al. Body size in ecological networks. Trends Ecol Evol. 2005. July 1;20(7):402–9. 10.1016/j.tree.2005.04.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.White EP, Ernest SKM, Kerkhoff AJ, Enquist BJ. Relationships between body size and abundance in ecology. Trends Ecol Evol. 2007. June 1;22(6):323–30. 10.1016/j.tree.2007.03.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Allen CR, Garmestani AS, Havlicek TD, Marquet PA, Peterson GD, Restrepo C, et al. Patterns in body mass distributions: sifting among alternative hypotheses. Ecol Lett. 2006. May 1;9(5):630–43. 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2006.00902.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Violle C, Enquist BJ, McGill BJ, Jiang L, Albert CH, Hulshof C, et al. The return of the variance: intraspecific variability in community ecology. Trends Ecol Evol. 2012. April 1;27(4):244–52. 10.1016/j.tree.2011.11.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.La VT, Cooke SJ. Advancing the Science and Practice of Fish Kill Investigations. Rev Fish Sci. 2011. January 3;19(1):21–33. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Crampton WGR. Effects of anoxia on the distribution, respiratory strategies and electric signal diversity of gymnotiform fishes. J Fish Biol. 1998;53(sA):307–30. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Moraes G, Avilez IM, Altran AE, Barbosa CC. Biochemical and hematological responses of the banded knife fish Gymnotus carapo (Linnaeus, 1758) exposed to environmental hypoxia. Braz J Biol. 2002;62:633–40. 10.1590/s1519-69842002000400011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Markham MR, Ban Y, McCauley AG, Maltby R. Energetics of Sensing and Communication in Electric Fish: A Blessing and a Curse in the Anthropocene? Integr Comp Biol. 2016;56(5):889–900. 10.1093/icb/icw104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Osório R, Rosa IL, Cabral H. Territorial defence by the Brazilian damsel Stegastes fuscus (Teleostei: Pomacentridae). J Fish Biol. 2006. July 1;69(1):233–42. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Börger L, Dalziel BD, Fryxell JM. Are there general mechanisms of animal home range behaviour? A review and prospects for future research. Ecol Lett. 2008. June 1;11(6):637–50. 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2008.01182.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Gavassa S, Silva AC, Stoddard PK. Tight hormonal phenotypic integration ensures honesty of the electric signal of male and female Brachyhypopomus gauderio. Horm Behav. 2011. September 1;60(4):420–6. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.07.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Pedraja F, Perrone R, Silva A, Budelli R. Passive and active electroreception during agonistic encounters in the weakly electric fish Gymnotus omarorum. Bioinspir Biomim. 2016. October 21;11(6):065002 10.1088/1748-3190/11/6/065002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Caputi A, Budelli R. The electric image in weakly electric fish: I. A data-based model of waveform generation inGymnotus carapo. J Comput Neurosci. 1995. June 1;2(2):131–47. 10.1007/BF00961884 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Caputi AA, Budelli R. Peripheral electrosensory imaging by weakly electric fish. J Comp Physiol A. 2006. February 25;192(6):587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Gavassa S, Silva AC, Gonzalez E, Stoddard PK. Signal modulation as a mechanism for handicap disposal. Anim Behav. 2012. April 1;83(4):935–44. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.01.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.West-Eberhard MJ. Sexual Selection, Social Competition, and Speciation. Q Rev Biol. 1983. June 1;58(2):155–83. [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Tobias JA, Montgomerie R, Lyon BE. The evolution of female ornaments and weaponry: social selection, sexual selection and ecological competition. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2012. August 19;367(1600):2274–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Whittingham LA, Kirkconnell A, Ratcliffe LM. Differences in Song and Sexual Dimorphism between Cuban and North American Red-Winged Blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus). The Auk. 1992;109(4):928–33. [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Tobias JA, Gamarra-Toledo V, García-Olaechea D, Pulgarín PC, Seddon N. Year-round resource defence and the evolution of male and female song in suboscine birds: social armaments are mutual ornaments. J Evol Biol. 2011. October 1;24(10):2118–38. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2011.02345.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Odreitz U, Sefc KM. Territorial competition and the evolutionary loss of sexual size dimorphism. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2015. April 1;69(4):593–601. 10.1007/s00265-014-1870-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.McEwen BS, Wingfield JC. The concept of allostasis in biology and biomedicine. Horm Behav. 2003. January 1;43(1):2–15. 10.1016/s0018-506x(02)00024-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Hirschenhauser K, Oliveira RF. Social modulation of androgens in male vertebrates: meta-analyses of the challenge hypothesis. Anim Behav. 2006. February 1;71(2):265–77. [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Albert DJ, Jonik RH, Walsh ML. Hormone-dependent aggression in female rats: Testosterone implants attenuate the decline in aggression following ovariectomy. Physiol Behav. 1990. April 1;47(4):659–64. 10.1016/0031-9384(90)90074-e [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Rendon NM, Amez AC, Proffitt MR, Bauserman ER, Demas GE. Aggressive behaviours track transitions in seasonal phenotypes of female Siberian hamsters. Funct Ecol. 2017. May 1;31(5):1071–81. 10.1111/1365-2435.12816 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Scaia MF, Morandini L, Noguera C, Trudeau VL, Somoza GM, Pandolfi M. Can estrogens be considered as key elements of the challenge hypothesis? The case of intrasexual aggression in a cichlid fish. Physiol Behav. 2018. October 1;194:481–90. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.06.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Kohda M, Shibata J, Awata S, Gomagano D, Takeyama T, Hori M, et al. Niche differentiation depends on body size in a cichlid fish: a model system of a community structured according to size regularities. J Anim Ecol. 2008. September 1;77(5):859–68. 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2008.01414.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Melissa J Coleman

17 Mar 2020

PONE-D-20-02253

Seasonal and social factors associated with spacing in a wild territorial electric fish

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Silva,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Unfortunately the second reviewer did not return the review after being given extra time to respond. Please respond to the revisions to the one reviewer. I will send the revisions to one additional reviewer at that point in time. Sorry for the delay.

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by May 01 2020 11:59PM. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Melissa J. Coleman

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements:

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at http://www.plosone.org/attachments/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and http://www.plosone.org/attachments/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. In your Methods section, please provide additional information regarding the permits you obtained for the work. Please ensure you have included the full name of the authority that approved the field site access and, if no permits were required, a brief statement explaining why.

3. To comply with PLOS ONE submissions requirements, please provide methods of sacrifice in the Methods section of your manuscript.

4. We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For more information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially sensitive information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors examine the physio-chemical, hormonal, behavioral and morphological correlates of territory size in a natural population of weakly electric fish. They conclude that territory size varies with oxygen concentration, season, hormonal/gonadal state (in breeding females) and body size (in non-breeding fish). Finally, fish show different affilation seasonally based on sex. This is an impressive data set, one that is rare in teleost fish and unprecedented in electric fish. The data appear carefully collected and statistically analyzed appropriately. The authors test in the field certain relationships that have been thoroughly documented in the laboratory. This paper makes a significant extention of behavioral physiology in the real world of the fish. It will be of great interest to the electric fish community, but also to a broad range of researchers that try to link behavioral physiology with ecology.

Major suggestions:

1. One highlighted conclusion of the paper is that territory size is sexually monomorphic in the non-breeding season, but dimorphic in the breeding season, with females having larger territory sizes than males in the breeding season. The authors speculate that this larger territory size corresponds to a the greater energetic demands of breeding females.

However, this sexual difference only applies when they examine body size-corrected territory size (DNN/body length). Several issues:

a) the authors should be more explicit about why they used body-size corrected territory size for this comparison, but not for other analyses. If it is important to size correct in this comparison, wouldn’t it be important in the general statistical analyses?

b) It is not clear why they corrected for body size using body length rather than body mass. (They collected both these measurements.) Presumably, energetic demands (e.g. metabolic rate) is a function of body mass, not length. They could even analyze the gonad-free body mass as well as the whole body mass to see if part of the apparent seasonal change in territory size is attributable to the relatively large gonads of females during the breeding season. Or even assess whether DNN correlated to absolute size in female to see if there is evidence that the added mass of a large gonad necessitates a larger territory.

c) In looking at the population means for DNN and body length (Table 1), it looks like the dimorphism in size-corrected DNN arises more from the fact that mean female body size changes less (~32%) seasonally than mean male body size changes (~41%)… rather than sexual differences in the seasonal change in DNN. That is, the seasonal change in this ratio is driven more by the denominator than the numerator. In this regard, the seasonal change may be more about demographic changes that behavioral/physiological changes. This, then, begs the question of why mean body size changes seasonally (see below) and why the change in mean body size differs between sexes

2) Because one of the main conclusions (size corrected sex-differences in territorial behavior) depends on seasonal changes in mean body length, the authors should provide some speculation about why this occurs. What happens to the large fish in the non-breeding season? Do they die? emigrate? Conversely, do the large fish drive out the small fish from the study area during the breeding season?

3) The correlation of territory size with 02 concentration is very interesting. Indeed, it is remarkable that fish can be larger in body size, discharge at high rates, respire at high temperature, defend larger territories and grow their gonads--- all very metabolically expensive -- when many of them exist in waters that are <2mg/L O2. But, again, why was DNN not size-corrected, since O2 requirements depend on body mass? Also, why was O2 concentration not simply incorporated as a covariate in the generalized linear model? It would also be interesting to know if EOD rate or GSI (two metabolically expensive traits) correlated with O2 concentration.

4) Finally, it would help to state if there were any correlations based on the position of the territory within the field site. For example, were territories on the edge of the mat of plants different in size or O2 concentration? Was one sex more likely to inhabit “edge” territories?

Minor comments:

Line 144 & 396: The authors synonomize DNN and territory size, given that DNN is a “proxy” for territory size. Please provide a reference that DNN (a linear measurement) is widely recognized as a reliable predictor of territory area. One could easily imagine that a fish might affiliate closely with another fish (small DNN) but still defend a large territory area.

Line 268: I am don’t understand how the population mean of DNN in the non-breeding season could be 1.4m when the mean for males is 1.2 and for females is 1.13 (Table 1). Also the sample sizes in line 268 are different that those in Table 1. Is this a different data set? Also, it would be helpful to put the mean size-corrected DNN here, since it would show the size-corrected DNN changes less seasonally than absolute DNN because mean body size increases seasonally.

Line 304: It would be helpful to include the correlational structure in E2, GSI and EOD rate, given that all three correlate significantly with DNN. Do relatively large gonads indeed predict high E2 levels? Is there experimental evidence in Gymnotus that E2 causally reduces EOD rate?

Line 319 (Table 1) it might also be useful to include raw values of EOD rate, since this would give the reader a sense of how much less active (in the EOD sense) the fish are in the non-breeding season, and thereby require smaller territory for foraging.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2020 Jun 15;15(6):e0228976. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0228976.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


6 Apr 2020

Response to Editor and Reviewer comments are all included in the document ''Response to Reviewers'', and some general aspects in the document ''Cover letter''.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers_Zubizarreta et al.docx

Decision Letter 1

Melissa J Coleman

8 May 2020

PONE-D-20-02253R1

Seasonal and social factors associated with spacing in a wild territorial electric fish

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Silva,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Both reviewers had some suggestions for editing the writing. 

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by Jun 22 2020 11:59PM. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Melissa J. Coleman

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors have thoroughly addressed all previous comments. They have done an excellent job reanalyzing portions of their data sets, clarifying their analyses, and revising their manuscript in response.

I have only a few remaining editorial suggestions, listed below.

Line 70: “body size”

Line 80 “By contrast”

Line 110, comma before “which”

Line 164, no need to use parentheses here

Line 165, “through two electrodes lowered into the vicinity of the animal and connected to an amplifier…”

Line 169, probably no need to define Q10 here.

Line 177, “was carried out during both the breeding and non-breeding season”

Line 185, “Netting requires…”

Line 193 “Dissected gonads were …”

Line 218: Data analysis was carried out in three steps.

Line 226: “”… whether individual morphometric and physiological traits correlated with DNN…

Line 230: “Because body weight and length were…”

Line 244: “… and considered up to second…”

Line 247 “two”

Line 263 “… its sex confirmed”

Line 320 “The second step of the analysis explored…”

Line 327 “Mean adult body size was significantly larger in the breeding season than non-breeding season.”

Line 445: Move citation forward. That is “… to analyzed interspecific interactions (66), “

Line 459 “… production (70), which can be….”

Line 496: “… body size influence habitat….” (eliminate ‘on’)

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Decision Letter 2

Melissa J Coleman

26 May 2020

Seasonal and social factors associated with spacing in a wild territorial electric fish

PONE-D-20-02253R2

Dear Dr. Silva,

We are pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it complies with all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you will receive an e-mail containing information on the amendments required prior to publication. When all required modifications have been addressed, you will receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will proceed to our production department and be scheduled for publication.

Shortly after the formal acceptance letter is sent, an invoice for payment will follow. To ensure an efficient production and billing process, please log into Editorial Manager at https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the "Update My Information" link at the top of the page, and update your user information. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, you must inform our press team as soon as possible and no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

With kind regards,

Melissa J. Coleman

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Melissa J Coleman

29 May 2020

PONE-D-20-02253R2

Seasonal and social factors associated with spacing in a wild territorial electric fish

Dear Dr. Silva:

I am pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

For any other questions or concerns, please email plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE.

With kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Melissa J. Coleman

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers_Zubizarreta et al.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers_second revision.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    Raw data are available from the https://osf.io/dashboard database Seasonal and social factors associated with spacing in a wild territorial electric fish (DOI 10.17605/OSF.IO/65D8A).


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES